Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Feb 28.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2013 Feb;25(1):133–149. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000958

Four factors for the initiation of substance use by young adulthood: A 10-year follow-up twin and sibling study of marital conflict, monitoring, siblings, and peers

JENAE M NEIDERHISER a, KRISTINE MARCEAU a, DAVID REISS b
PMCID: PMC3938097  NIHMSID: NIHMS551571  PMID: 23398758

Abstract

This study examined genetic and environmental influences on associations among marital conflict about the child, parental monitoring, sibling relationship negativity, and peer delinquency during adolescence and initiation of illegal drug use by young adulthood. The sample comprised data collected longitudinally from same-sex sibling pairs and parents when the siblings were 10–18 years old (M = 14.5 and 12.9 years for Child 1 and Child 2, respectively) and 20–35 years old (M = 26.8 and 25.5 years for Child 1 and Child 2, respectively). Findings indicate four factors that explain the initiation of illegal drug use: two shaped by genetic influences and two shaped by environments shared by siblings. The two genetically shaped factors probably have distinct mechanisms: one a child-initiated coercive process in the family and the other parent and peer processes shaped by the child’s disclosure. The environmentally influenced factors seem distinctively shaped by poor parental monitoring of both sibs and the effects of siblings on each other’s deviancy.


The links between family climate, peer characteristics, and drug use have been the focus of much research over the past several decades. Parenting, especially parental knowledge about their children’s friends and whereabouts (i.e., monitoring), is the family measure most thoroughly examined as a predictor of adolescent initiation and continued use of drugs and related behaviors. The handful of studies that have considered other aspects of the family like marital conflict and hostility in sibling relationships have also found evidence for links with problematic adolescent outcomes including drug use. Outside of the family, peer characteristics (i.e., delinquency) have well-established links with adolescent behavior problems and drug use. However, there have been fewer studies that have considered how these different interpersonal relationships may work together in influencing drug use, and there have not been satisfactory studies of whether these influences reflect exogenous pressures acting on the child or whether characteristics of the child initiate and shape some or all of these apparent environmental risks.

To help clarify how these influences are balanced, and the relative role of the child and exogenous social influences, it is fortunate that there is a rapidly growing literature on the role of genetic factors on interpersonal relationships and on the covariation of interpersonal relationships and the adjustment of family members. When children’s genes influence the objective features of their social environment or their own experience of it, genotype–environment correlation is indicated. The links between interpersonal relationships and adolescent drug use may be due at least partly to genetic influences of the adolescent on his or her family and peers as well as on his or her likelihood of using drugs. The current report will examine genetic and environmental influences on the covariation among marital conflict, parental knowledge, sibling conflict, and deviant peer group characteristics during adolescence and initiation of drug use by young adulthood. The interpersonal relationships are considered in order of proximal relevance to the adolescent’s likelihood of initiation of drug use by young adulthood, with the most distal to the child accounted for first (marital conflict) and the most proximal to the child considered last (peer group characteristics). In order to better understand the role of the adolescent, via genes, a sample of twins and siblings varying in degree of genetic relatedness was used. Thus, in this report we will be able to identify genotype–environment correlations among interpersonal relationships on the initiation of drug use.

Family Relationships and Drug Initiation

There is evidence from developmental and family process literature that marital conflict is linked with adolescent adjustment problems. Conflict between parents, especially conflict that children experience directly, is associated with increased internalizing, externalizing, and a host of other undesirable outcomes in adolescents (e.g., Amato, 1991; Cummings, 1994; Cummings & Davies, 2002; El-Sheikh & Elmore-Staton, 2004; Harold & Conger, 1997; Rhoades et al., 2011). There are, however, few studies that have examined the impact of marital conflict on drug use in adolescents, with most studies focusing on more general adjustment problems like externalizing. There is evidence that impulse control, aggression, and other externalizing behaviors predict initiation of drug use in adolescents (e.g., Ernst et al., 2006); thus, the associations between marital conflict and externalizing behaviors also suggest an increased risk of initiation of drug use.

A number of possible mechanisms through which marital conflict may be associated with child functioning have been identified. There may be a direct effect of marital conflict on the child, as described above. It is also likely that marital conflict disrupts or impairs parenting, thus indirectly influencing child adjustment through parental behaviors by, for example, decreasing effective monitoring (e.g., Cummings, 1994; Gable, Belsky, & Crnic, 1992; Peterson & Zill, 1986). A few studies have attempted to systematically examine the ordering of effects among marital conflict, parenting, and child adjustment. One such found that the effects of marital hostility on adolescent externalizing behaviors were mediated by multiple types of parenting behaviors, including monitoring, with some differences for mothers and fathers (Buehler, Benson, & Gerard, 2006). Overall, it is clear that marital conflict is associated with child adjustment and that parenting behaviors, including monitoring, at least partially mediate this effect on children. Because adolescent drug initiation is an adjustment problem highly comorbid with externalizing problems (e.g., Disney, Elkins, McGue, & Iacono, 1999; Helstrom, Bryan, Hutchison, Riggs, & Blechman, 2004; Molina & Pelham, 2003), it is likely that marital conflict impacts drug initiation via similar mechanisms, operating in part through parenting behaviors.

There are a plethora of studies that have found that more effective parental monitoring is related to a reduced likelihood to initiate drug use and related behaviors during adolescence (Dishion & Tipsord, 2011; Lac & Crano, 2009). Studies examining initiation and continued drug use have found parental monitoring to be one of the most predictive factors in adolescent drug use (e.g., Chilcoat & Anthony, 1996; Dishion, Capaldi, Spracklen, & Li, 1995). In general, these studies have found that when parents know who their children are with, where they are, and when they will return (parental monitoring and/or knowledge), children show fewer externalizing behaviors, associate with fewer delinquent peers, and are less likely to initiate use of drugs. This pattern of findings has been shown for initiation of commonly used substances like tobacco, alcohol, and marijuana (Crano, Siegel, Alvaro, Lac, & Hemovich, 2008; Dishion et al., 1995; Dishion & Loeber, 1985; Dorius, Bahr, Hoffmann, & Harmon, 2004; Duncan, Duncan, Biglan, & Ary, 1998; Lac & Crano, 2009; Piko & Kovacs, 2010) as well as for use of other less commonly used drugs, like ecstasy (Martins, Storr, Alexandre, & Chilcoat, 2008; Wu, Liu, & Fan, 2010) and inhalants (Nonnemaker, Crankshaw, Shive, Hussin, & Farrelly, 2011). Although other parenting behaviors (i.e., hostility, warmth, involvement, and coercive discipline) have also been shown to predict adolescent substance use (e.g., Cheng & Lo, 2010; Fletcher, Steinberg, & Williams-Wheeler, 2004; Siebenbruner, Englund, Egeland, & Hudson, 2006), generally parental monitoring and knowledge continues to be a significant predictor above and beyond other parenting behaviors (see Chen, Storr, & Anthony, 2005 for an exception to this). Together, this evidence highlights the importance of parental monitoring in particular on the likelihood of children to initiate a number of different substances. More effective parental monitoring is associated with a decreased likelihood to initiate drugs.

What is not clear from the studies reported above is the direction of effects: do parents who effectively monitor their adolescent have more control, thus taking an active part in preventing drug initiation, or is effective monitoring an adolescent-driven construct with adolescents who are less likely to initiate drug use more likely to tell their parents about their whereabouts? This latter explanation is consistent with the notion that parental knowledge is due to adolescent disclosure and that it is adolescent disclosure that predicts the likelihood of delinquent behaviors, not parental monitoring or attempts at control (Kerr & Stattin, 2000; Kerr, Stattin, & Burk, 2010; Stattin & Kerr, 2000). Findings from genetically informed designs indicate that there are genetic influences on parental monitoring (e.g., Neiderhiser et al., 2004; Neiderhiser, Reiss, Lichtenstein, Spotts, & Ganiban, 2007), thus supporting an adolescent-driven mechanism of effect.

Siblings are another source of potential influence on adolescent initiation of drug use. There have been a number of studies that have examined deviancy training in sibling pairs with clear evidence that having a deviant sibling increases an adolescent’s risk of deviant behavior, including initiation of substance use (e.g., Avenevoli & Merikangas, 2003; Compton, Snyder, Schrepferman, Bank, & Shortt, 2003; Natsuaki, Ge, Reiss, & Neiderhiser, 2009; Rowe & Gulley, 1992). The quality of the sibling relationship appears to be an important factor for the similarity of siblings for several problematic behaviors. For example, siblings with close relationships were more likely to have similar attitudes and experiences with sexual behavior in a large sample of adolescent siblings (McHale, Bissell, & Kim, 2009). At least one study has found no association between sibling relationship quality and substance use when sibling deviancy was accounted for (Stormshak, Comeau, & Shepard, 2004), suggesting that deviancy training is a likely mechanism for associations between sibling deviancy and initiation of drug use in adolescents. In a different study not directly assessing drug use, hostile sibling relationships predicted delinquency in boys and girls, while warm sibling relationships predicted delinquency in boys only (Slomkowski, Rende, Conger, Simons, & Conger, 2001). These studies clearly indicate that siblings can exert influence on the likelihood of adolescents to initiate drug use.

Peers have been implicated in the development of drug use for several decades (e.g., Kandel, 1973). There are many studies that have found that the risk of drug use increases with the number of peers who use (Brook, Kessler, & Cohen, 1999; Ennett et al., 2006) and that the perceptions of peer use is a strong predictor of adolescent use (Agrawal & Lynskey, 2007). Peers have also been implicated in deviancy training such that friendships with deviant peers increase delinquency, substance use, violence, and adult maladjustment (Dishion, McCord, & Poulin, 1999; Dishion & Owen, 2002). There has been some debate about the direction of effects of these associations. Is it that deviant peers train friends in deviance or do individuals at risk of drug use select deviant peers? Studies that have attempted to disentangle the direction of effects have found that this is most likely a bidirectional relationship. For example, longitudinal studies suggest that both peer drug use and participant drug use are highly stable over time, with somewhat more evidence that peers influence use of substances over time (Sieving, Perry, & Williams, 2000). A study using a short-term longitudinal design found that adolescents who were already using substances tended to select friends who also used and that these friends contributed to changes in adolescent substance use over time (Poulin, Kiesner, Pedersen, & Dishion, 2011). An earlier study found that both selection and socialization occurs in friendships and marijuana use over time (Kandel, 1978). Thus, although peers are clearly implicated in the initiation of drug use, this association appears to be bidirectional in nature and fairly stable over time.

Integrating Across Marital Conflict, Parental Monitoring, Sibling Relationships, and Peers

Although marital conflict, parental monitoring, sibling relationships, and peer characteristics have all been shown to have a direct association with initiation of drug use and related behaviors, as reviewed above, it is unlikely that any of these constructs are operating to influence the adolescent in isolation from the others. As noted above, the effect of marital hostility on adolescent externalizing problems is mediated in part by parenting behaviors, including less effective monitoring (e.g., Buehler et al., 2006; Cummings, 1994; Gable et al., 1992; Peterson & Zill, 1986). However, no studies were identified linking marital quality and parenting to substance use. Studies that have attempted to clarify how parenting and peers may operate together to influence adolescent delinquency and drug use have found some evidence that parents have an important influence on the types of friends with whom their adolescent(s) associate. For example, ineffective parental monitoring predicted involvement with a deviant peer network that in turn was associated with a rise in substance use in the transition to high school (Dishion et al., 1995). There is also some evidence that associating with deviant peers may mediate the effect of parenting on substance use (Kiesner, Poulin, & Dishion, 2010; Kung & Farrell, 2000), although at least one study found important differences for the association between drug use with friends and drug use alone, with parental monitoring moderating both associations (Kiesner et al., 2010). In other words, the impact of parenting behaviors, including monitoring, influences adolescent drug use indirectly via peer characteristics. However, the likelihood that an adolescent who uses drugs with his friends will also use when alone is decreased when parental monitoring is effective. Other studies of the links between parenting and peer characteristics have found that parental monitoring is related to peer group characteristics indirectly via adolescent drug use and self-reliance (Brown, Mounts, Lamborn, & Steinberg, 1993) and that poor parenting practices, including ineffective monitoring, increase the influence of deviant peers on adolescent drug use (Duncan et al., 1998; Kung & Farrell, 2000).

Fewer studies have examined associations among sibling relationships and peer characteristics. Peer and sibling substance use are better predictors of adolescent substance use than parental alcohol use, and sibling substance use also predicts peer substance use (Windle, 2000). In other words, the transmission of substance use within a family appears to be lateral (via sibling influences) rather than vertical (via parental influences), and sibling factors predict and probably precede peer factors. In another study both sibling and peer deviance directly predicted adolescent substance use, although when both were included as predictors of change in substance use over time, only sibling deviance remained significant (Stormshak et al., 2004). The findings from this latter study suggest that sibling factors may be more important in predicting adolescent drug use. Both of these studies suggest that siblings play an important role in influencing adolescent drug use, preceding the influence of drug using peers and possibly overriding the influence of peers.

We found no studies that considered marital conflict, parental monitoring, sibling relationships, and peer characteristics influences on initiation of drug use or related behaviors. The studies that have considered more than one of these constructs in the same model have found evidence for richer patterns of association than indicated by examining any one of these constructs alone. Both parental behavior and sibling relationships have been linked to peer group characteristics, and mediation and moderation of associations between interpersonal relationships and adolescent substance use is indicated in some studies. There is a clear need to consider how these different relationships may work together to influence the initiation of drug use in order to better understand the processes that may be involved. The findings reviewed above suggest that the effects of family factors appear to have a more profound effect when they are more proximal to the adolescent, supporting an approach that progresses from more distal to more proximal in regard to order of effect on adolescent initiation of drug use. Specifically, we propose an ordering from marital conflict about the child, parental monitoring, sibling relationship, peer characteristics, to initiation of drug use.

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Interpersonal Relationships and Drug Use

Strong causal inferences about the role of social factors in drug use and abuse cannot be drawn from studies that do not use genetically informed designs for three reasons. First, there is ample evidence that substance use itself is influenced by genetic factors; drug abuse shows very high heritability estimates, typically accounting for more than half of the total variance (e.g., Kendler, Meyers, & Prescott, 2007). These same genetic factors may influence adolescent relationships with parents and peers, but this second effect of genetic factors might simply be a noncausal epiphenomenon. Second, adolescent relationships may mediate genetic influences on substance use. Genetic factors related to substance use may first influence relationships and, as a consequence of that influence, go on to influence substance use. Third, interpersonal relationships may have their own main effects on substance use independent of genetic influences either on the relationships or on substance use. These possibilities are essential in constructing any causal model of substance use. They are based on a well-established literature substantiating the role of genetic influences of the adolescent on measures of family relationships like parenting and sibling relationships (Bussell et al., 1999; Elkins, Iacono, & Doyle, 1997; Feinberg, Reiss, Neiderhiser, & Hetherington, 2005; Rasbash, Jenkins, O’Connor, Tackett, & Reiss, 2011) and even for marital conflict about each child (Reiss, Neiderhiser, Hetherington, & Plomin, 2000). Genetic influences have also been found for peer relationships and peer group characteristics (Beaver et al., 2009; Bullock, Deater-Deckard, & Leve, 2006; Cleveland, Wiebe, & Rowe, 2005; Iervolino et al., 2002; Kendler, Jacobson, et al., 2007; Pike & Atzaba-Poria, 2003). In general, these studies have found that parenting is influenced by genetic, shared environmental, and nonshared environmental influences; peer relationships primarily by genetic and nonshared environmental influences; and sibling relationships primarily by genetic and shared environmental influences (Kendler & Baker, 2007; Ulbricht & Neiderhiser, 2010). In other words, parenting may reflect evoked responses of parents to genetically influenced characteristics of the child, while peer characteristics may be the result of adolescents’ selecting peers or by being selected on the basis of the adolescent’s genetically influenced characteristics. In both cases, genotype–environment correlation is indicated, although the type of genotype–environment correlation is unclear. At least one study has reported that genetic influences explain the covariation between parental discipline and quality of peer relationships during adolescence (Pike & Eley, 2009), a finding consistent with the univariate findings reported above. Marital conflict about the child, a marital variable that is distinct for each twin and/or sibling, shows substantial and significant genetic influence, with the remaining variance due to primarily nonshared environmental influences (Reiss et al., 2000). This can also be interpreted as evocative effects of the child on his or her parents’ marital conflict because the similarity in levels of conflict about each sibling increases with the genetic relatedness of the children. In sum, there is ample evidence of genetic influence on measures of interpersonal relationships with systematic variation based on which relationship is examined.

Genetic and environmental influences on substance use

Substance abuse disorders are highly heritable, with genetic influences typically explaining more than half of the total variance (e.g., Kendler, Myers, et al., 2007; Lynskey, Agrawal, & Heath, 2010; Rhee et al., 2003). Different patterns of genetic and environmental influences have been found, however, based on the developmental progression of substance use. In other words, initiation of use is largely due to shared environmental and some genetic influences, while continued use and dependence is primarily due to genetic influences (Agrawal & Lynskey, 2008; Derringer, Krueger, McGue, & Iacono, 2008; Hopfer, Crowley, & Hewitt, 2003; Kendler, Schmitt, Aggen, & Prescott, 2008). The shared environmental influences on initiation of drug use are typically described as having to do with availability of substances and sibling effects rather than common rearing environment factors. These findings suggest that although individuals are more likely to progress from experimentation to problem use for reasons to do with their genes, the opportunity to experiment is largely attributable to the environment that they share with their siblings. Thus, minimizing the opportunities to initiate drug use may be a very effective intervention strategy even in individuals who may be at greater genetic “risk” of becoming dependent.

There are also fairly clear findings from genetically informed studies that the bulk of genetic influence on substance use is not specific to a particular substance. A review of twin and adoption studies examining genetic and environmental influences on adolescent substance use found evidence for common genetic influence across substances (Hopfer et al., 2003). More recent reports support this conclusion. For example, common genetic influences have been found across multiple classes of illicit drugs in male twins (Kendler, Jacobson, Prescott, & Neale, 2003), although a subsequent analysis of male and female twins found that illicit and legal drug dependence loaded on different genetic factors, indicating that there are distinct genetic influences for the two categories of drug dependence (Kendler, Myers, et al., 2007). In both studies, although there are genetic influences specific to each drug, the bulk of genetic variance was shared across the drugs. These data suggest that a single index of drug use in early adulthood, one that combines all substances of abuse, adequately reflects the cumulative genetic and environmental influences on substance use in adulthood. We use this strategy in the current study.

Genetic and environmental influences on associations among interpersonal relationships and outcomes

A powerful way to understand how interpersonal relationships may impact outcomes, including initiation of drug use, is to examine genetic and environmental influences on the associations among relationships and outcomes. By examining genetic and environmental influences on these associations, we can understand whether evocative adolescent effects contribute to both interpersonal relationships and drug initiation, or whether interpersonal relationships are potential environmental mechanisms (or operate via passive genotype–environment correlation) predicting adolescent drug initiation. Few studies have investigated the role of adolescents’ genes and environments on associations between marital conflict and adolescent adjustment problems. There is some evidence that the association between marital conflict about the child and adolescent antisocial behavior is explained in near equal parts by genetic and shared environmental influences (Reiss et al., 2000). A children of twins approach found that marital instability was linked to young adult alcohol, drug, and behavioral problems for environmental reasons rather than through passive genotype–environment correlation (D’Onofrio et al., 2005). Another study using the children of twins design found that genetic influences mediated the association between marital conflict frequency and conduct problems in offspring (Harden et al., 2007). Together, these few studies suggest both genetic and environmental mediation of associations between marital relationships and offspring adjustment, indicating that marital conflict likely impacts offspring adjustment via both evocative and passive genotype–environment correlation processes.

Many studies have examined genetic and environmental influences underlying the association between parenting and adolescent outcomes. Most of the covariation between parenting and externalizing behaviors can be explained by genetic influences (e.g., Evrony, Ulbricht, & Neiderhiser, 2010; Ulbricht & Neiderhiser, 2010). In other words, the genetic influences of the child on parenting behaviors also account for variation in externalizing behaviors. One of the first studies to take this approach examined parental negativity and adolescent antisocial behavior using a sample of adolescent twins and siblings and their parents (Pike, McGuire, Hetherington, Reiss, & Plomin, 1996). Although the focus of that report was on finding nonshared environmental influences that contributed to covariation between family relationships and adolescent adjustment, genetic influences explained over half of the covariation between parental negativity and adolescent antisocial behavior. Many studies have found a similar pattern of findings using different samples, with most concluding that genetic factors are significant influences on the covariation between parenting and adolescent antisocial and related behaviors (Burt, Krueger, McGue, & Iacono, 2003; Button, Lau, Maughan, & Eley, 2008; Narusyte, Andershed, Neiderhiser, & Lichtenstein, 2007). These findings, similar to those of genetic influences on parenting alone, indicate that genotype–environment correlation is likely to be operating, although it provides little information on whether these effects are evocative or passive (because parents and children share genes and environments).

A number of studies have used different designs or taken a longitudinal approach to examining associations between parenting and adolescent externalizing behaviors to better understand how and why these constructs are related. For example, two adoption studies found that birth parent psychopathology is linked to adoptive parents’ harsh parenting via the adopted child’s behavior, indicating evocative genotype–environment correlation (Ge et al., 1996; Riggins-Caspers, Cadoret, Knutson, & Langbehn, 2003), and a different adoption study found a direct association between birth parent antisocial behavior and negative parenting (O’Connor, Deater-Deckard, Fulker, Rutter, & Plomin, 1998). A sibling adoption design examined parenting behaviors and later delinquent behaviors in adolescents and found that the associations were best explained by shared environmental and genetic influences (Burt, McGue, Krueger, & Iacono, 2007). Finally, there have been at least two longitudinal studies of parenting and adolescent outcomes. In both, parenting was associated with change in adolescent antisocial behavior, and a substantial portion of this association was due to genetic influences (Burt, McGue, Krueger, & Iacono, 2005; Neiderhiser, Reiss, Hetherington, & Plomin, 1999). In sum, a variety of studies using different samples, designs, and measures find evidence for genetic influences on the covariation of parenting and externalizing and related outcomes in offspring.

A handful of studies have examined genetic and environmental influences on links between other sibling and peer characteristics and relationships, and substance use and related outcomes. A series of studies using a large representative sample of twins and siblings found that sibling relationship was linked with smoking and drinking and that these associations are shared environmental, not genetic, in nature (Rende, Slomkowski, Lloyd-Richardson, & Niaura, 2005; Slomkowski, Rende, Novak, Lloyd-Richardson, & Niaura, 2005). In addition, sibling negativity was linked with adolescent externalizing behaviors for mostly shared environmental reasons (Reiss et al., 2000). Shared environmental influences on family functioning and adolescent substance use were also found in a large sibling adoption study, with the shared environmental influences due to sibling effects, not parent effects (McGue, Sharma, & Benson, 1996). A report from the same group using a sample of 14-year-old twins found that only shared environmental influences explained the covariation among peer deviance, parent–child relationship problems, and early substance use (Walden, McGue, Iacono, Burt, & Elkins, 2004). A study of a large sample of adult twin men who rated their peer characteristics and conduct problems during adolescence retrospectively via the Life History Calendar method found that genetic influences on conduct problems were linked to later peer deviance and did not change over time, while shared environmental influences explained the covariance between peer deviance and conduct problems and decreased over time (Kendler, Jacobson, Myers, & Eaves, 2008). A subsequent report using the same sample found that genetic and environmental influences on cannabis use were correlated with peer deviance and that the direction of effects was from cannabis use to peer deviance, not vice versa (Gillespie, Neale, Jacobson, & Kendler, 2009). A similar pattern of findings was found for adolescent peer delinquency and smoking during young adulthood in a longitudinal sample of twins and siblings, with the association explained by only genetic and nonshared environmental influences (Harakeh et al., 2008), and between peer and adolescent delinquency in a different sample of twins and siblings (Beaver, DeLisi, Wright, & Vaughn, 2009). In sum, sibling relationships have been found to be linked to externalizing and related behaviors because of shared environmental influences, which is most likely a sibling deviancy training process, whereas findings from studies examining peer delinquency and adolescent and young adult delinquency and substance use consistently show that genetic influences were important in explaining the association, suggesting an important role for genotype–environment correlation in these associations.

Therefore, although deviancy training is a proposed mechanism for the influence of both sibling and peer relationships on adolescent drug use, findings from behavioral genetics studies suggest that the evocative effects of the drug-using adolescent are particularly important in the deviancy training of his/her own peers, while shared environmental influences contribute to similarities between siblings. It is important to note here, however, that the shared environmental influences from siblings are conceptualized differently than those from parents (although there are no differences in the modeling). As noted earlier, shared environmental influences due to siblings are likely the result of reciprocal processes, while shared environmental influence due to rearing environment from parents are more commonly considered the result of the rearing environment acting on the child (Rowe & Rogers, 1997).

Current report

The current report seeks to build on and extend the existing work examining genetic and environmental influences on interpersonal relationships and the initiation of illegal drug use by young adulthood. Although a number of studies have examined bivariate associations among these relationships and adolescent or young adult behaviors, some longitudinally, few have considered multiple relationships. The phenotypic literature has indicated that marital conflict and parental monitoring are important family factors that may increase the likelihood of drug initiation and sibling conflict and peer delinquency as possible avenues for contagion or deviancy training. We will consider all four interpersonal factors: marital conflict about each adolescent, parental monitoring, sibling negativity, and peer group delinquency during adolescence in relation to initiation of drug use by young adulthood, using a genetically informed design. Clarifying the patterns of genetic and environmental contributions to these associations will help to advance our understanding of how genotype–environment correlation and direct environmental influences may be operating within the context of these interpersonal relationships to influence the initiation of drug use.

Method

Sample

The current report is based on data from the longitudinal Non-shared Environment and Adolescent Development (NEAD) project (Neiderhiser, Reiss, & Hetherington, 2007; Reiss et al., 2000). The NEAD project assessed 720 same-sex (48% female) twin and sibling pairs from families across the United States. The sample comprised two-parent families, including nondivorced and stepfamilies, with a pair of adolescent siblings no more than 4 years apart in age. NEAD comprises six sibling types in two family types: monozygotic (MZ, N =93 pairs) twins, dizygotic (DZ, N =99 pairs) twins, and full siblings in nondivorced families (FI, N = 95 pairs); and full siblings (FS, N = 182 pairs), half-siblings (HS, N = 109 pairs), and stepsiblings (US, N = 130 pairs) in step-families (12 pairs could not be classified). The parents of children in stepfamilies were required to be married at least 5 years prior to data collection to ensure that none of the step-families were in the unstable early phase of family formation. The nondivorced families with full siblings were recruited through random-digit dialing of 10,000 telephone numbers throughout the United States. Except for a small subsample of the other sibling types who were also recruited through random-digit dialing procedures, most of the other sibling types were recruited through a national market survey of 675,000 households.

Three cross-sectional assessments occurred in NEAD, the first two during adolescence and the third during young adulthood. The Time 1 assessment included 720 families. Families with adolescents still living at home at least 50% of the time and who participated in Time 1 were eligible for the Time 2 assessment 2–3 years later (N = 408). All families who participated in the Time 1 assessment were eligible to participate in the Time 3 assessment as young adults. In order to maximize the analytic sample size, data from the Time 1 and Time 3 assessments were used in the current report. At Time 1 siblings were 10–18 years old (average age was 14.5 years for Sibling 1 and 12.9 for Sibling 2) and were on average 1.6 years apart (SD =1.3 years). The median family income was $25,000–$35,000. The Time 3 assessment took place 7–13 years later. At Time 3 siblings were 20–35 years old (average age = 26.8 for Sibling 1 and 25.5 for Sibling 2) and were on average 1.23 years apart (SD =1.6 years). The median family income for parents was higher than at the Time 1 assessment ($60,000–$69,000). At Time 3 the median income was $40,000–$49,000 for Sibling 1 and $30,000–$39,000 for Sibling 2. Of the original 720 families, 516 families could be recontacted, and some data were collected on 413 families. Families who did not participate in the Time 3 assessment were generally younger, F (340) = 1.44, p < .05, and had lower family income, F (361) = 1.27, p < .05, but did not differ from families who did participate in the Time 3 assessment on other demographic and study variable characteristics (e.g., gender of siblings, sibling antisocial behavior, peer college orientation, peer delinquency and substance use, parents’ age or education, and family income). Demographic information for the Time 1 and Time 3 assessments (used in the present report) is presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Sample demographic information

Characteristic Time 1
Time 3
Sibling 1 Sibling 2 Sibling 1 Sibling 2
Age 13.5 (2.0) 12.1 (1.3) 26.8 (2.5) 25.5 (2.6)
Age range 10–18 9–18 20–35 20–35
Age difference 1.6 (1.3) 1.23 (1.6)
Female 48.4% 48%
Married 59% 50%
Mean education 14.9 (2.3) 14.6 (2.3)

Mother Father Mother Father

Parent age 38.1 (5.2) 41.0 (6.5) 51.4 (4.6) 54.2 (6.1)
Parent education (years) 13.8 (2.3) 13.9 (2.7) 14.2 (2.7) 14.6 (2.8)

Note: The values are means (standard deviations).

Procedure

At Time 1 most families were visited twice by two interviewers. Both parents and the two adolescents completed questionnaires and were videotaped during the visit in dyadic, triadic, and tetradic family interactions. Additional questionnaire data were obtained from questionnaires that were mailed ahead to be completed prior to the visit. Family members specified areas of disagreement that were then discussed and videotaped in the 10-min interactions. A global coding system on a 5-point Likert scale was used to rate the videotaped interactions (Hetherington & Clingempeel, 1992). In the present study we focused on the dyadic interactions between the siblings.

Twins were rated for physical similarity (e.g., eye and hair color) by the interviewer, the parents, and self-report using a questionnaire designed for adolescents (Nichols & Bilbro, 1966). If any differences in physical characteristics were reported or if respondents reported that people never were confused about the identity of the twins, the twin pair was classified as DZ. Twelve of the twin pairs could not be classified as MZ or DZ and were excluded from these analyses (<2% of the twin pairs). The accuracy of this method for assigning zygosity has been shown to be more than 95% accurate when compared with DNA tests (Nichols & Bilbro, 1966; Spitz, Moutier, Reed, Busnel, & Marchaland, 1996). Our analysis sample reflects the 708 families for which we have zygosity data. Missing data were accommodated using full information maximum likelihood during analysis.

Measures

Measurement included parent and adolescent reports and videotaped dyadic interactions.

Marital conflict about the child

At Time 1 marital conflict about the child was assessed using an average of mother and father reports of conflict with spouse about each sibling on the Child Rearing Issues-Self and Spouse scale (Hetherington & Clingempeel, 1992). Mothers and fathers reported on how often, on a scale of 0 (never) to 7 (more than once a day), they had arguments about Sibling 1 and Sibling 2 (separately) in the past month (e.g., children’s behaviors toward self and siblings, children’s grades, chores, or allowances). Mother and father reports were moderately correlated (Sibling 1: r = .31, p < .05; Sibling 2: r = .25, p < .05).

Parental monitoring

At Time 1 parental monitoring was computed as the sum of mother and father reports of their knowledge about the adolescent’s friends and whereabouts (Hetherington & Clingempeel, 1992; αs > 0.90). Mothers and fathers were asked 13 items about how much they know about Sibling 1’s and Sibling 2’s lives in several areas (e.g., choice of friends, who they are, and what they are like; and child’s school life, such as teachers, homework, and grades) on a scale of 1 (never) to 5 (always). Mother and father reports were moderately correlated (Sibling 1: r = .46, p < .05; Sibling 2: r = .40, p< .05).

Sibling negativity

Time 1 sibling conflict was assessed using a composite of child, parent, and observer ratings (Furman & Buhrmester, 1985; Hetherington et al., 1999; Schaefer & Edgerton, 1981). Parents reported on the sibling rivalry, aggression, and avoidance subscales of the Sibling Inventory of Behavior (Hetherington & Clingempeel, 1992; αs >0.85). Siblings reported on the amount of criticism they felt from their sibling using the criticism subscale of the Network of Relationship Inventory (Hetherington & Clingempeel, 1992; s αs>0.72), self-report on the sibling rivalry, aggression, and avoidance subscales of the Sibling Inventory of Behavior (Hetherington & Clingempeel, 1992; αs >0.71), the total score for the sibling interaction task (a questionnaire where siblings rate the frequency of disagreement on a number of topics, higher scores indicating more frequent disagreements; Hetherington & Clingempeel, 1992; αs >0.30), and the symbolic and physical aggression subscales of the Conflict Tactics Inventory (Straus, 1979, 1987; αs >0.81). Observers rated anger, coercion, and transactional conflict during the dyadic sibling interactions (intraclass correlations [ICCs] > .62). Scores for parent, adolescent, and observer reports were standardized and summed to form composite sibling negativity scores (r = .24–.60, ps < .05; see Reiss et al., 2000).

Peer group delinquency

Combined mother and father reports were used to index the adolescent’s delinquent peer group (Iervolino et al., 2002; Manke, McGuire, Reiss, Hetherington, & Plomin, 1995). Mothers and fathers reported on Sibling 1’s and Sibling 2’s peer group delinquency using 8 items assessing how like the child’s friends are to adjectives (i.e., delinquent or rebellious) on a 1 (very much unlike) to 4 (very much like) scale. Scores were acceptably reliable (αs > 0.76). Mother and father reports were moderately correlated (Sibling 1: r = .38, p < .05, Sibling 2: r = .35, p < .05).

Initiation of illegal drugs by young adulthood

Substance use was assessed at Time 3 using a construct indicating the number of illicit drugs young adults had initiated (i.e., marijuana, pills, crack, cocaine, LSD, PCP, heroin, mushrooms, inhalants/paint/glue, or meth; Elliott & Huizinga, 1983; Jessor, Donovan, & Widmer, 1980; Jessor & Jessor, 1977). Across siblings, 47% of the sample did not initiate any drugs, 28% of the sample initiated one drug, and 10% of the sample had initiated four or more drugs.

Analytic strategy

Data analyses proceeded in several steps. Child age, sex, the interaction between age and sex, and age difference between nontwin siblings were controlled for by regressing the variance in each variable out of the study variables (McGue & Bouchard, 1984). The continuous residual scores were used in all analyses.

First, phenotypic correlations were computed to examine the associations between interpersonal variables (marital conflict about the child, parental monitoring, sibling negativity, and peer group delinquency) and the number of illegal drugs initiated by young adulthood, as well as the intercorrelations among the interpersonal variables. We also regressed all four interpersonal variables on drug use in a phenotypic multiple regression to determine whether marital conflict about the child, parental monitoring, sibling negativity, and peer group delinquency each explained unique variance in the number of illegal drugs initiated by young adulthood.

Second, for number of drugs initiated by young adulthood, means and standard deviations were examined across sibling type (MZ twins, DZ twins, and FI in nondivorced families; FS, HS, US in stepfamilies), and sibling ICCs were computed using double-entered data separately for each sibling type for each sample (correlating Sibling 1 with Sibling 2). Genetic influences are suggested if ICCs decrease according to decreasing genetic similarity (MZ > DZ = FI = FS > HS > US). Shared environmental influences are suggested if ICCs are similar for genetically nonidentical siblings (DZ = FI = FS > 0, HS > O, US > 0). Finally, MZ ICCs < 1 indicate non-shared environmental influences.

Third, we conducted multivariate Cholesky biometrical models of the raw data using Mx (Neale, 1994) to systematically estimate genetic and environmental influences on the associations among the interpersonal relationship variables and initiation of illegal drug use. Figure 1 depicts the model for one sibling. The parameter estimates are set to be equal across the two sibling pairs, with the correlations between distinctive pairs of latent factors representing genetic influences (i.e., A1 for Sibling 1 and A1 for Sibling 2, A2 for Sibling 1 and A2 for Sibling 2, A3 for Sibling1 and A3 for Sibling 2, A4 for Sibling 1 and A4 for Sibling 2, and A5 for Sibling 1 and A5 for Sibling 2) set according to the genetic similarity of the siblings. Correlations between pairs of distinct shared environmental latent variables (C1–C5) were each set to 1, and nonshared environmental latent factors (E1–E5) were uncorrelated with each other or any other latent factor.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

An illustration of the multivariate Cholesky model. The latent factors A1, C1, and E1 refer to the respective genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on marital conflict. A2, C2, and E2 refer to the respective unique genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on parental monitoring, or the genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on parental monitoring that are not shared with marital conflict. A3, C3, and E3 refer to the respective unique genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on sibling negativity, not in common with either marital conflict or parental monitoring. A4, C4, and E4 refer to the respective unique genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on peer group delinquency not shared with any of the previous constructs; and A5, C5, and E5 represent the respective unique genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on number of drugs initiated by young adulthood not in common with any of the predictors. All paths are estimated in the fully saturated model.

The order in which variables are entered into the model is important in multivariate Cholesky decompositions because the genetic and environmental influences contributing to associations among the first and subsequent variables are constrained by the genetic and environmental influences on the first variable entered into the model. As described earlier, we derived the order of our variables based on the patterns reviewed in the literature from predictors most distal to most proximal to initiation of drug use: (a) marital conflict about the child, (b) parental monitoring, (c) sibling negativity, (d) peer group delinquency, and (e) the number of drugs initiated by young adulthood, the outcome.

Multivariate Cholesky decompositions first estimate the genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on the first variable (i.e., marital conflict about the child). Then, the association between the first and second variables (i.e., the association between marital conflict about the child and parental monitoring) is decomposed into genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences, followed by the association between the first and third variables, and so on. Unique genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences are then estimated for the second variable (parental monitoring), followed by the associations between the second and third variables, second and fourth variables, and so forth. This is repeated for each subsequent variable entered into the model, culminating in estimating the unique genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on the outcome variable (i.e., number of drugs initiated by young adulthood).

We tested a saturated model estimating all genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental paths possible, and then systematically dropped paths that were not significantly different from zero (as judged by 95% confidence interval [CI]). Model differences were explored using a nested model approach that compared the constrained models to the saturated model. Differences in chi-square estimates between models tested whether the constrained models resulted in a significant decrement of model fit. If a model removing paths did not result in a significant decrement in fit from the saturated model, we concluded the path was no different from zero and therefore the model dropping those paths (the constrained model) was the better fitting and more parsimonious model. We first dropped each nonsignificant path individually. If there was a significant decrement in model fit (despite non-significant CIs in the full model), the path was considered significant and included during the next step in model fitting, when nonsignificant paths were dropped in pairs. We also tested dropped paths in larger groups until we arrived at the best fitting model. Results from both the full model and the best fitting model are presented.

Model assumptions

These types of behavioral genetic models assume that shared and nonshared environmental effects are the same across sibling types and that assortative mating (i.e., nonrandom mating that could result in suppressed genetic influences because DZs and full siblings would share more than 50% of their genes) is not occurring. These assumptions have previously been tested and have been generally found to be valid (Kendler, 2001; Pike, McGuire, Hetherington, & Reiss, 1996). Further, there were no systematic differences for the validity of equal twin and sibling environments in NEAD (Reiss et al., 2000), indicating that the equal environments assumption is reasonable for the current study. We also are assuming that the order of influence of predictors flows in the direction previously specified.

Results

Descriptive statistics

Behavioral genetic models are also particularly sensitive to skewness (Purcell, 2002). Therefore, we used log transformations to obtain a normal distribution for both marital conflict about the child and peer group delinquency. We also used SAS PROC RANK to normalize data by substituting values with ranks using a Blom transformation, to assure that smaller variations within the range of normality of skewness and kurtosis in the distributions of each variable would not affect modeling (Eaves et al., 1997). The data were double entered prior to control for within-family confounds before performing any ICCs and phenotypic associations.

Genetic and environmental influences on drug use

The intraclass twin/sibling correlations showed a slightly decreasing pattern, suggesting substantial shared environment, but also genetic and nonshared environmental influences on drug use (MZ = 0.71, DZ = 0.70, FI = 0.40, FS = 0.56, HS = 0.31, US = 0.41). Univariate biometric analyses supported this pattern of findings. Results showed significant genetic (32%; 95% CI = 0.06–0.57), shared environmental (41%; 95% CI = 0.19–0.59), and nonshared environmental influences (27%; 95% CI = 0.18–0.41) for the number of drugs initiated by young adulthood with the variance nearly evenly split between the three.

Phenotypic associations

Phenotypic correlations are presented in Table 2. Overall, each interpersonal predictor was positively associated with the number of drugs initiated in young adulthood (r > .13, ps < .05), except parental monitoring, which was related to fewer drugs initiated, as expected (r = −.17, p < .05). All of the interpersonal predictors were correlated with one another (rs > .06, ps < .05).

Table 2.

Correlations among study variables

Marital Conflict Parental Monitoring Sibling Negativity Peer Delinquency
Marital conflict
Parental monitoring .06*
N = 657
Sibling negativity .22*
N = 667
.09*
N = 646
Peer delinquency .14*
N = 695
.27*
N = 658
.13*
N = 669
Number of drugs initiated .13*
N = 257
–.17*
N = 246
.15*
N = 250
.25*
N = 259
*

p < .05.

The next step was to conduct a multiple regression analysis to estimate the extent to which each interpersonal variable predicted unique variance in the outcome, drug initiation. Marital conflict, parental monitoring, sibling negativity, and peer group delinquency were simultaneously added as predictors, with drug initiation as the dependent variable. The overall model was significant, F (4, 472) = 12.74, p < .001, and predicted about 10% of the variance in drug initiation (R2 = .097). Parental monitoring (β = 0.08, t = 2.05, p < .05), sibling negativity (β = 0.09, t = 2.21, p < .05), and peer delinquency (β = 0.19, t = 4.49, p < .001) each contributed significant unique variance in drug initiation, controlling for the other interpersonal variables. Marital conflict contributed marginally significant unique variance in drug initiation, controlling for the other interpersonal variable (β = 0.08, t = 1.85, p = .065). Thus, we continued with the proposed model including all four interpersonal variables and drug initiation for the behavioral genetic analyses.

Model-fitting results

Unstandardized parameter estimates and 95% CIs for the fully saturated and best fitting models are presented in Table 3 (fully saturated), Table 4 (best fitting), and Figure 2 (best fitting). The constrained model (dropping nonsignificant paths) fit the data better than the fully saturated model, fully saturated model: −2 log likelihood (−2LL) (3088) = 7326.5, Akaike information criterion (AIC) = 1150.3; constrained model: −2LL (5971) = 14173.6, AIC = 2231.6; χ2 diff (22) = 30.3, p = .11, AIC diff = −13.7.

Table 3.

Model fitting results for the fully saturated model

A1 C1 E1 A2 C2 E2 A3 C3 E3 A4 C4 E4 A5 C5 E5
Marital conflict 0.50 (0.43, 0.57) 0.84 (0.79, 0.87) 0.23 (0.19, 0.28)
Parental monitoring 0.02 (−0.14, 0.18) 0.03 (−0.09, 0.15) −0.01 (−0.16, 0.14) 0.18 (0.04, 0.40) 0.71 (0.63, 0.76) 0.68 (0.62, 0.74)
Sibling negativity 0.1 (0.01, 0.19) 0.13 (−0.01, 0.25) −0.04 (−0.11, 0.04) −0.03 (−0.29, 0.26) 0.09 (−0.05, 0.23) 0.01 (−0.05, 0.07) 0.27 (0.06, 0.37) 0.89 (0.85, 0.92) 0.31 (0.26, 0.37)
Peer group delinquency 0.26 (0.13, 0.38) −0.02 (−0.14, 0.10) 0.03 (−0.04, 0.10) 0.77 (0.20, 0.86) 0.24 (0.09, 0.37) −0.01 (−0.07, 0.05) 0.05 (−0.70, 0.73) 0.08 (−0.03, 0.18) 0.02 (−0.04, 0.09) −0.04 (−0.76, 0.76) 0.45 (0.27, 0.57) 0.26 (0.21, 0.31)
Drug use 0.21 (0.01, 0.40) 0 (−0.13, 0.14) 0 (−0.14, 0.16) 0.26 (−0.41, 0.69) 0.15 (−0.03, 0.31) 0.01 (−0.11, 0.12) 0.1 (−0.45, 0.62) 0.1 (−0.02, 0.18) 0.01 (−0.04, 0.09) 0.51 (−0.75, 0.75) −0.14 (−0.49, 0.11) 0.07 (−0.07, 0.71) 0 (−0.71, 0.71) 0.55 (−0.71, 0.71) 0.51 (0.42, 0.63)

Note: Unstandardized beta weights are presented (95% confidence intervals). The latent factors A1, C1, and E1 refer to the respective genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on marital conflict. A2, C2, and E2 refer to the respective unique genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on parental monitoring or the genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on parental monitoring that are not shared with marital conflict. A3, C3, and E3 refer to the respective unique genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on sibling negativity not in common with either marital conflict or parental monitoring. A4, C4, and E4 refer to the respective unique genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on peer group delinquency not shared with any of the previous constructs; and A5, C5, and E5 represent the respective unique genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on number of drugs initiated by young adulthood not in common with any of the predictors.

Table 4.

Model fitting results for the best-fitting model

A1 C1 E1 A2 C2 E2 A3 C3 E3 A4 C4 E4 A5 C5 E5
Marital conflict 0.49 (0.43, 0.54) 0.83 (0.80, 0.86) 0.26 (0.22, 0.30)
Parental monitoring 0
0.05a (0, 0.12) 0
0.1 (0.02, 0.18) 0.71 (0.67, 0.75) 0.7 (0.66, 0.74)
Sibling negativity 0.11 (0.06, 0.16) 0.2 (0.12, 0.28) 0
0
0
0
0.27 (0.12, 0.35) 0.88 (0.85, 0.90) 0.33 (0.29, 0.38)
Peer group delinquency 0.26 (0.19, 0.33) 0
0
0.74 (0.68, 0.79) 0.27 (0.18, 0.36) 0
0
0
0
0
0.51 (0.42, 0.58) 0.25 (0.22, 0.29)
Drug Use 0.21 (0.07, 0.35) 0
0
0.21 (0.10, 0.32) 0.17 (0.04, 0.27) 0
0
0.11 (0.01, 0.20) 0
0
0
0
0
0.71 (0.62, 0.77) 0.62 (0.55, 0.69)

Note: Unstandardized beta weights are presented (95% confidence intervals). Parameter estimates of 0 indicate that the parameter was not included in the best-fitting model. The latent factors A1, C1, and E1 refer to the respective genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on marital conflict. A2, C2, and E2 refer to the respective unique genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on parental monitoring or the genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on parental monitoring that are not shared with marital conflict. A3, C3, and E3 refer to the respective unique genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on sibling negativity not in common with either marital conflict or parental monitoring. A4, C4, and E4 refer to the respective unique genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on peer group delinquency not shared with any of the previous constructs; and A5, C5, and E5 represent the respective unique genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on number of drugs initiated by young adulthood not in common with any of the predictors.

a

A parameter estimate is not significantly different from zero but is required to be in the model (or the model fails).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Significant paths for the best fitting model. The latent factors A1, C1, and E1 refer to the respective genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on marital conflict. A2, C2, and E2 refer to the respective unique genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on parental monitoring, or the genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on parental monitoring that are not shared with marital conflict. A3, C3, and E3 refer to the respective unique genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences on sibling negativity, not in common with either marital conflict or parental monitoring. C4 and E4 refer to the respective unique shared and nonshared environmental influences on peer group delinquency not shared with any of the previous constructs, and C5 and E5 represent the respective unique shared and nonshared environmental influences on number of drugs initiated by young adulthood not in common with any of the predictors. Significant paths are shown with a solid line. The nonsignificant path estimate is depicted with a hashed line.

The best fitting model (Figure 2) revealed a genetic factor explaining variance common to marital conflict about the child, sibling negativity, peer group delinquency, and drug initiation (A1). There was also a discrete genetic factor explaining variance in parental monitoring, peer group delinquency, and drug initiation that was distinct from the genetic influences shared with marital conflict (A2). A final genetic factor explained unique variance in sibling negativity (A3). Peer group delinquency and drug initiation showed no unique genetic influences after accounting for the genetic variance shared with marital conflict and parental monitoring. There was a shared environmental factor explaining variance in marital conflict about the child and sibling negativity (C1). A separate shared environmental factor explained variance shared by parental monitoring, peer group delinquency, and drug initiation that was distinct from variance shared with marital conflict (C2). A third distinct shared environmental factor explained shared environmental influences on sibling negativity and initiation of drug use independent of the shared environmental influences on marital conflict and parental monitoring (C3). Finally, there were unique shared environmental factors influencing peer group delinquency (C4) and drug initiation (C5). Nonshared environmental factors did not explain any of the covariation among interpersonal variables and drug use. Instead, there were discrete nonshared environmental influences for each construct (E1–E5).

Discussion

This study sought to clarify the associations among interpersonal relationships within the family and peer group characteristics during adolescence influencing initiation of drug use by young adulthood. Findings indicate that interpersonal relationships during adolescence are associated with initiation of drug use for both genetic and environmental reasons and that these associations cluster in meaningful ways, suggesting four distinct factors for the initiation of drug use. In other words, marital conflict, sibling negativity, and peer group delinquency are all associated with initiation of drug use and with one another for shared genetic, but not environmental, reasons. Parental monitoring and peer group delinquency are associated with initiation of drug use through both genetic and shared environmental factors. Finally, an additional shared environmental factor is common to only sibling negativity and initiation of drug use. It is interesting that even after all of the variance shared with interpersonal relationships during middle adolescence had been accounted for there were still sizable and significant shared and non-shared environmental influences on initiation of drug use by young adulthood but no remaining genetic influences. These findings and their implications are discussed.

The first genetic pathway identified in this report reflects adolescent characteristics eliciting conflict in their relationships and also increasing their likelihood of associating with deviant peers and initiating drug use. This pathway is consistent with the coercive family cycle well documented by Patterson (1982) and others (e.g., Compton et al., 2003; Conger, Ge, Elder, Lorenz, & Simons, 1994; Dishion, Duncan, Eddy, Fagot, & Fetrow, 1994; Loeber & Dishion, 1984). In his initial work on the coercive cycle, Patterson focused on families of younger aggressive boys. He was clear that cycles of aggression between parent and child were initiated by the child, a finding that would fit with the importance of the child’s genetic influences shown here. However, our study arrived on the developmental scene later than Patterson’s. Here deviant child behaviors may be having a wider effect, on sibling and on marital conflict about the adolescent. Patterson tracked the links between the coercive cycles in the family and the child’s entering delinquent peer groups with the link occurring through the school. Aggressive boys were not only aggressive at home but also aggressive at school where, rejected by nonaggressive and/or nondeviant peers, they sought the company of other deviant children. By adolescence, the child’s genetically influenced characteristics seem, from our findings, to have had a detrimental impact on multiple relationships and thereby have increased their risk for adult substance use.

The second genetically influenced pathway centers on inadequate parental monitoring, peer group delinquency, and initiation of illegal drug use and may reflect lack of adolescent disclosure of their whereabouts and activities. In other words, the genetic influences of the child that influence the likelihood that they will elicit monitoring from their parents are correlated with their association with deviant peers and their likelihood of initiating illegal drug use by young adulthood. This pattern of findings is consistent with the adolescent disclosure explanation of parental monitoring and knowledge first proposed by Stattin and Kerr a decade ago (Kerr & Stattin, 2000; Stattin & Kerr, 2000). According to this explanation, the effects of parental monitoring on adolescent delinquent behavior is not the result of parents’ active efforts to find out who their children are associating with and where and what they are doing but is instead due to their adolescent’s disclosure of their activities to their parents.

We also identified two factors shaped by environments siblings share. Recent studies have drawn attention to this domain of influences, especially for family relationships, externalizing behaviors, and drug use. For example, as behavioral genetic studies include more direct measures of the environment like parenting behaviors, shared environmental influences have been found to account for substantial proportions of the total variance (e.g., Elkins, McGue, & Iacono, 1997; Plomin, Reiss, Hetherington, & Howe, 1994). Shared environmental influences have consistently been indicated for adolescent delinquency and antisocial behaviors (Rhee & Waldman, 2002), and a recent meta-analysis found evidence for significant and meaningful shared environmental influences on behaviors within the externalizing and internalizing spectrums (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder was an exception with no shared environmental influence; Burt, 2009). Given the findings that shared environmental influences are important for both interpersonal relationships and measures of adjustment, identifying two shared environmental factors that explain variation of initiation of illegal drug use is especially interesting.

The first shared environmental factor reflects a different type of parental monitoring effect: one that is parent driven rather than child driven. In this case, inadequate parental monitoring, peer group delinquency, and initiation of drug use covary because of nongenetic influences that make family members similar. Thus, consistent with the literature on parental monitoring and drug use (e.g., Dishion & Tipsord, 2011), parents who are more effective monitors have children who are less likely to associate with deviant peers and to initiate drug use and these associations. A parsimonious explanation for this sequence is that ineffective parental monitoring initiates a sequence of events that pivots around subsequent peer group delinquency and risk for substance abuse. The developmental sequence of parental monitoring and peer group delinquency is not established in this study but, as we have reviewed, has been documented elsewhere.

The second pathway shaped by shared environment reflects what may be a sibling deviancy training (e.g., Compton et al., 2003) effect, with shared environmental influences contributing to the covariation of sibling negativity and initiation of illegal drug use. These findings may provide one of the clearest examples of sibling deviancy training to date because this construct includes only the shared environmental influences that are un-correlated with a more general family climate construct (marital conflict about the child) and are uncorrelated with peer group delinquency. In fact, another, distinct shared environmental factor emerged that seems to represent a general family climate of conflict and negativity for associations between marital conflict about the child and sibling deviancy. This family climate shared environmental factor, however, is uncorrelated with the sibling deviancy shared environmental factor.

Limitations and future directions

There are a number of limitations of the current report. We cannot clearly distinguish among the different types of genotype–environment correlations that may be operating, and it is likely that many different types of genotype–environment correlation are operating simultaneously. However, we can say that we find clear evidence for genotype–environment correlation in that genetic influences of the child explain associations among interpersonal relationships and initiation of drug use. Another limitation of this report is the cross-sectional nature of the design. Although we assessed the sample longitudinally, the age range was wide at each assessment. Nonetheless, all subjects who participated during the young adult assessment were beyond the age at which initiation of drug use typically occurs. The outcome variable, initiation of any illegal drug, could be seen as a shortcoming of the current report. Based on previous work that has found clear evidence for a common genetic liability for use of illegal drugs (Kendler et al., 2003; Kendler, Karkowski, Corey, Prescott, & Neale, 1999; Kendler, Karkowski, Neale, & Prescott, 2000; Kendler, Karkowski, & Prescott, 1999), we feel that this aggregate variable is appropriate. Finally, the use of parent reports of adolescent peer group characteristics limits our confidence that this is an index of the actual characteristics of the adolescent’s peer group characteristics. Prior research using the NEAD sample using this measure and the child report measure (collected only at Time 2) found different patterns of genetic and environmental influences (Iervolino et al., 2002; Manke et al., 1995). Parent reports showed more genetic influences, while child self-reports showed primarily nonshared environmental and modest shared environmental influences. Because the child self-report version was not administered at Time 1, we were limited to parent reports of peer group characteristics, and thus, those findings should be interpreted with some caution.

Despite these limitations, this study has clear implications for our understanding of the pathogenesis and prevention of drug use in early adulthood. First, it clarifies that, for samples similar in demographic characteristics to the one used here, environmental causes of substance abuse are important. Our data emphasize a pattern of recent findings that siblings are an important subsystem in the family and are a potential breeding ground for deviant behavior and substance abuse (e.g., Compton et al., 2003; Rende et al., 2005; Slomkowski et al., 2005). Preventionists often overlook siblings in their exclusive attention on other aspects of the family.

Second, our findings provide some support for both sides of the current debate about the pivotal role of parental monitoring in the initiation of drug use. Stattin and Kerr (2000) have argued, as we noted, that inadequate parental monitoring reflects problems of the child: they do not disclose what they do and hence the parents cannot act to prevent or reduce deviant behavior. Findings from this study give only partial support to that view. The shared environmental pathway we identified, the one that pivots around parental monitoring, is not consistent with Stattin and Kerr’s argument. Taken together, our findings support the position that inadequate parental monitoring has two sources: a child problem, perhaps inadequate disclosure, and a parent problem, inadequate surveillance. Moreover, these are noncontingent and separable family problems. These findings suggest some families may show one type of problem and not the other, thus assessing families based on this critical distinction and tailoring interventions to improve child disclosure, where warranted, or to embolden parental surveillance, where warranted, or both is one way that these findings could be used to reduce the likelihood of the initiation of drug use.

Third, the lack of genetic influences on initiation of drug use by young adulthood that are not correlated with interpersonal relationships during adolescence is a novel finding that could have important implications. Our model accounts for all the genetic influences on initiation of drug use. It weights heavily a general hypothesis that the mechanisms by which genetic influences on drug use are expressed are mediated via their impact on family and peer relationships. If their influence on these relationships could be blunted, then the adverse effect of these genetic influences could be partially or entirely prevented. We need, of course, more rigorous studies here to establish, with certainty, that the genetic influences on relationship systems are not just epiphenomena. The surest way to do this is to conduct preventive interventions within the context of a twin or adoption design. Interventions aimed at marital conflict and sibling deviancy training should, in such a design, eliminate entirely the heritability of substance use. The data we report here not only suggest the importance of this crucial next step in drug abuse prevention but also raise a strong caution about the current passion to use gene identification studies to identify “druggable targets” (Uhl et al., 2008) for the prevention or treatment of substance abuse. Family and peer relationships now clearly qualify as intermediate phenotypes and, if genetically informed randomized controlled trials support their causal role in expression of genetic influences on drug abuse, they become legitimate therapeutic targets not for drugs, with all their unwanted side effects, but for psychosocial interventions that may be safer and equally effective.

Acknowledgments

The Nonshared Environment in Adolescent Development project was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant R01MH43373 (Time 1, David Reiss, Principal Investigator [PI]) and Grant R01MH59014 (Time 3, Jenae M. Neiderhiser, PI) and by the William T. Grant Foundation (Time 1, David Reiss, PI).

References

  1. Agrawal A, Lynskey MT. Does gender contribute to heterogeneity in criteria for cannabis abuse and dependence? Results from the national epidemiological survey on alcohol and related conditions. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2007;88:300–307. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Agrawal A, Lynskey MT. Are there genetic influences on addiction: Evidence from family, adoption and twin studies. Addiction. 2008;103:1069–1081. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02213.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Amato PR. Psychological distress and the recall of childhood family characteristics. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1991;53:1011–1019. [Google Scholar]
  4. Avenevoli S, Merikangas KR. Familial influences on adolescent smoking. Addiction. 2003;98:1–20. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.98.s1.2.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Beaver KM, DeLisi M, Wright JP, Vaughn MG. Gene–environment interplay and delinquent involvement: Evidence of direct, indirect, and interactive effects. Journal of Adolescent Research. 2009;24:39. [Google Scholar]
  6. Beaver KM, Shutt JE, Boutwell BB, Ratchford M, Roberts K, Barnes JC. Genetic and environmental influences on levels of self-control and delinquent peer affiliation: Results from a longitudinal sample of adolescent twins. Criminal Justice and Behavior. 2009;36:8. [Google Scholar]
  7. Brook JS, Kessler RC, Cohen P. The onset of marijuana use from preadolescence and early adolescence to young adulthood. Development and Psychopathology. 1999;11:901–914. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499002370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brown BB, Mounts N, Lamborn SD, Steinberg L. Parenting practices and peer group affiliation in adolescence. Child Development. 1993;64:467–482. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb02922.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Buehler C, Benson MJ, Gerard JM. Interparental hostility and early adolescent problem behavior: The mediating role of specific aspects of parenting. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2006;16:265–291. [Google Scholar]
  10. Bullock BM, Deater-Deckard K, Leve LD. Deviant peer affiliation and problem behavior: A test of genetic and environmental influences. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2006;34:29–41. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-9004-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Burt SA. Rethinking environmental contributions to child and adolescent psychopathology: A meta-analysis of shared environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin. 2009;135:6. doi: 10.1037/a0015702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Burt SA, Krueger RF, McGue M, Iacono W. Parent–child conflict and the comorbidity among childhood externalizing disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:505–513. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.5.505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Burt SA, McGue M, Krueger RF, Iacono WG. How are parent–child conflict and childhood externalizing symptoms related over time? Results from a genetically informative cross-lagged study. Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17:145–165. doi: 10.1017/S095457940505008X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Burt SA, McGue M, Krueger RF, Iacono WG. Environmental contributions to adolescent delinquency: A fresh look at the shared environment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2007;35:787–800. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9135-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bussell DA, Neiderhiser JM, Pike A, Plomin R, Simmens S, Howe GW, et al. Adolescents’ relationships to siblings and mothers: A multivariate genetic analysis. Developmental Psychology. 1999;35:1248–1259. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Button TMM, Lau JYF, Maughan B, Eley TC. Parental punitive discipline, negative life events and gene–environment interplay in the development of externalizing behavior. Psychological Medicine. 2008;38:29–39. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Chen CY, Storr CL, Anthony JC. Influences of parenting practices on the risk of having a chance to try cannabis. Pediatrics. 2005;115:1631–1639. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-1926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cheng TC, Lo CC. The roles of parenting and child welfare services in alcohol use by adolescents. Children and Youth Services Review. 2010;32:38–43. [Google Scholar]
  19. Chilcoat HD, Anthony JC. Impact of parent monitoring on initiation of drug use through late childhood. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1996;35:91–100. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199601000-00017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cleveland HH, Wiebe RP, Rowe DC. Sources of exposure to smoking and drinking friends among adolescents: A behavioral–genetic evaluation. Journal of Genetic Psychology. 2005;166:153–169. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Compton K, Snyder J, Schrepferman L, Bank L, Shortt JW. The contribution of parents and siblings to antisocial and depressive behavior in adolescents: A double jeopardy coercion model. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15:163–182. doi: 10.1017/s0954579403000099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Conger RD, Ge X, Elder GH, Lorenz FO, Simons RL. Economic stress, coercive family process, and developmental problems of adolescents. Child Development. 1994;65:541–561. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Crano WD, Siegel JT, Alvaro EM, Lac A, Hemovich V. The at-risk adolescent marijuana nonuser: Expanding the standard distinction. Prevention Science. 2008;9:129–137. doi: 10.1007/s11121-008-0090-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Cummings E. Marital conflict and children’s functioning. Social Development. 1994;3:16–36. [Google Scholar]
  25. Cummings E, Davies PT. Effects of marital conflict on children: Recent advances and emerging themes in process-oriented research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2002;43:31–63. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Derringer J, Krueger RF, McGue M, Iacono WG. Genetic and environmental contributions to the diversity of substances used in adolescent twins: A longitudinal study of age and sex effects. Addiction. 2008;103:3. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02305.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Dishion TJ, Capaldi D, Spracklen KM, Li F. Peer ecology of male adolescent drug use. Development and Psychopathology. 1995;7:803–824. [Google Scholar]
  28. Dishion TJ, Duncan TE, Eddy JM, Fagot BI, Fetrow R. The world of parents and peers: Coercive exchanges and children’s social adaptation. Social Development. 1994;3:255–268. [Google Scholar]
  29. Dishion TJ, Loeber R. Adolescent marijuana and alcohol use: The role of parents and peers revisited. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 1985;11:11–25. doi: 10.3109/00952998509016846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Dishion TJ, McCord J, Poulin F. When interventions harm: Peer groups and problem behavior. American Psychologist. 1999;54:755–764. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.54.9.755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Dishion TJ, Owen LD. A longitudinal analysis of friendships and substance use: Bidirectional influence from adolescence to adulthood. Developmental Psychology. 2002;38:480–491. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.38.4.480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Dishion TJ, Tipsord JM. Peer contagion in child and adolescent social and emotional development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2011;62:189–214. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Disney ER, Elkins IJ, McGue M, Iacono WJ. Effects of ADHD, conduct disorder, and gender on substance use and abuse in adolescence. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;156:1515–1521. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.10.1515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. D’Onofrio BM, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, Slutske WS, Heath AC, Madden PA, et al. A genetically informed study of marital instability and its association with offspring psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2005;114:570–586. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Dorius CJ, Bahr SJ, Hoffmann JP, Harmon EL. Parenting practices as moderators of the relationship between peers and adolescent marijuana use. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2004;66:163–178. [Google Scholar]
  36. Duncan SC, Duncan TE, Biglan A, Ary D. Contributions of the social context to the development of adolescent substance use: A multivariate latent growth modeling approach. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 1998;50:57–71. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(98)00006-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Eaves LJ, Silberg JL, Maes HH, Simonoff E, Pickles A, Rutter M, et al. Genetics and developmental psychopathology: 2. The main effects of genes and environment on behavioral problems in the Virginia Twin Study of Adolescent Behavioral Development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 1997;38:965–980. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01614.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Elkins IJ, Iacono WG, Doyle AE. Characteristics associated with the persistence of antisocial behavior: Results from recent longitudinal research. Aggression and Violent Behavior. 1997;2:101–124. [Google Scholar]
  39. Elkins IJ, McGue M, Iacono WG. Genetic and environmental influences on parent–son relationships: Evidence for increasing genetic influence during adolescence. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33:351–363. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.33.2.351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Elliott DS, Huizinga D. Social class and delinquent-behavior in a national youth panel—1976–1980. Criminology. 1983;21:149–177. [Google Scholar]
  41. El-Sheikh M, Elmore-Staton L. The link between marital conflict and child adjustment: Parent–child conflict and perceived attachments as mediators, potentiators, and mitigators of risk. Development and Psychopathology. 2004;16:631–648. doi: 10.1017/s0954579404004705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Ennett ST, Bauman KE, Hussong A, Faris R, Foshee VA, Cai L, et al. The peer context of adolescent substance use: Findings from social network analysis. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2006;16:159–186. [Google Scholar]
  43. Ernst M, Luckenbaugh DA, Moolchan ET, Leff MK, Allen R, Eshel N, et al. Behavioral predictors of substance-use initiation in adolescents with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2006;117:2030–2039. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-0704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Evrony H, Ulbricht J, Neiderhiser JM. Understanding gene, environment, and Gene Environment interaction effects: The example of childhood externalizing disorders. In: Tercyak KP, editor. Handbook of genomics and the family: Psychosocial context for children and adolescents. New York: Springer; 2010. pp. 59–86. [Google Scholar]
  45. Feinberg ME, Reiss D, Neiderhiser JM, Hetherington EM. Differential association of family subsystem negativity on siblings’ maladjustment: Using behavior genetic methods to test process theory. Journal of Family Psychology. 2005;19:601–610. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.19.4.601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Fletcher AC, Steinberg L, Williams-Wheeler M. Parental influences on adolescent problem behavior: Revisiting Stattin and Kerr. Child Development. 2004;75:781–796. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00706.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Furman W, Buhrmester D. Children’s perceptions of the personal relationships in their social networks. Developmental Psychology. 1985;21:1016–1024. [Google Scholar]
  48. Gable S, Belsky J, Crnic K. Marriage, parenting, and child development: Progress and prospects. Journal of Family Psychology. 1992;5:276–294. [Google Scholar]
  49. Ge X, Conger RD, Cadoret RJ, Neiderhiser JM, Yates W, Troughton E. The developmental interface between nature and nurture: A mutual influence model of child antisocial behavior and parent behaviors. Developmental Psychology. 1996;32:574–589. [Google Scholar]
  50. Gillespie NA, Neale MC, Jacobson K, Kendler KS. Modeling the genetic and environmental association between peer group deviance and cannabis use in male twins. Addiction. 2009;104:420–429. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02457.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Harakeh Z, Neiderhiser JM, Spotts EL, Engels R, Scholte RHJ, Reiss D. Genetic factors contribute to the association between peers and young adults smoking: Univariate and multivariate behavioral genetic analyses. Addictive Behaviors. 2008;33:1113–1122. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.02.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Harden KP, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, D’Onofrio BM, Slutske WS, Heath AC, et al. Marital conflict and conduct problems in children of twins. Child Development. 2007;78:1–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00982.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Harold GT, Conger RD. Marital conflict and adolescent distress: The role of adolescent awareness. Child Development. 1997;68:333–350. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb01943.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Helstrom A, Bryan A, Hutchison KE, Riggs PD, Blechman EA. Tobacco and alcohol use as an explanation for the association between externalizing behavior and illicit drug use among delinquent adolescents. Prevention Science. 2004;5:267–277. doi: 10.1023/b:prev.0000045360.23290.8f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Hetherington EM, Clingempeel WG. Coping with marital transitions: A family systems perspective. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1992;57(2–3 Serial No 227):1–238. [Google Scholar]
  56. Hetherington EM, Henderson SH, O’Connor TG, Insabella GM, Taylor L, Anderson ER, et al. Adolescent siblings in step-families: Family functioning and adolescent adjustment. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1999;64(4 Serial No 259):1–200. [Google Scholar]
  57. Hopfer CJ, Crowley TJ, Hewitt JK. Review of twin and adoption studies of adolescent substance use. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2003;42:710–719. doi: 10.1097/01.CHI.0000046848.56865.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Iervolino AC, Pike A, Manke B, Reiss D, Hetherington E, Plomin R. Genetic and environmental influences in adolescent peer socialization: Evidence from two genetically sensitive designs. Child Development. 2002;73:162–174. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Jessor R, Donovan JE, Widmer K. Psychosocial factors in adolescent alcohol and drug use: The 1978 National Sample Study and the 1974–78 Panel Study. Boulder, CO: University of Colorado, Institute of Behavioral Science; 1980. [Google Scholar]
  60. Jessor R, Jessor SL. Problem behavior and psychosocial development. New York: Academic Press; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  61. Kandel D. Adolescent marihuana use: Role of parents and peers. Science. 1973;181:1067–1070. doi: 10.1126/science.181.4104.1067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Kandel DB. Homophily, selection, and socialization in adolescent friendships. American Journal of Sociology. 1978;84:427–436. [Google Scholar]
  63. Kendler KS. Twin studies of psychiatric illness: An update. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2001;58:1005–1014. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.11.1005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Kendler KS, Baker JH. Genetic influences on measures of the environment: A systematic review. Psychological Medicine. 2007;37:615–626. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706009524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Kendler KS, Jacobson KC, Gardner CO, Gillespie N, Aggen SA, Prescott CA. Creating a social world—A developmental twin study of peer-group deviance. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64:958–965. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.8.958. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Kendler KS, Jacobson K, Myers JM, Eaves LJ. A genetically informative developmental study of the relationship between conduct disorder and peer deviance in males. Psychological Medicine. 2008;38:1001–1011. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001821. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Kendler KS, Jacobson KC, Prescott CA, Neale MC. Specificity of genetic and environmental risk factors for use and abuse/ dependence of cannabis, cocaine, hallucinogens, sedatives, stimulants, and opiates in male twins. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:687–695. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Kendler KS, Karkowski LM, Corey LA, Prescott CA, Neale MC. Genetic and environmental risk factors in the aetiology of illicit drug initiation and subsequent misuse in women. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;175:351–356. doi: 10.1192/bjp.175.4.351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Kendler KS, Karkowski LM, Neale MC, Prescott CA. Illicit psychoactive substance use, heavy use, abuse, and dependence in a US population-based sample of male twins. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2000;57:261–269. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.3.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Kendler KS, Karkowski L, Prescott CA. Hallucinogen, opiate, sedative and stimulant use and abuse in a population-based sample of female twins. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 1999;99:368–376. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1999.tb07243.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Kendler KS, Myers J, Prescott CA. Specificity of genetic and environmental risk factors for symptoms of cannabis, cocaine, alcohol, caffeine, and nicotine dependence. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64:1313–1320. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.11.1313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Kendler KS, Schmitt E, Aggen SH, Prescott CA. Genetic and environmental influences on alcohol, caffeine, cannabis, and nicotine use from early adolescence to middle adulthood. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2008;65:674–682. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.6.674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Kerr M, Stattin HK. What parents know, how they know it, and several forms of adolescent adjustment: Further support for a reinterpretation of monitoring. Developmental Psychology. 2000;36:366–380. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Kerr M, Stattin H, Burk WJ. A reinterpretation of parental monitoring in longitudinal perspective. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2010;20:39–64. [Google Scholar]
  75. Kiesner J, Poulin F, Dishion TJ. Adolescent substance use with friends moderating and mediating effects of parental monitoring and peer activity contexts. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly. 2010;56:529–556. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2010.0002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Kung EM, Farrell AD. The role of parents and peers in early adolescent substance use: An examination of mediating and moderating effects. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2000;9:509–528. [Google Scholar]
  77. Lac A, Crano WD. Monitoring matters: Meta-analytic review reveals the reliable linkage of parental monitoring with adolescent marijuana use. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2009;4:578–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01166.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Loeber R, Dishion TJ. Boys who fight at home and school: Family conditions influencing cross-setting consistency. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1984;52:759–768. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.52.5.759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Lynskey MT, Agrawal A, Heath AC. Genetically informative research on adolescent substance use: Methods, findings, and challenges. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2010;49:1202–1214. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.09.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Manke B, McGuire S, Reiss D, Hetherington EM, Plomin R. Genetic contributions to adolescents’ extrafamilial social interactions: Teachers, best friends, and peers. Social Development. 1995;4:238–256. [Google Scholar]
  81. Martins SS, Storr CL, Alexandre PK, Chilcoat HD. Adolescent ecstasy and other drug use in the National Survey of Parents and Youth: The role of sensation-seeking, parental monitoring and peer’s drug use. Addictive Behaviors. 2008;33:919–933. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. McGue M, Bouchard TJ., Jr Adjustment of twin data for the effects of age and sex. Behavior Genetics. 1984;14:325–343. doi: 10.1007/BF01080045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. McGue M, Sharma A, Benson P. The effect of common rearing on adolescent adjustment: Evidence from a US adoption cohort. Developmental Psychology. 1996;32:604–613. [Google Scholar]
  84. McHale SM, Bissell J, Kim JY. Sibling relationship, family, and genetic factors in sibling similarity in sexual risk. Journal of Family Psychology. 2009;23:562–572. doi: 10.1037/a0014982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Molina BSG, Pelham WE. Childhood predictors of adolescent substance use in a longitudinal study of children with ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2003;112:497–507. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.112.3.497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Narusyte J, Andershed AK, Neiderhiser JM, Lichtenstein P. Aggression as a mediator of genetic contributions to the association between negative parent–child relationships and adolescent antisocial behavior. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2007;16:128–137. doi: 10.1007/s00787-006-0582-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Natsuaki MN, Ge X, Reiss D, Neiderhiser JM. Aggressive behavior between siblings and the development of externalizing problems: Evidence from a genetically sensitive study. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45:11. doi: 10.1037/a0015698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Neale MC. Mx: Statistical modeling. 2. Richmond, VA: Vir-ginia Commonwealth University, Department of Psychiatry; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  89. Neiderhiser JM, Reiss D, Hetherington EM. The Nonshared Environment in Adolescent Development (NEAD) Project: A longitudinal family study of twins and siblings from adolescence to young adulthood. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2007;10:74–83. doi: 10.1375/twin.10.1.74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Neiderhiser JM, Reiss D, Hetherington E, Plomin R. Relationships between parenting and adolescent adjustment over time: Genetic and environmental contributions. Developmental Psychology. 1999;35:680–692. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.35.3.680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Neiderhiser JA, Reiss D, Lichtenstein P, Spotts EL, Ganiban J. Father–adolescent relationships and the role of genotype–environment correlation. Journal of Family Psychology. 2007;21:560–571. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.4.560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Neiderhiser JM, Reiss D, Pedersen NL, Lichtenstein P, Spotts EL, Hansson K. Genetic and environmental influences on mothering of adolescents: A comparison of two samples. Developmental Psychology. 2004;40:335–351. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.3.335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Nichols RC, Bilbro WC. The diagnosis of twin zygosity. Acta Geneticae et Statistica Medica. 1966;16:265–275. doi: 10.1159/000151973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Nonnemaker JM, Crankshaw EC, Shive DR, Hussin AH, Farrelly MC. Inhalant use initiation among U.S. adolescents: Evidence from the National Survey of Parents and Youth using discrete-time survival analysis. Addictive Behaviors. 2011;36:878–881. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.03.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. O’Connor TG, Deater-Deckard K, Fulker D, Rutter M, Plomin R. Genotype–environment correlations in late childhood and early adolescence: Antisocial behavioral problems and coercive parenting. Developmental Psychology. 1998;34:970–981. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.34.5.970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Patterson G. Coercive family process: A social learning approach. Eugene, OR: Castalia; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  97. Peterson JL, Zill N. Marital disruption, parent–child relationships and behavior problems in children. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1986;48:295–307. [Google Scholar]
  98. Pike A, Atzaba-Poria N. Do sibling and friend relationships share the same temperamental origins? A twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2003;44:598–611. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Pike A, Eley TC. Links between parenting and extra-familial relationships: Nature or nurture? Journal of Adolescence. 2009;32:519–533. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Pike A, McGuire S, Hetherington E, Reiss D. Family environment and adolescent depressive symptoms and antisocial behavior: A multivariate genetic analysis. Developmental Psychology. 1996;32:590–604. [Google Scholar]
  101. Pike A, McGuire S, Hetherington EM, Reiss D, Plomin R. Family environment and adolescent depressive symptoms and antisocial behavior: A multivariate genetic analysis. Developmental Psychology. 1996;32:590–603. [Google Scholar]
  102. Piko BF, Kovacs E. Do parents and school matter? Protective factors for adolescent substance use. Addictive Behaviors. 2010;35:53–56. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Plomin R, Reiss D, Hetherington E, Howe GW. Nature and nurture: Genetic contributions to measures of the family environment. Developmental Psychology. 1994;30:32–43. [Google Scholar]
  104. Poulin F, Kiesner J, Pedersen S, Dishion TJ. A short-term longitudinal analysis of friendship selection on early adolescent substance use. Journal of Adolescence. 2011;34:249–256. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Purcell S. Variance components models for gene–environment interaction in twin analysis. Twin Research. 2002;5:554–571. doi: 10.1375/136905202762342026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Rasbash J, Jenkins J, O’Connor TG, Tackett J, Reiss D. A social relations model of observed family negativity and positivity using a genetically informative sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2011;100:474–491. doi: 10.1037/a0020931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Reiss D, Neiderhiser JM, Hetherington EM, Plomin R. The relationship code: Deciphering genetic and social influences on adolescent development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  108. Rende R, Slomkowski C, Lloyd-Richardson E, Niaura R. Sibling effects on substance use in adolescence: Social contagion and genetic relatedness. Journal of Family Psychology. 2005;19:611–618. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.19.4.611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Rhee SH, Hewitt JK, Young SE, Corley RP, Crowley TJ, Stallings MC. Genetic and environmental influences on substance initiation, use, and problem use in adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:1256–1264. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.12.1256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Rhee SH, Waldman ID. Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128:490–529. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Rhoades KA, Leve LD, Harold GT, Neiderhiser JM, Shaw DS, Reiss D. Longitudinal pathways from marital hostility to child anger during toddlerhood: Genetic susceptibility and indirect effects via harsh parenting. Journal of Family Psychology. 2011;25:282–291. doi: 10.1037/a0022886. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Riggins-Caspers KM, Cadoret RJ, Knutson JF, Langbehn D. Biology–environment interaction and evocative biology–environment correlation: Contributions of harsh discipline and parental psychopathology to problem adolescent behaviors. Behavior Genetics. 2003;33:205–220. doi: 10.1023/a:1023434206261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Rowe DC, Gulley BL. Sibling effects on substance use and delinquency. Criminology. 1992;30:217–233. [Google Scholar]
  114. Rowe DC, Rogers JL. Behavioral genetics, adolescent deviance, and “d”: Contributions and issues. In: Adams GR, editor. Advances in adolescent psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  115. Schaefer E, Edgerton M. The sibling inventory of behavior. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina Press; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  116. Siebenbruner J, Englund MM, Egeland B, Hudson K. Developmental antecedents of late adolescence substance use patterns. Development and Psychopathology. 2006;18:551–571. doi: 10.1017/S0954579406060287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Sieving RE, Perry CL, Williams CL. Do friendships change behaviors, or do behaviors change friendships? Examining paths of influence in young adolescents’ alcohol use. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2000;26:27–35. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(99)00056-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Slomkowski C, Rende R, Conger KJ, Simons RL, Conger RD. Sisters, brothers, and delinquency: Evaluating social influence during early and middle adolescence. Child Development. 2001;72:271–283. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Slomkowski C, Rende R, Novak S, Lloyd-Richardson E, Niaura R. Sibling effects on smoking in adolescence: Evidence for social influence from a genetically informative design. Addiction. 2005;100:430–438. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00965.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Spitz E, Moutier R, Reed T, Busnel MC, Marchaland C. Comparative diagnoses of twin zygosity by SSLP variant analysis, questionnaire, and dermatoglyphic analysis. Behavior Genetics. 1996;26:55–63. doi: 10.1007/BF02361159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Stattin H, Kerr M. Parental monitoring: A reinterpretation. Child Development. 2000;71:1072–1085. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Stormshak EA, Comeau CA, Shepard SA. The relative contribution of sibling deviance and peer deviance in the prediction of substance use across middle childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2004;32:635–649. doi: 10.1023/b:jacp.0000047212.49463.c7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Straus MA. Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The Conflict Tactics (CT) Scales. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1979;41:75–85. [Google Scholar]
  124. Straus MA. Conflict Tactics Scales. In: Freedman N, Sherman R, editors. Handbook of measurements for marriage and family therapy. New York: Brunner/Mazel; 1987. pp. 159–161. [Google Scholar]
  125. Uhl GR, Drgon T, Johnson C, Li CY, Contoreggi C, Hess J, et al. Molecular genetics of addiction and related heritable phenotypes: Genome-wide association approaches identify “connectivity constellation” and drug target genes with pleiotropic effects. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2008;1141:318–381. doi: 10.1196/annals.1441.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Ulbricht JA, Neiderhiser JM. Genotype–environment correlation and family relationships. 2010 Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
  127. Walden B, McGue M, Iacono WG, Burt SA, Elkins I. Identifying shared environmental contributions to early substance use: The respective roles of peers and parents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2004;113:440–450. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.3.440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Windle M. Parental, sibling, and peer influences on adolescent substance use and alcohol problems. Applied Developmental Science. 2000;4:98–110. [Google Scholar]
  129. Wu P, Liu XL, Fan B. Factors associated with initiation of ecstasy use among US adolescents: Findings from a national survey. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2010;106:193–198. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.08.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES