Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Feb 25.
Published in final edited form as: Biol Psychol. 2012 Feb 25;90(1):80–87. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.02.013

Heart Rate Responses to Parental Behavior in Depressed Adolescents

Nicholas B Allen 1, Peter Kuppens 2, Lisa Sheeber 3
PMCID: PMC3934559  NIHMSID: NIHMS366250  PMID: 22391522

Abstract

In order to more fully understand the abnormalities in emotional responding associated with adolescent depression we examined clinically depressed and non-depressed adolescents’ physiological responses to their parents’ negative emotional behavior, as indexed by their heart rate responses to parental angry and dysphoric behavior during laboratory-based interactions. Maternal angry and dysphoric behavior predicted heart rate deceleration amongst non-depressed adolescents, a response that was not observed in depressed adolescents. Fathers’ angry behavior predicted significant heart rate acceleration in depressed (but not non-depressed) adolescents, whereas fathers’ dysphoric behavior predicted heart rate deceleration amongst depressed but not amongst non-depressed adolescents. These findings are interpreted within the framework of orienting and defense cardiac responses, and suggest that reactivity in adolescent depression is characterized by the absence of a normative orienting response towards aversive maternal behaviors, and a defensive physiological response to aggressive paternal behavior.

Keywords: Depression, Adolescence, Parent-child interactions, Psychophysiology, Behavioral observation

Introduction

The prevalence of depressive symptoms and disorder increases greatly during adolescence, a developmental period of marked changes in affective functioning (Allen & Sheeber, 2008). Indeed, depression is frequently conceptualized as a disorder of affect or affect regulation (Cole & Kaslow, 1988; Davidson, Pizzagalli, Nitschke, & Putnam 2002; Gross & Muñoz, 1995), which presumably has considerable significance for the way depressed people respond to interpersonal environments. Specifically, depressed persons experience mood states characterized by low levels of positive affects (PA) and high levels of negative affects (NA; see Watson, 2000), and demonstrate poor regulation of emotions as assessed at neurological, physiological, and behavioral levels (Garber, Dodge, & Shantz, 2007; Gross & Muñoz, 1995; Johnstone, van Reekum, Urry, Kalin & Davidson, 2007; Tomarken & Keener, 1998). Notably these abnormalities also characterise depressive disorders during childhood and adolescence (Cole & Kaslow, 1988; Joiner, Catanzaro & Laurent, 1996; Sheeber, Allen, Davis & Sorensen, 2000; Silk, Steinberg & Morris, 2003). However, one critical issue is the relationship between heightened depressed mood and alterations in reactivity to social and other affective stimuli, especially during adolescence when developmental changes in affective functioning can interact with personal vulnerabilities to result in the emergence of depressive disorders (Allen & Sheeber, 2008).

The conventional view is that mood states potentiate similar emotional reactions, and inhibit incompatible ones. In the case of depression, this would suggest that those with high levels of depressed mood are likely to have stronger emotional reactions to sadness-inducing stimuli, and weaker emotional reactions to pleasant stimuli. Some studies in our labs (though not all, cf. Sheeber et al., 2000) have demonstrated that higher levels of adolescent depressive symptoms are associated with greater adolescent reciprocity of mothers’ dysphoric and aggressive affect (Yap, Allen, O’Shea, DiParsia, Simmons, & Sheeber, 2011), supporting the notion that depression is associated with greater reactivity to aversive social stimuli.

Conversely, other recent studies have provided evidence of depressive hypo-reactivity to sadness-inducing stimuli. Rottenberg (2005), synthesizing results from a series of studies demonstrating that depressed individuals were less emotionally reactive to sadness-inducing stimuli, proposed that clinical depression is characterized by Emotion Context Insensitivity (ECI). The ECI model predicts that, despite the high levels of negative mood experienced by depressed persons, emotional reactivity (including responses to normatively sadness-inducing stimuli) are blunted during clinically depressed states. Indeed, the ECI model has been supported by a meta-analysis that concluded that depressed individuals evidenced reduced emotional reactivity to both positive and depressive stimuli (Bylsma, Morris, & Rottenberg, 2007).

Most functional or evolutionary views of depression have conjectured that depressed states are defensive reactions to threats to social wellbeing, such as defeat and humiliation (Price, Sloman, Gardner, Gilbert, & Rohde, 1994) or rejection (Ingram, Miranda, & Segal, 1998; see Allen & Badcock 2003, for a review). These views suggest that depressed states will be associated with enhanced reactivity to personally relevant stimuli associated with social threats (e.g., interpersonal conflict or rejection), but not to stimuli depicting threats that are non-social in nature (e.g., physical threats), or even social threats that are not directed at the self (e.g., conflict or rejection directed at another person). As such, studies that have used either putatively “depression-relevant” stimuli that depict sadness (such as movies portraying people grieving the loss of a loved one; e.g., Rottenberg, Kasch, Gross & Gotlib, 2002), or stimuli depicting physical threats (e.g., Allen, Trinder, & Brennan, 1999; Dichter et al., 2004) may not be utilizing the types of affective stimuli to which depressed states most strongly alter reactivity.

The present study

In this investigation, we examined adolescent reactivity in response to aversive (i.e., dysphoric and angry) interpersonal behaviors displayed by the adolescents’ parents during parent-adolescent interactions. Several considerations lead us to select aversive parent behavior as exemplars of the type of stimuli to which depressed persons may be differentially reactive. First, family conflict peaks during adolescence (Paikoff & Brooks-Gunn, 1991), with parent-child disagreements being not only more frequent than before, but also more intense than those that adolescents experience in peer relationships (Laursen & Collins, 1994). Second, threats to social well-being are richly represented in the types of family environments known to play an important role in the aetiology and maintenance of depressed states (Butzlaff & Hooley, 1998; Sheeber, Hops, Alpert, Davis & Andrews, 1997; Stice, Ragan & Randall, 2004). In particular, family environments characterized by harsh and conflictual family interactions are associated with depressive symptoms and disorder during adolescence (e.g., Ge et al., 1994; Patton et al., 2008; Sheeber et al., 2007). It is notable in this regard that family relationships appear to be more predictive of depressive symptoms in adolescence than are peer relationships (Barrera & Garrison-Jones, 1992; Stice, Ragan & Randall, 2004). The family environment thus appears to provide the type of personally relevant stimulus context in which instances of altered emotional reactivity may be observed in depressed persons.

Heart Rate as an Index of Reactivity to the Interpersonal Environment

In order to track adolescents’ reactivity to the interpersonal environment in real time we examined their psychophysiological responses during interactions with their parents. Though we often use behavioral data to index emotional reactivity (e.g., Yap et al., 2011; Sheeber et al., 2000), psychophysiological data have important advantages. In contrast to behavioral expression, which may be regulated or suppressed due to social constraints and display rules (e.g., Gross, 1998), psychophysiological responses can provide a more direct index of reactivity that may bypass the regulatory controls exerted on overt behavior. Physiological indices, moreover, are able to capture more subtle or fleeting changes that may escape behavioral observation.

We selected heart rate change as the specific psychophysiological measure to examine in this investigation as it can be measured unobtrusively during interactions, allowing a relatively natural range of movement and non-verbal behavior. Also, the psychological concomitants of changes in heart rate are relatively well understood. In particular, deceleratory and acceleratory heart rate responses have been explicated within the theoretical framework of orienting (OR) and defense (DR) responses, respectively (Cook & Turpin, 1997).

The OR, or “what is it?” response, involves shifting attention towards a salient stimulus. It is distinguishable from the DR, also referred to as the “self-defense reflex”, which involves heightened emotional arousal and bodily shifts that turn receptor organs away from the stimulus (Sokolov & Cacioppo, 1997). In general, HR deceleration is understood to be related to information intake and orienting, while HR acceleration is associated with information rejection and defense (Berntson, Boysen & Cacioppo, 1992; Cook & Turpin, 1997; Graham, 1992). As such, phasic changes in heart rate in response to changing circumstances during social interactions can be used to index a person’s responses in terms of attentional orienting and information intake versus heightened emotional arousal geared towards self-defense, with consequent information rejection.

We hypothesised that in the personally relevant context of family interactions, depressed adolescents’ responses to their parents’ negative interpersonal behavior, as indexed by their heart rate responses to parental angry and dysphoric behavior during interactions, will be different from those of their non-depressed peers. Specifically, we hypothesized that depressed adolescents’ heart rate responses to parental anger would be more likely to reflect a defensive response, consistent with the (socially) threatening nature of such behavior. In contrast, we predicted that non-depressed adolescents would be more likely to show orienting responses to their parent’s angry behaviors, consistent with an adaptive recognition of the potential salience of these behaviors, and a non-defensive attending to the information that these behaviors might convey. In other words, we predicted that the depressed adolescents would experience greater heart rate acceleration, indicative of heightened emotional arousal and defensive reactions to angry parental behavior, whereas non-depressed adolescents would show greater heart rate deceleration, indicative of non-defensive orienting. Further we hypothesized that these differences would not be observed in reaction to parental dysphoria, as dysphoric behavior does not convey threats to the adolescent’s own social well being as directly as does parental aggression. Accordingly we predicted that both depressed and non-depressed adolescents would evidence a deceleratory heart rate response to these behaviors, suggesting non-defensive attending.

Finally, one of the strengths of the current data set is the inclusion of both fathers and mothers in the majority of the interactions. Given that few studies have examined differential responses to fathers and mothers within these interactions, we primarily examined this issue in an exploratory fashion. Nevertheless, we were guided by previous findings demonstrating that harsh parenting by fathers is more strongly associated with adolescent depression than is harsh parenting by mothers (Sheeber et al., 2007), and that dysphoric behavior by depressed adolescents appears to be negatively reinforced by its ability to suppress fathers’ aggressive behavior (Sheeber et al., 1998). Accordingly we tentatively hypothesized that fathers’ anger would be especially salient and distressing to depressed adolescents, and would be associated with a defensive cardiac acceleration in this group.

Methods

Participants

Participants were 141 adolescents (47 boys), aged 14 to 18, and their parents. To be included in the investigation, adolescents had to live with at least one parent or permanent guardian, and meet research criteria for placement in one of two groups. Depressed adolescents met DSM IV (APA, 2000) diagnostic criteria for a current unipolar depressive disorder during a diagnostic interview. Consistent with guidelines for establishing the offset of depressive episodes, a diagnosis was considered current if it was ongoing or had an offset within two months preceding the diagnostic interview (APA, 2000); using this definition, three of the depressed adolescents were in partial remission at the time of the assessment and were excluded from further analyses, leaving a final sample size of 138 (Depressed, n = 69 or Healthy, n= 69). Healthy adolescents had no current or lifetime history of psychopathology, and no history of mental health treatment. Adolescents were excluded if they evidenced comorbid externalizing or substance dependence disorders, were taking medications with known cardiac effects, or reported regular nicotine use.

Recruitment and Assessment Measures and Procedures

Families were recruited using a two-gate procedure consisting of an in-school screening and an in-home diagnostic interview. In order to facilitate recruitment of a representative sample of students, we used a combined passive parental consent and active student assent protocol for the school screening (Biglan & Ary, 1990; Severson & Ary, 1983). Active parent consent and adolescent assent for the full assessment were obtained prior to the diagnostic interview.

Depression Screener

The CES-D is a widely used, self-report measure of depressive symptoms that has acceptable psychometric properties for use with adolescents (e.g., Roberts et al., 1990). It has a well-established record of use as a screener for depressive symptoms in adolescent samples (e.g., Asarnow, Jaycox, Duan, LaBorde, Rea, Murray, et al., 2005; Sheeber et al., 2007). As described above the CES-D was used as the initial gate of a two-stage recruitment and screening procedure.

Diagnostic Interview

The K-SADS interview was conducted with the adolescents to obtain current and lifetime diagnoses. Interviewers, who were bachelor-and masters-level research staff, participated in a rigorous training program and demonstrated agreement with a senior interviewer (κ ≥ .80) on at least two interviews before conducting independent interviews. All interview-derived diagnoses were confirmed by supervisors who reviewed both item-endorsement and interviewers’ notes. Questions regarding the accuracy of diagnoses were resolved based upon discussion with the interviewer and review of the audiotaped interview as needed. Reliability ratings were obtained on approximately 20% of the interviews, chosen at random. The average agreement was κ = .94.

School screening

Students from area high schools completed the Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977) and a demographic data form during class. Approximately 70% of enrolled students who were living with at least one parent or permanent guardian participated (4182 of 5975), 12% (n=695) declined or had parents decline their participation and 18% (n=1098) were absent or off campus on the day of the assessment. CES-D cut-off scores for selecting potential participants were based on the distribution of scores obtained in an earlier screening of high school students (N= 4495) in the same area (Sheeber et al., 2007). Relatively high scores (≥ 31 for males and ≥ 38 for females) were selected to maximize the positive predictive power to identify adolescents experiencing depressive disorder. Approximately 8% (n=372) of the students scored above these cut-offs. The pool for the healthy group was defined as students not more than ½ SD above the mean in the earlier sample (< 21 for males and < 24 for females).

Diagnostic assessment

Interviewers conducted the Schedule of Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia-Children’s Version (K-SADS, Orvaschel and Puig-Antich, 1994) with adolescents who had elevated CES-D scores. Subsequent to the interviews, families of adolescents who met criteria for MDD were invited to participate in the lab-based assessment. After each adolescent in the depressed group completed the laboratory assessment, a healthy, demographically matched comparison participant was recruited from the pool of students who scored within the normal range on the CES-D.

Approximately 9% (n=52) of families contacted by phone were not eligible to participate as per the criteria described above (e.g., due to medicine regimen; moved out of family home). Of families invited to participate, approximately 26% (n=131) declined. Rates of decline did not vary as a function of pre-interview group status (i.e., elevated or healthy CES-D score), age, or race, though more males than females declined (31.6% vs. 23%), χ2 (1, n=498)=4.57, p<.05. Of adolescents with elevated CES-D scores who participated in the interview, 38% (n=81) met criteria for MDD. Of these, 13.9% (n=10) had a comorbid anxiety disorder and were retained in the MDD group given the high rate of comorbidity between mood and anxiety disorders. Five individuals were excluded due to psychotic diagnoses (mania or schizophrenia). Of adolescents with CES-D scores in the healthy range, approximately 76% (n=84) met criteria for inclusion.

Lab assessment

Families who met criteria for the investigation after the diagnostic interview were invited to participate in the lab assessment. Approximately 4% (n=7) of families declined. The decline rate did not vary as a function of group status, age, race, or gender. Additionally, 11 participants were excluded from this report due to missing physiological data. In approximately 93% of two-parent families, both parents participated in the assessments. The average time between the diagnostic assessment and the lab assessment was 33.2 days (SD=20.1; no between group differences).

The lab assessment included two family interaction tasks. Each task lasted 18 minutes, evenly divided across two discussions. The first task consisted of two consecutive problem-solving interactions in which families were asked to discuss and resolve two areas of conflict. In the second task, families were asked to discuss two areas of family life; one focused on identifying and describing the best and most difficult years the adolescent had experienced, and the other focused on the most challenging and most rewarding aspects of parenting the adolescent. Interactions were video recorded for subsequent behavioral coding. Although we have found that these tasks do preferentially elicit angry and dysphoric interactive behavior respectively (Sheeber et al., in press), each task has the capacity to elicit a wide range of affective behaviors, and as such data from the two tasks were combined in the present analyses in order to allow greater number of examples of each affective behavior to be included in the analyses.

In addition, measures of adolescent cardiac physiology were obtained during the interactions. Participants were instructed to abstain from alcohol and illicit drugs on the day of the assessment. Compliance with this instruction was confirmed on the day of the assessment via self-report.

Physiological Measures and Procedures

Heart-rate data were acquired using software and equipment from the James Long Company (www.jameslong.net). The ECG signals were recorded using Ag-AgCl electrodes. To record the ECG signal, we used a three-lead system to maximize the r-wave amplitude and minimize movement artefact and t-wave amplitude. The positive electrode was placed directly under the left armpit about 4 to 6 inches down, at heart elevation. The negative electrode was placed directly under the right armpit at the same elevation. The ground electrode was placed on the sternum, halfway between the positive and negative electrodes. The ECG signals were input to an isolated bioelectric amplifier custom, which amplified the signals with a gain of 250 and bandpass frequencies of 0.1 – 1000 Hz.

The ECGRWAVE program from the James Long Company identified r-waves from the ECG signal with an automated, multiple-pass, self-scaling algorithm. These signals were then visually inspected and manually corrected for missed or misrepresented r-waves, and sections of movement or noise artifact were removed. Overall, this accounted for only 0.5% (in seconds) of the total data that had to be marked and removed as artifact. Mean heart rate values were derived for each one-second epoch, based on a weighted average of the heart rate values associated with each of the inter-beat intervals (IBI) to fall either fully or partially within the epoch, with each of these intervals being weighted according to the time proportion of the epoch that each IBI constituted.

Behavioral observations

The Living in Family Environments coding system (LIFE; Hops, Biglan, Tolman, Arthur, & Longoria, 1995) was used to code parent behavior during the video-recorded family interactions. The LIFE is an event-based, microanalytic coding system in which a new code is entered each time there is a change in a participant’s verbal content or affective behavior. As such, duration of each behavior can be calculated as the time between onset of one code and onset of the next code, for each person in the interaction. Each entry is comprised of several components which identify the: a) target (i.e. whose behavior is being coded); b) verbal content; and c) nonverbal (or para-verbal) affect. These micro-level data are then combined into mutually exclusive constructs, which are operationalized as particular combinations of content and affect codes (Hops, Davis, and Longoria, 1995). Three binary constructs, angry, dysphoric, and happy were derived from individual affect and content codes (with 1 indicating presence of the behavior and 0 otherwise). The angry and dysphoric constructs were used in this report. Angry behavior included aggressive (e.g., raised voice; clenched teeth) or contemptuous (e.g., eye rolling; sneering) nonverbal behavior and cruel (e.g., mocking; insults; threats) or provoking (e.g., taunts; dares) statements. Dysphoric behavior was defined by sad nonverbal behavior (e.g., tearfulness, sighing) or complaining statements.

Extensively trained observers, with over a decade of experience, coded the parents’ nonverbal affect and verbal content of verbal statements. Observers were blind to diagnostic status. Approximately 25% of the videos were coded by an additional observer for reliability Kappas for mothers and fathers angry and dysphoric behavior ranged from .60-.64, which reflect good agreement (Fleiss, 1981; Landis & Koch, 1977). The validity of the LIFE system as a measure of family processes has been established in numerous studies of adolescent depression (e.g., Katz & Hunter, 2007; Sheeber et al., 2007).

Data Analysis

The data involve concurrent information on depressed or non-depressed adolescents’ heart rate and their parents’ behavior across time. The data require an analytical strategy that takes into account the dependencies in the data created by the nested structure (second-by-second observations nested in persons) and that allows examination of between group differences in the influence of parental behavior on adolescent heart rate (HR). To accomplish this, we used a crosscorrelation-autocorrelation approach embedded in a multilevel modeling framework (for a similar approach, see e.g., Gottman, 1990; Moberly & Watkins, 2008), using HLM software (Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992). The crosscorrelation-autocorrelation examines how change in one variable can be explained by another variable, controlling for the variable’s autocorrelation, and has been proposed to be the closest one can come to inferring causal relationships based on multivariate time series data (see e.g. Gottman, 1990; Granger, 1969). The multilevel or hierarchical linear modeling framework takes into account the nested data structure and facilitates the examination of the moderating role of depression (e.g., Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992; Snijders & Bosker, 1999). Specifically, we performed analyses in which at level 1 of the model adolescents’ HR at time t was predicted by the mother’s or father’s dysphoric and angry behavior at time t-5 seconds, controlling for the adolescents’ HR at time t-5 seconds. For instance, when using maternal behavior as predictor, the level -1 model equation was:

HRt,i=π0i+π1i(HRt5s,i)+π2i(Anger mothert5s,i)+π3i(Dysphoria mothert5s,i)+eti

with i indicating participant. All level 1 predictors were group-mean centered, and intercepts and slopes were allowed to vary randomly across participants (given by the index i). The crucial parameters in the equation are π2i and π3i, reflecting individual differences in reactivity of the adolescent’s HR towards angry and dysphoric behaviour by the mother. As can be seen from the equations, the intercept and slope values in the equation are allowed to vary randomly across participants. At level two, we examined how these parameters differed for depressed and non-depressed adolescents by including two binary variables, one indicating depression and one indicating non-depression, omitting the intercept:

π0i=β00Depressedi+β01NonDepressedi+ε0π1i=β10Depressedi+β11NonDepressedi+ε1π2i=β20Depressedi+β21NonDepressedi+ε2π3i=β30Depressedi+β31NonDepressedi+ε3

Of primary interest in these equations, β20 and β21 reflect HR reactivity of depressed and non-depressed participants towards angry behavior by the mother, and β30 and β31 reflect HR reactivity of depressed and non-depressed participants towards dysphoric behavior by the mother. χ2 hypothesis tests were used to determine the difference between depressed and non-depressed1. Because both parents were not always present during the interactions (see below), separate analyses for mothers’ and fathers’ behavior were performed.2

The crosscorrelation-autocorrelation approach thus allowed to examine how a parents’ behavior at one point in time is associated with a change in the adolescent’s physiology at a following point in time, and how this association was moderated by depressive status. We used a 5 second time-lag as previous research has documented that the difference between the orienting (deceleratory) and defense (acceleratory) HR response is most strongly observed in this time frame following the eliciting stimulus (e.g., Sokolov & Cacioppo, 1997).

Results

Table 1 reports the average adolescent HR, average proportion of time that each maternal and paternal behavior was displayed during the interaction, the average frequency of each parental behavior, and the average duration that each behavior lasted, as a function of adolescent depressive status. The results show that the average HR of depressed adolescents was higher than that of non-depressed adolescents during the interactions. Moreover, it showed that both fathers and mothers in depressed families displayed more, more frequent, and longer angry (but not dysphoric) behavior during the interactions.

Table 1.

Average adolescent heart rate and average proportion, frequency, and duration of maternal and paternal angry and dysphoric behavior during the interaction as a function of adolescent depressive status

Variable Depressed Mean (SD) Non-depressed Mean (SD) t(df) p
Adolescent Heart Rate 78.80 (10.42) 75.16 (10.22) 2.07 (136) .040
Proportion of duration of parental behavior
 Mother anger .12 (.09) .07 (.08) 3.29 (132) .001
 Mother dysphoria .15 (.13) .17 (.15) -0.99 (132) .33
 Father anger .11 (.15) .04 (.06) 3.04(97) .003
 Father dysphoria .16 (.12) .15 (.14) 0.39 (97) .69
Frequency of parental behavior
 Mother anger 26.94 (16.47) 18.48 (14.84) 3.11 (131) .002
 Mother dysphoria 36.54 (16.36) 41.19 (16.11) -1.61 (131) .11
 Father anger 18.14 (17.76) 10.41 (11.52) 2.57 (94) .01
 Father dysphoria 40.77 (16.08) 37.11 (16.28) 1.10 (94) .27
Duration in seconds
 Mother anger 7.5 (4.1) 5.9 (3.4) 2.36 (131) .02
 Mother dysphoria 8.4 (5.0) 8.5 (5.4) -0.18 (131) .86
 Father anger 7.7 (6.9) 4.8 (3.8) 2.59 (94) .01
 Father dysphoria 8.6 (4.5) 9.5 (8.0) -.58 (94) .56

Responses to maternal behavior

In the first analysis, we examined the role of the mothers’ behavior (Table 1). This analysis was based on 134 participants for whom the mother participated in the interaction task.3 The first two lines of Table 2 contain the intercepts (average HR) for depressed and non-depressed and show that depressed adolescents demonstrate elevated average HR relative to non-depressed adolescents during the interactions (as we’ve shown in a previous report, Byrne et al., 2010). Lines 3-4 show that HR is positively and similarly autocorrelated for all participants.

Table 2.

Hierarchical model predicting adolescent HR by dysphoric and anger behavior of the mother at t-5s controlling for HR autocorrelation at t-5s (level 1) as a function of depression (level 2)

Level 1 Level 2
Group B (fixed effect) SE T-ratio χ2D-ND Random effect (parameter SD)
Intercept D 78.97 1.32 59.61*** 5.14* 10.64***
ND 74.83 1.26 59.54***
HR t-5 sec D 0.22 0.02 9.25*** 0.52 0.19***
ND 0.20 0.02 8.78***
Anger mother t-5 sec D 0.17 0.18 0.93 4.74* 1.31***
ND -0.42 0.20 -2.11*
Dysphoria mother t-5 sec D -0.06 0.17 -0.32 5.54* 1.12***
ND -0.56 0.13 -4.39***

Note. χ2D-ND : χ2-test of difference between depressed and non-depressed.

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01,

***

p < .001.

Level 1 SD = 7.59.

Findings central to our research question, are presented in the bottom half of the table, and indicate significantly different patterns of reactivity between depressed and non-depressed adolescents. In particular, both angry and dysphoric behaviors by the mother were associated with a decrease in the HR of non-depressed adolescents only (the difference between non-depressed participants’ HR response to angry and dysphoric behavior was non-significant, χ2=0.83, p>.50). As an indication of effect size, the reported coefficients indicate that a 1SD increase in maternal negative behavior elicited close to half an SD decrease in HR (a change of approx. 4 bpm). In contrast, depressed adolescents showed no change in heart rate in response to maternal behavior, which was moreover significantly different from the response of non-depressed adolescents; the difference between depressed participants’ HR response to angry and dysphoric behavior was non-significant, χ2=0.34, p>.50.

Response to paternal behavior

Second, we evaluated the role of the fathers’ behavior in a parallel model, based on 99 families in which the father participated in the interaction task.4 The results, shown in Table 3, again indicate that depressed adolescents showed elevated average levels of HR relative to non-depressed adolescents. Heart rate was again positively and similarly autocorrelated for all participants.

Table 3.

Hierarchical model predicting adolescent HR by dysphoric and anger behavior of the father at t-5s controlling for HR autocorrelation at t-5s (level 1) as a function of depression (level 2)

Level 1 Level 2
Group B (fixed effect) SE T-ratio χ2D-ND Random effect (parameter SD)
Intercept D 80.41 1.50 53.75*** 7.91*** 9.87***
ND 74.84 1.30 57.65
HR t-5 sec D 0.25 0.03 8.69*** 0.94 0.19***
ND 0.21 0.03 8.12***
Anger father t-5s D 0.64 0.29 2.18* 6.88*** 1.58***
ND -0.40 0.27 -1.50
Dysphoria father t- 5s D -0.59 0.16 -3.68*** 4.08* 0.96***
ND -0.15 0.15 -0.98

Note. χ2D-ND : χ2-test of difference between depressed and non-depressed.

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01,

***

p < .001.

Level 1 SD = 7.36.

Concerning our central research question, significant between group differences in HR reactivity to fathers’ negative affective behavior were observed. Depressed adolescents displayed an increase in HR in response to fathers’ angry behavior, and a decrease in HR in response fathers’ dysphoric behavior (the difference between depressed participants’ HR response to angry and dysphoric behavior being significant, χ2=16.90, p<.001). In terms of effect size, the reported coefficients indicate that a change of 1SD in paternal angry behavior elicited on average a .64 SD (approx. 5bpm) increase in HR, while dysphoric behavior elicited a similar decrease in HR. Conversely, non-depressed adolescents showed no change in heart rate in response to either type of paternal behavior (the difference between non-depressed participants’ response to the two behaviors being non-significant, χ2=0.71, p>.50).5

Discussion

These findings demonstrate that clinically depressed and non-depressed adolescents show different patterns of heart-rate reactivity to parental dysphoric and angry behavior. Consistent with predictions, both maternal angry and dysphoric behavior were associated with heart-rate deceleration amongst non-depressed adolescents. Importantly, this was not observed in depressed adolescents. Neither, however, did we observe the hypothesized acceleration amongst depressed adolescents to angry maternal affective behavior. Responses to paternal affective behavior were more variable. Fathers’ angry behavior predicted significant heart-rate acceleration in depressed (but not non-depressed) adolescents. By contrast, paternal dysphoric behavior predicted heart-rate deceleration amongst depressed (but not amongst non-depressed) adolescents.

Viewed in light of the literature on the determinants of heart rate acceleration and deceleration in response to stimuli (Berntson et al., 1992; Cook & Turpin, 1997; Graham, 1992; Vila et al., 2007), these findings suggest that both dysphoric and angry maternal behaviors are salient interpersonal cues that strongly elicit an orienting response in non-depressed adolescents. This is consistent with other recent research that has demonstrated cardiac deceleration to social cues in non-clinical groups (e.g., social rejection; Gunther Moor, Crone & Van der Molen, in press). However, this response was absent in the depressed group. Whether this absence of response represents the habituation of a defensive response, as has been shown in studies of repeated presentation of aversive stimuli in high fear participants (Klorman et al., 1977), or the overlapping influence of simultaneous orienting and defense responses (i.e., with different modes of cardiac control essentially cancelling each other out; Vila et al., 1997) remains to be clarified. One argument, against the habituation explanation, however, is that parallel findings were obtained for reactivity to dysphoric and angry behaviors, though there were no group differences in the amount of dysphoric behavior displayed by mothers. We should note, however, that as the LIFE coding system does not capture intensity of behavior, it is feasible that the groups differed on the intensity of maternal dysphoric behavior, which could contribute to differences in adolescent reactivity. Although another possibile explanation for the blunted cardiac response patterns observed in the depressed group is reduced autonomic flexibility, which has previously been proposed to be a feature of anxiety and depressive disorders (Friedman, 2007), we consider this less likely as reduced heart rate variability and vagal tone, the proposed mechanism for this reduced autonomic flexibility, does not differentiate the depressed and non-depressed groups in this sample (Byrne et al., 2010).

The finding with regard to reactivity to paternal angry affect was consistent with our hypothesis that depressed participants would respond with heart-rate acceleration, indicating increased emotional arousal and a defensive stress response. It is intriguing that this increased reactivity was specific to angry behaviors displayed by fathers. Although both angry and dysphoric behaviors are potentially aversive to others, as noted above, they have somewhat different interpersonal implications and elicit different responses (Biglan et al., 1989). In particular, angry behavior reflects a clear threat signal (Blanchard & Blanchard, 2009), whereas this is not the case for dysphoric behavior.

Given the higher probability of physical aggression in men (Archer, 2004), it is therefore possible that paternal anger (albeit in this context reflecting verbal and expressive aggression rather than physical aggression) is more immediately threatening compared to such behavior by the mother. Indeed, the appearance of a threat response to parental angry behavior amongst depressed teenagers is interesting in light of evidence that harsh parenting by fathers is more strongly associated with adolescent depressive status than is harsh parenting by mothers (Sheeber et al., 2007). As noted earlier, fathers of depressed adolescents displayed more angry behavior and it is possible that depressed adolescents have been sensitized to the greater extent or intensity (which we didn’t directly examine here) of paternal anger. Furthermore, in previous work, we have reported that dysphoric behavior by depressed adolescents appears to be negatively reinforced by its ability to suppress fathers’ aggressive behavior (Sheeber et al, 1998; but cf. Slesnick & Waldron, 1997). To the extent that dysphoric behavior is, in part, a self-protective response to perceived threat in the father-child relationship, heart-rate acceleration may represent a psychophysiological concomitant of this behavioral response.

In turn, the finding of heart deceleration in depressed (but not non-depressed) adolescents in response to fathers’ dysphoric behavior was unexpected, and suggests that depressed participants were orienting more to this behavior than were the non-depressed participants. Combined, the results thus indicate that dysphoric paternal behavior elicits emotional orienting in depressed adolescents, whereas paternal angry behavior elicits defensive responding, with both these responses absent in non-depressed adolescents.

The differential elicitation of orienting and defense responses by the same aversive stimuli in different types of individuals has also been observed in studies of individual differences in fear. For example, Klorman, Weissberg and Wiesenfeld (1977) found that participants reporting low mutilation fear showed a deceleratory cardiac response to mutilation pictures (i.e. an orienting response), whereas participant high in mutilation fear showed an acceleratory response (i.e., a defense response). This finding, along with similar ones in the literature (see Cook & Turpin, 1997 for a review) suggests that individual differences in how fear prone an individual is to the specific class of aversive stimuli strongly moderates cardiac response patterns.

The findings of the current study are consistent with the interpretation that non-depressed adolescents respond to aversive (i.e., both angry and dysphoric) social behavior by mothers with attentional focus and information intake, whereas the depressed group lack this response. Moreover, in responding to paternal anger depressed adolescents show signs of a defensive physiological response that suggests autonomic arousal and preparation for action (e.g., fight or flight responses) more than attentional engagement. Further extending these ideas suggests that non-depressed participants might be thought of as processing aversive maternal behavior in a way that is analogous to the way that persons who have a low level of fearfulness process a potentially fear relevant stimulus (i.e., with an orienting response), whereas depressed participants responded to paternal anger in a way analogous to persons with a high fear response (cardiac acceleration; see Cook & Turpin, 1997; Klorman et al., 1977).

Given the importance of successfully negotiating aversive behaviors in interpersonal contexts, these findings may not only provide evidence of enhanced reactivity to personally relevant stimuli, but may also point towards processes that may contribute to dysfunctional family relationships amongst the families of depressed adolescents. If depressed adolescents are unable to process important interpersonal cues adequately, this may result in maladaptive behavioral responses that serve to escalate, rather than deescalate, conflict within family environments. Importantly, this study documents findings that are both consistent with the ECI model of emotion in depression (Rottenberg, 2005), as well as some potentially important exceptions. The lack of reactivity to maternal behavior observed in depressed adolescents is consistent with the notion that they are under-reactive to some aversive interpersonal behaviors, whereas the response to angry paternal behavior suggests that they may be over reactive to other such behaviors. This latter finding suggests, consistent with some evolutionary and interpersonal models of depression (e.g., Allen & Badcock, 2003; Price, Sloman, Gardner, Gilbert, & Rohde, 1994), that given the significance of interpersonal defeat and rejection for depressed states, affective reactivity in depression is especially enhanced to interpersonally aggressive rather than sadness inducing stimuli per se.

Although this study had a number of significant strengths, including the integration of behavioral and physiological data within personally and etiologically relevant social interactions, and the selection of a large clinically depressed cohort, there are also some important limitations that need to be considered. The reliance on behavioral and physiological data alone requires us to speculate as to the way in which participants interpreted environmental stimuli. Although self-report of such interpretations also suffers from limitations, and is difficult to implement in the context of dynamic naturalistic interactions, there are methodologies such as video mediated recall (Gottman & Levenson, 1985; Lorber, 2007) that can be useful in this regard. Indeed, we have recently have recently published findings using this procedure with a different cohort indicating that depressed adolescents do over-report parental aggressive affect and underreport parental happy and neutral affects when compared to neutral observers (Ehrmantrout, Allen, Leve, Davis & Sheeber, 2011). The integration of such techniques into future studies using behavioural and physiological data may help to provide an extra dimension of interpretation to the response patterns observed in this study. This is also important in light of the fact that heart rate acceleration cannot be unambiguously interpreted as a DR given that it has also been shown to associated with positive incentives (Fowles, Fisher & Tranel, 1982), active coping (Light, 1981) and challenge (as compared to threat; Blascovich, Mendes, Hunter, Lickel & Kowai-Bell, 2001). Although the context in which cardiac acceleration was observed in this study (i.e., in response to angry paternal behavior) renders an appetitive interpretation of the response unlikely, nevertheless some other form of data with which to confirm the interpretation that this response likely reflects a DR would enhance confidence in the inferences made.

Another important caveat is that the absence of a psychiatric control group means that we cannot be certain the extent to which these responses characterize depression specifically, as opposed to emotional disorders, or psychopathology, in general. Indeed, we did allow comorbidity with anxiety disorder in the depressed group, although only 10 depressed participants (13.9%) had a comorbid anxiety disorder, making it very unlikely that these participants alone were responsible for the observed effects. Furthermore, separate analyses of this group would have very low power. Nevertheless, given the well established fact that depressed samples have higher levels of anxious symptoms than healthy groups, establishing the differential association of anxious and depressive symptoms with effects observed here remains an important avenue for future research. Another limitation is that although most of interactions examined were triadic (i.e., mother, father and adolescent were all present) we have not examined the influence of the joint pattern of mothers’ and fathers’ behavior on adolescents (e.g., whether consistent or inconsistent affective behavior between the parents moderates the adolescents’ responses). Analysis of these issues goes beyond the scope of the current manuscript but remains an important topic for future research. Finally, given the delay between the diagnostic assessment and the laboratory assessment, it is possible that some depressed participants, despite still meeting diagnostic criteria for a depressive episode, may have experienced a reduction in the severity of their depressive symptoms. Future studies would do well to re-assess depressive symptoms on the day of the laboratory assessment to address this caveat.

In sum, our findings suggest that the cardiac reactivity of depressed adolescents to interpersonal behavior during family interactions is primarily characterized by the absence of a normative orienting response towards aversive maternal behaviors, along with some indications of defensive physiological responses to angry aversive paternal behavior specifically. This pattern combines both the idea of increased reactivity to socially threatening stimuli (in particular, paternal anger) with the possibility of under-reactivity (lack of orienting towards salient maternal behavior) in personally relevant stimulus contexts. As this is a cross-sectional study, we cannot determine the extent to which differences in depressed adolescents’ reactivity have developed consequent to differences in family interactional patterns associated with adolescent depression (e.g., Sheeber et al., 2007). To our knowledge, though, this study is among the first to provide such a detailed and direct examination of interpersonal reactivity in parent-child relationships in adolescent depression. Although more research is needed to pinpoint the precise processes that underlie the observed differential reactivity to paternal and maternal behavior, our findings emphasize the importance of taking into account differences between reactivity to the father and mother and the type (i.e., dysphoric versus angry) of their respective aversive behavior.

Research Highlights.

  • We examined clinically depressed and non-depressed adolescents heart rate responses to their parents’ negative emotional behavior.

  • Maternal angry and dysphoric behavior predicted heart rate deceleration amongst non-depressed adolescents, a response that was not observed in depressed adolescents.

  • Fathers’ angry behavior predicted significant heart rate acceleration in depressed (but not non-depressed) adolescents.

  • These findings suggest that reactivity in adolescent depression is characterized by the absence of a normative orienting response towards aversive maternal behaviors, and a defensive physiological response to aggressive paternal behavior.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by NIMH grant R01 MH65340 (PI Sheeber) and KULeuven Research Council Grant GOA/05/04 and OT/11/031. Nicholas Allen is partially supported by a grant from the Colonial Foundation. We gratefully acknowledge the assistance of Michelle Byrne in the processing of the psychophysiological data presented in this study.

Footnotes

1

This is not the case when formulating a model that includes an intercept term and only one dummy variable, which yields more difficult to interpret relative regression weights. Both models are mathematically equivalent, however, produce the same results, and can be used to extract identical information.

2

Because both father and mother were not always present during the interaction tasks, combined analyses including both maternal and paternal behavior results in a large loss of participants. For this reason, separate analyses were performed and reported. It is important to note, however, that a combined analyses (based on 95 participants for which both father and mother were present) yields similar results and supports similar conclusions as the separate results reported here.

3

The mother was absent for 3 of the depressed and 1 of the non-depressed participants (the difference being non-significant, χ2(df=1) = 1.03, p > .05), leaving a sample of 66 depressed and 68 non-depressed adolescents whose mother participated. The families with and without mothers did not differ with respect to adolescent HR (t(df=136) = -0.43, p > .05), paternal anger (t(df=97) = -0.48, p > .05), or paternal dysphoria (t(df=97) = -0.26, p > .05).

4

The father was absent for 24 of the depressed and 15 of the non-depressed participants (the difference being non-significant, χ2(df=1) = 2.90, p > .05), leaving a sample of 45 depressed and 54 non-depressed participants whose father participated. The families with and without fathers did not differ with respect to adolescent HR (t(df=136) = -1.03, p > .05), maternal anger (t(df=132) = 0.36, p > .05), or maternal dysphoria (t(df=132) = -1.06, p > .05).

5

We also performed all analyses adding gender (next to depressive status) as a predictor of HR reactivity at level 2 of the models, and analyses adding gender, depressive status and the interaction between both as predictors of HR reactivity. The findings showed that none of the gender main effects nor gender × depressive status interaction effects on the reactivity of HR to parental behavior were significant (all ps > .05)

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Contributor Information

Nicholas B. Allen, University of Melbourne

Peter Kuppens, University of Leuven and University of Melbourne.

Lisa Sheeber, Oregon Research Institute.

References

  1. Allen NB, Badcock PBT. The social risk hypothesis of depressed mood: Evolutionary, psychosocial, and neurobiological perspectives. Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129:887–913. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.6.887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Allen NB, Badcock PBT. Darwinian models of depression: A review of evolutionary accounts of mood and mood disorders. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 2006;30:815–626. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Allen NB, Trinder J, Brennan C. Affective startle modulation in clinical depression: Preliminary findings. Biological Psychiatry. 1999;46:542–550. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00025-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Allen NB, Sheeber LB. The importance of affective development for the emergence of depressive disorder during adolescence. In: Allen NB, Sheeber LB, editors. Adolescent emotional development and the emergence of depressive disorders. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  5. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4. Washington, DC: Author; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  6. Archer J. Sex differences in aggression in real-world settings: A meta-analytic review. Review of General Psychology. 2004;8:291–322. [Google Scholar]
  7. Asarnow JR, Jaycox LH, Duan N, LaBorde AP, Rea MM, Murray P, et al. Effectiveness of a quality improvement intervention for adolescent depression in primary care clinics: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2005;293:311–393. doi: 10.1001/jama.293.3.311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Barrera M, Jr, Garrison-Jones C. Family and peer social support as specific correlates of adolescent depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1992;20:1–16. doi: 10.1007/BF00927113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Berntson G, Boysen S, Cacioppo J. Cardiac orienting and defensive responses: Potential origins in autonomic space. In: Campbell BA, Hayne H, Richardson R, editors. Attention and information processing in infants and adults: Perspectives from human and animal research. LEA; Hillsdale NJ: 1992. pp. 163–200. [Google Scholar]
  10. Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC. Aggression (biological perspectives) In: Sander D, Scherer KR, editors. The Oxford companion to emotion and the affective sciences. New York: Oxford University Press; 2009. pp. 21–22. [Google Scholar]
  11. Biglan A, Ary DV. Methodological issues in research on smoking prevention. In: Bell CS, Battjes R, editors. Prevention research: Deterring drug abuse among children and adolescents. Rockville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services; 1990. pp. 170–195. NIDA Research Monograph Series, No. 63. [Google Scholar]
  12. Biglan A, Rothlind J, Hops H, Sherman L. Impact of distressed and aggressive behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1989;98:218–228. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.98.3.218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bryk AS, Raudenbush SW. Hierarchical linear models for social and behavioral research: Applications and data analysis methods. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  14. Blascovich J, Mendes WB, Hunter SB, Lickel B, Kowai-Bell N. Perceiver threat in social interactions with stigmatized others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;80:253–67. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bryk AS, Raudenbush SW. Hierarchical linear models for social and behavioural research: Applications and data analysis methods. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  16. Butzlaff AM, Hooley JM. Expressed emotion and psychiatric relapse. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1998;55:547–552. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.6.547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bylsma LM, Morris BH, Rottenberg J. A meta-analyses of emotional reactivity in major depressive disorder. Clinical Psychology Review. 2008;28:676–691. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Byrne M, Sheeber L, Simmons JG, Davis B, Shortt JW, Katz LF, Allen NB. Autonomic cardiac control in depressed adolescents. Depression and Anxiety. 2010;27:1050–1056. doi: 10.1002/da.20717. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cole PM, Kaslow NJ. Interactional and cognitive strategies for affect regulation: Developmental perspective on childhood depression. In: Alloy LB, editor. Cognitive processes in depression. New York: Guilford; 1988. pp. 310–343. [Google Scholar]
  20. Cook EC, III, Turpin G. Differentiating orienting, startle, and defense response: The role of affect and its implications for psychopathology. In: Lang PJ, Simons RF, Balaban M, editors. Attention and orienting Sensory and motivational processes. New Jersey: Lawrence, Erlbaum Associates Inc; 1997. pp. 137–164. [Google Scholar]
  21. Davidson RJ, Pizzagalli D, Nitschke JB, Putnam KM. Depression: Perspectives from affective neuroscience. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002;53:545–574. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dichter G, Tomarken A, Shelton R, Sutton S. Early- and late-onset startle modulation in unipolar depression. Psychophysiology. 2004;41:433–440. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.00162.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Ehrmantrout N, Allen NB, Leve C, Davis B, Sheeber L. Adolescent recognition of parental affect: Influence of depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2011;120:628–634. doi: 10.1037/a0022500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fleiss JL. Statistical methods for rates and proportions. 2. New York: Wiley; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  25. Fowles DC, Fisher AE, Tranel DT. The heart beats to reward: The effect of monetary incentive on heart rate. Psychophysiology. 1982;19:506–513. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1982.tb02577.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Friedman B. An autonomic flexibility–neurovisceral integration model of anxiety and cardiac vagal tone. Biological Psychology. 2007;74:185–199. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Garber J, Dodge KA, Shantz C. The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation. Cambridge University Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  28. Ge X, Conger RD, Lorenz FO, Simons RL. Parents’ stressful life events and adolescent depressed mood. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1994;35(1):28–44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gottman JM. Time-series analysis applied to physiological data. In: Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG, editors. Principles of Psychophysiology: Physical, Social and Inferential Elements. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1990. pp. 754–774. [Google Scholar]
  30. Gottman JM, Levenson RW. A valid procedure for obtaining self-report of affect in marital interaction. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1985;53:151–160. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.53.2.151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Graham FK. Attention: The heart beat, the blink, and the brain. In: Campbell BA, Hayne H, Richardson R, editors. Attention and information processing in infants and adults. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1992. pp. 3–29. [Google Scholar]
  32. Granger CWJ. Investigating causal relations by econometric models and cross-spectral methods. Econometrica. 1969;37:424–438. [Google Scholar]
  33. Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology. 1998;2:271–299. [Google Scholar]
  34. Gross JJ, Muñoz RF. Emotion regulation and mental health. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 1995;2:151–164. [Google Scholar]
  35. Gunther Moor B, Crone EA, van der Molen MW. The heart-brake of social rejection: Heart rate deceleration to unexpected peer rejection. Psychological Science. doi: 10.1177/0956797610379236. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Harrington R, Vostanis P. Longitudinal perspectives and affective disorder in children and adolescents. In: Goodyer IM, editor. The depressed child and adolescent: Developmental and clinical perspectives. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1995. pp. 311–341. [Google Scholar]
  37. Hops H, Biglan A, Tolman A, Arthur J, Longoria N. Living in Family Environments (LIFE) coding system: Manual for coders (Revised) Eugene, OR: Oregon Research Institute; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  38. Ingram RE, Miranda J, Segal ZV. Cognitive vulnerability to depression. New York: Guilford Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  39. Johnstone T, van Reekum CM, Urry HL, Kalin NH, Davidson RJ. Failure to regulate: Counter-productive recruitment of top-down prefrontal-subcortical circuitry in major depression. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:8877–8884. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2063-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Joiner TE, Jr, Catanzaro SJ, Laurent J. Tripartite structure of positive and negative affect, depression, and anxiety in child and adolescent psychiatric patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1996;105:401–409. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.105.3.401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Katz LF, Hunter EC. Maternal meta-emotion philosophy and adolescent depressive symptomatology. Social Development. 2007;16:343–360. [Google Scholar]
  42. Klorman R, Weissberg R, Wiesenfeld A. Individual differences in fear and autonomic reactions to affective stimulation. Psychophysiology. 1977;14(1):5–51. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1977.tb01154.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Landis JR, Koch GG. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics. 1977;33:159–174. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN. Motivated attention: Affect, activation, and action. In: Lang PJ, Simons RF, Balaban MT, editors. Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational processes. Mahwah, NJ: LEA; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  45. Laursen B, Collins WA. Interpersonal conflict during adolescence. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;115:197–209. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Light KC. Cardiovascular responses to effortful active coping: Implications for the role of stress in hypertension development. Psychophysiology. 1981;18:216–225. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb03021.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lorber MF. Validity of Video-Mediated Recall procedures for mother’s emotions and child ratings. Journal of Family Psychology. 2007;21:520–528. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.3.520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Moberly NJ, Watkins ER. Ruminative self-focus and negative affect: An Experience Sampling study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2008;117:314–323. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.2.314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Orvaschel H, Puig-Antich J. Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-epidemiologic version-5 (K-SADS-E-5) Nova Southeastern University; Ft. Lauderdale, Fl: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  50. Paikoff RL, Brooks-Gunn J. Do parent-child relationships change during puberty? Psychological Bulletin. 1991;110:47–66. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Patton GC, Olsson C, Bond L, Toumbourou JW, Carlin JB, Hempill S, Catalano RF. Predicting female depression across puberty: a two-nation longitudinal study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2008;47:1424–32. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181886ebe. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Price JS, Sloman L, Gardner R, Gilbert P, Rohde P. The social competition hypothesis of depression. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1994;164:309–135. doi: 10.1192/bjp.164.3.309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Roberts RE, Andrews JA, Lewinsohn PM, Hops H. Assessment of depression in adolescents using the center for epidemiologic studies depression scale. Psychological Assessment. 1990;2:122–128. [Google Scholar]
  54. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: A self report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
  55. Rottenberg J. Mood and emotion in major depression. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2005;14:167–170. [Google Scholar]
  56. Rottenberg J, Kasch KL, Gross JJ, Gotlib IH. Sadness and amusement reactivity differentially predict concurrent and prospective functioning in Major Depressive Disorder. Emotion. 2002;2:135–146. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.2.2.135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Severson HH, Ary DV. Sampling bias due to consent procedures with adolescents. Addictive Behaviors. 1983;8:433–437. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(83)90046-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Sheeber L, Allen N, Davis B, Sorensen E. Regulation of negative affect during mother-child problem-solving interactions: Adolescent depressive status and family processes. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2000;28:467–479. doi: 10.1023/a:1005135706799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Sheeber LB, Davis B, Leve C, Hops H, Tildesley E. Adolescents’ relationships with their mothers and fathers: Associations with depressive disorder and subdiagnostic symptomatology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2007;116:144–154. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sheeber L, Hops H, Alpert A, Davis B, Andrews J. Family support and conflict: prospective relations to adolescent depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1997;25:333–344. doi: 10.1023/a:1025768504415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Sheeber LB, Kuppens P, Shortt JW, Katz LF, Davis B, Allen NB. Depression is associated with escalation of adolescents’ dysphoric behavior during interactions with parents. Emotion. doi: 10.1037/a0025784. In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Silk JS, Steinberg L, Sheffield Morris A. Adolescents’ emotion regulation in daily life: Links to depressive symptoms and problem behavior. Child Development. 2003;74:1869–1880. doi: 10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00643.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Slesnick N, Waldron HB. Interpersonal problem-solving interactions of depressed adolescents and their parents. Journal of Family Psychology. 1997;11:234–245. [Google Scholar]
  64. Snijders TAB, Bosker RJ. Multilevel Analysis: An Introduction to Basic and Advanced Multilevel Modeling. London: Sage Publishers; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  65. Sokolov E, Cacioppo J. Orienting and defense reflexes: Vector coding the cardiac response. In: Lang PJ, Simons RF, Balaban M, editors. Attention and orienting Sensory and motivational processes. New Jersey: Lawrence, Erlbaum Associates Inc; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  66. Stice E, Ragan J, Randall P. Prospective relations between social support and depression: Differential direction of effects for parent and peer support? Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2004;113(1):155–159. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Tomarken AJ, Keener AD. Frontal brain symmetry and depression: A self-regulatory perspective. Cognition & Emotion. 1998;12:387–420. [Google Scholar]
  68. Vila J, Pérez MN, Fernández MC, Pegalajar J, Sánchez M. Attentional modulation of the cardiac defense response in humans. Psychophysiology. 1997;34:482–487. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02393.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Vila J, et al. Cardiac defense: from attention to action. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2007;66:169–82. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Watson D. Mood and temperament. New York: Guilford Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  71. Yap M, Allen NB, O’Shea M, Di Parsia P, Simmons JG, Sheeber L. Early adolescents’ temperament, emotion regulation during mother child interactions, and depressive symptoms. Development and Psychopathology. 2011;23:267–282. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES