Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Feb 26.
Published in final edited form as: J Psychosom Res. 2012 Jun 15;73(2):98–103. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2012.05.007

Low Conscientiousness and Risk of All-Cause, Cardiovascular and Cancer Mortality over 17 Years: Whitehall II Cohort Study

Gareth Hagger-Johnson 1,*, Séverine Sabia 1, Hermann Nabi 2, Eric Brunner 1, Mika Kivimaki 1, Martin Shipley 1, Archana Singh–Manoux 1,2,3
PMCID: PMC3936113  NIHMSID: NIHMS541989  PMID: 22789411

Abstract

Objective

To examine the personality trait conscientiousness as a risk factor for mortality and to identify candidate explanatory mechanisms.

Methods

Participants in the Whitehall II cohort study (N = 6800, aged 34 to 55 at recruitment in 1985) completed two self-reported items measuring conscientiousness in 1991–1993 (‘I am overly conscientious’ and ‘I am overly perfectionistic’, Cronbach's α = .72), the baseline for this study. Age, socio-economic status (SES), social support, health behaviours, physiological variables and minor psychiatric morbidity were also recorded at baseline. The vital status of participants was then monitored for a mean of 17 years. All-cause and cause-specific mortality was ascertained through linkage to a national mortality register until January 2010.

Results

Each 1 standard deviation decrease in conscientiousness was associated with a 10% increase in all-cause (hazard ratio [HR] = 1.10, 95% CI 1.003, 1.20) mortality. Patterns were similar for cardiovascular (HR = 1.17, 95% CI 0.98, 1.39) and cancer mortality (HR = 1.10, 95% CI 0.96, 1.25), not reaching statistical significance. The association with all-cause mortality was attenuated by 5% after adjustment for SES, 13% for health behaviours, 14% for cardiovascular risk factors, 5% for minor psychiatric morbidity, 29% for all variables. Repeating analyses with each item separately and excluding participants who died within five years of personality assessment did not change the results materially.

Conclusion

Low conscientiousness in midlife is a risk factor for all-cause mortality. This association is only partly explained by health behaviours, SES, cardiovascular disease risk factors and minor psychiatric morbidity in midlife.

Keywords: cohort study, conscientiousness, mortality, perfectionism, personality traits, socio-economic status

Introduction

There is increasing interest in the public health implications of personality traits and other stable psychological factors as risk factors for disease and mortality (13). Conscientiousness is a personality trait describing personal dispositions, such as self-efficacy, orderliness, dutifulness, achievement-striving, self-discipline and cautiousness (46). High scorers tend to be organized, follow instructions carefully, adhere to medical regimens, strive to achieve goals, and are more likely to internalise societal norms which guide their behaviour (6, 7). Low scorers are described as being more disorganized, reckless, laid back and prefer not to make long-term plans (8). Although conscientiousness is considered to be a stable personality trait (9), increases in the mean level of conscientiousness have been observed across the adult life course, which is described as a maturity effect (10).

An association between higher conscientiousness and longevity has been shown in several studies (11, 12), including the Terman Life-Cycle Study (1921) of gifted children (13) where the sample was fairly homogenous on cognitive ability and socio-economic status (SES) and therefore less obviously confounded by these variables. The association between low conscientiousness and poor prognosis or adverse health outcomes has also been observed in clinical studies on patients with chronic renal insufficiency (14), coronary heart disease (15) and those receiving Medicare (16). Adjustment for potential confounding and mediating factors does not appear to attenuate the association markedly (1720). The effect sizes are comparable to those between SES and between IQ and mortality risk (3). A recent review based on 20 studies estimated the effect size to be consistent with a 1.25 times increase in relative risk of mortality per standard deviation decrease in conscientiousness (5). The relative risk tends to be larger in recent rather than pre-1993 studies, among patients vs. healthy adults, and for longer than shorter (<5 years) follow-up periods (5).

Several factors may link low conscientiousness with mortality risk (8, 21). Possible mediators which have been proposed include low SES (2224), unhealthy behaviours (4, 23, 25), worse cardiovascular risk profiles including higher inflammation (23, 26, 27) and minor psychiatric morbidity (28). To date, however, no study has included all these possible mediators in a single study and considered disease-specific effects.

The purpose of our study was to evaluate the association between conscientiousness and all-cause and cause-specific mortality over a mean follow-up time of 17 years in the Whitehall II study, a large occupational cohort of British civil servants. We adjusted the analyses for a various sets of possible explanatory variables and examined their explanatory role in these associations. We also calculated the direct, indirect and total effects from low conscientiousness, via possible mediators, to mortality risk.

Methods

Study population and procedure

The Whitehall II cohort recruited 10308 participants (6895 male) at baseline (phase 1, 1985–1988) with a response rate of 73% from 20 London based Civil Service departments (29). The University College London Medical School Committee on the Ethics of Human Research provided ethical approval for the study. Phase 3 (1991–1993), the baseline for the present analysis, was composed of both a questionnaire, which included two self-reported items measuring conscientiousness, and a clinical examination. Informed consent was provided by all participants. The analytic sample comprised 6800 participants (age range 39 to 63) with data at phase 3 on conscientiousness, covariates, candidate mediators and vital status.

Measures

Conscientiousness

Two items were administered at phase 3 of the study, which measured two different components of conscientiousness: ‘I am over-perfectionistic’ and ‘I am over-conscientious’. Response options were presented as ordered categories: ‘very much like me’ (1), ‘fairly like me’ (2), ‘not really like me’ (3), ‘very unlike me’ (4). The two items were combined into a summed composite to reduce measurement error. The combined scale achieved satisfactory internal consistency reliability (range 2 to 8; Cronbach’s alpha α = .72).

Mortality

Participants were traced for mortality through the national mortality register kept by the National Health Services (NHS) Central Registry using the NHS identification number assigned to each British citizen. In our analysis, mortality follow-up began at the measurement of conscientiousness (1991–1993, Phase 3) and ended on the 31st January 2010.

Covariates and mediators

Covariates were age in 1991–1993 and sex, both considered confounding factors. Mediators were selected on the basis that they had been identified in previous studies or in dominant theories of personality and disease.

Socio-economic status (SES)

Employment grade was used as a marker of socio-economic status, classified as low, intermediate or high. The civil service grades cover a wide range of salaries (>10-fold difference from highest to lowest), and reflect social status and level of responsibility.

Social support

Participants were asked to report whether they visited, or were regularly visited by, friends and relatives. Response scores were averaged for friends and relatives, and range from 0 to 4 per month. Social support was also indexed by marital status, scored 1 if participants were married or co-habiting (vs. 0 if not married or co-habiting), and by divorce status, scored 1 if participants were divorced (vs. 0 if not divorced).

Psychiatric morbidity

Participants completed the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-30), a previously validated and widely used measure of minor psychiatric morbidity, particularly psychological distress and anxiety (30). Participants are asked ‘Have you recently…’ in relation to 30 self-reported items, such as ‘Felt that life isn’t worth living’ and ‘Found at times you couldn’t do anything because your nerves were too bad’? Total scores range from 0 to 30.

Health behaviours

Cigarette smoking was classified into separate categories for never smoker (the reference category), current regular smoker and ex-smoker. Alcohol units consumed in the previous week were categorized into abstainers, moderate drinkers (representing 1–14 units/week in women and 1–21 units/week in men; the reference category) and heavy drinkers (representing >14 units in women and >21 units in men). Dietary quality was assessed using the frequency of fresh fruits and vegetables consumed, on an eight-point scale ranging from seldom/never, to two or more times a day. Duration of moderate/vigorous physical activity was recorded in hours per week. Missing data on health behaviours was replaced with the last known value.

Cardiovascular disease (CVD) risk factors

Body Mass Index (BMI) was categorized according to the WHO criteria (31): BMI<18.5 = underweight; BMI 18.5–24.99 = normal weight (the reference category); BMI 25–29.99 = overweight; BMI ≥30 = obese. Blood pressure (systolic and diastolic) was measured as part of the clinical examination, measured twice in a sitting position after 5 minutes rest with the Hawksley random-zero sphygmomanometer. Measured blood pressure was considered the average of these two measures. As part of the clinical examination, fasting serum was collected between 8am and 1pm and stored at −70°C until analysis. A high-sensitivity immunonephelometric assay in a BN ProSpec nephelometer (Dade Behring, Milton Keynes, UK) was used to record CRP values. IL-6 was recorded using a high-sensitivity ELISA assay. Values lower than the detection limit (0.154 mg/L for CRP and 0.08 pg/mL for IL-6) were assigned a value equal to half the detection limit. Intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation were 4.7% for CRP, and 7.5% for IL-6. C-reactive protein and interleukin-6 values were natural log-transformed prior to analysis, no constant added.

Data analysis

We first assessed the differences in all covariates, using t-tests for continuous variables and chi-square tests for categorical variables, between those who were alive and those who had died at the end of the follow-up period and between three categories of conscientiousness.

Cox regression with follow-up time as the time scale was used to evaluate the association between the conscientiousness scale and mortality risk, using SPSS version 19. The assumption of proportional hazards was tested by creating a new time-varying covariate (ln(T)*conscientiousness) where T was the follow-up time since personality assessment. This variable was not significant (p=.88) when entered into a model containing age, sex and conscientiousness, suggesting that the proportional hazards assumption was not violated.

Preliminary analyses screened for interaction terms between conscientiousness and sex or SES. The interaction terms suggested that the association was not significantly different in men and women (p for interaction = .96), and for different employment grades (p for interaction = .10). Therefore, we did not separate the sample into groups according to sex or employment grade.

Analyses were conducted for all-cause mortality and major causes of death (CVD and cancer). We first assessed the association between low conscientiousness and all-cause mortality risk in a model adjusted for age and sex (model 1). Subsequently, the extent to which this association was attenuated was examined by adding SES, health behaviours, biological risk factors, and minor psychiatric morbidity, first sequentially and then simultaneously, to the model adjusted for age and sex (model 1). The attenuation from model 1 attributed to the group of possible mediators under consideration was calculated using the formula “100 × (βModel 1 − βModel 1+covariates)/(βModel 1)”. This approach is used widely in epidemiology and is broadly equivalent to testing for mediation, since the percentage attenuation estimates the proportion of the association that is explained by the proposed mediator. An alternative approach is to model the variables explicitly in a hypothesized causal chain, using structural equation modelling combined with Cox regression analysis (32). This involves calculating the direct, indirect and total effects using the MODEL INDIRECT feature of Mplus (version 6.1). The indirect effects are calculated as products of the regression coefficients from the path Cox regression coefficients, which are then exponentiated to obtain the hazard ratio (32). The possibility that effects ran in different directions for different items was tested by repeating models for each item separately. Concerns about reverse causality were addressed by repeating the analysis after excluding participants who died within 5 years of personality assessment. Cox regression models were then repeated for CVD and cancer mortality using SPSS.

Results

Sample description

Compared to the 6800 participants included in the present analysis, the 3508 participants excluded from the analytic sample were slightly older (44.7 vs. 44.3 years at recruitment, p = .002) and comprised more females (38.9 vs. 30.1% female, p <.001). Descriptive statistics for the analytic sample are shown in Table 1. During the follow-up period (mean 17.26 years, SD= 2.15) a total of 468 (6.9%) of participants died. The cause of death was not known for five participants. Of the 463 participants with known cause of death, 125 (27.0%) were CVD deaths and 227 (49.0%) were cancer deaths. For the CVD/cancer mortality models, two/five participants were additionally excluded automatically from the models, because they were censored before the event.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics as a function of conscientiousness profile and vital status (N=6800)

Alive
(N = 6332)
Died
(N = 468)
Least
conscientious
(N = 1676)
Intermediate
conscientious
(N = 3742)a
Most
conscientious
(N = 1382)
Age-adjusted meanb (95% CI) pc M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) pc

Low conscientiousnessd (SD) .00 (−.02, .03) .10 (.01, .19) .04 1.35 (0.48) −0.07 (0.36) −1.39 (0.36) <.001
Age at baseline (range 39, 63) 49.3 (49.1, 49.4) 52.7 (52.1, 53.2) <.001 49.3 (6.0) 49.6 (6.1) 49.5 (6.2) .26
Physical activity (hours/week) 3.5 (3.5, 3.6) 3.3 (2.9, 3.6) .05 3.4 (3.9) 3.6 (4.0) 3.6 (4.1) .12
Systolic blood pressure (mmHg) 120.3 (120.0, 120.7) 122.9 (121.6, 124.1) <.001 121.2 (13.9) 120.5 (13.4) 119.7 (13.5) .003
Diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) 79.7 (79.5, 79.9) 81.1 (80.3, 82.0) <.001 80.4 (9.5) 79.8 (9.3) 79.0 (9.1) <.001
C-reactive proteine (mg/L) 0.89 (0.86, 0.91) 1.20 (1.08, 1.34) <.001 1.82 (2.83) 1.80 (3.48) 2.10 (6.45) .47e
Interleukin-6e (pg/mL) 1.49 (1.47, 1.52) 1.81 (1.72, 1.91) <.001 1.93 (2.73) 1.90 (1.95) 1.99 (2.64) .93e

Age-adjusted %b (95% CI) pc N (%) N (%) N (%) pc

Male 69.7 (68. 6, 70.8) 70.0 (65.3, 74.6) .29 1244 (74.2) 2617 (69.9) 889 (64.3) <.001
Low socio-economic status (SES)f 15.6 (14.7, 16.5) 20.1 (16.1, 24.1) .10 286 (17.1) 578 (15.4) 212 (15.3) .28
Married or co-habiting 77.1 (76.1, 78.1) 69.5 (64.7, 74.2) .003 1318 (78.6) 2874 (76.8) 1024 (74.1) .01
Divorced 6.8 (6.2, 7.4) 8.2 (5.5, 10.9) .18 115 (6.9) 258 (6.9) 98 (7.1) .96
< 2 friends or relatives seen monthlyf 15.8 (14. 9, 16.7) 15.2 (11.5, 18.9) .79 246 (14.7) 566 (15.1) 259 (18.7) .003
Current regular smoker 12.6 (11.8, 13.4) 22.5 (18.3, 26.7) <.001 221 (13.2) 514 (13.7) 168 (12.2) .33
Heavy alcohol drinker 15.4 (14.5, 16.3) 24.0 (19.6, 28.5) <.001 322 (19.2) 586 (15.7) 175 (12.7) <.001
Poor dietf 37.7 (36.5, 38.9) 44.1 (39.1, 49.1) .004 643 (38.4) 1456 (38.9) 497 (36.0) .15
Underweight 0.9 (0.7, 1.1) 1.4 (0.2, 2.6) .06 12 (0.7) 33 (0.9) 19 (1.4) .17
Overweight 38.0 (36.8, 39.1) 39.8 (34.8, 44.7) .90 650 (38.8) 1423 (38.0) 507 (36.7) .49
Obese 9.0 (8.3, 9.7) 15.0 (11.4, 18.7) <.001 164 (9.8) 356 (9.5) 118 (8.5) .45
GHQ-30 case (>=5 points) 21.9 (20.9, 23.0) 21.0 (16.7, 25.3) .22 262 (15.6) 801 (21.4) 417 (30.2) <.001

Note.

a

Tertile cut points are 4 and 5 (range 2 to 8), with a large intermediate group.

b

Adjusted for age, allowing for age differences in the two groups (age at baseline is unadjusted).

c

p values are for linear trends (analysis of variance or covariance for continuous variables, χ2 test or logistic regression for categorical variables).

d

Standardised low conscientiousness scale.

e

C-reactive protein and Interleukin-6 were natural log transformed, p value refers to log value.

f

In Cox models, SES, number of friends/relatives seen and diet were treated as continuous.

Lower conscientiousness was associated with higher blood pressure, being male (vs. female), seeing fewer friends or relatives, and heavy alcohol drinking (Table 1).

Association between conscientiousness and mortality

Each 1 SD decrease in conscientiousness was associated with a 10% increase in all-cause mortality risk (HR = 1.10, 95% CI 1.003, 1.20). The association was only slightly attenuated when controlling for each set of possible mediators. Adjustment for SES attenuated the association by 5%, health behaviours by 13%, biological factors by 14%, minor psychiatric morbidity by 5% (Table 2). Similar patterns of associations were observed for CVD mortality (HR = 1.17, 95% CI 0.98, 1.39) and cancer mortality (HR = 1.10, 95% CI 0.96, 1.25), but they were not statistically significant partly due to a lack of statistical power.

Table 2.

Hazard ratios (95% confidence intervals) for all-cause associated with a one unit increase in low conscientiousness

All-cause mortality
(468 deaths/6800 participants)

Model description HR (95% CI) % attenuationa
Model 1: adjusted for age and sexb 1.10 (1.003, 1.20)
Model 2 (socio-economic status): Model 1 +employment grade 1.09 (0.998, 1.19) 5.2
Model 3 (social support): Model 1 +social support from friends/relatives, divorce and marital status 1.10 (1.01, 1.21) −6.0
Model 4 (health behaviours): Model 1 + current regular smoking, physical activity, diet, abstinent and heavy (vs. moderate) alcohol drinking 1.08 (0.99, 1.19) 13.1
Model 5 (biological risk factors): Model 1 +underweight/overweight/obesity (vs. normal), systolic and diastolic blood pressure, C-reactive protein and IL-6. 1.08 (0.99, 1.19) 13.7
Model 6 (minor psychiatric morbidity): Model 1 + GHQ score. 1.09 (0.998, 1.20) 4.6
Fully adjusted model 1.07 (0.98, 1.17) 28.6

Note.

a

The percent attenuation refers to the beta coefficient, not the hazard ratio.

b

For model 1, the associations with all-cause mortality for age and sex (male) are HR = 1.10 (95% CI 1.08, 1.11) and HR = 1.11 (95% CI .91, 1.35).

Results from the structural equation model are shown in Table 3. The model includes low conscientiousness, covariates and all proposed mediators. To facilitate convergence, BMI was treated as continuous because the underweight category contained relatively few participants. To reduce multicollinearity between systolic/diastolic blood pressure, the worst (higher) z score for each measure was retained (33). The effects are decomposed into indirect (mediating) effects, which are summed to provide the total indirect effect (32). To illustrate, the sum of all the indirect effects is β=0.032 (HR = 1.03). The direct effect is β=0.063 (HR = 1.07). The combined direct and indirect effect is β=0.095 (HR = 1.10), consistent with results from Cox regression analysis.

Table 3.

Unstandardised model parameters decomposed into direct and indirect effects, using structural equation modelling

Indirect pathway Indirect Effect Hazard Ratio
Employment grade 0.002 1.002
Married or cohabiting (vs. not) −0.002 0.998
Divorced (vs. not divorced) 0.000 1.000
Number of friends or relatives seen monthly −0.002 0.998
Current regular smoker (vs. non-smoker) 0.003 1.003
Ex-smoker (vs. non-smoker) 0.002 1.002
Heavy drinker (vs. moderate drinker) 0.010 1.010
Abstinent drinker (vs. moderate drinker) −0.002 0.998
Fruit/vegetable consumption 0.001 1.001
Physical activity 0.002 1.002
Body Mass Index 0.008 1.008
Blood pressure (highest z score) 0.004 1.004
GHQ score 0.007 1.007

Total of indirect effects 0.032 1.033

Direct effect 0.063 1.065

Note. In the structural equation model, BMI was treated as continuous (facilitating convergence) and blood pressure was the most unfavourable z-score for systolic or diastolic (reducing multicollinearity). All other variables were treated in the same way as for Cox Regression models.

Sensitivity analyses showed that the all-cause mortality association was very similar when analyses were repeated using each of the two items separately. Results did not change materially after excluding participants who died within five years of personality assessment, mitigating concerns about reverse causality (available on request).

Discussion

In a large sample of middle-aged British working adults, a 1-SD lower conscientiousness score was associated with a 10% higher risk of all-cause mortality over a mean follow-up period of 17 years. Socio-economic status (SES), social support, health behaviours, biological pathways and minor psychiatric morbidity only partially mediated the association. The effects were similar for CVD and cancer mortality although these analyses had less statistical power and the results were statistically non-significant.

Strengths of the study include the large sample size and follow-up period which are larger and longer than in many previous studies of conscientiousness and survival (1214, 1620, 23, 34, 35). Personality trait stability peaks at the third and fourth decades of life (36) and is considered relatively stable from age 30 (37). Comparatively few studies have measured conscientiousness in adulthood and subsequent mortality risk, particularly in a large cohort such as Whitehall II. We were able to control for a wide range of possible mediators, which have been proposed in previous studies, but not been available for analysis collectively. We were able to compare competing explanatory variables, by calculating the percentage attenuation observed in the association after adjustment for different kinds of candidate mediators. The causal sequence was specified according to dominant models of personality and health (2123) which assume that personality influences adult SES rather than vice versa. We cannot rule out the possibility that SES might have influenced the expression of conscientiousness later in time, because conscientiousness and SES were both measured cross-sectionally. It is possible that work-related stressors may influence the expression of personality traits, a research question that has been identified as a priority area for future study (38). The apparent trend showing more GHQ cases in the more conscientious group could be driven by the larger proportion of participants responding ‘very like me’ to both items (N = 175; 11.8% of GHQ cases) compared to those responding ‘very unlike me’ to both items (N = 19; 1.3% of GHQ cases). The combination of excessive conscientiousness, perfectionism, anxiety and depression may indicate psychopathology or personality disorder (39).

Brief measures of personality traits have many advantages (40), as may single-item measures, given that results were similar for each of the two items separately or when combined. There are clear disadvantages however, since brief measures cannot capture the full construct of conscientiousness (40). The brief scale used in our study does not correspond to longer conscientiousness scales as measured in the big five. It does however, have face validity and what has been termed the ‘ultimate validity’ of a psychological test (41) – it predicts mortality risk. Pathological levels of perfectionism may overlap with neuroticism (e.g. concerns over trivial mistakes, excessive self-criticism), but we were not able to control for other personality traits or personality disorders. This is a further limitation, although we found little evidence that minor psychiatric morbidity explained the association, and GHQ scores share genetic variance with neuroticism (28). Our results using a global measure of conscientiousness differ from those of a recent study, where perfectionism concerning self-expectations was associated with increased risk of mortality, but perfectionism concerning others’ expectations and socially prescribed perfectionism showed no association (42). In the same model, conscientiousness was associated with decreased mortality risk. Similar results in a recent study of diabetic patients pointed to self-oriented perfectionism decreasing mortality risk (43). Additional limitations of our study include the unusual term ‘overly’ which preceded both items, which could influence how participants responded. Civil servants may have interpreted these questions in relation to their occupational responsibilities, rather than their natural ‘shoes off’ self. Despite these limitations, the combined measure was associated with mortality, supporting its predictive validity.

Our study is among the first with the specific aim to describe and explain the association between single-item measures of low conscientiousness in midlife and survival in a large sample of British civil servants. Other studies have utilized measures of personality in childhood (11, 13), in late adulthood and old age (35), or among specific patient groups with existing disease (14, 15). We measured conscientiousness by combining two questionnaire items in order to reduce measurement error, but longer inventories would bring other advantages, such as allowing analysis of more specific ‘facets’ of conscientiousness (40). The recent meta-analytic review of conscientiousness and mortality risk found stronger effects for the achievement (persistence, industriousness) and order (organization, discipline) facets (5) which measure different components of the overall trait. Finally, we were not able to screen for interactions with other personality traits in the big five model. For example, data from the Midlife Development Study (44) suggested that conscientiousness was protective of mortality risk - but only at the higher levels of agreeableness.

We found no evidence of effect modification by SES, in contrast to the Terman cohort where socioeconomic ‘success’ interacted with conscientiousness to predict mortality [34]. Participants high in conscientiousness but low in success were more likely to die younger in that study, although the cohort had high mean levels of cognitive ability and SES which could influence the meaning of ‘success’ and its relationship to mortality. In contrast, the Whitehall II cohort covers a wide range of SES, with a greater than 10-fold difference between the highest and lowest salary.

From a life course perspective, conscientiousness has often been treated as a stable trait, which is associated with educational attainment and career success, and with healthier behaviours in early adulthood. There is growing evidence however, that conscientiousness and other personality traits do change over time (45), having both within- and between-person variation. Mean level of conscientiousness, for example, are known to rise over adulthood (45).Our data point to health behaviours as partial mediators of the all-cause mortality association, consistent with several recent studies (22, 23). Conscientious individuals may internalise social norms about healthy behaviour, including public health advice, or find advice from health professionals easier to follow diligently (7, 27). Those who are less conscientious may find it difficult to follow health advice, or to organize activities in a way that promotes health. We emphasise the importance of collecting detailed measures of health behaviour, particularly the four major unhealthy behaviours: smoking, heavy alcohol use, poor diet and physical inactivity (46) in attempting to understand how low conscientiousness might influence mortality risk. Other health behaviours may be relevant (4), such as adherence to medical regimens and attending screening appointments (47). Adult with low conscientiousness may find it more difficult to adopt public health messages about health behaviours. Our results suggest that other mechanisms are involved in the association however, beyond health behaviours. These mechanisms may include immune system functioning (47), genetic factors(8), neurophysiological mechanisms (5) or personality disorders and psychopathology (39).

In summary, a brief measure of conscientiousness administered at midlife was associated with all-cause mortality risk. The association may arise partly because low conscientiousness is a strong risk factor for unhealthy behaviours (25), low SES (22, 23), or through shared common causes. In our cohort, conscientiousness appeared to influence mortality risk through several different pathways, although none of these pathways offered a full explanation.

Acknowledgements

We thank all of the participating civil service departments and their welfare, personnel, and establishment officers; the British Occupational Health and Safety Agency; the British Council of Civil Service Unions; all participating civil servants in the Whitehall II study; and all members of the Whitehall II study team. The Whitehall II Study team comprises research scientists, statisticians, study coordinators, nurses, data managers, administrative assistants and data entry staff, who make the study possible. We are grateful to Alexis Elbaz for helpful comments and suggestions.

Funding/Support: Gareth Hagger-Johnson and Séverine Sabia are supported by a grant from the National Institute on Aging, NIH (R01AG034454, PI: Singh-Manoux & Kivimaki). ASM is supported by a “European Young Investigator Award” from the European Science Foundation and the National Institute on Aging, NIH (R01AG013196; R01AG034454). MK is supported by the Academy of Finland, the BUPA Foundation, the National Institutes of Health (R01HL036310; R01AG034454). MJS is supported by the British Heart Foundation. The Whitehall II study is also supported by the British Medical Research Council (G0902037). The funding organizations had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of data; and preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.

Acronyms

BMI

body mass index

CRP

C-reactive protein

CHD

coronary heart disease

CVD

cardiovascular disease

DBP

diastolic blood pressure

ELISA

enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay

GHQ

General Health Questionnaire

HR

hazard ratio

IQ

Intelligence Quotient

IL-6

Interleukin-6

NHS

National Health Service

SD

standard deviation

SES

socio-economic status

SBP

systolic blood pressure

WHO

World Health Organisation

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Mackenbach JP. New trends in health inequalities research: now it's personal. Lancet. 2010;376:854–855. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)60313-3. [Editorial Material]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lahey BB. Public health significance of neuroticism. American Psychologist. 2009;64:241–256. doi: 10.1037/a0015309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Chapman BP, Roberts B, Duberstein P. Personality and longevity: knowns, unknowns, and implications for public health and personalized medicine. Journal of aging research. 2011;2011:759170. doi: 10.4061/2011/759170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hagger-Johnson G, Whiteman M. Conscientiousness facets and health behaviors: A latent variable modeling approach. Personality and Individual Differences. 2007;43:1235–1245. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kern ML, Friedman HS. Do conscientious individuals live longer? A quantitative review. Health Psychol. 2008;27:505–512. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.5.505. [Article]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Goldberg LR. The structure of phenotypic personality traits. Am Psychol. 1993;48:26–34. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.48.1.26. [Article; Proceedings Paper]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Roberts B, Smith J, Jackson J, Edmonds G. Compensatory conscientiousness and health in older couples. Psychological science : a journal of the American Psychological Society/APS. 2009;20:553–559. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02339.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Deary I, Weiss A, Batty D. Intelligence and Personality as Predictors of Illness and Death. Psychological Science in the Public Interest. doi: 10.1177/1529100610388753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Costa PT, Herbst JH, McCrae RR, Siegler IC. Personality at midlife: stability, intrinsic maturation, and response to life events. Psychological Assessment. 2000;7:365–378. doi: 10.1177/107319110000700405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Marsh H, Lüdtke O, Muthén B, Asparouhov T, Morin A, Trautwein U, Nagengast B. A new look at the big five factor structure through exploratory structural equation modeling. Psychological Assessment. 2010;22:471–491. doi: 10.1037/a0019227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Friedman HS, Kern ML, Reynolds CA. Personality and Health, Subjective Well-Being, and Longevity. J Pers. 2010;78:179–215. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00613.x. [Article]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Martin P, da Rosa G, Siegler IC. Personality and longevity: findings from the Georgia Centenarian Study. Age. 2006;28:343–352. doi: 10.1007/s11357-006-9022-8. [Article]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Friedman HS, Tucker JS, Tomlinson-Keasey C, Schwartz JE, Wingard DL, Criqui MH. Does childhood personality predict longevity? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1993;65:176–185. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.65.1.176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Christensen AJ, Ehlers SL, Wiebe JS, Moran PJ, Raichle K, Ferneyhough K, Lawton WJ. Patient personality and mortality: A 4-year prospective examination of chronic renal insufficiency. Health Psychology. 2002;21:315–320. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.21.4.315. [Article]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Boyle SHMD, Siegler IC, Williams RB, Barefoot JC, editors. Conscientiousness as a predictor of survival in coronary patients. Annual meeting of the American Psychosomatic Society; 2003; Phoenix, AZ. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Weiss A, Costa PT. Domain and facet personality predictors of all-cause mortality among medicare patients aged 65 to 100. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2005;67:724–733. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000181272.58103.18. [Article]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Wilson RS, de Leon CFM, Bienias JL, Evans DA, Bennett DA. Personality and mortality in old age. Journals of Gerontology Series B-Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2004;59:P110–P116. doi: 10.1093/geronb/59.3.p110. [Article]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Iwasa H, Masui Y, Gondo Y, Inagaki H, Kawaai C, Suzuki T. Personality and all-cause mortality among older adults dwelling in a Japanese community: A five-year population-based prospective cohort study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatr. 2008;16:399–405. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181662ac9. [Article]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Masui Y, Gondo Y, Inagaki H, Hirose N. Do personality characteristics predict longevity? Findings from the Tokyo Centenarian Study. Age. 2006;28:353–361. doi: 10.1007/s11357-006-9024-6. [Article]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Terracciano A, Lockenhoff CE, Zonderman AB, Ferrucci L, Costa PT. Personality predictors of longevity: Activity, emotional stability, and conscientiousness. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2008;70:621–627. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31817b9371. [Article]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Smith T. Personality as Risk and Resilience in Physical Health. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2006;15:227–231. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Lodi-Smith J, Jackson J, Bogg T, Walton K, Wood D, Harms P, Roberts B. Mechanisms of health: Education and health-related behaviours partially mediate the relationship between conscientiousness and self-reported physical health. Psychology & Health. 2010;25:305–319. doi: 10.1080/08870440902736964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hampson SE, Goldberg LR, Vogt TM, Dubanoski JP. Mechanisms by which childhood personality traits influence adult health status: Educational attainment and healthy behaviors. Health Psychology. 2007;26:121–125. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.26.1.121. [Article]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Soldz S. The Big Five Personality Traits and the Life Course: A 45-Year Longitudinal Study. Journal of Research in Personality. 1999;33:208–232. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Bogg T, Roberts B. Conscientiousness and health-related behaviors: a meta-analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to mortality. Psychological bulletin. 2004;130:887–919. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ironson GH, O'Cleirigh C, Weiss A, Schneiderman N, Costa PT. Personality and HIV disease progression: Role of NEO-PI-R openness, extraversion, and profiles of engagement. Psychosom Med. 2008;70:245–253. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31816422fc. [Article]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Goodwin RD, Friedman HS. Health Status and the Five-factor Personality Traits in a Nationally Representative Sample. J Health Psychol. 2006;11:643–654. doi: 10.1177/1359105306066610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Shipley B, Weiss A, Der G, Taylor M, Deary I. Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Mortality in the UK Health and Lifestyle Survey: A 21-Year Prospective Cohort Study. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2007;69:923–931. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31815abf83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Marmot M, Brunner E. Cohort Profile: The Whitehall II study. International journal of epidemiology. 2005;34:251–256. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyh372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Goldberg DP, Williams P. A user’s guide to general health questionnaire. Windsor: NFER-Nelson; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.WHO. Preventing and managing the global epidemic: Report of a WHO consultation. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2000. Contract No.: 894. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ploubidis G, Grundy E. Personality and all cause mortality: Evidence for indirect links. Personality and Individual Differences. 2009;47:203–208. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Kerkhof GF, Duivenvoorden HJ, Leunissen RWJ, Hokken-Koelega ACS. Pathways Leading to Atherosclerosis. Hypertension. 2010 doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.110.163600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kern ML, Friedman HS, Martin LR, Reynolds CA, Luong G. Conscientiousness, Career Success, and Longevity: A Lifespan Analysis. Ann Behav Med. 2009;37:154–163. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9095-6. [Article]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Taylor MD, Whiteman MC, Fowkes GR, Lee AJ, Allerhand M, Deary IJ. Five Factor Model Personality Traits and All-Cause Mortality in the Edinburgh Artery Study Cohort. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2009;71:631–641. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181a65298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Roberts BW, DelVecchio WF. The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: a quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychological bulletin. 2000;126:3–25. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Costa PT, McCrae RR. Set like plaster? Evidence for the stability of adult personality. In: Weinberger TFHJL, editor. Can personality change? Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1994. pp. 21–40. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Nabi H, Kivimäki M, Marmot M, Ferrie J, Zins M, Ducimetière P, Consoli S, Singh-Manoux A. Does personality explain social inequalities in mortality? The French GAZEL cohort study. International journal of epidemiology. 2008;37:591–602. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyn021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Wheeler HA, Blankstein KR, Antony MM, McCabe RE, Bieling PJ. Perfectionism in Anxiety and Depression: Comparisons across Disorders, Relations with Symptom Severity, and Role of Comorbidity. Int J Cogn Ther. 2011;4:66–91. [Article]. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Woods S, Hampson S. Measuring the Big Five with single items using a bipolar response scale. Eur J Pers. 2005;19:373–390. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.O'Toole B, Stankov L. Ultimate validity of psychological tests. Personality and Individual Differences. 1992;13:699–716. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Fry P, Debats D. Perfectionism and the Five-factor Personality Traits as Predictors of Mortality in Older Adults. Journal of Health Psychology. 2009;14:513–524. doi: 10.1177/1359105309103571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Fry P, Debats D. Perfectionism and other related trait measures as predictors of mortality in diabetic older adults: A six-and-a-half-year longitudinal study. Journal of Health Psychology. 2011;16:1058–1070. doi: 10.1177/1359105311398684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Chapman B, Fiscella K, Kawachi I, Duberstein P. Personality, Socioeconomic Status, and All-Cause Mortality in the United States. Am J Epidemiol. 2010;171:83–92. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwp323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Caspi A, Roberts BW, Shiner RL. Personality Development: Stability and Change. Annual Review of Psychology. 2005;56:453–484. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Babor T, Sciamanna C, Pronk N. Assessing multiple risk behaviors in primary care: Screening issues and related concepts. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2004;27:42–53. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2004.04.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.O'Cleirigh C, Ironson G, Weiss A, Costa PT. Conscientiousness predicts disease progression (CD4 number and viral load) in people living with HIV. Health Psychology. 2007;26:473–480. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.26.4.473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES