Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Feb 26.
Published in final edited form as: J Adolesc Health. 2013 Feb;52(2 0 2):S22–S32. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.05.007

Biological Contributions to Addictions in Adolescents and Adults: Prevention, Treatment and Policy Implications

Marc N Potenza 1
PMCID: PMC3935152  NIHMSID: NIHMS382908  PMID: 23332567

Abstract

Purpose

Despite significant advances in our understanding of the biological bases of addictions, these disorders continue to represent a huge public health burden that is associated with substantial personal suffering. Efforts to target addictions require consideration of how the improved biological understanding of addictions may lead to improved prevention, treatment and policy initiatives.

Method

In this article, we provide a narrative review of current biological models for addictions with a goal of placing existing data and theories within a translational and developmental framework targeting the advancement of prevention, treatment and policy strategies.

Results

Data regarding individual differences, intermediary phenotypes, and main and interactive influences of genetic and environmental contributions in the setting of developmental trajectories that may be influenced by addictive drugs or behavior indicate complex underpinnings of addictions.

Conclusions

Consideration and further elucidation of the biological etiologies of addictions hold significant potential for making important gains and reducing the public health impact of addictions.

Keywords: Addiction, Neurobiology, Adolescence, Public Health, Development

Introduction

Over the past several decades, substantial research has investigated the biological factors leading to and resulting from addictions [1, 2]. The findings emanating from this work are vitally important if we are to continue to make inroads against addictions, particularly with respect to improving prevention and treatment strategies [3]. Despite significant efforts, excessive patterns of alcohol, tobacco and other drugs have been estimated to cost the United States alone over $400 billion annually [4]. Worldwide, addictions are prevalent and low- and middle-income countries may not have the resources to adequately address these disorders [5, 6]. The impact of addictions typically is widespread, with some estimates indicating seven people being affected for each identified addicted individual, and there often exist substantial social consequences [7]. Addictions may influence employers as well as families, and the impact may be felt trans-generationally as parents with addictions may neglect children or model unhealthy behaviors [8]. Certain developmental groups, particularly adolescents and young adults, may be particularly vulnerable to developing addictions as specific brain regions, specifically those involved in exerting behavioral control, typically mature less rapidly than do brain regions involved in promoting motivated behaviors like substance use [9, 10]. Consistent with this notion, adolescents and young adults as compared to children and older adults have high rates of addictions [11]. As biological studies identify specific brain pathways and chemicals that may underlie specific aspects of addictions and addiction vulnerability [12], the knowledge gained holds significant potential to advance prevention, treatment and policy interventions.

The Boundaries of Addiction

Prior to embarking on a discussion of the biological factors contributing to addiction and addiction vulnerability, it is important to consider which disorders are encompassed by the term “addiction.” Historically, there has been variation in the application of the word. “Addiction” is derived from the Latin verb addicere meaning “bound to” or “enslaved by,” and in its original usage was not associated with substance use behaviors [13]. Dating back several hundred years, the term became linked to excessive patterns of alcohol use and later to excessive patterns of drug use such that by the 1980’s there was apparent consensus amongst some groups of experts that addiction could be defined as “compulsive drug use” [14]. However, over the past fifteen years or so, there has been debate as to whether excessive participation in non-drug behaviors like gambling, eating, sex, shopping, internet use and video gaming, to name several, might be considered addictions [15-17]. All of these domains appear to hold relevance to adolescents as rates of problem and pathological gambling have been estimated to be two-to-four-fold higher in adolescents than adults, problematic gambling, shopping, internet use have each been associated with adverse measures of health and functioning in adolescents, and obesity rates have risen dramatically in youth over the past several decades [17-22]. Additionally, these behaviors may follow developmental frequencies similar to substance use behaviors, with high rates of use and addiction in adolescence and early adulthood and lower rates in older adulthood [11, 23]. Amongst adolescents, it also appears important to consider levels of engagement that fall short of addiction as subsyndromal engagement has been associated with immediate and longer-term adverse measures of health and functioning [18, 24, 25]. The unique characteristics of adolescents as compared to adults (e.g., more likely to have school as compared to work be a primary emphasis, more likely to be influenced by parental monitoring, less likely to have head-of-household obligations, less likely to seek treatment for these behaviors, and less likely to have large sums of money to support engagement in addictive behaviors) also warrant consideration and may explain some differences in frequencies (e.g., with respect to compulsive shopping behaviors) in adolescents versus adults [26, 27].

The debate over what behaviors, and the level of engagement in such behaviors, might be considered as addictions has involved consideration of the core components of addiction [23, 28]. One proposition is that central features of addiction include continued engagement in a behavior despite adverse consequences, diminished control over participation in the behavior, compulsive participation, and a craving or appetitive urge state immediately preceding engagement in the behavior [23, 29]. If one adopts these features as the defining aspects of addiction, then non-substance behaviors like gambling might be considered within an addictions framework. Consistent with this notion, pathological gambling is being proposed for categorization together with substance use disorders in a “Substance Use and Addictive Disorders” category in DSM-5 [30]. Such a definition for addiction and such groupings could substantially increase the estimated costs of addictions to society. For example, if foods and food consumption might be considered addictive [31], the costs to society could increase tremendously given the high prevalence estimates of obesity and the associated health costs related to type 2 diabetes, hypertension, heart disease and other obesity-related conditions [32]. The consumption of energy drinks and other caffeinated beverages may also be considered within an addiction framework, and this may be particularly relevant to adolescents given their patterns of consumption of these drinks [33].

It should be noted that while many of the common substances of abuse (tobacco, alcohol, and cannabis amongst the most common, with a recent rise in prescription medication abuse in some countries like the US) show patterns of initiation and escalation of use during adolescence, food consumption follows a different pattern. That being said, many of the features that might converge to make adolescents vulnerable to addiction (maturation and associated hormonal and other biological changes, greater independence, greater access to addictive substances/materials, emerging non-addiction psychopathology) many represent factors associated with altered eating behaviors and obesity.

Biological Models of Addiction

Multiple biological models have been proposed to understand addictions and addiction vulnerability, and many of these models are complementary and not mutually exclusive. As an extensive review of each of these models is beyond the scope of this manuscript, interested readers are directed to the references cited for additional aspects of each model. Additionally, theories of addiction as related to current neurobiological understandings are reviewed in chapters 2-5 of [34].

Early reward-centric models focused on pleasurable aspects of taking drugs and proposed that drugs may “hijack” brain circuits involved in responses to “natural” rewards like sex or food [35, 36]. A central component in this circuitry is the nucleus accumbens located in the ventral striatum and receiving dopaminergic innervation from the ventral tegmental area (termed the mesolimbic dopamine system). This nucleus accumbens has at times been termed the brain’s “reward center” given that all known drugs with abuse potential, as well as natural rewards, lead to dopamine release in this structure [37, 38]. However, a broader range of neurotransmitters (including opioids, cannabinoids, serotonin, norepinephrine, acetyl-choline, glutamate, and gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) [39-41]) contributes to addiction, and molecular entities (receptors, transporters) for sensing these neurotransmitters are expressed in specific brain regions (e.g., see figure 1 in [41] for details). Recent studies suggest that the functions of the nucleus accumbens and dopamine function therein are more complex and involve learning (particularly reward-based) and reward anticipation and valuation, salience attribution (that is, assigning degrees of relevance to items, decisions or behaviors), as well as loss processing [1, 36]. Consistent with a role for rewarding effects of drugs in addictive processes and a role for dopamine in this process, an incentive salience model of drug addiction proposes that “liking” a drug may be separated from “wanting” [42, 43]. Another reward-based model suggests a “reward deficiency syndrome” in which individuals with addictions seek out and engage in addictive behaviors to compensate for hypo-functioning reward signals in the mesolimbic dopamine pathway [44]. In contrast to the incentive salience model, the reward deficiency model may be particularly relevant to self-medication theories of addiction [45, 46]. Despite their differences, these two models share some theoretically consistency with other motivational theories like the “IRISA” (impaired response inhibition and salience attribution) and others detailed below that describe behavioral and biological differences in transitions from initial, sporadic to regular, habitual use of drugs [2, 47, 48]. Specific brain regions or circuits may be particularly important in different aspects of addiction – for example, the mesolimbic dopamine system has been proposed to be particularly relevant to initiation and binge/intoxication, noradrenergic and stress pathways particularly relevant to withdrawal and negative affect components, and prefrontal cortical, hippocampal, amygdalar and insular contributions particularly relevant to preoccupation and craving (see figure 2 in [41] for details).

Motivation-focused models have proposed that addiction might be considered a disorder of misdirected motivation in which relatively greater priority is given to drug use and relatively lesser priority is given to other motivated behaviors like familial care, work or school [49-51]. In these processes, decisions to pursue typically smaller, immediate rewards (e.g., a drug-related high) at the expense of typically larger, delayed rewards (e.g., longer term life possibilities emanating from studying for an exam or taking children to school). These behaviors and choices may be modeled from a biological perspective and specific brain regions including the ventral striatum and ventromedial prefrontal cortex appear closely linked to reward processing, risk/reward decision-making and the selection of smaller immediate rewards, whereas the selection of larger, delayed rewards has been found to recruit more dorsal prefrontal cortical brain regions [52-54]. These findings suggest that more developed brain regions involved in higher-order (so-called executive) processes are important in risk-reward decision-making relevant to addictions [2]. From a developmental perspective, these prefrontal cortical brain regions are amongst the last to mature and this feature of brain development may in part contribute to adolescent vulnerability to addictions and other risk behaviors and mental health disorders (see figure 1 in [9]) [50, 55, 56].

Adolescent Addiction Vulnerability

Other motivation-focused models have proposed arguably more extensive involvement of brain regions whose functions may contribute to motivated behaviors, addiction vulnerability and addictions. One model focusing on adolescent vulnerability to addiction separated primary and secondary motivational neurocircuitry (see figure 1 in [50]). The primary circuitry involves the prefrontal cortex, striatum (including the caudate and putamen), and thalamus. Parallel loops involving these structures have been proposed as primary to motivations and behaviors, including those in addictions [57, 58]. The limbic loops that involve more ventral regions of cortex and striatum have been proposed to be particularly relevant to novel or impulsive behaviors whereas the associative and sensorimotor loops that involve more dorsal regions of cortex and striatum have been proposed to be particularly relevant to habitual or compulsive behaviors [59, 60]. This model, as well as others, appears applicable to both substance and non-substance addictions, including behaviors related to excessive food intake and obesity [16, 60, 61].

A secondary motivational neurocircuitry has been proposed to explain how other brain circuits may influence motivational decision-making processes and behaviors within the primary circuitry [50]. Specifically, multiple factors (both external influences like parental monitoring, peer behavior and access to drugs or addictive materials, as well as internal states, all of which are particularly relevant to adolescents) may influence decisions to use drugs or engage in addictive behaviors [62]. Both internal and external influences may be relevant to adolescents’ initiation and continued engagement in addictive behaviors. For example, one’s emotional state may contribute, and periods of feeling upset or stressed may lead to drug use [63, 64]. As such, brain regions involved in emotional processing, including the amygdala and anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortices, may provide important information into primary motivational circuitry and contribute to decisions to use drugs in emotionally reactive “hot” states as compared to reflective, “cold” states [63-65].

Given the relative immaturity in adolescents of brain regions like the prefrontal cortex involved in emotional and motivational processing including in the regulation of craving for drugs and food [66, 67], adolescents may be biologically vulnerable to engagement in addictive behaviors. Consistent with this notion, adolescents show largely subcortical/limbic responses to favorite food cues and individualized stress cues [68], whereas adults show both subcortical/limbic and prefrontal cortical responses [69, 70]. In biological models focusing specifically on adolescent addiction vulnerability [50], the function of brain regions contributing to other states (for example, relating to hunger, thirst or sex drive) relating to motivational drives and behaviors has been cited as important. For example, brain regions such as the hypothalamus and septum that are involved in these homeostatic processes may contribute importantly [50, 71, 72].

Personally relevant experiences may also influence motivations and decisions to use drugs, and in the setting of relatively smaller contributions of prefrontal cortically mediated self-control in adolescence, such experiential recollection may play a relatively larger role in adolescent decision-making related to addictive behaviors. Brain regions such as the hippocampus or temporal cortices that have been implicated in storing and recalling memories, particularly emotional ones, related to prior drug use (or other relevant situations) may thus provide important contextual memory contributions [50, 73-75]. Other brain regions such as the insula (involved in sensing physical or somatic states) and parietal cortex (involved in attentional processing) may also participate by influencing motivations and decisions to engage in addictive behaviors [76-78]. The emotional volatility of adolescents and its influence on how attention is directed thus may contribute significantly to adolescent participation in substance use and other behaviors with addictive potential, and brain regions involved in emotionally processing (e.g., the amygdala) are important contributors [79]. Importantly, input from brain regions involved in higher order executive function (for example the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) may allow for “top-down” control over motivations, in part driven by “bottom-up” subcortical processes, to engage in addictive behaviors [2, 80]. Consistently, prefrontal cortical brain regions like the inferior frontal gyrus are amongst those most frequently implicated in studies of impaired impulse control [81] as well as in the control of craving or desire [66, 67]. Taken together, adolescents may not be able to regulate emotional or motivational states to the same degree as adults given their neurodevelopmental status.

The extent to which specific neurobiological or behavioral features described above reflect normal or aberrant development is currently incompletely understood, and it appears as if arguments for both cases could be made. Importantly, characteristics that are developmentally appropriate (e.g., increased risk-taking) are also associated with real-life measures of adverse functioning (including with respect to addictive behaviors) [82-84], and it follows that the neurobiological underpinnings would show a similar pattern. However, while some studies indicate that adolescents in general show increased reward-related and risk-taking-related responses [9, 85], other studies indicate that they show relatively diminished activation [86, 87]. Similarly, some studies indicate that adolescents with addictions as compared to those without show relatively diminished ventral striatal activation during reward anticipation, much like adults with addictions compared to those without [88-90]. Similar patterns of ventral striatal activation also appear to apply to risk-taking in adolescents and adults with addictions [91, 92]. Thus, although adolescents in general may show exaggerated reward- or risk-related brain activations in reward circuitry, it is possible that those who may be those showing relatively blunted activations who are most important to target with respect to addictions. However, other data suggest that features associated with substance abuse (e.g., externalizing tendencies) correlate positively with reward-anticipation-related activation of the ventral striatum in adolescence [93]. Some of these differences might reflect study design (e.g, with respect to conflating anticipatory phases of reward processing [94]), differences in samples (e.g., with respect to substance use or other measures [95]) or other factors. As described below, understanding the biological correlates of such individual differences represents a major area of research in that it might help advance individualized interventions. Despite these gaps in our understanding, given that some of the normative developmental features of adolescence may represent risk factors for addiction and adolescent engagement is associated with poorer outcome, navigation through this developmental epoch in a healthy fashion is important.

How Might Drug Use or the Addictive Process Influence Brain Structure and Function?

Using the above-described frameworks to consider the neurocircuitry involved in addictions, it is important to consider that changes may occur over time in the structure and function of these brain motivational pathways. Some changes may reflect normative developmental processes [55, 56, 96], some may reflect changes directly related to the addictive process [49, 97], and others may reflect changes related to recent or chronic substance exposure that may or may not be central to addictive processes [98, 99]. Models and studies have begun to examine these influences. One model posits that there are “allostatic” changes (that is, alterations in baseline set-points) that may occur upon repeated exposure to drugs or stressors [97, 100]. Such exposures may differentially influence specific neural structures, with initial involvement of the mesolimbic dopamine system progressing to the nucleus accumbens, prefrontal cortex and extended amygdala with continued drug exposure and increasing compulsivity (see figure 4 in [41]). Such progressive involvement of brain regions and their function in brain circuits may underlie a recalibration of baseline set-points in the functioning of motivational circuitry that could contribute importantly to repeated drug-taking and complicate attempts to cease engagement in addictive behaviors. A non-mutually exclusive possibility involves the formation of the progressive involvement of more dorsal cortico-striato-thalamo-cortical circuits as behavior moves from more consciously decision-oriented to more habitually driven with repeated engagement over time (see figures 1 in [59] and [60] and figures 1, 3 and 11 in [58]). How changes related to normal development (including the complex ones described above during adolescence) interact with behavioral engagement and substance use thus may involve complex interactions, particularly when one considers individual differences in genetic composition and life experiences and their interactive effects (see below). Such environmental influences may come from multiple domains salient to adolescents including parents, peers, school, church, and extra-curricular involvement, to list several, and may include positive pro-social influences and negative ones such as bullying or other forms of abuse.

Predisposing Factors Versus Sequelae of Use

Disentangling the influences of chronic and recent effects of specific drugs on brain structure and function in addiction can be complicated. That being said, drugs like cocaine appear to have significant influences on cortical structures, with repeated exposure progressively involving ventral to lateral to dorsal regions of prefrontal cortex [98]. Alcohol can also influence brain structure and function, and decreased gray matter and poorer white matter integrity have been found in individuals with alcoholism [101-104]. Amongst adolescents, both structural volumetric and white matter changes have been observed in association with one to two years of drinking alcohol, particularly with respect to binge-pattern drinking [105]. Both gray matter and white matter integrity are important to brain function, with the latter particularly relevant to how brain regions connect and therefore operate in conjunction with one another. Alcohol’s influences on gray and white matter structures may explain in part differences seen in performance on cognitive tests in groups of individuals with different addictions [106, 107]. However, longitudinal studies in people with carefully assessed measures of drug-taking behaviors will help further clarify to what extent differences may reflect characteristics (e.g., neurobiological features related to impulsive tendencies) existing prior to drug exposure, those relating to drug exposure, those relating more precisely to changes in the addictive process or a combination thereof (possibly evolving in an interactive fashion). Upon this framework it is important to consider developmental changes in brain structure and function that occur naturally as people age [9, 55, 56]. For example, in rats, exposure to alcohol during adolescence increases risky or impulsive decision-making in adulthood [108]. These findings suggest that if adolescents consume alcohol, such consumption may lead to tendencies promoting alcohol consumption, generating a vicious cycle of addictive behavior. However, controlled studies investigating such questions are lacking in humans. Thus, at this point in time, it is important to be cautious about inferring causality, particularly as many human studies involve associational rather than longitudinal designs.

Consideration of Individual Differences

It is reasonable to consider that certain factors (including individual differences in genetic composition and/or environmental exposures) may exist, develop or be experienced early in life, precede the exposure to addictive substances or engagement in addictive behaviors, and thus predispose to addiction vulnerability or resilience [109]. For example, there exists a genetic variation coding for an enzyme (acetylaldehyde dehydrogenase) involved in the metabolism of alcohol. Individuals with the variant that is associated with slower metabolism of alcohol and the accumulation of acetylaldehyde upon alcohol consumption (leading to an unpleasant or aversive response) are protected against the development of alcoholism. The ways in which other genetic differences may contribute to the development of addictions are arguably less clear. However, twin data suggest that 30% to 70% of the risk for developing addictions may be genetic in nature [110], suggesting that an improved understanding of specific genetic factors relating to addictions and addiction vulnerability is relevant [111].

Moreover, environmental factors may interact with genetic factors, and these are important to consideration in the stages of addictions [112-114]. That is, individuals with one type of genetic background may respond differently than individuals of another genetic background to the same environmental stimulus. Such gene-by-environment interactions have been suggested in brain imaging studies and may have relevance to addictions and other mental health conditions. For example, specific variants of the gene coding for the serotonin transporter that are associated with different functioning of the transporter protein are associated with differences in amygdala activation to emotional stimuli [115]. Given findings linking emotional dysregulation to addictive behaviors [116], amygdala function to motivation and addictions [50, 117, 118], and serotonin transporter gene variation relating to externalizing tendencies in youth as a function of socioeconomic status [119], such variations may in part explain how different individuals respond differently to environments with respect to developing addictions. Furthermore, the timing of exposure to specific environmental stimuli (for example, childhood trauma) should be considered within this framework as well as within the context of developmental brain changes [120]. This example is meant to reflect one of multiple possible genes and gene-environment interactions that may contribute to addictions and other conditions. As many genes with commonly occurring variations have been proposed to contribute to aspects of addiction [111, 112, 121], it will be important to examine a broad range of genetic and environmental factors relating to addiction vulnerability and resilience.

Sex/Gender

Other individual differences also warrant consideration. For example, males as compared to females tend to more frequently encounter problems with addiction, although the gender composition varies somewhat according to addictive substance or behavior and developmental stage. For example, alcohol and cocaine dependence and pathological gambling are typically male predominant whereas compulsive shopping is more typically identified in females across the lifespan [122]. However, some recent US data have found that girls ages 12-17 years have rates of alcohol and illicit drug abuse or dependence equal to or greater than those for boys [123, 124]. Despite these differences, some gender-related differences appear relatively consistent across disorders. For example, a telescoping phenomenon, initially described for alcoholism, later for drug use and more recently for gambling, exists whereby women as compared to men begin engagement in the behavior on average later in life than do men but the time between initial participation and development of a problem is shorter (or telescoped) in women as compared to men [125, 126]. Differences in motivations for engaging in addictive behaviors also exist between females and males, with women more likely to participate to escape from negative mood states (negative reinforcement) and men more likely to participate to experience positive feelings (positive reinforcement) [127-129]. These differences have important implications. First, they may relate to important differences in co-occurring disorders whereby addictive behaviors like gambling are more closely linked to depression in girls and women as compared to boys and men, respectively [130, 131]. Second, they suggest that differences exist in biological underpinnings of addictions in women and men, particularly with respect to responses to negative (stress/anxiety) and positive (addiction cue) responses. Consistently, as compared to same-sexed non-addicted comparison subjects, women with cocaine dependence show more robust patterns of brain activation differences in brain motivation circuitry in responses to stress cues and men with cocaine dependence show more robust patterns of brain activation differences in brain motivation circuitry in responses to drug cues [132]. Third, these findings have treatment implications as interventions like mindfulness-based approaches that target stress reduction might be differentially helpful for women and men with addictions [133]. The extent to which gender-related differences relate to biological sex hormones (e.g., progesterone, estrogens, testosterone) and/or environmental factors like gender-related differences in social acceptability of specific behaviors warrants additional investigation, as well as does how these might best be targeted in interventions [134-137].

Race, Culture and Ethnicity

Factors related to culture, race and ethnicity also warrant consideration in the propensity to develop addictions. Differences in genetic compositions may vary according to race and in part explain differences observed in rates of addictions across racial and ethnic groups [138, 139]. Environmental factors related to differences in acculturation, cultural expectations, socioeconomics, stress exposure and other domains also warrant consideration as these might differ across cultural groups [137, 140]. Some of these factors (e.g., stress exposure like childhood trauma) have been linked both to the propensity to develop addictions and to brain structure and function, including in regions implicated in reward, motivation and addictions [141, 142], although the precise natures of these relationships warrant further investigation in longitudinal studies in people [143-145]. As such, disentangling the precise contributions to addictions amongst different racial/ethnic groups is both an important and complex undertaking.

Intermediary Phenotypes

One important approach that has been employed for the past decade involves the study of intermediary phenotypes or endophenotypes [146]. This approach considers that multiple factors, including multiple gene variations, likely contribute to psychiatric disorders like addictions, and that these disorders represent heterogeneous groupings. Intermediary phenotypes or endophenotypes represent constructs that are not readily visible but represent measurable constructs that may more closely link to biological factors (and by extension their prevention and treatment) than do the heterogeneous diagnostic groupings. Endophenotypes also are proposed to be identifiable, albeit to a lesser extent, in unaffected family members of people with the disorder.

An example of an endophenotype that has been proposed for addictions and some other psychiatric disorders is impulsivity [2, 61, 111, 147]. Impulsivity has been defined as propensity towards rapid, unplanned reactions to internal or external stimuli with diminished regard to the negative consequences of these reactions to the impulsive individual or others [147, 148]. Animal studies involving controlled designs indicate that impulsivity prior to drug exposure can predict the propensity to develop drug addictions [2, 149, 150], and substance exposure (for example, alcohol exposure during adolescence) can lead to increased impulsivity [108]. Thus, these animal studies indicate that impulsivity may predispose to the development of addictions and may increase following chronic substance exposure, potentially leading to worsening addiction. Amongst people, impulsivity measures are higher not only in stimulant-dependent individuals but also in their siblings, consistent with the notion that impulsivity represents an endophenotype for addictions [151]. Additionally, these similarities appear to relate to neural regions that have been linked in prior studies to poor impulse control [152]. Better impulse control in children as young as four years of age has been associated with better scholastic functioning during adolescence as well as differences in prefrontal cortical and ventral striatal functioning during performance of an impulse control task as adults [153, 154].

Although these findings are encouraging and suggest that impulsivity may represent a relevant target for treatment development in addictions [3, 155-157], caution should be noted for several reasons.

First, impulsivity is a complex construct that can be fractionated, and components related to decision-making (choice impulsivity) and action (response impulsivity) have been identified in multiple studies [158-160]. Thus, facets of impulsivity may represent separate and dissociable endophenotypic constructs. Choice and response impulsivity often do not associate, may involve different neurochemical contributions, and appear to relate differentially to aspects of addiction [2, 157, 159, 161, 162]. For instance, while response impulsivity has been found in animal models to predict compulsive or addictive drug use [2, 149], choice impulsivity has not and has rather been associated with resistance to extinction and propensity to relapse [163]. As such, these aspects of impulsivity may relate specifically to different aspects of addictive processes (e.g., initiation versus persistence).

Similarly, “hot” and “cold” processes that might contribute to impulsive tendencies and behaviors may have different neural underpinnings that reflect different genetic, environmental and interactive components (see description above and figure 1 in [9] describing subcortical/limbic and prefrontal cortical contributions to motivated behaviors and their changes during adolescence). Additionally, it is important to consider in this context genetic factors that may influence adolescent behavior including psychopathic tendencies, risk-taking and distress tolerance [119, 164, 165], as well as gene-environment interactions, such as with respect to brain responses and psychopathology related to early childhood trauma [115, 166, 167]. Such interactions appear to have important clinical implications with respect to addictive behaviors in adolescents; for example, greater stress-induced risk-taking has been linked to poorer treatment outcome in adolescent smokers [168].

Second, self-report and behavioral measures of impulsivity, even within the same domain, may not correlate, suggesting that how people perceive their behavioral tendencies may differ from their actual behavior [157]. These measures may thus relate differentially to specific aspects of addictions relevant to prevention and treatment efforts [3, 157]. They may also relate differentially to the interaction between environmental exposures and substance use behaviors. For example, self-reported but not behavioral measures of impulsivity have been reported to mediate the relationships between different forms of stress (trauma, major and recent life events, and chronic stressors) and alcohol consumption [169]. Amongst adolescents seeking treatment for addictive behaviors, real-life behavioral and hypothetical self-report measures of discounting at treatment onset have been associated with treatment outcome in tobacco and marijuana smokers, respectively, indicating that these constructs relate importantly to clinically relevant measures and have the potential to identify subgroups of adolescents warranting particular attention [157, 170]. Additional therapies (e.g., those like dialectical behavioral therapy or mindfulness based stress reduction) may be helpful for adolescents who show impaired impulse control during emotionally arousing or stressful states [171, 172].

Third, impulsivity, and facets thereof, appear to follow developmental trajectories that are important to consider [173]. For example, while self-reported impulsivity has been reported to decrease in a linear fashion from adolescence to adulthood, sensation-seeking appears to follow a curvi-linear pattern, increasing during early adolescence and decreasing thereafter [174]. Individual differences in impulsivity appear important to substance use behavior; for example, during late adolescence/early adulthood (ages 18-25 years), groups showing the greatest decreases in impulsivity demonstrated accelerated decreases in alcohol involvement [175].

Fourth, impulsivity represents only one of multiple potential endophenotypes relevant to addictions. Other constructs (e.g., compulsivity, emotional reactivity, stress responsiveness) represent other potential endophenotypes that warrant consideration in understanding the biologies of addictions [2, 63]. Each of these intermediary phenotypes has potential relevance for adolescent addiction vulnerability, particularly given the neurobiological and behavioral changes during this developmental epoch.

Prevention, Treatment and Policy Implications

A major goal in advancing our understanding of the biologies of addictions involves the translation of this knowledge into improved prevention, treatment and policy strategies. Arguably these efforts might be most easily understood for treatment development, particularly with respect to pharmacological therapies (see figure 1 in [3]). That is, an improved understanding of the neurobiological underpinnings of addictions, for example with respect to the function of specific neurotransmitters in specific brain regions, might help to develop medications for the specific receptors or transporters they target. In some ways, efforts in this area have fallen short. Specifically, despite the findings that mesolimbic dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens is considered a central component of drug addictions, medications that block dopamine receptors in this brain region have shown limited efficacy in the treatment of addictions, and in non-substance addictions like pathological gambling have been associated with pro-gambling motivations and behaviors [3, 176]. However, drugs that may influence mesolimbic dopamine function indirectly, such as opioid receptor antagonists like naltrexone and nalmefene and glutamatergic compounds like N-acetyl cysteine, have shown more consistent findings in both substance and non-substance addictions like pathological gambling [16, 177, 178]. Other molecular targets that may influence mesolimbic dopamine function, like the serotonin 1B receptor, show similarities in substance and non-substance addictions [179, 180], and these entities may represent better targets for treatment development than do less specific serotonergic proteins like the serotonin transporter targeted by serotonin reuptake inhibitors, a class of compounds which has shown only modest effects in treating addictions [16, 178]. Given the biological links between substance addictions and obesity (e.g., with respect to striatal dopamine function), some of these targets may extend to excessive eating behaviors [16]. Given the involvement of cannabinoids in both eating behaviors and substance use disorders [16, 181], medication development targeting cannabinoid function also warrants consideration across addictive behaviors. The cross-addiction targeting of cannabinoids would fit with their roles in ventral striatal functioning and stress system responsiveness, as well as the clinical population of adolescent cannabis users who tend to use multiple substances. It is also important to consider heterogeneities with respect to patterns of excessive food consumption, and that some patterns (e.g., those related to binge-eating disorder or “food addiction”) might help identify important sub-groups with respect to underlying biologies and effective prevention and treatment strategies [182].

Intermediary phenotypes or endophenotypes also warrant consideration as treatment targets, and preliminary findings with impulsivity appear encouraging [155, 156]. Specific groups of individuals (e.g., adolescents who demonstrate greater choice impulsivity as evidenced by steeper discounting) may respond preferentially to different interventions like contingency management [170, 183]. Additionally, medications and behavioral treatments that target cognitive enhancement may help improve decision-making and behavioral control in addictions, and these may operate by influencing the brain circuits underlying impulse control [3, 184, 185].

It will be important to consider neurodevelopmental changes, particularly with respect to adolescence and adolescent addiction vulnerability and how this may impact adult functioning, and how differences in maturational rates of cortical and subcortical regions may influence both addiction and other mental health vulnerabilities and the mechanisms of actions (and effectiveness) of specific therapies [9, 10, 50, 186, 187]. For example, the utilization of instructions derived from cognitive behavioral therapies for addictions have been shown in adult tobacco smokers to increase connectivity between prefrontal cortical regions implicated in behavioral control and subcortical regions implicated in cravings [66]. The extent to which these strategies might work in adolescents who may as a group show less prefrontal cortical maturity and ability to harness such cortical control may offer both challenges and opportunities. However, preliminary data indicate that adolescents demonstrate benefit from cognitive behavioral therapies (for example, with respect to smoking cessation). Similarly, the efficacy and tolerability of medications in the treatment of youth warrant consideration; e.g., potential risks associated with widely used medications like serotonin reuptake inhibitors or stimulants. Specific medications effective in adults need to be evaluated separately in youth for efficacy and tolerability, with both short-term and long-term outcomes in mind. Individual differences (e.g., with respect to past trauma exposure) also warrant consideration [188].

Multiple interventions for adolescents have received empirical support. The prevention strategies with the most empirical support involve targeting important risk factors and bolstering important protective factors at individual, familial and community levels [189]. Multiple behavioral approaches, including contingency management, motivational interviewing, and cognitive behavioral and family therapies, have empirical support, with varying levels of data to support each approach in specific populations [183, 190, 191]. Comparatively few medications have been tested for their efficacy and tolerability amongst adolescents with substance abuse or dependence [192], and even less research has examined the extent to which pharmacotherapies might be helpful amongst non-substance addictions [193]. As in adults, other considerations (e.g., co-occurring disorders and after-care) are important in the treatment of adolescent addictions [194, 195].

Other potential targets exist. For example, poor white matter integrity has been found to contribute to both substance and non-substance addictions like pathological gambling as well as to obesity [107, 196-199]. The extent to which pharmacological and behavioral mechanisms might alter white matter integrity to improve treatment outcome warrants consideration [3, 200-202].

Biological knowledge of addictions may help inform advances in policy and prevention [203]. An improved understanding of genetic factors or related endophenotypes might help identify individuals with vulnerability factors that could be targeted preventively for interventions. Similarly, an improved understanding of gene-by-environment interactions, and how specific environmental exposures may influence gene expression (epigenetic phenomena), may also improve prevention strategies. Identification of brain imaging measures that reliably link to addictions could aid in both prevention and treatment strategies. Such prevention and treatment interventions would be most effective with policies and related resources that facilitate their enactment, and this may be particularly difficult in countries that devote limited resources to mental health interventions [5, 204].

Other considerations relevant to prevention, treatment and policy, such as the potential influences of low socio-economic status, may also be informed by biological advances. For example, early life adversity has been linked to altered brain structure and function [141, 142]. Additionally, individuals lower in social status show hypo-functioning striatal systems, and this may influence reward- and motivation-related behaviors including addiction propensity [205]. The extent to which this impact operates at a communal or national level warrants consideration.

Importantly, policy may be informed across addictive behaviors in a manner that benefits from effective interventions in other domains. For example, effective tax strategies that have helped curtail tobacco use particularly amongst adolescents and young adults may be used to model similar efforts with respect to food taxation [31, 206]. It may also be that certain foods (e.g., highly caloric, “hyper-palatable” processed foods) may possess greater addictive potential than do other foods and thus may warrant increased attention from public health and policy perspectives [31]. With respect to adolescents, limiting fast food and sugared sodas (e.g., in school cafeterias and vending machines) warrant consideration. Similarly, policy efforts could restrict the availability of substances with addictive potential that might lead to greater adolescent initiation or use (“bidis” or flavored cigarettes and alcohol-containing caffeinated beverages). Using information related to individual differences in biologies may help to optimize such policies, and the resulting policies may have substantial impact on reducing the societal burdens of addictions. From a global perspective, having resources and policies that would help increase the currently scarce mental health and addiction efforts in low- and middle-income countries could have a major impact on world health [207-209].

Conclusions

The growing body of data on the neurobiology of addiction has the potential to address more effectively one of the major public health problems facing societies today. A neurodevelopmental perspective with a focus on youth vulnerability could help advance efforts related to early interventions.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by NIH grants R01 DA019039, R01 DA020908, R01 DA018647, R21 DA029445, P20 DA027844, P50 DA09241, P50 DA016556, and RL1 AA017539, the Office of Research on Women’s Health, the NIH Roadmap for Medical Research/Common Fund, a grant from the National Center for Responsible Gaming and its Institute for Research on Gambling Disorders, the Connecticut Department of Mental Health and Addiction Services and the Connecticut Mental Health Center. The statements in the manuscript do not necessarily reflect those of the funding agencies.

Footnotes

Disclosures: The author reports no financial conflicts of interest with respect to the content of this manuscript. Dr. Potenza has received financial support or compensation for the following: Dr. Potenza has consulted for and advised Boehringer Ingelheim; has consulted for and has financial interests in Somaxon; has received research support from the National Institutes of Health, Veteran’s Administration, Mohegan Sun Casino, the National Center for Responsible Gaming and its affiliated Institute for Research on Gambling Disorders, and Forest Laboratories, Ortho-McNeil, Oy-Control/Biotie, Glaxo-SmithKline, and Psyadon pharmaceuticals; has participated in surveys, mailings or telephone consultations related to drug addiction, impulse control disorders or other health topics; has consulted for law offices and the federal public defender’s office in issues related to impulse control disorders; provides clinical care in the Connecticut Department of Mental Health and Addiction Services Problem Gambling Services Program; has performed grant reviews for the National Institutes of Health and other agencies; has guest-edited journal sections; has given academic lectures in grand rounds, CME events and other clinical or scientific venues; and has generated books or book chapters for publishers of mental health texts.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Schultz W. Potential effects of addictive drugs on neuronal reward, risk and decision mechanisms. Neuron. 2011;69:603–617. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Dalley JW, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Impulsivity, compulsivity and top-down cognitive control. Neuron. 2011;69:680–694. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Potenza MN, Sofuoglu M, Carroll KM, et al. Neuroscience of behavioral and pharmacological treatments for addictions. Neuron. 2011;69:695–712. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Uhl GR, Grow RW. The burden of complex genetics in brain disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:223–229. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.3.223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Patel V, Flisher A, Nikapota A, et al. Promoting child and adolescent mental health in low and middle income countries. J Child Psychology Psychiatry. 2008;19:313–334. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01824.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Organization WH. Mental Health Atlas. 2011.
  • 7.Volkow ND, Baler RD, goldstein RZ. Addiction: Pulling at the thread of social behaviors. Neuron. 2011;69:599–602. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Strathearn L, Mayes LC. Cocaine addition in mothers: Potential effects on maternal care and infant development. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2010;1187:172–183. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05142.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Somerville LH, Jones RM, Casey BJ. A time of change: behavioral and neural correlates of adolescent sensitivity to appetitive and aversive environmental cues. Brain Cog. 2010;72:124–133. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Rutherford HJV, Mayes LC, Potenza MN. Neurobiology of adolescent substance abuse: implications for prevention and treatment. Child Adol Psychiatry Clin N Am. 2010;19:479–492. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2010.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Wagner F, Anthony J. From first drug use to drug dependence: Developmental periods of risk for dependence upon marijuana, cocaine, and alcohol. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26:479–488. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00367-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Luscher C, Malenka RC. Drug-evoked synaptic plasticity in addiction: From molecular changes to circuit remodeling. Neuron. 2011;69:650–663. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Maddux JF, Desmond DP. Addiction or dependence? Addiction. 2000;95:661–665. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2000.9556611.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.O’Brien CP, Volkow N, Li TK. What’s in a word? Addiction versus dependence in DSM-V. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:764–765. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.5.764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Holden C. ‘Behavioral’ addictions: Do they exist? Science. 2001;294:980–982. doi: 10.1126/science.294.5544.980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Frascella J, Potenza MN, Brown LJ, et al. Shared brain vulnerabilities open the way for nonsubstance addictions: carving addiction at a new joint? Ann NY Acad Sci. 2010;1187:294–315. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05420.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Grant JE, Potenza MN, Weinstein A, et al. Introduction to behavioral addictions. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2010;36:233–241. doi: 10.3109/00952990.2010.491884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Yip SW, Desai RA, Steinberg MA, et al. Health/functioning characteristics, gambling behaviors and motivations in adolescents stratified by gambling severity: Findings from a High School Risk Survey. Am J Addict. 2011;20:495–508. doi: 10.1111/j.1521-0391.2011.00180.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Desai RA, Krishnan-Sarin S, Cavallo DA, et al. Video-gaming among high school students: health correlates, gender differences and problematic gaming. Pediatrics. 2010;126:e1414–e1424. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-2706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Liu TC, Desai RA, Krishnan-Sarin S, et al. Problematic Internet Use and Health in Adolescents: Data from a High School Survey in Connecticut. J Clin Psychiatry. 2011;72:836–845. doi: 10.4088/JCP.10m06057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Potenza MN, Wareham JD, Steinberg MA, et al. Correlates of at-risk/problem internet gambling in adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adol Psychiatry. 2011;50:150–159. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Grant JE, Potenza MN, Krishnan-Sarin S, et al. Shopping problems among high school students. Comp Psychiatry. 2011;52:247–252. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2010.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Potenza MN. Should addictive disorders include non-substance-related conditions? Addiction. 2006;101(s1):142–151. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01591.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Lynch WJ, Maciejewski PK, Potenza MN. Psychiatric correlates of gambling in adolescents and young adults grouped by age of gambling onset. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:1116–1122. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.11.1116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Jacobs D. Juvenile gambling in North America: An analysis of long-term trends and future prospects. J Gambling Stud. 2000;16:119–152. doi: 10.1023/a:1009476829902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Grant JE, Levine L, Kim D, et al. Impulse control disorders in adult psychiatric inpatients. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:2184–2188. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.11.2184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Grant JE, Williams KA, Potenza MN. Impulse control disorders in adolescent inpatients: Co-occurring disorders and sex differences. J Clin Psychiatry. 2007;68:1584–1591. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v68n1018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Petry NM. Should the scope of addictive behaviors be broadened to include pathological gambling? Addiction. 2006;101(s1):152–160. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01593.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Shaffer HJ. Strange bedfellows: a critical view of pathological gambling and addiction. Addiction. 1999;94:1445–1448. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999.941014451.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Holden C. Behavioral addictions debut in proposed DSM-V. Science. 2010;327:935. doi: 10.1126/science.327.5968.935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Gearhardt AN, Grilo CM, DiLeone RJ, et al. Can food be addictive? Public health and policy implications. Addiction. 2011;106:1208–1212. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03301.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ogden CL, Carroll MD, McDowell MA, et al. Obesity among adults in the United States— no change since 2003–2004. National Center for Health Statistics; Hyattsville, MD: NCHS data brief no 1 2007. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Kaminer Y. Problematic use of energy drinks by adolescents. Child Adol Psychiatric Clin North America. 2010;19:643–650. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2010.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Robbins TW, Everitt BJ, Nutt DJ. The neurobiology of addictions: new vistas. Cambridge University Press; New York: 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Nestler EJ. Is there a common molecular pathway for addiction. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1445–1449. doi: 10.1038/nn1578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Volkow N, Li T. Drug addiction: the neurobiology of behaviour gone awry. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2004;5(12):963–970. doi: 10.1038/nrn1539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Sulzer D. How addictive drugs disrupt dopamine neurotransmission. Neuron. 2011;69:628–649. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kenny PJ. Reward mechanisms in obesity: New insights and future directions. Neuron. 2011;69:664–679. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Kalivas PW, Volkow ND. The Neural Basis of Addiction: A Pathology of Motivation and Choice. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162:1403–1413. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Leeman RF, Potenza MN. Similarities and Differences between Pathological Gambling and Substance Use Disorders: A Focus on Impulsivity and Compulsivity. Psychopharmacol. 2012;219:469–490. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2550-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Koob GF, Volkow ND. Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;35:217–238. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Berridge K, Robinson TE. What is the role of dopamine in reward: Hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Res Rev. 1998;28:309–369. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(98)00019-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Berridge KC. The debate over dopamine’s role in reward: the case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology. 2007;191:391–431. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0578-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Blum K, Cull JG, Braverman ER, et al. Reward deficiency syndrome. Am Scientist. 1996;84:132–145. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Khantzian EJ. The self-medication hypothesis of addictive disorders: Focus on heroin and cocaine dependence. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1985;142(11):1259–1264. doi: 10.1176/ajp.142.11.1259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Khantzian EJ, Mack JE, Schatzberg AF. Heroin use as an attempt to cope: Clinical observations. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1974;131:160–164. doi: 10.1176/ajp.131.2.160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND. Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: Neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:1642–1652. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.10.1642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Everitt B, Robbins TW. Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: From actions to habits to compulsion. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1481–1489. doi: 10.1038/nn1579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Chambers RA, Bickel WK, Potenza MN. A scale-free systems theory of motivation and addiction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2007;31:1017–1045. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.04.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Chambers RA, Taylor JR, Potenza MN. Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: A critical period of addiction vulnerability. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:1041–1052. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Kalivas PW, Volkow ND. The neural basis of addiction: a pathology of motivation and choice. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:1403–1413. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.McClure SM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, et al. Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science. 2004;306:503–507. doi: 10.1126/science.1100907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Bechara A. Risky business: Emotion, decision-making, and addiction. J Gambling Stud. 2003;19:23–51. doi: 10.1023/a:1021223113233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Knutson B, Greer SM. Anticipatory affect: neural correlates and consequences for choice. Phil Trans Roy Soc B. 2008;363:3771–3786. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Giedd JN. Structural Magnetic Resonance Imaging of the Adolescent. Brain Ann Rev NY Acad Sci. 2004;1021:77–85. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Amso D, Casey BJ. Curr Dir Psycholog Sci. 2006;15:24–29. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Alexander GE, Crutcher MD, DeLong MR. Basal ganglia-thalamocortical circuits: Parallel substrates for motor, oculomotor, “prefrontal” and “limbic” functions. Prog Brain Res. 1990;85:119–147. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Haber SN, Knutson B. The Reward Circuit: Linking Primate Anatomy and Human Imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:4–26. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Everitt B, Robbins TW. Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8(11):1481–1489. doi: 10.1038/nn1579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Brewer JA, Potenza MN. The Neurobiology and Genetics of Impulse Control Disorders: Relationships to Drug Addictions. Biochem Pharmacology. 2008;75:63–75. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2007.06.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Fineberg NA, Potenza MN, Chamberlain SR, et al. Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviors, from animal models to endophenotypes; a narrative review. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;35:591–604. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Sinha R. Stress and Addiction: A Dynamic Interplay of Genes, Environment and Drug Intake. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66:100–101. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.05.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Sinha R. Chronic stress, drug use, and vulnerability to addiction. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2008;1141:105–130. doi: 10.1196/annals.1441.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Belujon P, Grace AA. Hippocampus, amygdala, and stress: interacting systems that affect susceptibility to addictions. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2011;1216:114–121. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05896.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Metcalfe J, Mishel W. A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psycholog Rev. 1999;106:3–19. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.106.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Kober H, Mende-Siedlecki P, Kross EF, et al. Prefrontal-striatal pathway underlies cognitive regulation of craving. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107(33):14811–14816. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1007779107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Hollman M, Hellrung L, Pleger B, et al. Neural correlates of the volitional regulation of the desire for food. Int J Obesity. 2012;36:648–655. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2011.125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Hommer RE, Seo D, Lacadie CM, et al. [accessed May 14, 2012];Neural Correlates of Stress and Favorite-Food Cue Exposure in Adolescents: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study. Human Brain Mapping. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22089. in press;online before print: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/hbm.22089/abstract. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 69.Potenza MN, Hong KA, Lacadie CM, et al. Neural Correlates of Stress-Induced and Cue-Induced Craving: Influences of Gender and Cocaine Dependence. Am J Psychiatry. 2012;169:406–414. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Jastreboff AM, Sinha R, Lacadie C, et al. Neural correlates of stress- and food-cue-induced food craving in obesity: association with insulin. Diabetes Care. doi: 10.2337/dc12-1112. under review. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Sheehan TP, Chambers RA, Russell DS. Regulation of affect by the lateral septum: implications for neuropsychiatry. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2004;46:71–117. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2004.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Davidson S, Lear M, Shanley L, et al. Differential Activity by Polymorphic Variants of a Remote Enhancer that Supports Galanin Expression in the Hypothalamus and Amygdala: Implications for Obesity, Depression and Alcoholism. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011 Oct;36(11):2211–2221. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Balodis IM, Lacadie CM, Potenza MN. A Preliminary Study of the Neural Correlates of the Intensities of Self-Reported Gambling Urges and Emotions in Men with Pathological Gambling. J Gambling Stud. doi: 10.1007/s10899-011-9259-8. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Olson IR, Plotzker A, Ezzyat Y. The enigmatic temporal pole: a review of findings on social and emotional processing. Brain. 2007;130:1718–1731. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Robbins TW, Ersche KD, Everitt BJ. Drug addiction and the memory systems of the brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1141:1–21. doi: 10.1196/annals.1441.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Naqvi NH, Bechara A. The hidden island of addiction: the insula. Trends Neurosci. 2009;32:56–67. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Behrmann M, Geng JJ, Shomstein S. Parietal cortex and attention. Curr Opin Neurobio. 2004;14:212–217. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Wilens TE, Biederman J. Alcohol, drugs, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a model for the study of addictions in youth. J Psychopharmacol. 2006;20:580–588. doi: 10.1177/0269881105058776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Ernst M, Pine DS, Hardin M. Triadic model of the neurobiology of motivated behavior in adolescence. Psychological Med. 2006;36:299–312. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705005891. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Jentsch JD, Taylor JR. Impulsivity resulting from frontostrial dysfunction in drug abuse: Implications for the control of behavior by reward-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology. 1999;146(4):373–390. doi: 10.1007/pl00005483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Chamberlain SR, Sahakian BJ. The neuropsychiatry of impulsivity. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2007;20:255–261. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3280ba4989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Lejuez CW, Read JP, Kahler CW, et al. Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk-taking: the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART) J Expt Psychology Appl. 2002;8:75–84. doi: 10.1037//1076-898x.8.2.75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Lejuez C, Aklin WM, Jones HA, Strong DR, Richards JB, Kahler CW, Read JP. The Balloon Analogue Risk Taking Task (BART) Differentiates Smokers and Nonsmokers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2003;11:26–33. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.11.1.26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Lejuez C, Aklin WM, Zvolensky MJ, Pedulla CM. Evaluation of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) as a predictor of adolescent real-world risk-taking behavior. J Adolesc. 2003;26:475–479. doi: 10.1016/s0140-1971(03)00036-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Galvan A, Hare TA, Parra CE, et al. Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in adolescents. J Neurosci. 2006;26:6885–6892. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1062-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Bjork JM, Smith AR, Chen G, et al. Adolescents, adults and rewards: comparing motivational neurocircuitry recruitment using fMRI. PLoS One. 2010;6(5):e11440. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Bjork J, Knutson B, Fong G, et al. Incentive-elicited brain activation in adolescents: Similarities and differences from young adults. J Neurosci. 2004;24:1793–1802. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4862-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Wrase J, Schlagenhauf F, Kienast T, et al. Dysfunction of reward processing correlates with alcohol craving in detoxified alcoholics. Neuroimage. 2007;35:787–794. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Balodis IM, Kober H, Worhunsky PD, et al. Diminished fronto-striatal activity during processing of monetary rewards and losses in pathological gambling. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71:749–757. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.01.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Beck A, Schlagenhauf F, Wustenberg T, et al. Ventral striatal activation during reward anticipation correlates with impulsivity in alcoholics. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66:734–742. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.04.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Schneider S, Peters J, Bromberg U, et al. Risk taking and the adolescent reward system: a potential common link to substance abuse. Am J Psychiatry. 2012;169:39–46. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11030489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Rao H, Mamikonyan E, Detre JA, et al. Decreased Ventral Striatal Activity with Impulse Control Disorders in Parkinson’s Disease. Mov Disord. 2010;25:1660–1669. doi: 10.1002/mds.23147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Bjork JM, Chen G, Smith AR, et al. Incentive-elicited mesolimbic activation and externalizing symptomatology in adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;51:827–837. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02201.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Andrews MM, Meda SA, Thomas AD, et al. Individuals Family History Positive for Alcoholism Show fMRI Abnormalities in Reward Sensitivity that are Related to Impulsivity Factors. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69:675–683. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Jia Z, Worhunsky PD, Pearlson GD, et al. An initial study of neural responses to monetary incentives as related to treatment outcome in cocaine dependence. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70:553–560. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Giedd J, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, Paus T, Evans AC, Rapoport JL. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neurosci. 1999;2:861–863. doi: 10.1038/13158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and allostasis. Neurpsychopharmacology. 2001;24:97–129. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00195-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Beveridge TJR, Gill KE, Hanlon CA, et al. Parallel studies of cocaine-related neural and cognitive impairment in humans and monkeys. Phil Trans Royal Soc B. 2008;363:3257–3266. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Eiden LE, Weihe E. VMAT2: a dynamic regulator of brain monoaminergic neuronal function interacting with drugs of abuse. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2011;1216:86–98. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05906.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Koob GF, Kreek MJ. Stress, dysregulation of drug reward pathways, and the transition to drug dependence. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:1149–1159. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.05030503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Moselhy HF, Goergiou G, Kahn A. Frontal lobe changes in alcoholism: A review of the literature. Alcohol Alcoholism. 2001;36:357–368. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/36.5.357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Pfefferbaum A, Sullivan EV. Disruption of Brain White Matter Microstructure by Excessive Intracellular and Extracellular Fluid in Alcoholism: Evidence from Diffusion Tensor Imaging. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2005;30:423–432. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Mann K, Agartz I, Harper C, et al. Neuroimaging in alcoholism: Ethanol and brain damage. Alc Clin Expt Res. 2001;25:104S–109S. doi: 10.1097/00000374-200105051-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Bjork JM, Grant SJ, Hommer DW. Cross-Sectional Volumetric Analysis of Brain Atrophy in Alcohol Dependence: Effects of Drinking History and Comorbid Substance Use Disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:2038–2045. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.11.2038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Squeglia LM, Jacobus J, Tapert SF. The influence of substance use on adolescent brain development. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2009;40:31–38. doi: 10.1177/155005940904000110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Lawrence AJ, Luty J, Bogdan NA, et al. Problem gamblers share deficits in impulsive decision-making with alcohol dependent individuals. Addiction. 2009;104:1006–1015. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02533.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Rosenbloom MJ, Sassoon SA, Fama R, et al. Frontal Callosal Fiber Integrity Selectively Predicts Coordinated Psychomotor Performance in Chronic Alcoholism. Brain Imaging Behav. 2008;2:74–83. doi: 10.1007/s11682-007-9017-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Nasrallah NA, Yang TWH, Bernstein IL. Long-term risk preference and suboptimal decision making following adolescent alcohol use. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106:17600–17604. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0906629106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Swendson J, Le Moal M. Individual vulnerability to addiction. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2011;1216:73–85. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05894.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Tsuang M, Lyons MJ, Meyer JM, Doyle T, Eisen SA, Goldberg J, True W, Lin N, Toomey R, Eaves L. Co-occurrence of abuse of different drugs in men. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55:967–972. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.11.967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Kreek MJ, Nielsen DA, Butelman ER, et al. Genetic influences on impulsivity, risk-taking, stress responsivity and vulnerability to drug abuse and addiction. Nature Neurosci. 2005;8:1450–1457. doi: 10.1038/nn1583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Bierut LJ. Genetic vulnerability and susceptibility to substance dependence. Neuron. 2011;69:618–627. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Maze I, Nestler EJ. The epigenetic landscape of addiction. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2011;1216:99–113. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05893.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Duncan LE, Keller MC. A critical review of the first 10 years of candidate gene-by-environment interaction research in psychiatry. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168:1041–1049. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Hariri AR, Mattay VS, Tessitore A, et al. Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala. Science. 2002 Jul 19;297(5580):400–403. doi: 10.1126/science.1071829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Fox HC, Axelrod SR, Paliwal P, et al. Difficulties in emotion regulation and impulse control during cocaine abstinence. Drug Alc Dep. 2007;89:298–301. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.12.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Kilts C, Gross RE, Ely TD, Drexler KP. The neural correlates of cue-induced craving in cocaine-dependent women. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:233–241. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.2.233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Breiter H, Gollub RL, Weisskopf RM, Kennedy DN, Makris N, Berke JD, Goodman JM, Kantor HL, Gastfriend DR, Riorden JP, Mathew RT, Rosen BR, Hyman SE. Acute effects of cocaine on human brain activity and emotion. Neuron. 1997;19:591–611. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80374-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Sadeh N, Javdani S, Jackson J, et al. Serotonin transporter gene associations with psychopathic traits in youth vary as a function of socioeconomic resources. J Abnorm Psychology. 2010;119:604–609. doi: 10.1037/a0019709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Kaufman J, Yang BZ, Douglas-Palumberi H, Houshyar S, Lipschitz D, Krystal JH, Gelernter J. Social supports and serotonin transporter gene moderate depression in maltreated children. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA. 2004;101:17316–17321. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0404376101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Dong L, Bilbao A, Laucht M, et al. Effects of the circadian rthythm gene period 1 (Per1) on psychosocial stress-induced alcohol drinking. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168:1090–1098. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10111579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Grant JE, Potenza MN, editors. The Oxford Handbook of Impulse Control Disorders. Oxford University Press; Oxford, UK: 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Latimer W, Zur J. Epidemiologic trends of adolescent use of alcohol, tobacco, and other drugs. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2010;19:451–464. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2010.03.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Office_of_Applied_Studies [Accessed May 21, 2012 2009];Adolescent Behavioral Health in Brief. http://wwwsamhsagov/samhsanewsletter/volume_17_number_1/youthsubstanceabuseaspx.
  • 125.Piazza N, Vrbka JL, Yaeger RD. Telescoping of alcoholism in women alcoholics. Int J Addict. 1989;24:19–28. doi: 10.3109/10826088909047272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Potenza MN, Steinberg MA, McLaughlin SD, et al. Gender-related differences in the characteristics of problem gamblers using a gambling helpline. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:1500–1505. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.9.1500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Brady K, Randall CL. Gender differences in substance use disorders. Psychiatr Clin N Am. 1999;22:241–252. doi: 10.1016/s0193-953x(05)70074-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Potenza MN, Maciejewski P, Mazure C. A gender-based examination of past-year recreational gamblers. J Gambling Stud. 2005 doi: 10.1007/s10899-005-9002-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Blanco C, Hasin DS, Petry N, et al. Sex differences in subclinical and DSM-IV pathological gambling: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Psychological Med. 2006;36:943–953. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706007410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Desai RA, Maciejewski PK, Pantalon MV, et al. Gender differences in adolescent gambling. Ann Clin Psychiatry. 2005;17:249–258. doi: 10.1080/10401230500295636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Desai RA, Potenza MN. Gender differences in the associations between problem gambling and psychiatric disorders. Soc Psychol Psychiatr Epi. 2008;43:173–183. doi: 10.1007/s00127-007-0283-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Potenza MN, Hong KA, Lacadie CM, et al. Neural Correlates of Stress-Induced and Cue-Induced Craving: Influences of Gender and Cocaine Dependence. under review. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 133.Brewer JA, Smith JT, Bowen S, et al. Applying mindfulness-based treatments to co-occurring depression and substance use disorders: What can we learn from the brain? Addiction. 2010;105:1698–1706. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02890.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.de Wit H. Sex hormones: A new treatment for cocaine abuse? Neuropsychopharmacol. 2011;36:2155–2156. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Lynch WJ, Sofuoglu M. Role of progesterone in nicotine addiction: evidence from initiation to relapse. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2010;18:451–461. doi: 10.1037/a0021265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Royce JM, Corbett K, Sorensen G, et al. Gender, social pressure, and smoking cessations: The Community Intervention Trial for Smoking Cessation (COMMIT) at baseline. Soc Sci Med. 1997;44:359–370. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(96)00149-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Barry DT. Culture, ethnicity, race and men’s mental health. In: Grant JE, Potenza MN, editors. Textbook of Men’s Mental Health. American Psychiatric Press, Inc; Washington, DC: 2006. pp. 343–362. [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Ioannidis JPA, Ntzani EA, Trikalinos TA. ‘Racial’ differences in genetic effects for complex diseases. Nature Genetics. 2004;36:1312–1318. doi: 10.1038/ng1474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Gelernter J, Panhuysen C, Wilcox M, et al. Genomewide linkage scan for opioid dependence and related traits. Am J Hum Genet. 2006;78:759–769. doi: 10.1086/503631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Barry DT, Beitel M. Cultral and ethnic considerations in young adult mental health. In: Grant JE, Potenza MN, editors. Young adult mental health. Oxford University Press; Oxford, UK: 2009. pp. 110–125. [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Edmiston EE, Wang F, Mazure CM, et al. Corticostriatal-limbic gray matter morphology in adolescents with self-reported exposure to childhood maltreatment. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2011;165:1069–1077. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.565. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Liu J, Chaplin TM, Wang F, et al. Stress reactivity and corticolimbic response to emotional faces in adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adol Psychiatry. 2012;51:304–312. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.12.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Schmahl CG, Vermetten E, Elzinga BM, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging of hippoccampal and amygdala volume in women with childhood abuse and borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2003;122:193–198. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(03)00023-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Vythilingam M, Heim C, Newport J, et al. Childhood Trauma Associated With Smaller Hippocampal Volume in Women With Major Depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:2072–2080. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.12.2072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Sapolsky RM. Why stress is bad for your brain. Science. 1996;273:749–750. doi: 10.1126/science.273.5276.749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Gottesman I, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:636–645. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, et al. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:1783–1793. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Potenza MN. To do or not to do? The complexities of addiction, motivations, self-control and impulsivity. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:4–6. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.1.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Dalley JW, Fryer TD, Brichard L, et al. Nucleus Accumbens D2/3 Receptors Predict Trait Impulsivity and Cocaine Reinforcement. Science. 2007;315:1267–1270. doi: 10.1126/science.1137073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Belin D, Mar AC, Dalley JW, et al. High impulsivity predicts the switch to compulsive cocaine-taking. Science. 2008;320:1352–1355. doi: 10.1126/science.1158136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Ersche KD, Turton AJ, Pradhan S, et al. Drug Addiction Endophenotypes: Impulsive Versus Sensation-Seeking Personality Traits. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68:770–773. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.06.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Ersche KD, Jones PS, Williams GB, et al. Abnormal Brain Structure Implicated in Stimulant Drug Addiction. Science. 2012;6068:601–604. doi: 10.1126/science.1214463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Mischel W, Shoda Y, Rodriguez MI. Delay of gratification in children. Science. 1989;244(4907):933–938. doi: 10.1126/science.2658056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 154.Casey BJ, Somerville LH, Gotlib IH, et al. Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years later. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1108561108. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 155.Grant JE, Odlaug BL, Chamberlain SR, et al. Open-Label Memantine Treatment of Pathological Gambling Reduces Gambling Severity and Cognitive Inflexibility. Psychopharmacology. 2010;4:603–612. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1994-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Blanco C, Potenza MN, Kim SW, et al. A pilot study of impulsivity and compulsivity in pathological gambling. Psychiatric Res. 2009;167:161–168. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.04.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Krishnan-Sarin S, Reynolds B, Duhig AM, et al. Behavioral Impulsivity Predicts Treatment Outcome in a Smoking Cessation Program for Adolescent Smokers. Drug Alc Dep. 2007;88:79–82. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Potenza MN, de Wit H. Control yourself: alcohol and impulsivity. Alc Clin Expt Res. 2010;34:1–3. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01214.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 159.Meda SA, Stevens MC, Potenza MN, et al. Investigating the behavioral and self-report constructs of impulsivity domains using principal component analysis. Behav Pharmacol. 2009;20 doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e32833113a3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 160.Verdejo-Garcıa A, Lawrence AJ, Clark L. Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: Review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32:777–810. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 161.Winstanley CA, Theobald DE, Dalley JW, et al. Double dissociation between serotonergic and dopaminergic modulation of medial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex during a test of impulsive choice. Cerebral Cortex. 2006;16:106–114. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 162.Cardinal R, Winstanley CA, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Limbic corticostriatal systems and delayed reinforcement. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2004;1021:33–50. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 163.Broos N, Diergaarde L, Schoffelmeer ANM, et al. Trait impulsive choice predicts resistance ot extinction and propensity to relapse to cocaine seeking: a bidirectional investigation. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2012;37:1377–1386. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 164.Amstadter AB, Daughters SB, Macpherson L, et al. Genetic associations with performance on a behavioral measure of distress intolerance. J Psychiatr Res. 2012;46:87–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.09.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 165.Amstadter AB, MacPherson L, Wang F, et al. [accessed May 21, 2012];The relationship between risk-taking propensity and the COMT Val158Met polymorphism among early adolescents as a function of sex. J Psychiatr Res. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.04.010. in press; http://www.journalofpsychiatricresearch.com/article/PIIS0022395612001240/related?article_id=S0022-3956%2812%2900124-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 166.Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffit TE, et al. Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science. 2003;301:386–389. doi: 10.1126/science.1083968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 167.Karg K, Burmeister M, Shedden K, et al. The serotonin transporter promoter variant (5-HTTLPR), stress, and depression meta-analysis revisited: evidence of genetic moderation. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68:444–454. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 168.Schepis TS, McFetridge A, Chaplin TM, et al. A pilot examination of stress-related changes in impulsivity and risk taking as related to smoking status and cessation outcome in adolescents. Nicotine Tob Res. 2011;13:611–615. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntr022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 169.Hamilton KR, Ansell EB, Reynolds B, et al. Self-reported impulsivity, but not behavioral choice or response impulsivity, partially mediates the effect of stress on drinking behavior. Stress. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2012.671397. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 170.Stanger C, Ryan SR, Fu H, et al. [accessed May 21, 2012];Delay discounting predicts adolescent substance abuse treatment outcome. Expt Clin Psychopharmacol. doi: 10.1037/a0026543. in press; http://psycnet.apa.org/index.cfm?fa=search.displayRecord&uid=2011-29376-001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 171.Fleischhaker C, Böhme R, Sixt B, et al. Dialectical Behavioral Therapy for Adolescents (DBT-A): a clinical Trial for Patients with suicidal and self-injurious Behavior and Borderline Symptoms with a one-year Follow-up. Child Adol Psychiatr Mental Health. 2011;5(3):1–10. doi: 10.1186/1753-2000-5-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 172.Burke CA. Mindfulness-Based Approaches with Children and Adolescents: A Preliminary Review of Current Research in an Emergent Field. J Child Family Stud. 2010;19:133–144. [Google Scholar]
  • 173.Hammond CJ, Potenza MN, Mayes LC. Development of impulse control, inhibition and self-regulatory behaviors. In: Grant JE, Potenza MN, editors. The Oxford Handbook of Impulse Control Disorders. Oxford University Press; New York, NY: 2012. pp. 232–244. [Google Scholar]
  • 174.Steinberg L, Albert D, Cauffman E, et al. Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: evidence for a dual systems model. Dev Psychology. 2008;44:1764–1778. doi: 10.1037/a0012955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 175.Littlefield AK, Sher KJ, Steinley D. Developmental Trajectories of Impulsivity and Their Association With Alcohol Use and Related Outcomes During Emerging and Young Adulthood I. Alc Clin Expt Res. 2010;34:1409–1416. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01224.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 176.Zack M, Poulos CX. A D2 antagonist enhances the rewarding and priming effects of a gambling episode in pathological gamblers. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2007;32:1678–1686. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 177.Grant JE, Kim SW, Odlaug BL. N-Acetyl cysteine, a glutamate-modulating agent, in the treatment of pathological gambling: a pilot study. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:652–657. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 178.Wareham JD, Potenza MN. Pathological gambling and substance use disorders. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2010;36:242–247. doi: 10.3109/00952991003721118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 179.Hu J, Henry S, Gallezot J-D, et al. Serotonin 1B Receptor Imaging in Alcohol Dependence. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;63:300–303. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 180.Potenza MN, Walderhaug E, Henry S, et al. Serotonin 1B receptor imaging in pathological gambling. World J Biol Psychiatry. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2011.598559. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 181.Yip SW, Potenza MN. Understanding ‘behavioral’ addictions: insights from research. In: Riess, Fiellin, Miller, et al., editors. Principles of Addiction Medicine. 4th Edition Wolters Kluwer; New York, NY: 2009. pp. 45–63. [Google Scholar]
  • 182.Gearhardt A, White M, Potenza M. Binge eating disorder and food addiction. Current Drug Alcohol Rev. doi: 10.2174/1874473711104030201. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 183.Stanger C, Budney AJ. Contingency Management Approaches for Adolescent Substance Use Disorders. Child Adol Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2010;19:547–562. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2010.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 184.Bickel WK, Yi R, Landes RD, et al. Remember the Future: Working Memory Training Decreases Delay Discounting Among Stimulant Addicts. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69:260–265. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.08.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 185.DeVito EE, Worhunsky PD, Carroll KM, et al. A Preliminary Study of the Neural Effects of Behavioral Therapy for Substance Use Disorders. Drug Alc Depend. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.10.002. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 186.Sun H, Cocker PJ, Zeeb FD, et al. Chronic atomoxetine treatment during adolescence decreases impulsive choice, but not impulsive action, in adult rats and alters markers of synaptic plasticity in the orbitofrontal cortex. Psychopharmacol. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2419-9. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 187.Shaw P, Eckstrand K, Sharp W, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA. 2007;104:19649–19654. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707741104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 188.Shamseddeen W, Asarnow JR, Clarke G Gregory, et al. Impact of Physical and Sexual Abuse on Treatment Response in the Treatment of Resistant Depression in Adolescent Study (TORDIA) J Am Acad Child Adol Psychiatry. 2011;50:293–301. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.11.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 189.Griffin KW, Botvin GJ. Evidence-Based Interventions for Preventing Substance Use Disorders in Adolescents Child. Adol Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2010;19:505–526. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2010.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 190.Macgowan MJ, Engle B. Evidence for Optimism: Behavior Therapies and Motivational Interviewing in Adolescent Substance Abuse Treatment. Child Adol Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2010;19:527–545. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2010.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 191.Rowe CL. Multidimensional Family Therapy: Addressing Co-occurring Substance Abuse and Other Problems Among Adolescents with Comprehensive Family-based Treatment. Child Adol Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2010;19:563–576. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2010.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 192.Simkin DR, Grenoble S. Pharmacotherapies for Adolescent Substance Use Disorders. Child Adol Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2010;19:591–608. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2010.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 193.Brezing CA, Derevensky JL, Potenza MN. Non-substance addictive behaviors in youth: pathological gambling and problematic internet use. Child Adol Psychiatric Clin North Am. 2010;19:625–641. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2010.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 194.Bukstein OG, Horner MS. Management of the Adolescent with Substance Use Disorders and Comorbid Psychopathology. Child Adol Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2010;19:609–623. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2010.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 195.Kaminer Y, Godley M. From Assessment Reactivity to Aftercare for Adolescent Substance Abuse: Are We There Yet? Child Adol Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2010;19:577–590. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2010.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 196.Yip SW, Lacadie C, Xu J, et al. Reduced genual corpus collosal white matter integrity in pathological gambling and its relationship to alcohol abuse or dependence. World J Biol Psychiatry. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2011.568068. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 197.Xu J, DeVito EE, Worhunsky PD, et al. White Matter Integrity is Associated with Treatment Outcome Measures in Cocaine Dependence. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;35:1541–1549. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 198.Xu J, Li Y, Lin H, et al. Body Mass Index Correlates Negatively With White Matter Integrity in The Fornix And Corpus Callosum: A Diffusion Tensor Imaging Study. Human Brain Mapping. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21491. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 199.McQueeny T, Schweinsburg BC, Schweinsburg AD, et al. Altered white matter integrity in adolescent binge drinkers. Alc Clin Exp Res. 2009;33:1278–1285. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.00953.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 200.Tang Y, Lu Q, Geng X, et al. Short-term meditation induces white matter changes in the anterior cingulate. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107:15649–15652. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011043107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 201.Harsan LA, Steibel J, Zaremba A, et al. Recovery from chronic demyelination by thyroid hormone therapy: myelinogenesis induction and assessment by diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci. 2008;28:14189–14201. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4453-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 202.Schlaug G, Marchina S, Norton A. Evidence for plasticity in white-matter tracts of patients with chronic Broca’s aphasia undergoing intense intonation-based speech therapy. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1169:385–394. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04587.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 203.Feinstein EC, Richter L, Foster SE. Addressing the Critical Health Problem of Adolescent Substance Use Through Health Care, Research, and Public Policy. J Adol Health. 2012;50:431–436. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.12.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 204.World_Health_Organization [accessed May 21, 2012 2012];Mental Health Atlas. 2011 http://whqlibdocwhoint/publications/2011/9799241564359_engpdf.
  • 205.Nader MA, Czoty PW, Gould RW, et al. Positron emission tomography imaging studies of dopamine receptors in primate models of addiction. Phil Trans Royal Soc B. 2008;363:3223–3232. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 206.Brownell KD, Farley T, Willett WC, et al. The public health and economic benefits of taxing sugar-sweetened beverages. New Engl J Med. 2009;361:1599–1605. doi: 10.1056/NEJMhpr0905723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 207.Morris J, Belfer M, Daniels A, et al. Treated prevalence of and mental health services received by children and adolescents in 42 low and middle income countries. J Child Psychology Psychiatry. 2011;52:1239–1246. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02409.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 208.Nikapota A, et al. Commentary: The how and what of the WHO aims of extending CAMH services in developing countries: a response to Morris. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52:1247–1248. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02471.x. (2011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 209.Eaton J, McCay L, Semrau M, et al. Scale up of services for mental health in low income and middle income countries. Lancet. 2011;378:1592–1603. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60891-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES