Abstract
A response to Maskiewicz and Lineback's essay in the September 2013 issue of CBE-Life Sciences Education.
Dear Editor:
Maskiewicz and Lineback (2013) have written a provocative essay about how the term misconceptions is used in biology education and the learning sciences in general. Their historical perspective highlights the logic and utility of the constructivist theory of learning. They emphasize that students’ preliminary ideas are resources to be built upon, not errors to be eradicated. Furthermore, Maskiewicz and Lineback argue that the term misconception has been largely abandoned by educational researchers, because it is not consistent with constructivist theory. Instead, they conclude, members of the biology education community should speak of preconceptions, naïve conceptions, commonsense conceptions, or alternative conceptions.
We respectfully disagree. Our objections encompass both the semantics of the term misconception and the more general issue of constructivist theory and practice. We now address each of these in turn. (For additional discussion, please see Leonard, Andrews, and Kalinowski, “Misconceptions Yesterday, Today, and Tomorrow,” CBE—Life Sciences Education [LSE], in press, 2014.)
Is misconception suitable for use in scholarly discussions? The answer depends partly on the intended audience. We avoid using the term misconception with students, because it could be perceived as pejorative. However, connotations of disapproval are less of a concern for the primary audience of LSE and similar journals, that is, learning scientists, discipline-based education researchers, and classroom teachers.
An additional consideration is whether misconception is still used in learning sciences outside biology education. Maskiewicz and Lineback claim that misconception is rarely used in journals such as Cognition and Instruction, Journal of the Learning Sciences, Journal of Research in Science Teaching, and Science Education, yet the term appears in about a quarter of the articles published by these journals in 2013 (Table 1). In almost all instances, the authors deployed the word unapologetically, not stating any reservations regarding its appropriateness (see also National Research Council, 2012).
Table 1.
Journal (total articles published in 2013a) | Articles using misconception (“nondisapproving” articles/total articles) | Articles using other terms |
---|---|---|
LSE (59) | 23/24 | Alternative conception (4) |
Commonsense conception (2) | ||
Naïve conception (1) | ||
Preconception (4) | ||
Cognition and Instruction (16) | 3/3 | None |
Journal of the Learning Sciences (17) | 4/4 | Commonsense science knowledge (1) |
Naïve conception (1) | ||
Prior conception (1) | ||
Journal of Research in Science Teaching (49) | 11/13 | Commonsense idea (1) |
Naïve conception (1) | ||
Preconception (5) | ||
Science Education (36) | 10/11 | Naïve conception (1) |
aAs of November 25, 2013. Does not include very short editorials, commentaries, corrections, or prepublication online versions.
A final consideration is whether any of the possible alternatives to misconception are preferable. We feel that the alternatives suggested by Maskiewicz and Lineback are problematic in their own ways. For example, naïve conception sounds more strongly pejorative to us than misconception. Naïve conception and preconception also imply that conceptual challenges occur only at the very beginning stages of learning, even though multiple rounds of conceptual revisions are sometimes necessary (e.g., see figure 1 of Andrews et al., 2012) as students move through learning progressions. Moreover, the terms preferred by Maskiewicz and Lineback are used infrequently (Table 1) and may be perceived as jargon by many readers of LSE, whereas misconception is less cryptic to the average university faculty member. Thus, despite its history and its limitations, the word misconception remains a staple of science education research, and its use should continue.
Our concerns extend beyond the word misconception to a broader debate on constructivism. Table 2 presents several recent LSE papers that, in Maskiewicz and Lineback's opinion, ignore or misunderstand constructivist theory. Maskiewicz and Lineback do not identify the specific passages they find problematic, but we infer from their use of Smith et al. (1993) that they object to statements that misconceptions should be actively confronted, challenged, overcome, corrected, and/or replaced (Table 2). Smith et al. (1993) argue on theoretical grounds that confrontation does not allow refinement of students’ pre-existing, imperfect ideas; instead, the students must simply choose among discrete prepackaged ideas. From Maskiewicz and Lineback's perspective, the papers listed in Table 2 are flawed because they accept or promote confrontation.
Table 2.
Article | Example of constructivist language | Example of language suggesting confrontation |
---|---|---|
Andrews et al., 2011 | “Constructivist theory argues that individuals construct new understanding based on what they already know and believe.… We can expect students to retain serious misconceptions if instruction is not specifically designed to elicit and address the prior knowledge students bring to class” (p. 400). | Instructors were scored for “explaining to students why misconceptions were incorrect” and “making a substantial effort toward correcting misconceptions” (p. 399). “Misconceptions must be confronted before students can learn natural selection” (p. 399). “Instructors need to elicit misconceptions, create situations that challenge misconceptions.” (p. 403). |
Baumler et al., 2012 | “The last pair [of students]'s response invoked introns, an informative answer, in that it revealed a misconception grounded in a basic understanding of the Central Dogma” (p. 89; acknowledges students’ useful prior knowledge). | No relevant text found |
Cox-Paulson et al., 2012 | No relevant text found | This paper barely mentions misconceptions, but cites sources (Phillips et al., 2008; Robertson and Phillips, 2008) that refer to “exposing,” “uncovering,” and “correcting” misconceptions. |
Crowther, 2012 | “Prewritten songs may explain concepts in new ways that clash with students’ mental models and force revision of those models” (p. 28; emphasis added). | “Songs can be particularly useful for countering … conceptual misunderstandings.… Prewritten songs may explain concepts in new ways that clash with students’ mental models and force revision of those models” (p. 28). |
Kalinowski et al., 2010 | “Several different instructional approaches for helping students to change misconceptions … agree that instructors must take students’ prior knowledge into account and help students integrate new knowledge with their existing knowledge” (p. 88). | “One strategy for correcting misconceptions is to challenge them directly by ‘creating cognitive conflict,’ presenting students with new ideas that conflict with their pre-existing ideas about a phenomenon… In addition, study of multiple examples increases the chance of students identifying and overcoming persistent misconceptions” (p. 89). |
aWhile these papers do not adhere to Smith et al.'s (1993) version of constructivism, they do adhere to the constructivist approach that advocates cognitive dissonance.
Our own stance differs from that of Maskiewicz and Lineback, reflecting a lack of consensus within constructivist theory. We agree with those who argue that, not only are confrontations compatible with constructivist learning, they are a central part of it (e.g., Gilbert and Watts, 1983; Hammer, 1996). We note that Baviskar et al. (2009) list “creating cognitive dissonance” as one of the four main tenets of constructivist teaching. Their work is consistent with research showing that focusing students on conflicting ideas improves understanding more than approaches that do not highlight conflicts (e.g., Kowalski and Taylor, 2009; Gadgil et al., 2012). Similarly, the Discipline-Based Education Research report (National Research Council, 2012, p. 70) advocates “bridging analogies,” a form of confrontation, to guide students toward more accurate ways of thinking. Therefore, we do not share Maskiewicz and Lineback's concerns about the papers listed in Table 2. To the extent that these papers are about misconceptions, they seem consistent with forms of constructivism that incorporate confrontation.
Above all, science is about evaluating competing ideas. We help our students when we teach them how to improve their understanding of the natural world by comparing hypotheses. As instructors, our task is to provide a safe, supportive environment in which students can grapple with challenging, conflicting ideas and undergo mini–paradigm shifts (e.g., Price, 2012). We embrace collegial disagreement.
Maskiewicz and Lineback imply that labeling students’ ideas as misconceptions essentially classifies these ideas as either right or wrong, with no intermediate stages for constructivist refinement. In fact, a primary goal of creating concept inventories, which use the term misconception profusely (e.g., Morris et al., 2012; Prince et al., 2012), is to demonstrate that learning is a complex composite of scientifically valid and invalid ideas (e.g., Andrews et al., 2012). A researcher or instructor who uses the word misconceptions can agree wholeheartedly with Maskiewicz and Lineback's point that misconceptions can be a good starting point from which to develop expertise.
As we have seen, misconception is itself fraught with misconceptions. The term now embodies the evolution of our understanding of how people learn. We support the continued use of the term, agreeing with Maskiewicz and Lineback that authors should define it carefully. For example, in our own work, we define misconceptions as inaccurate ideas that can predate or emerge from instruction (e.g., Andrews et al., 2012). We encourage instructors to view misconceptions as opportunities for cognitive dissonance that students encounter as they progress in their learning.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We benefited from excellent conversations with Tessa Andrews (University of Georgia), Sara Brownell and Mary Pat Wenderoth (University of Washington), and Steven Kalinowski (University of Montana), who do not necessarily agree with our opinions.
REFERENCES
- Andrews TM, Leonard MJ, Colgrove CA, Kalinowski ST. Active learning not associated with student learning in a random sample of college biology courses. CBE Life Sci Educ. 2011;10:394–405. doi: 10.1187/cbe.11-07-0061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrews TM, Price RM, Mead LS, McElhinny TL, Thanukos A, Perez KE, Herreid CF, Terry DR, Lemons PP. Biology undergraduates’ misconceptions about genetic drift. CBE Life Sci Educ. 2012;11:248–259. doi: 10.1187/cbe.11-12-0107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baumler DJ, Banta LM, Hung KF, Schwarz JA, Cabot EL, Glasner JD, Perna NT. Using comparative genomics for inquiry-based learning to dissect virulence of Escherichia coli O157:H7 and Yersinia pestis. CBE Life Sci Educ. 2012;11:81–93. doi: 10.1187/cbe.10-04-0057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baviskar SN, Hartle RT, Whitney T. Essential criteria to characterize constructivist teaching: derived from a review of the literature and applied to five constructivist-teaching method articles. Int J Sci Educ. 2009;31:541–550. [Google Scholar]
- Cox-Paulson EA, Grana TM, Harris MA, Batzli JM. Studying human disease genes in Caenorhabditis elegans: a molecular genetics laboratory project. CBE Life Sci Educ. 2012;11:165–179. doi: 10.1187/cbe-11-06-0045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crowther G. Using science songs to enhance learning: an interdisciplinary approach. CBE Life Sci Educ. 2012;11:26–30. doi: 10.1187/cbe.11-08-0068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gadgil S, Nokes-Malach TJ, Chi MTH. Effectiveness of holistic mental model confrontation in driving conceptual change. Learn Instr. 2012;22:47–61. [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert JK, Watts DM. Concepts, misconceptions, and alternative conceptions: changing perspectives in science education. Stud Sci Educ. 1983;10:61–98. [Google Scholar]
- Hammer D. More than misconceptions: multiple perspectives on student knowledge and reasoning, and an appropriate role for education research. Am J Phys. 1996;64:1316. [Google Scholar]
- Kalinowski ST, Leonard MJ, Andrews TM. Nothing in evolution makes sense except in the light of DNA. CBE Life Sci Educ. 2010;9:87–97. doi: 10.1187/cbe.09-12-0088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kowalski P, Taylor AK. The effect of refuting misconceptions in the introductory psychology class. Teach Pyschol. 2009;36:153–159. [Google Scholar]
- Leonard MJ, Andrews TM, Kalinowski ST. Misconceptions yesterday, today, and tomorrow. CBE Life Sci Educ. 2014 doi: 10.1187/cbe.13-12-0244. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maskiewicz AC, Lineback JE. Misconceptions are “so yesterday!”. CBE Life Sci Educ. 2013;12:352–356. doi: 10.1187/cbe.13-01-0014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris GA, Harshman N, Branum-Martin L, Mazur E, Mzoughi T, Baker SD. An item response curves analysis of the Force Concept Inventory. Am J Phys. 2012;80:825–831. [Google Scholar]
- National Research Council. Discipline-Based Education Research: Understanding and Improving Learning in Undergraduate Science and Engineering. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Phillips AR, Robertson AL, Batzli J, Harris M, Miller S. Aligning goals, assessments, and activities: an approach to teaching PCR and gel electrophoresis. CBE Life Sci Educ. 2008;7:96–106. doi: 10.1187/cbe.07-07-0052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price RM. How we got here: an inquiry-based activity about human evolution. Science. 2012;338:1554–1555. doi: 10.1126/science.1215221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prince M, Vigeant M, Nottis K. Development of the Heat and Energy Concept Inventory: preliminary results on the prevalence and persistence of engineering students’ misconceptions. J Eng Educ. 2012;101:412–438. [Google Scholar]
- Robertson AL, Phillips AR. Integrating PCR theory and bioinformatics into a research-oriented primer design exercise. CBE Life Sci Educ. 2008;7:89–95. doi: 10.1187/cbe.07-07-0051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith JP, diSessa AA, Roschelle J. Misconceptions reconceived: a constructivist analysis of knowledge in transition. J Learn Sci. 1993;3:115–163. [Google Scholar]