Abstract
Objective
To determine whether late-onset schizophrenia (LOS, onset after age 40) should be considered a distinct subtype of schizophrenia.
Method
Participants included 359 normal comparison subjects (NCs) and 854 schizophrenia outpatients age > 40 (110 LOS, 744 early-onset schizophrenia or EOS). Assessments included standardized measures of psychopathology, neurocognition, and functioning.
Results
EOS and LOS groups differed from NCs on all measures of psychopathology and functioning, and most cognitive tests. EOS and LOS groups had similar education, severity of depressive, negative, and deficit symptoms, crystallized knowledge, and auditory working memory, but LOS patients included more women and married individuals, had less severe positive symptoms and general psychopathology, and better processing speed, abstraction, verbal memory, and everyday functioning, and were on lower antipsychotic doses. Most EOS-LOS differences remained significant after adjusting for age, gender, severity of negative or deficit symptoms, and duration of illness.
Conclusions
LOS should be considered a subtype of schizophrenia.
Keywords: Schizophrenia, aging, cognition, negative symptoms, quality of life, positive symptoms
“…more than two-thirds of the cases [of dementia praecox] begin between the fifteenth and thirtieth year…However, there can be no talk of an inviolable connection of dementia praecox with the period of youth… a not inconsiderable number of cases still reach development in the fourth, fifth, and even in the sixth decade”
Emil Kraepelin, 1913 (1)
INTRODUCTION
Over the past century there has been considerable debate regarding late-onset schizophrenia (LOS) (2–6). The roots of this debate were present at the close of the 19th century when Kraepelin conceptualized and labeled schizophrenia as dementia praecox (referring to mental decline starting at a precocious age) (7,8). Kraepelin’s terminology proved controversial (9). As illustrated by the above quote, by 1913 Kraepelin himself abandoned the notion that the onset of this disorder was restricted to adolescence and early adulthood. As Kraepelin (1) and Eugen Bleuler (10) were still actively delineating dementia praecox/schizophrenia from affective psychoses, there were a number of reports in the German literature of schizophrenia-like syndromes with mid- or late-life onset (11). Larger-scale systematic investigation began in the 1940’s with the work of Manfred Bleuler (12), who coined the term “spätschizophrenie” (“late schizophrenia”) to describe persons whose symptoms first manifested after age 40 (11). Excluding individuals whose symptoms might be attributed to amnestic or other neurologic or medical conditions, Manfred Bleuler observed that approximately 15% of schizophrenia patients manifested onset of symptoms after age 40.
Systematic studies of LOS (with onset after age 40) continued in Germany (11,13). In other European countries (especially UK) there were scholarly reports in the 1950s and 1960s of patients with very-late onset (often age 60 or older) psychoses that were described with labels such as “late paraphrenia” (14–16), “persistent persecutory states” (17,18), or “senile schizophrenia” (19). Late-onset psychoses were virtually ignored in American psychiatry until the 1980s (20), but the term “late paraphrenia” gained some traction in Europe and Japan (21,22).
As noted in a review of the literature by Riecher-Rössler, Häfner, & Munk-Jørgensen (23), methodological limitations plagued definitive interpretation and comparison of results from studies of LOS, late paraphrenia, and similar conditions. In addition to inconsistencies in diagnostic terminology and criteria (15,19,22,24), a key methodological problem was use of divergent age cut-offs for what constituted “late onset” (10,14,16,25,26). Whereas LOS, as invested by Manfred Bleuler and subsequent German researchers generally focused on those with onset between ages 40 and 60, studies of late paraphrenia and related psychoses in the UK and elsewhere often focused on persons with onset after age 50, 60, or 65 years. The term “late paraphrenia” was sometimes used as a synonym for LOS but others employed it to describe a more circumscribed paranoid psychosis similar to paraphrenia (without reference to age of onset) that Kraepelin (1) had delineated in 1913 (23,27).
In order to bring greater consistency to the study and clinical recognition/treatment of late-onset psychotic disorders, an international conference was held in 1998, attended by experts in late-life schizophrenia from 12 different countries. This conference resulted in a consensus statement which concluded that LOS, with onset of prodromal symptoms after age 40, had at least face validity (2). The diagnostic label for persons whose symptoms emerged after age 60 was “very-late onset schizophrenia-like psychosis,” reflecting less certainty about the nature of schizophrenia-like symptoms when they manifested at an age at which risk of primary dementias begins to increase. The recommendation to designate LOS as a formal subtype was not incorporated into either the ICD-10 (28) or DSM-IV-TR (29,30). The term “paraphrenia (late)” had been present under “paranoid states” in some earlier editions of the ICD; it was dropped from the initial draft of ICD-10, but that deletion generated controversy so it was added in the final version of ICD-10 but only as a part of “delusional disorder” rather than as a type of LOS (13). As planning progresses for the development of the ICD-11 and DSM-V, now appears an opportune time to consider available empirical data to address the question: Should LOS be considered a formal subtype of schizophrenia?
Implicit in the above question is the issue of what basis any subtyping should be proposed. The present subtyping of schizophrenia is largely based on phenomenology and historical concepts. Most of the schizophrenia subtypes in the ICD-10 (28) and DSM-IV-TR (30) directly parallel subtype distinctions (paranoid, catatonic, undifferentiated, etc.) suggested a century ago by Kraepelin (1) and Bleuler (10). There is little empirical support or clinical utility of these phenomenology-based subtypes. In contrast, age of illness onset has been used to subtype medical disorders such as diabetes mellitus (31), Huntington’s disease (32), and Alzheimer’s disease (33) in a more useful manner. It has also been used to subtype other serious mental illnesses such as depression (34). Early-onset subtypes generally have worse course and outcome than late-onset ones, and also differ in biology (when it is known). Clinically, the distinction between early- versus late-onset illness may be particularly critical for schizophrenia because, when the illness emerges in adolescence or early adulthood, many of the normal psychosocial developmental tasks are interrupted. When the onset is delayed until mid-life, functional skills needed to survive in the adult-world are likely to be better established and ingrained. An optimal way to subtype a disorder should be based on its underlying etiology or at least known pathophysiology (35); however, despite advances in research (36,37), there are presently no proven biomarkers of schizophrenia (38), let alone its subtypes. Therefore, clinical relevance becomes the main criterion for subtyping schizophrenia.
Paralleling the historical lack of research attention to LOS that characterized American psychiatry until the 1980s (20,39), most of the contemporary published research on LOS originates from outside North America. We searched the PubMed database for articles published between January 1, 2004 and August 18, 2009; of 18 citations, 6 were published in non-English language journals; of the remaining 12, only one originated from the US (40), whereas four came, at least in part, from UK (41–44), three from Germany (45–47), one from Greece (48), one from France (49), one from Israel (50), and one from Brazil (51). Over the past two decades our research group at the University of California, San Diego (UCSD) has been systematically studying middle-aged and older patients with schizophrenia, as well as normal comparison subjects (NC). We have gathered a large dataset with comprehensive demographic, clinical, neurocognitive, and functioning data. The analyses conducted for the current study were designed to help address the question of whether older adults with LOS present with sufficient differences from those with EOS to warrant distinction of LOS as a formal schizophrenia subtype. We have published data from several smaller studies comparing EOS and LOS patients (25,52–55); however, the present report represents the first attempt to examine the combined data on LOS and EOS patients, and NCs, collected over the past two decades in our research program funded by the U.S. National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH). We evaluated the issue of LOS as a subtype of schizophrenia from three perspectives.
We postulated that, as LOS is a form of schizophrenia, the LOS and EOS groups would be similar to each other and different from NCs on several core characteristics of schizophrenia. Based on the larger literature showing the association of schizophrenia with not only psychopathology, but also with cognitive impairment and functional disability (56), we hypothesized that the LOS and EOS groups would have greater level of psychopathology, cognitive impairment, and functional disability than NCs. (Although our primary focus was on the similarities and differences between EOS and LOS patients, it was thought to be important to show first that the LOS group differed from the NCs in all critical dimensions.)
Based on the existing empirical literature (albeit of studies with much smaller sample sizes (2), we hypothesized that LOS would differ from EOS on several relevant characteristics – i.e., gender, psychopathology, cognitive impairment, everyday functioning, and antipsychotic dosages. For example, observations going back to the early 20th century (e.g., see Eugen Bleuler (10) pp. 341–342) suggest that the LOS group would have a higher proportion of women than the EOS group. Similarly, based on prior studies of psychopathology in LOS and EOS, we hypothesized that LOS patients would have less severe overall psychopathology (57). Early-onset forms of illness tend to have worse course and outcome than later-onset variants (31–33). Accordingly, we postulated that LOS would be associated with less severe impairment in neurocognitive performance (especially executive functions). Given the reported importance of executive functions to independent functioning (58), we also hypothesized that LOS patients would have less severe impairment in everyday functioning than EOS patients. Finally, as noted by Howard et al. (2) open label observations suggest that LOS patients may be effectively managed on antipsychotic doses that are only about 40% as high as those needed with younger patients. Thus, we hypothesized that the mean daily dose of antipsychotic medications among LOS patients would be lower than that among EOS patients.
A potential counter-explanation for EOS-LOS differences is that these may be epiphenomena secondary to differences in some other primary characteristic(s) such as age, gender, negative symptoms, deficit syndrome, or duration of illness,. For example, Carpenter and colleagues have described the concept of Deficit Syndrome schizophrenia (59,60), characterized by insidious onset, worse neurocognitive functioning, and greater familial risk of schizophrenia (60–62). If Deficit Syndrome disproportionately manifests in a subset of patients with EOS relative to its prevalence among LOS patients, it might imply that the key distinction driving the EOS-LOS differences may be an over-representation of Deficit Syndrome among patients with EOS. According to this view, among the non-Deficit Syndrome patients with schizophrenia, there may be no systematic EOS-LOS differences. Similarly, although most longitudinal research indicates that cognitive deficits remain relatively stable throughout the post-onset course of schizophrenia (55,63,64), a few studies suggest that longer disease duration may be associated with worse executive functions and/or episodic declarative memory (65–67). On the other hand, based on the prior conclusions from the International Consensus Statement (2) that LOS warrants designation as a formal subtype, we hypothesized that the EOS-LOS differences in psychopathology, cognitive impairment, and everyday functioning would remain significant after adjusting for differences in age- and gender-corrected scores on severity of negative symptoms and of (a validated Proxy for) deficit syndrome, as well as after adjusting for duration of illness.
METHODS
Participants
The sample consisted of persons age > 40 years including 359 NCs, 744 individuals with EOS (age at first manifestation of prodromal symptoms < 40 years), and 110 with LOS (age at first manifestation of prodromal symptoms ≥ 40 years). Data were collected between August 1987 and July 2008, as a part of studies conducted through an NIMH-funded research program at UCSD. Recruitment sources included the UCSD and Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System psychiatry services, the San Diego County Adult and Older Adult Mental Health Services, assisted living facilities in San Diego metropolitan region, as well as private practitioners.
Although the present report focuses on baseline data, many of the patients were reevaluated on an annual basis as part of longitudinal studies in our Research Center; among those, 93 % evidenced no subsequent change in the diagnosis of schizophrenia in either EOS or LOS group. Where a diagnostic change did occur, it involved bipolar disorder (7 patients with EOS and 0 with LOS), depressive disorder (4 EOS and 3 LOS), and other conditions (2 EOS and 1 LOS).
Selection criteria
Inclusion criteria were: (1) DSM-III-R, DSM–IV diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, and no major psychiatric diagnosis for NCs, (2) current age > 40 years, and (3) English fluency. Exclusion criteria were: (1) presence of DMS-III-R or DSM–IV diagnosis of alcohol or other substance use disorder within 3 months preceding enrollment, (2) DSM-III-R or –IV diagnosis of dementia, or (3) presence or history of major neurological or other medical conditions likely to affect current cognitive functioning. All the subjects who met these criteria and had valid information on age of onset of illness and Positive And Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) (68) were selected for this study. The NCs were recruited via flyers, advertisements in local papers and newsletters, and word of mouth.
The research was approved by the UCSD Human Research Protections Program, and a written informed consent was obtained from all the participants.
Measures
Demographic characteristics
Age, education, gender, self-identified ethnic background, and current as well as past marital status were determined via interview and/or review of available records.
Clinical characteristics and motor symptoms
Age of onset of prodromal symptoms, duration of illness, and current antipsychotic medications (type and current daily antipsychotic dosage in mg chlorpromazine equivalent (CPZE) (69) were determined though patient or collateral interview, and/or review of available records. Severity of dyskinesia and other abnormal involuntary movements was determined with the Abnormal Involuntary Movement Scale (AIMS) (70).
Psychopathology
Severity of positive, negative, and general psychopathologic symptoms was measured with the respective subscales of the PANSS (68). Severity of depressive symptoms was measured with the 17-item Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HAM-D) (71).
Deficit syndrome
Based on the select items from the PANSS we also calculated the Proxy for Deficit Syndrome (or PDS) (72) score for each participant, i.e.:
The PDS was developed by the same investigators who originally proposed the concept of Deficit Syndrome, and has proved to be adequately sensitive and specific to Deficit Syndrome as defined by more comprehensive structured interview (72,73). As each of the PANSS items is scored on a 7 point scale: 1 (absent) to 7 (severe), the PDS has a potential range of −27 to +3; scores toward the positive range tend to be more strongly predictive of presence of Deficit Syndrome (72,73). We categorized each patient with schizophrenia as having “Deficit Syndrome” or “no Deficit Syndrome” using the originally published cut-point (PDS ≥ −2 defining Deficit Syndrome status) (72). However, as that cut-off has not been validated for use with an older adult sample such as ours, we also compared the study groups treating PDS total as a continuous variable.
Cognitive Performance
Subjects were assessed with a comprehensive neurocognitive test battery, including the Information, Vocabulary, Similarities, Picture Arrangement, Block Design, mental Arithmetic, and Digit Symbol subtests from the 1981 or 1997 versions of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-R or WAIS-III) (74,75). Factor analyses suggest that these subtests measure many of the abilities key to characterizing cognitive status, including crystallized verbal knowledge (Information, Vocabulary, Similarities), perceptual-organization (Picture Completion, Block Design), auditory working memory (mental Arithmetic), and processing speed (Digit Symbol) (76,77). We also used perseverative response scores from the 128- or 64-card versions of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) (78,79) to assess abstraction/cognitive flexibility. Verbal memory was measured with the long-delay free recall score, adjusted for performance on the last learning trial (Trial 5), of the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) (80). Raw scores from the WAIS-R or WAIS-III and CVLT were expressed as Scaled Scores (normative mean 10, SD = 3). (Scores provided in the Results below are expressed in terms of the WAIS/WAIS-R “Reference Group” norms.) The WCST preservative response scores were expressed as T-scores (normative mean = 50, SD = 10) using the published census matched norms (78,79).
Everyday Functioning
Functioning measures included the UCSD Performance-based Skills Assessment (UPSA) (81) and the Social Skills Performance Assessment (SSPA) (82). Health-related quality of life was measured with the Quality of Well-Being (QWB) scale (83).
Procedures
For data collected in the initial years of the study, diagnoses were established with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R or DSM-IV (SCID) (84,85), conducted by trained MD or PhD postdoctoral fellows, with diagnostic confirmation provided by a board-certified geriatric psychiatrist based on the information collected through the SCID. In general, there was a high correspondence between chart diagnoses and SCID-based diagnoses. In later years, diagnoses were established by the treating clinicians using DSM-IV or DSM-IV-TR criteria. We have previously reported on reliability of determination of age of onset of illness in our research (25). All other assessments were administered individually by trained bachelors-level research assistants (RAs). To avoid participant fatigue, the evaluations were completed over two or three days, as appropriate. Interrater reliability for the AIMS, PANSS, and HAM-D was regularly established for studies in our research center by having each RA rate videotaped interviews, and his/her ratings then compared to those of criterion standard by established investigators; training was continued until each RA achieved Intra-class Correlation Coefficients relative to the criterion standard of ≥ 0.80; this training was repeated annually to diminish rater drift. Cognitive and functioning measures were also administered by trained RAs; appropriate investigators and/or experienced postdoctoral fellows in neuropsychology met with the RAs on a regular basis to foster consistency in administration and scoring.
Statistical analyses
For categorical variables, the groups were compared using Pearson’s Chi-square tests. For continuous variables Welsh’s robust analysis of variance was used due to significantly unequal variances between the NC group and the patient groups. T-tests were used for pairwise comparisons. Due to significant age and gender differences among the groups, each of the outcome variables was replaced with a standardized age- and gender- corrected version. The latter values were obtained by first fitting linear regression models in the EOS group with each outcome variable as a dependant measure, and age, age-squared, and gender as predictors. From these models, standardized residuals for all the subjects were calculated and employed as age- and gender- corrected outcomes. To control for possible confounding influence of group differences in severity of negative symptoms and of deficit syndrome (with PDS scores treated as a continuous variable), as well as differences in duration of illness, we also conducted additional analyses to adjust for these variables. Two-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were performed, including one confounder and one outcome variable in the model at a time. To avoid making strict assumptions about the linearity of the relationship between confounders and the outcome, the confounders were stratified into dummy variables for the analysis. Both variables were stratified into bins of the size of one-half of the standard deviation for the EOS group. Differences in medication dosages were also computed using a two-way ANOVA allowing for heteroskedasticity. All the tests were two-tailed; significance was defined as p <.05.
RESULTS
The EOS patients were younger than the NCs and LOS patients (Table 1). The NCs included a larger proportion of women than the LOS group, and the LOS group had a higher percentage of women than EOS subjects. No significant differences were found between EOS and LOS groups in levels of education, but both had less education than the NC subjects. The NCs were more likely than the participants with schizophrenia to be to be currently or ever married, LOS patients being intermediate between EOS and NC groups. The diagnosis remained stable in most patients followed longitudinally.
Table 1.
N (NC, EOS, LOS) | Normal Comparison Subjects (NC) | Early- Onset Schizophrenia (EOS) | Late-Onset Schizophrenia (LOS) | F, t, or X2 | df | p | Pairwise Significant Differences | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Demographic characteristics | ||||||||
Age (years) | (359,744,110) | 59.8 (14.5) | 51.0 (8.3) | 57.6 (8.7) | 75.5 | 2,278 | <.001 | EOS<LOS=NC |
Education (years) | (359, 738,110) | 13.9 (2.3) | 12.2 (2.4) | 12.5 (3.1) | 64.6 | 2,277 | <.001 | LOS=EOS<NC |
Gender (% women) | (359, 744,110) | 63% | 34% | 47% | 84.0 | 2 | <.001 | NC>LOS>EOS |
Ethnic background % Caucasian | (359, 744,110) | 70% | 64% | 66% | 3.6 | 2 | .163 | |
Marital status | (358, 736,110) | 208.6 | 4 | <.001 | EOS≠LOS EOS≠NC LOS≠NC |
|||
Divorced/Widowed/Separated | 41% | 41% | 55% | |||||
Married/Cohabitating | 43% | 10% | 16% | |||||
Single | 16% | 49% | 29% | |||||
Clinical characteristics | ||||||||
Age of illness onset (years) | (n/a, 744,110) | n/a | 22.9 (7.4) | 48.1 (7.3) | n/aa | n/aa | n/aa | |
% Paranoid subtype | (n/a, 541,71) | n/a | 33% | 36% | 0.2 | 1 | .615 | |
Duration of illness (years) | (n/a, 744,110) | n/a | 28.1(10.2) | 9.4 (7.6) | n/aa | /n/aa | n/aa | |
Daily dose of antipsychotic medication (mg chlorpromazine equivalent) | (n/a, 485, 65) | n/a | 477 (888) | 135 (96) | −8.1 | 541.2 | 0.002 | |
Abnormal Involuntary Movement Scale score | (168, 637,101) | 1.8 (1.9) | 4 (3.7) | 4.4 (4.6) | 150.6 | 2,234 | <.001 | NC<EOS=LOS |
Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale | ||||||||
Positive Syndrome score | (356, 740,110) | 8.3 (1.9) | 15.5 (6.1) | 14.0 (6) | 367.9 | 2,278 | <.001 | NC<LOS<EOS |
Negative Syndrome score | (355, 741,109) | 8.2 (1.7) | 15.0 (5.6) | 14.0 (5.3) | 545.0 | 2,272 | <.001 | NC<LOS=EOS |
General Psychopathology score | (354, 740,109) | 19.3 (3.1) | 30.2 (8.5) | 27.9 (6.6) | 482.1 | 2,291 | <.001 | NC<LOS<EOS |
Proxy for Deficit Syndrome (PDS) scoreb | (743,110) | n/a | n/a | −6.5 (3.5) | 72.7 | 2,292 | <.001 | EOS=LOS |
% with Deficit Syndrome (PDS ≥ −2 defining Deficit Syndrome status) b.59 | (743, 110) | n/a | n/a | 6.4% | 1.18 | 1 | .278 | EOS=LOS |
Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression score | (354, 688,110) | 3.6 (3.7) | 10.4 (6.4) | 9.7 (6.1) | 218.7 | 2,295 | <.001 | NC<EOS=LOS |
Cognitive performance | ||||||||
WAIS-R or WAIS-III (Reference group Scaled Scores) | ||||||||
Information | (152, 270,54) | 10.4 (2.7) | 9.4 (3.2) | 9.2 (3.2) | 49.9 | 2,139 | <.001 | EOS=LOS<NC |
Vocabulary | (165, 299,56) | 11.1 (2.8) | 8.4 (3.2) | 8.9 (3.1) | 42.4 | 2,149 | <.001 | EOS=LOS<NC |
Similarities | (145, 284,56) | 10.1 (2.7) | 7.9 (3.3) | 8 (3.3) | 71.2 | 2,146 | <.001 | EOS=LOS<NC |
Picture Arrangement | (147, 270,53) | 8 (2.8) | 6.8 (2.9) | 6.6 (3.2) | 31.9 | 2,135 | <.001 | EOS=LOS<NC |
Block Design | (146, 263,55) | 7.9 (2.5) | 6.5 (2.4) | 7 (3.1) | 56.4 | 2,135 | <.001 | EOS<LOS<NC |
Arithmetic | (148, 276,57) | 9.1 (2.8) | 7.1 (2.8) | 7.6 (3.5) | 52.3 | 2,141 | <.001 | EOS=LOS<NC |
Digit Symbol | (150, 431,70) | 7 (2.4) | 4.7 (1.8) | 5.3 (2.4) | 140.3 | 2,155 | <.001 | EOS<LOS<NC |
WCST Perseverative Responses (T-score) | (155, 362,62) | 49.8 (12.9) | 39.3 (13.2) | 45 (15.7) | 31.4 | 2,151 | <.001 | EOS<LOS<NC |
California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) | (157, 197,42) | 10.1 (3.3) | 9.2 (3.6) | 10.7 (3.4) | 3.9 | 2,117 | .023 | EOS<LOS=NC |
Functional capacity and Quality of life | ||||||||
UCSD Performance-based Skills Assessment score | (31, 417,53) | 92.6 (5.8) | 67.6 (20.6) | 72 (18.8) | 112.4 | 2,71 | <.001 | EOS<LOS<NC |
Social Skills Performance Assessment score | (110, 462,56) | 37.3 (4) | 26.4 (8) | 28.4 (8.2) | 416.8 | 2,133 | <.001 | EOS<LOS<NC |
Quality of Well-Being scale (Adjusted score) | (158, 494,77) | 0.7 (0.1) | 0.5 (0.1) | 0.6 (0.1) | 99.2 | 2,179 | <.001 | EOS<LOS<NC |
Note: Higher scores on Abnormal Involuntary Movement Scale, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale, and Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression represent worse symptoms. Higher scores on the Proxy for Deficit Syndrome, as well as all the cognitive, functional capacity, and Quality of Well-Being measures represent better status.
Means (and SDs) for age of onset and duration of illness are provided for descriptive purposes only. The significance of these differences was not statistically tested because the null hypotheses (no difference in the EOS versus LOS populations) are, by definition, known to be false.
Deficit syndrome measures were not applied to normal comparison subjects.
Abbreviations:
EOS = Early-Onset Schizophrenia
LOS = Late-Onset Schizophrenia
n/a = not applicable
NC = Normal Comparison subject
UCSD = University of California, San Diego
WAIS-R = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised
WAIS-III = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Third Edition
There were no significant differences between EOS and LOS groups in terms of the proportion of patients with paranoid subtype. The EOS group had a higher mean daily dose of antipsychotics than the LOS group. The AIMS total was worse for the two schizophrenia groups relative to the NCs, with no differences between EOS and LOS patients. Both the schizophrenia groups had worse psychopathology scores than the NCs, with the LOS group having less severe positive symptoms and general psychopathology relative to EOS group. There were no significant differences between the EOS and LOS groups in severity of negative, deficit (PDS total), or depressive symptoms. There was also no significant difference between the two patient groups in prevalence of Deficit Syndrome (using the published cut-score of PDS total ≥ −2 defined “Deficit Syndrome Status”).
On cognitive testing, both the schizophrenia groups had significantly worse scores than NC subjects. There were no significant EOS-LOS differences on tests of crystallized verbal knowledge (Information, Vocabulary, and Similarities) or auditory working memory (Arithmetic). The LOS group was less impaired than the EOS group on tests of processing speed (Digit Symbol), abstraction/cognitive flexibility (WCST), and verbal memory (CVLT). The LOS patients also had a better performance on one of the two tests of perceptual-organization abilities (Block Design), but not on the other (Picture Arrangement). With regard to everyday functioning and health-related quality of life, the NCs performed better than either of the patient groups, and the LOS group performed better than the EOS group on all three measures (UPSA, SSPA, and QWB).
With the sole exception of severity of positive symptoms (PANSS positive syndrome score), all of the above mentioned EOS-LOS differences remained significant after adjusting for severity of negative or deficit symptoms. On co-varying for duration of illness, two additional differences became non-significant – i.e., differences in cognitive flexibility (WCST), and verbal memory (CVLT); however, the other differences remained significant, including severity of general psychopathology (PANSS general psychopathology score), processing speed (Digit Symbol), perceptual organization (Block Design), everyday functioning (UPSA, SSPA), and health-related quality of life (QWB).
DISCUSSION
We compared the demographic, clinical, neurocognitive, and everyday functioning characteristics of a large sample of middle-aged and older persons with LOS relative to EOS patients and NC subjects. Consistent with our a priori hypotheses: (1) LOS and EOS groups had greater levels of psychopathology, cognitive impairment, and functional disability than NC subjects. (2) Relative to the EOS group, LOS patients had a higher proportion of women, less severe positive symptoms and general psychopathology, less severe impairment in abstraction/cognitive flexibility, verbal memory as well as in everyday functioning, and were on lower doses of antipsychotics. (3) With the sole exception of severity of positive symptoms, all of the EOS-LOS differences remained significant after adjusting for age- and gender-corrected scores on severity of negative or deficit symptoms. On co-varying for duration of illness, two additional differences became non-significant – i.e., differences in cognitive flexibility, and verbal memory; however, the other differences remained significant, including severity of general psychopathology, processing speed, everyday functioning, health-related quality of life, and one of the two measures of perceptual organization.
One hypothesis that was not supported was that of a lower severity of negative and deficit symptoms (or prevalence of Deficit Syndrome) in LOS. An unexpected finding was that LOS patients also had faster performance on a measure of processing speed and one (Block Design) but not the other (Picture Arrangement) test of perceptual-organization skills. The difference in processing speed was particularly surprising as the LOS group was, on average, six years older than the EOS group, and processing speed tends to decline steadily with normal aging (86). The better performance of the LOS patients on Block Design could, however, be viewed as suggesting better perceptual organization, and thus consistent with better score on the executive function test (WCST), although the lack of significant differences on the Picture Arrangement task, which also tends to be sensitive to perceptual organization, suggests a need to further examine this potential relationship with additional measures in follow-up research. The differences in results with the two tests of perceptual organization may reflect on the relative psychometric strengths of each of these measures (87).
Although we had a large sample size and employed standardized instruments, our study also had several limitations. While our analyses were directed as tests of several specific a priori hypotheses, doing so involved a large number of comparisons to determine if LOS differed from EOS across clinical symptoms, cognition, and daily function, as well as the analyses controlling for potential confounds such as duration of illness, severity of negative symptoms and deficit syndrome. There is a possibility of type I errors because of multiple comparisons, but the direction of significant differences was generally consistent with the patterns seen in earlier (albeit smaller sample size) studies (reviewed by Howard et al. (2)).
As hypothesized, we found a higher proportion of women among LOS than EOS patients. As a non-epidemiologic sample, the relative population prevalence rates of EOS and LOS among women versus men cannot be definitively determined from the present data. (Unfortunately, the prevalence of LOS can also not be estimated from the Epidemiologic Catchment Area Study data because the diagnostic criteria that were employed in that study excluded a diagnosis of schizophrenia in anyone whose symptoms first manifested at age 40 or later (88,89)). Nonetheless, the observed higher frequency of women in the LOS relative to EOS group is consistent with a large body of published empirical data dating back a century. For instance, in his 1911 textbook Eugen Bleuler (10), commenting on age-of-onset data published in a 1907 (German-language) paper by Ryssia Wolfsohn, wrote that:
“It seems noteworthy though, that in Wolfsohn’s material the curve of male patients drops in fairly regular fashion from a maximum in the fifth quinquennium; while that of the female patients shows a small elevation between the ages of 40 and 45 years; this rise becomes even more striking in the two following pentads in which we find more female cases than male. It may very well be due to the influence of climacterium which leaves a much stronger imprint on the female psyche than on the male who after all does not need to resign yet at this particular age period” (pp. 341-242).
Although Bleuler’s psychogenic explanation is open to debate, his comments are remarkable in anticipating contemporary theories on the role of menopause (albeit referencing its biological rather than psychosocial aspects) as an explanation for the higher proportion of women among persons with LOS. The so-called “estrogen hypothesis” is that estrogen may mimic some properties of antipsychotic compounds and thus delay symptom onset in women predisposed to schizophrenia until the post-menopausal years (90,91). Definitive evidence for the estrogen hypothesis is lacking, however. Indeed, it would not help explain the occurrence of LOS among men, and it would seem to suggest a stronger antipsychotic effect of estrogen than has been empirically demonstrated. In regard to the latter, clinical trials examining the effectiveness of estrogen replacement as an adjuvant antipsychotic treatment in post-menopausal women with schizophrenia have yielded mixed results, with some reporting positive benefits (92) but others showing no clear added value (93). One potential direction for future research on LOS would be to compare large samples of men and women with LOS to see whether the women have a more severe form of schizophrenia (as the male LOS patients presumably had no or minimal hormonal protection masking the manifestation of schizophrenia).
Another potential interpretive limitation of the present study is that most of the patient participants were clinically stable, receiving maintenance outpatient psychiatric care, and thus had relatively low severity of symptoms. Thus, our results may not generalize to institutionalized patients. However, among other (positive as well as deleterious) effects of the move toward deinstitutionalization of those with chronic mental illnesses that occurred in the last five decades (94), one clear result has been that most of the older persons with schizophrenia currently reside in the community, not in institutions (95). The sample was collected over a period of two decades, and the sample sizes for individual measures varied, but we conducted annual inter-rater reliability training to ensure a sustained high level of reliability. The analyses in this report focus on cross-sectional comparison of baseline data, but the diagnosis of schizophrenia remained stable in most of the patients who were followed longitudinally in our Research Center. It is also important to emphasize that our observed distinctions between EOS and LOS were based primarily on clinical assessments rather than on validated biomarkers (which are not established yet).
The higher antipsychotic dosages among EOS relative to LOS patients are consistent with findings from case registry (96) and open label studies suggesting that LOS patients may be effectively managed on lower doses (reviewed in Howard et al (2)). However, as ours was not an intervention trial, medication type and doses varied across subjects as determined by their individual treating clinicians. It is, therefore, conceivable that the LOS-EOS differences in antipsychotic dosages could be partially due to a tendency to continue prescribing higher doses to EOS patients (who received their first antipsychotic prescription in an era when average dose recommendations were higher for first-break psychosis). Thus the currently higher doses among EOS patients could potentially reflect the residual/anchoring effects (clinicians being hesitant to change medication doses for clinically stable patients.) It is also possible that dosing differences could be partially attributable to secondary factors, such as episodes of hostility or aggression being less prevalent among LOS compared to EOS patients. These possibilities warrant controlled clinical trials to determine the lowest effective doses in each group.
One conclusion that can be drawn from the present data is that most of the EOS-LOS differences cannot be explained by differential presence of Deficit Syndrome in the EOS group. The absence of significant differences in the mean PDS scores or the proportion of patients categorized as having Deficit Syndrome when PDS scores were treated categorically, suggests that differential presence of Deficit Syndrome was not responsible for EOS-LOS differences in other dimensions. The latter conclusion was further supported by the lack of a change in the pattern of findings when comparisons were adjusted for differences in PDS scores.
A comparison of the neurocognitive deficits between EOS and LOS groups revealed results that were, on the one hand, consistent with the notion that schizophrenia is best characterized by a general pattern of multiple cognitive deficits of mild-to-moderate severity (56). On the other hand, our finding of less severe impairment among LOS patients in abstraction/cognitive flexibility and verbal memory is also noteworthy. As abstraction/cognitive flexibility tends to be mediated by the executive functions associated with the frontal and frontal-subcortical systems (97,98), and performance on verbal memory measures can be affected by executive functions as well as by the memory consolidation systems in the medial temporal lobe (99), these findings are consistent with the long-standing focus on the prefrontal and temporal lobe regions, as well as their subcortical circuits as viable candidates for neurobiologic systems relevant to the manifestation of schizophrenia (100,101)
One of the challenges in studying the effects of age of onset is that it tends to be confounded with duration of illness, cumulative treatment, and number of prior periods of acute psychotic exacerbations - i.e., relative to similarly aged patients with EOS, those with LOS have had less time for exposure to the potential neurotoxic (and cognitively and socially “toxic”) effects of chronic schizophrenia and its treatment. There is, however, strong basis in the literature to argue against attributing all the EOS versus LOS differences to duration/chronicity or related effects. For instance, cognition tends to be relatively stable throughout the post-adult lifespan in schizophrenia, and there is no significant influence of duration of illness on the severity or pattern of neurocognitive functioning, even when examined within specific ability areas (54,55,63,64). Thus, our finding that EOS-LOS differences in WCST and CVLT no longer reached significance after adjusting for duration of illness could reflect a Type II error. Given that the comparisons were made in terms of residual scores that were, in part, age adjusted, and that with current age, age of illness onset, and duration of illness can be directly triangulated, it may be expected that several of the LOS-EOS differences would fail to reach significance when adjusting for duration of illness.
As expected, both EOS and LOS groups were less likely to have been married and had worse everyday functioning than NCs, but LOS patients were more likely to have been currently or previously married, had better scores on measures of daily functioning and health-related quality of life than EOS patients, and were on lower dosages of antipsychotics, suggesting better prognosis for LOS than for EOS. As was noted earlier, later age of onset has been associated with better course and outcome in several medical disorders such as diabetes mellitus (31), Huntington disease (32), and Alzheimer disease (33).
One may question whether age of onset of schizophrenia is a continuous variable or a categorical one that may be used to define LOS (52). As the current debate regarding appropriate classification and terminology of schizophrenia and associated conditions continues (102), consistent with the recommendations that emerged from the International Consensus Statement (2), the present results support denoting LOS as a subtype in the future ICD-11 and DSM-V. On the other hand, the value of categorical subtypes over dimensional ratings is itself an issue of ongoing discussion. We believe that these two views are not mutually exclusive. In social life, we use specific age cut-offs to determine the minimum ages for driving, voting, being elected to a public office, or for retirement, although there are no magic changes that take place on those predetermined birthdays.
Given some of the observed group differences, including those in everyday functioning, the EOS-LOS distinction may have clinical value. Obtaining information on age of onset of illness is a part of any well conducted psychiatric interview, and thus does not require administration of additional rating scales or other complex research instruments as is required for some of the other suggested subtypes of schizophrenia. The EOS-LOS differentiation may also have relevance in elucidating the neurobiological basis of schizophrenia. Most researchers accept neurodevelopmental theories of schizophrenia (103). Prior reports demonstrate similarity among EOS and LOS patients in terms of family history of schizophrenia in first-degree relatives, as well as minor physical anomalies and early childhood maladjustment, which may indicate prenatal or early postnatal damage (2,104). Thus, the concept of LOS does not negate neurodevelopmental theories, but rather suggests that neurobiologic changes occurring in mid- or later-life may also be important in the phenotypic manifestation of schizophrenia in some individuals (105). Those changes could represent a diminution of earlier protective factors and/or an accumulation of risk factors. The eventual validity of any subtypes of schizophrenia will be determined by reliable biomarkers, but at present, there are no established biomarkers to define any of the established or suggested subtypes.
In sum, our results suggest that LOS and EOS show sufficient overlap to warrant consideration as a single disorder (commensurate with the heterogeneity between and within other schizophrenia subtypes), yet there are important differences with regard to gender, symptom severity, executive impairment (relevant to adaptive functioning and cognitive rehabilitation efforts (58,106,107)), health-related quality of life, everyday functioning, and other prognostic indicators that should warrant specification of LOS as a subtype. Doing so has heuristic value in facilitating further study of LOS; the insights gained from such research may also open new avenues for prevention and treatment that could be applied to persons with EOS and even to those at high risk for schizophrenia.
Table 2.
Severity of Negative Symptoms | Proxy for Deficit Syndrome | Duration of Illness | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||||||
Dependant Variables | F | df | p-value | F | df | p-value | F | df | p-value |
PANSS positive syndrome score | 2.51 | 1, 827 | 0.114 | 3.75 | 1, 834 | 0.053 | 1.56 | 1, 839 | 0.212 |
PANSS general psychopathology | 5.48 | 1, 827 | 0.019 | 9.00 | 1, 834 | 0.003 | 4.99 | 1, 838 | 0.026 |
WCST Perseverative responses | 4.06 | 1, 410 | 0.044 | 5.13 | 1, 411 | 0.024 | 0.96 | 1, 414 | 0.328 |
CVLT Memory score | 6.94 | 1, 224 | 0.009 | 6.70 | 1, 229 | 0.010 | 0.04 | 1, 228 | 0.844 |
Digit Symbol | 12.44 | 1, 486 | 0.001 | 17.85 | 1, 487 | 0.001 | 8.31 | 1, 490 | 0.004 |
Block Design | 4.95 | 1, 304 | 0.027 | 5.92 | 1, 307 | 0.016 | 5.24 | 1, 307 | 0.023 |
UPSA | 10.40 | 1, 459 | 0.001 | 10.96 | 1, 456 | 0.001 | 8.43 | 1, 460 | 0.004 |
SSPA | 10.19 | 1, 505 | 0.002 | 10.58 | 1, 505 | 0.001 | 8.88 | 1, 508 | 0.003 |
Adjusted QWB | 6.82 | 1, 552 | 0.009 | 10.14 | 1, 558 | 0.002 | 4.64 | 1, 560 | 0.032 |
Note: Due to significant age and gender differences among the groups (Table 1), each of the outcome variables was replaced with a standardized age- and gender-corrected version. (Please see the section on Statistical Analysis for details.)
Abbreviations:
CVLT – California Verbal Learning Test
PANSS – Positive And Negative Syndrome Scale
QWB – Quality of Well-Being scale
WCST – Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
Acknowledgments
The authors express their gratitude to Brian Kirkpatrick, M.D., for his guidance and advice on the application of the Proxy for Deficit Syndrome score in the present analyses, as well as to each of the research staff involved in data collection, and most of all to each of the research participants for their time and effort in completing the study assessments.
This work was supported, in part, by the National Institute of Mental Health (grant #s MH080002, MH019934, MH43693, MH 064722), and by the Department of Veterans Affairs.
References
- 1.KRAEPLIN E. Dementia Praecox and Paraphrenia [Translated in 1919 from the eighth German edition of the ‘Test-book of Psychiatry,’. ii. iii. Huntington, NY: Krieger; 1971. section on Endogenous Dementias published in 1913. [Google Scholar]
- 2.HOWARD R, RABINS PV, SEEMAN MV, JESTE DV AND THE INTERNATIONAL LATE-ONSET SCHIZOPHRENIA GROUP. Late-onset schizophrenia and very-late-onset schizophrenia-like psychois: an international consensus. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:172–178. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.2.172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.ROTH M, KAY DWK. Late paraphrenia: a variant of schizophrenia manifest in late life or an organic clinical syndrome? A review of recent evidence. Int J Geriat Psychiatry. 1998;13:775–784. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-1166(1998110)13:11<775::aid-gps868>3.0.co;2-e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.RIECHER-ROSSLER A, LOFFLER W, MUNK-JORGENSEN P. What do we really know about late-onset schizophrenia? Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1997;247:195–208. doi: 10.1007/BF02900216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.TAYLOR MA. Late-onset schizophrenia-like psychosis. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:1334–1335. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.8.1334-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.SACHDEV P, BRODATY H, ROSE N, CATHCART S. Schizophrenia with onset after age 50 years. 2: Neurological, neuropsychological and MRI investigation. Br J Psychiatry. 1999;175:416–421. doi: 10.1192/bjp.175.5.416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.ADITYANJEE AYA, THEODORIDIS D, VIEWEG VR. Dementia praecox to schizophrenia: the first 100 years. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1999;53:437–448. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.1999.00584.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.OXFORD UNIVERSITY PRESS. Oxford English Dictionary Online. 2008 Available at: http://www.oed.com.
- 9.NOLL R. Non-dementia non-praecox: note on the advantages to mental hygiene of extirpating a term. In: Southard EE, editor. Hist Psychiatry. Vol. 18. 2007. pp. 483–502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.BLEULER E. Dementia Praecox or the Group of Schizophrenias. New York: International Universities Press; 1950. [Google Scholar]
- 11.RIECHER-ROSSLER A. Late onset schizophrenia: the German concept and literature. In: Howard R, Rabins PV, Castle DJ, editors. Late Onset Schizophrenia. Philadelphia: Wrightson Biomedical Publishing Ltd; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 12.BLEULER M. Die spatschizophrenen krankheitsbilder. Fortschritte der Neurologie Psychiatrie. 1943;15:259–290. [Google Scholar]
- 13.RIECHER-ROSSLER A, ROSSLER W, FORSTL H, MEISE U. Late-onset schizophrenia and late paraphrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1995;21:345–354. doi: 10.1093/schbul/21.3.345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.ROTH M, MORRISSEY JD. Problems in the diagnosis and classification of mental disorder in old age: With a study of case material. Journal of Mental Science. 1952;98:66–80. doi: 10.1192/bjp.98.410.66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.ROTH M. The natural history of mental disorder in old age. Journal of Mental Science. 1955;101:281–301. doi: 10.1192/bjp.101.423.281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.KAY DWK, ROTH M. Environmental and hereditary factors in the schizophrenias of old age (“late paraphrenia”) and their bearing on the general problem of causation in schizophrenia. Journal of Mental Science. 1961;107:649–686. doi: 10.1192/bjp.107.449.649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.POST F. Persistent Persecutory States of the Elderly. London: Pergamon Press; 1966. [Google Scholar]
- 18.POST F. The schizophrenic reaction-type in late life. Proc R Soc Med. 1967;60:249–254. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.FISH F. Senile schizophrenia. Journal of Mental Science. 1960;106:938–946. doi: 10.1192/bjp.106.444.938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.BRIDGE TP, WYATT RJ. Paraphrenia: paranoid states of late life. II. American research. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1980;28:201–205. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1980.tb00519.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.TAKEI N. Late onset psychosis in Japan. In: Howard R, Rabins PV, Castle DJ, editors. Late-Onset Schizophrenia. Philadelphia, PA: Wrightson Biomedical Publishing; 1999. pp. 93–100. [Google Scholar]
- 22.ABLESKOV K. Paraphrenia: the Scandinavian view. In: Howard R, Rabins PV, Castle DJ, editors. Late-Onset Schizophrenia. Philadelphia, PA: Wrightson Biomedical Publishing; 1999. pp. 89–91. [Google Scholar]
- 23.RIECHER-RÖSSLER A, HÄFNER H, MUNK-JØRGENSEN P. Late onset schizophrenia: a European view. In: Howard R, Rabins PV, Castle DJ, editors. Late-Onset Schizophrenia. Philadelphia, PA: Wrightson Biomedical Publishing; 1999. pp. 55–78. [Google Scholar]
- 24.BERRIOS GE. The insanities of the third age: a conceptual history of paraphrenia. J Nutr Health Aging. 2003;7:394–399. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.JESTE DV, SYMONDS LL, HARRIS MJ, PAULSEN JS, PALMER BW, HEATON RK. Non-dementia non-praecox dementia praecox? Late-onset schizophrenia. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 1997;5:302–317. doi: 10.1097/00019442-199700540-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.RABINS PV, LAVRISHA M. Long-term follow-up and phenomenologic differences distinguish among late-onset schizophrenia, late-life depression and progressive dementia. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2003;11:589–594. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajgp.11.6.589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.HOWARD R, JESTE DV. Late-onset schizophrenia. In: Weinberger DR, Harrison P, editors. Schizophrenia. 3. Oxford: Blackwell Science Ltd; 2009. in press. [Google Scholar]
- 28.WORLD HEALTH ORGANIZATION. International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, 10th Revision, Version for 2007. 2007 Available at: http://www.who.int/classifications/icd/en.
- 29.AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC ASSOCIATION. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- 30.AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC ASSOCIATION. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. Text Revision. [Google Scholar]
- 31.VAAG A. On the pathophysiology of late onset non-insulin dependent diabetes mellitus. Current controversies and new insights. Dan Med Bull. 1999;46:197–234. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.LIPE H, BIRD T. Late onset Huntington Disease: clinical and genetic characteristics of 34 cases. J Neurol Sci. 2009;276:159–162. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2008.09.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.TERRY RD. Biologic differences between early- and late-onset Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 1995;9:S26–S27. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.CORRUBLE E, GORWOOD P, FALISSARD B. Association between age of onset and symptom profiles of late-life depression. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2008;118:389–394. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01239.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.KUPFER DJ, KUHL EA, REGIER DA. Research for improving diagnostic systems: consideration of factors related to later life development. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2009;17:355–358. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31819e2c40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.JOHN ER, PRICHEP LC, WINTERER G, HERRMANN WM, DI MICHELE F, HALPER J, et al. Electrophysiological subtypes of psychotic states. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2007;116:17–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2006.00983.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.BORA F, YUCEL M, FORNITO A, BERK M, PANTELIS C. Major psychoses with mixed psychotic and mood symptoms: are mixed psychoses associated with different neurobiological markers? Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2008;118:172–187. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01230.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.KRAEMER HC, SCHULTZ SK, ARNDT S. Biomarkers in psychiatry: methodological issues. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2002;10:653–659. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.RABINS PV. Schizophrenia beginning in late life: an American view. In: Howard R, Rabins PV, Castle DJ, editors. Late Onset Schizophrenia. Philadelphia: Wrightson Biomedical Publishing, Ltd; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 40.REEVES RR, STRUVE FA, PARKER JD. P300 event-related potential in late-onset schizophrenia. Int Psychogeriatr. 2004;16:487–489. doi: 10.1017/s1041610204210912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.MOORE R, BLACKWOOD N, CORCORAN R, ROWSE G, KINDERMAN P, BENTALL R, et al. Misunderstanding the intentions of others: an exploratory study of the cognitive etiology of persecutory delusions in very late-onset schizophrenia-like psychosis. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14:410–418. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000200604.47367.38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.JONES DK, CATANI M, PIERPAOLI C, REEVES SJ, SHERGILL SS, O’SULLIVAN M, et al. A diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging study of frontal cortex connections in very-late-onset schizophrenia-like psychosis. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2005;13:1092–1099. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajgp.13.12.1092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.MITTER P, REEVES S, ROMERO-RUBIALES F, BELL P, STEWART R, HOWARD R. Migrant status, age, gender and social isolation in very late-onset schizophrenia-like psychosis. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2005;20:1046–1051. doi: 10.1002/gps.1396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.ROMERO-RUBIALES F, REEVES S, HOWARD R. Have risk factors for very late-onset schizophrenia-like psychosis changed in the last 40 years? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2004;19:806–807. doi: 10.1002/gps.1152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.BERGEMANN N, ABU-TAIR F, STROWITZKI T. Estrogen in the treatment of late-onset schizophrenia. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2007;27:718–720. doi: 10.1097/JCP.0b013e31815a6af9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.REULBACH U, BLEICH S, BIERMANN T, PFAHLBERG A, SPERLING W. Late-onset schizophrenia in child survivors of the holocaust. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2007;195:315–319. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000261878.55882.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.SATO T, BOTTLENDER R, SCHROTER A, MOLLER HJ. Psychopathology of early-onset versus late-onset schizophrenia revisited: an observation of 473 neuroleptic-naive patients before and after first-admission treatments. Schizophr Res. 2004;67:175–183. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00015-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.PSARROS C, THELERITIS CG, PAPARRIGOPOULOS TJ, POLITIS AM, PAPADIMITRIOU GN. Amisulpride for the treatment of very-late-onset schizophrenia-like psychosis. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2009;24:518–522. doi: 10.1002/gps.2146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.POULET E, BRUNELIN J, KALLEL L, D’AMATO T, SAOUD M. Maintenance treatment with transcranial magnetic stimulation in a patient with late-onset schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:537–538. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07060868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.MAZEH D, ZEMISHLANI C, AIZENBERG D, BARAK Y. Patients with very-late-onset schizophrenia-like psychosis: a follow-up study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2005;13:417–419. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajgp.13.5.417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.LAKS J, FONTENELL LF, CHALITA A, MENDLOWICZ MV. Absence of dementia in late-onset schizophrenia: a one year follow-up of a Brazilian case series. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2006;64:946–949. doi: 10.1590/s0004-282x2006000600011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.JESTE DV, MCADAMS LA, PALMER BW, BRAFF D, JERNIGAN TL, STOUT J, et al. Relationship of neuropsychological and MRI measures with age of onset of schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1998;98:156–164. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1998.tb10058.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.COREY-BLOOM J, JERNIGAN T, ARCHIBALD S, HARRIS MJ, JESTE DV. Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of the brain in late-life schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1995;152:447–449. doi: 10.1176/ajp.152.3.447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.HEATON R, PAULSEN J, MCADAMS LA, KUCK J, ZISOOK S, BRAFF D, et al. Neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia: relationship to age, chronicity and dementia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51:469–476. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950060033003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.HEATON RK, GLADSJO JA, PALMER BW, KUCK J, MARCOTTE TD, JESTE DV. Stability and course of neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:24–32. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.1.24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.PALMER BW, DAWES SE, HEATON RK. What do we know about neuropsychological aspects of schizophrenia? Neuropsychol Rev. 2009;19:365–384. doi: 10.1007/s11065-009-9109-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.GIRARD C, SIMARD M. Clinical characterization of late- and very late-onset first psychotic episode in psychiatric inpatients. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;16:478–487. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31816c7b3c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.GREEN MF. What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153:321–330. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.3.321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.CARPENTER JRWT, HEINDRICHS DW, WAGMAN AMI. Deficit and nondeficit forms of schizophrenia: the concept. Am J Psychiatry. 1988;145:578–583. doi: 10.1176/ajp.145.5.578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.BUCHANAN RW, KIRKPATRICK B, HEINRICHS DW, CARPENTER TW. Clinical correlates of the deficit syndrome of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1990;147:290–294. doi: 10.1176/ajp.147.3.290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.COHEN AS, SAPERSTEIN AM, GOLD JM, KIRKPATRICK B, CARPENTER WT, BUCHANAN RW. Neuropsychology of the deficit syndrome: new data and meta-analysis of findings to date. Schizophr Bull. 2007;33:1201–1212. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.KIRKPATRICK B, BUCHANAN RW, ROSS DE, CARPENTER WT. A separate disease within the syndrome of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:165–171. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.2.165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.RUND BR. A review of longitudinal studies of cognitive functions in schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Bull. 1998;24:425–435. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.KURTZ MM. Neurocognitive impairment across the lifespan in schizophrenia: an update. Schizophr Res. 2005;74:15–26. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.SULLIVAN EV, SHEAR PK, ZIPURSKY RB, SAGAR HJ, PFEFFERBAUM A. A deficit profile of executive, memory, and motor functions in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 1994;36:641–653. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(94)91173-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.PALMER BW, SAVLA GN, FELLOWS I, TWAMLEY E, JESTE DV, LACRO JP. Do people with schizophrenia have differential impairment in episodic memory and/or working memory relative to other cognitive abilities? Schizophrenia Research. 2009 doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.11.002. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 67.FUCETOLA R, SEIDMAN LJ, KREMEN WS, FARAONE SV, GOLDSTEIN JM, TSUANG MT. Age and neuropsychologic function in schizophrenia: a decline in executive abilities beyond that observed in healthy volunteers. Society of Biological Psychiatry. 2000;48:137–146. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00240-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.KAY S, FISZBEIN A, OPLER LA. The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13:261–276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.JESTE DV, WYATT RJ. Understanding and Treating Tardive Dyskinesia. New York, NY: Guilford Press, Inc; 1982. [Google Scholar]
- 70.GUY W. ECDEU Assessment Manual for Psychopharmacology: US Department of Health, Education, and Welfare (ADM) Rockville, MD: National Institute of Mental Health; 1976. pp. 76–338. [Google Scholar]
- 71.HAMILTON M. Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness. Br J Soc Clin Psychol. 1967;6:278–296. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1967.tb00530.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.KIRKPATRICK B, BUCHANAN RW, BREIER A, CARPENTER WT. Case identification and stability of the deficit syndrome of schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 1993;47:47–56. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(93)90054-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.GOETZ RR, CORCORAN C, YALE S, et al. Validity of a ‘proxy’ for the deficit syndrome derived from the Positive And Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) Schizophr Res. 2007;93:169–177. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.02.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.WECHSLER D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Revised. Cleveland, OH: Psychological Corporation; 1981. [Google Scholar]
- 75.WECHSLER D. Adminstration and Scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1997. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Third Edition (WAIS-III) [Google Scholar]
- 76.GLADSJO JA, MCADAMS LA, PALMER BW, MOORE D, JESTE DV, HEATON RK. A six-factor model of cognition in schizophrenia and related psychotic disorders: relationships with clinical symptoms and functional capacity. Schizophr Bull. 2004;30:739–754. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.TULSKY DS, PRICE LR. The joint WAIS-III and WMS-III factor structure: development and cross-validation of a six-factor model of cognitive functioning. Psychological Assessment. 2003;15:149–162. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.15.2.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.HEATON RK, CHELUNE GJ, TALLEY JL, KAY GG, CURTIS G. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual. Revised and Expanded. Odessa, Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 79.KONGS SK, THOMPSON LL, IVERSON GL, HEATON RK. Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 2000. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test - 64 Card Version. [Google Scholar]
- 80.DELIS DC, KRAMER JH, KAPLAN E, OBER BA. California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- 81.PATTERSON TL, GOLDMAN S, MCKIBBIN CL, HUGHS T, JESTE DV. UCSD performance-based skills assessment (UPSA): development of a new measure of everyday functioning for severely mentally ill adults. Schizophr Bull. 2001;27:235–245. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.PATTERSON TL, MOSCONA S, MCKIBBIN CL, DAVIDSON K, JESTE DV. Social skills performance assessment among older patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2001;48:351–360. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(00)00109-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.KAPLAN RM, ANDERSON JP. The Quality of Well-Being scale: rationale for a single quality of life index. In: Rosser R, Walker SR, editors. Quality of Life: Assessment and Application. London: MTP Press; 1988. pp. 51–77. [Google Scholar]
- 84.SPITZER RL, WILLIAMS JB, GIBBON M, FIRST MB. Structured clinical interview for the DSM-III-R, Patient Edition/Non-patient Edition. (SCID-P/SCID-NP) Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- 85.FIRST MB, SPITZER RL, GIBBON M, WILLIAMS JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition With Psychotic Screen (SCID-I/P W/PSY SCREEN) New York: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 86.SALTHOUSE TA. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol Rev. 1996;103:403–428. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.103.3.403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.CHAPMAN LJ, CHAPMAN JP. Problems in the measurement of cognitive deficits. Psychol Bull. 1973;79:380–385. doi: 10.1037/h0034541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.FEIGHNER JP, ROBINS E, GUZE SB, WOODRUFF RA, WINOKUR G, MUNOZ R. Diagnostic criteria for use in psychiatric research. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1972;26:57–63. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1972.01750190059011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.ROBINS LN, REGIER DA. Psychiatric Disorders in America: The Epidemiologic Catchment Area Study. New York: The Free Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 90.HÄFNER H, HAMBRECHT M, LOFFLER W, MUNK-JØRGENSEN P, RIECHER-RÖSSLER A. Is schizophrenia a disorder of all ages? A comparison of first episodes and early course across the life-cycle. Psychol Med. 1998;28:351–365. doi: 10.1017/s0033291797006399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.SEEMAN MV, LANG M. The role of estrogens in schizophrenia gender differences. Schizophr Bull. 1990;16:185–194. doi: 10.1093/schbul/16.2.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.RIECHER-ROSSLER A, DE GEYTER C. The forthcoming role of treatment with oestrogens in mental health. Swiss Med Wkly. 2007;137:565–572. doi: 10.4414/smw.2007.11925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.CHUA WL, DE IZQUIERDO SA, KULKARNI J, MORTIMER A. Estrogen for schizophrenia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2005;4:CD004719. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD004719.pub2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.MUNK-JØRGENSEN P. Has deinstitutionalization gone too far? Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1999;249:136–143. doi: 10.1007/s004060050078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.COHEN CI, COHEN GD, BLANK K, GAITZ C, KATZ IR, LEUCHTER A, et al. Schizophrenia and older adults: an overview: directions for research and policy. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2000;8:19–28. doi: 10.1097/00019442-200002000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.UCHIDA H, SUZUKI T, MAMO DC, et al. Effects of age and age of onset on prescribed antipsychotic dose in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: a survey of 1,418 patients in Japan. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;16:584–593. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e318172b42d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.CIRILLO MA, SEIDMAN LJ. Verbal declarative memory dysfunction in schizophrenia: from clinical assessment to genetics and brain mechanisms. Neuropsychol Rev. 2003;13:43–77. doi: 10.1023/a:1023870821631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.PALMER BW, HEATON RK. Executive dysfunction in schizophrenia. In: Sharma T, Harvey PD, editors. Cognition in Schizophrenia: Impairments, Importance and Treatment Strategies. New York: Oxford University Press; 2000. pp. 52–72. [Google Scholar]
- 99.PAULSEN JS, HEATON RK, SADEK JR, PERRY W, DELIS DC, BRAFF D, et al. The nature of learning and memory impairments in schizophrenia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 1995;1:88–99. doi: 10.1017/s135561770000014x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.RAGLAND JD, YOON J, MINZENBERG MJ, CARTER CS. Neuroimaging of cognitive disability in schizophrenia: Search for a pathophysiological mechanism. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2007;19:417–427. doi: 10.1080/09540260701486365. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.HARRISON PJ. The hippocampus in schizophrenia: A review of the neuropathological evidence and its pathophysiological implications. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004;174:151–162. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1761-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.VAN OSJ. ‘Salience syndrome’ replaces ‘schizophrenia’ in DSM-V and ICD-11: psychiatry’s evidence-based entry into the 21st century? Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2009;120:363–372. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01456.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.RAPOPORT JL, ADDINGTON A, FRANGOU S. The neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia: what can very early onset cases tell us? Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2005;7:81–82. doi: 10.1007/s11920-005-0001-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.LOHR JB, ALDER M, FLYNN K, HARRIS MJ, MCADAMS LA. Minor physical anomalies in older patients with late-onset schizophrenia, early-onset schizophrenia, depression, and Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 1997;5:318–323. doi: 10.1097/00019442-199700540-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.PALMER BW, JESTE DV. Neurodevelopmental theories of schizophrenia: application to late-onset schizophrenia. Indian Journal of Psychiatry. 1996;38:13–22. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.MEDALIA A, CHOI J. Cognitive Remediation in Schizophrenia. Neuropsychology Review. 2009 May 15; doi: 10.1007/s11065-009-9097-y. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.TWAMLEY EW, SAVLA GN, ZURHELLEN CH, HEATON RK, JESTE DV. Development and pilot testing of a novel compensatory cognitive training intervention for people with psychosis. American Journal of Psychiatric Rehabilitation. 2008;11:144–163. doi: 10.1080/15487760801963678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]