Abstract
Purpose:
The current study sought to characterize the language phenotype of fragile X syndrome (FXS), focusing on the extent of impairment in receptive syntax, within-syndrome variability in those impairments in relation to gender, and the syndrome specificity of those impairments.
Method:
The Test for the Reception of Grammar-2 (Bishop, 2003) was used to examine the overall receptive syntactic skills of adolescents with FXS (n = 35; 30 males), adolescents with Down syndrome (DS; n = 28; 18 males), and younger typically developing children (n = 23; 14 males) matched on nonverbal cognition. Performance on specific grammatical constructions and error types were examined for a subset of matched participants.
Results:
Participants with FXS had overall receptive syntax scores that were lower than typically children. Males and females with FXS had higher overall receptive syntax scores than participants with DS; however, there was no difference in performance when females were excluded. Grammatical constructions that were especially difficult for those with FXS and DS were identified, especially relative clause constructions and reversible constructions requiring attention to word order encoded by syntactic features.
Conclusions:
The current findings have implications for understanding the nature of the language learning difficulties of FXS and for language interventions.
Fragile X syndrome (FXS), the most common form of inherited intellectual disability, results from a mutation in the FMR1 gene located on the X chromosome (Crawford, Acuna, & Sherman, 2001). It is estimated that 1 in 4,000 males and 1 in 6,000 to 8,000 females are affected with FXS (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [CDC], 2010). Because it is an X-linked condition, FXS is more common in males than females and males are more severely affected, on average, than females (Crawford et al., 2001). The cognitive abilities presumed to be important for language (e.g., auditory memory) are typically impaired or delayed in FXS (Ornstein et al., 2008). Consequently, most individuals with FXS have language impairments, although there is wide variability in the extent of the impairment even within each gender (Abbeduto, Brady, & Kover, 2007). It is not clear, however, whether impairments are variable across the different components of language. The aim of the current study was to further characterize the language phenotype of FXS. The focus was on receptive syntax, with the goals being to examine the extent and source(s) of impairments in receptive syntax, within-syndrome variability in those impairments in relation to gender, and the syndrome specificity of the impairments through a comparison with Down syndrome (DS).
The majority of previous research on language in FXS has focused on the expressive domain (Finestack & Abbeduto, 2010; Finestack, Palmer, & Abbeduto, 2012; Levy, Gottesman, Borochowitz, Frydman, & Sagi, 2006; Mazzocco et al., 2006; Murphy & Abbeduto, 2007; Price et al., 2008). Most of this research has relied on broad summary measures, such as mean length of utterance (MLU) in spontaneous language samples or total scores on standardized tests (Abbeduto et al., 2007). Some studies, however, have assessed more specific aspects of expressive language, such as the use of complex clauses (Levy et al., 2006) or various elements of narrative structure (Finestack et al., 2012), with the goal being to provide a more detailed and clinically useful picture of language development in FXS. In general, individuals with FXS display delays in expressive language relative to their levels of nonverbal cognitive ability, with expressive syntax posing especially serious challenges (Finestack & Abbeduto, 2010; Finestack, Sterling, & Abbeduto, 2013; Levy et al., 2006; Price et al., 2008). It is important to extend these findings to the receptive modality in FXS because the extent and profile of impairments has been found to be quite different across modalities in some disorders (Volden et al., 2011). Variation in impairments across modalities can provide insights into mechanisms and potential intervention targets.
Unfortunately, there have been only two studies of language comprehension in FXS. In the first study, Abbeduto and colleagues (2003) compared the receptive language skills of male and female adolescents and young adults with FXS without comorbid autism to those of typically developing (TD) children matched on nonverbal mental age (NVMA). Participants were assessed using the Test for Auditory Comprehension of Language-Revised (TACL-R; Carrow-Woolfolk, 1985), which includes subtests to measure vocabulary, grammatical morphology, and multi-word syntactic patterns. Abbeduto et al. found that there were no differences in age-equivalent scores on any of the three subtests between the participants with FXS and those with TD, suggesting that vocabulary, grammatical morphology, and syntax more generally keep pace with nonverbal cognition in adolescents and young adults with FXS. In the second study Price, Roberts, Vandergrift, and Martin (2007) focused only on males with FXS and used the more recent TACL-3. Price et al. also found that performance was equally delayed across the domains of vocabulary, grammatical morphology, and syntax; however, they also found that boys with FXS regardless of comorbid autism status had lower age-equivalents than expected based on their nonverbal cognitive levels on all three subtests.
These inconsistent findings regarding performance relative to NVMA across the two studies might relate to the fact that the sample in the Abbeduto et al. (2003) study was older on average and included males and females. Differences in the two versions of the TACL might have had an impact on the findings as well. In the present study, an attempt was made to clarify the picture by using the Test for Reception of Grammar-2 (TROG-2; Bishop, 2003) rather than the TACL and systematically examining the impact of gender on the findings. In addition, the sample for the present study fell in the middle of, and overlapped with, the samples of Abbeduto et al. and Price et al. in terms of age.
Any attempt to characterize receptive language must recognize its complex and multifaceted nature. Many standardized assessments of receptive language, including those that purport to examine specific domains of language (e.g., receptive syntax), are seldom sufficient for characterizing profiles of receptive language impairments because they fail to fully probe mastery of specific linguistic elements or constructions. An individual who fails to understand a sentence such as The man is chasing the dog, for example, might do so because he/she does not understand the meanings of individual words, fails to recognize that the syntactic frame specifies that the first noun phrase is the agent and the second noun phrase the recipient, or simply cannot maintain the sentence in memory long enough to get to its meaning. Deciding among these (and other alternatives) is likely to require a more careful assessment of performance on specific linguistic constructions or an examination of the types of errors made during processing so as to isolate the source(s) of difficulty (Laws & Bishop, 2003). Isolating the source(s) of receptive language difficulties is important for adequately characterizing the phenotype of FXS as well as for identifying and prioritizing targets for intervention. In the present study, therefore, the comprehension of specific language forms and the types of errors were examined in addition to the overall level of receptive language performance.
In the case of FXS, virtually all aspects of cognition are impaired and thus, there are likely to be numerous contributors to receptive language problems (Abbeduto et al., 2007). Nevertheless, there are two areas of especially severe cognitive impairment in FXS that are likely to have an important impact on receptive language. First, it has been documented that individuals with FXS are quite poor at processing sequential patterns (Burack et al., 1999; Dykens, Hodapp, & Leckman, 1987). This observation raises the possibility that language forms whose comprehension hinges on attending to the order of linguistic elements will be especially challenging for those with FXS. Second, auditory memory is also an area of special weakness (Baker et al., 2011), which raises the possibility that language forms that place particularly heavy demands on auditory memory during comprehension will also be especially challenging for individuals with FXS. In the present study, therefore, special attention was focused on the comprehension of (a) reversible sentences with the prepositions in and on (e.g., The duck is on the ball) and (b) reversible SVO sentences (e.g., The man is chasing the dog), each of which depends on processing information about word order, and (c) sentences containing four lexical elements (e.g., There is a yellow star and a big flower) and (d) sentences with subject relative clauses (e.g., The man that is eating looks at the cat), each of which places heavy demands on auditory memory. Data on these forms will help illuminate the ways in which other aspects of the FXS cognitive phenotype contribute to language difficulties.
Because individuals with FXS, particularly males, also have an intellectual disability (i.e., an IQ of 70 or less), it is useful to determine the extent to which the FXS language is syndrome specific or common to individuals with an intellectual disability. Such a determination typically requires comparison with individuals who have an intellectual disability of a different etiology (Dykens, Hodapp, & Finucane, 2000). DS provides a useful comparison for FXS in part because the language phenotype of DS has been well described (Abbeduto & McDuffie, 2010). In particular, individuals with DS have especially severe deficits in the area of syntax, with these deficits being seen in both the expressive and the receptive modalities (Abbeduto et al., 2003; Chapman, Schwartz, & Kay-Raining Bird, 1991; Glenn & Cunningham, 2005, Laws & Bishop, 2003; Miller, 1988). Moreover, the receptive syntax deficits in DS do not appear to be attributable solely to auditory memory impairments, hearing loss, or other nonlinguistic impairments (Chapman & Hesketh, 2001; Laws & Bishop, 2003); instead, syntactic processing appears to pose its own unique challenges or, at least, to magnify impairments in auditory memory, hearing, or other nonlinguistic domains of processing (Laws & Bishop, 2003).
Current Study
The present study evaluated the possibility that FXS, like DS, is characterized by especially severe impairments in receptive syntax. This possibility was addressed by comparing the receptive syntax performance of individuals with FXS to that of individuals with DS matched on NVMA. The measure of receptive syntax was the TROG-2, which has only recently been used in studies of FXS (McDuffie et al., 2012; Pierpont et al., 2011). Four constructions from the TROG-2 were selected for additional in-depth analysis: reversible in and on sentences, reversible SVO sentences, sentences containing four lexical elements, and sentences with subject relative clauses (see Table 1 for examples of all constructions). These forms were selected because they were administered to a relatively large number of participants in the study and because they make it possible to determine whether syntactic information per se placed an added burden on the poor sequential processing and limited auditory memory of individuals with FXS. In particular, comparison of reversible in and on sentences to reversible SVO sentences is useful because the relationships among words are encoded lexically in the former sentences and syntactically in the latter sentences. Similarly, comparison of four-element sentences to sentences with subject relatives is useful because, despite being of similar length, only the latter are multiclause and thus, highly syntactically complex (Bishop, 1997; Karmiloff-Smith et al., 1997; van der Lely & Harris, 1990). Finally, the types of comprehension errors committed by the participants were examined to determine whether they had their origin in syntactically or lexically based decisions during comprehension. Examination of such error types has helped uncover similarities and differences between individuals with DS and those with specific language impairment in regards to the source of receptive language difficulties (Laws & Bishop, 2003). Thus, the present study was designed to clarify the extent and nature of receptive impairments in FXS, thereby yielding insights into the factors that contribute to those impairments and identify targets and avenues for intervention.
Table 1.
Block | Construction | Example item |
---|---|---|
A | Two elements | The sheep is running. |
B | Negative | The fork is not big. |
C | Reversible in and on | The duck is on the ball. |
D | Three elements | The girl pushes the box. |
E | Reversible SVO | The man is chasing the dog. |
F | Four elements | There is a yellow star and a big flower. |
G | Relative clause in subject | The man that is eating looks at the cat. |
H | Not only X but also Y | The man is not only running but also pointing. |
I | Reversible above and below | The cup is below the star. |
J | Comparative/absolute | The flower is longer than the comb. |
K | Reversible passive | The cow is chased by the girl. |
L | Zero anaphor | The book is on the scarf and is blue. |
M | Pronoun gender/number | They are carrying him. |
N | Pronoun binding | The girl sees that the lady is pointing at her. |
O | Neither nor | The girl is neither pointing nor running. |
P | X but not Y | The man but not the horse is jumping. |
Q | Postmodified subject | The scarf on the shoe is blue. |
R | Singular/plural inflection | The cat chases the ducks. |
S | Relative clause in object | The man pushes the cow that is standing. |
T | Centre-embedded sentence | The sheep the girl looks at is running. |
Study Questions and Predictions
-
1.
Are there diagnostic group and gender differences in overall receptive syntactic skills among young adolescents with FXS and DS? In light of previous findings on DS (Abbeduto et al., 2003; Chapman et al., 1991; Glenn & Cunningham, 2005, Laws & Bishop, 2003; Miller, 1988), as well as the inconsistent findings for FXS (Abbeduto et al., 2003; Price et al., 2007), it was hypothesized that after controlling for nonverbal cognitive ability, young adolescents with FXS would score higher in terms of overall receptive syntactic skills than would young adolescents with DS. No prediction was possible, however, as to how the receptive syntactic skills of young adolescents with FXS would compare to TD participants. Additionally, based on the X-linked nature of FXS, it was predicted that males with FXS would score lower on overall receptive syntax than females with FXS.
-
2.
Are there diagnostic group differences in comprehension of sentences differing in length, reversibility, and clause embedding among young adolescents with FXS and DS? Due to the cognitive and auditory memory deficits found in both FXS (Baker et al., 2011; Ornstein et al., 2008) and DS (Chapman & Hesketh, 2001; Laws & Bishop, 2003), it was hypothesized that both group profiles would reflect special difficulty with syntax and thus, word order and clause embedding.
-
3.
Are there diagnostic group differences in the pattern of lexical and syntactic errors across these sentence distinctions (i.e., length, reversibility, and syntactic complexity) among young adolescents with FXS and DS? Due to the cognitive and auditory memory deficits found in both FXS (Baker et al., 2011; Ornstein et al., 2008) and DS (Chapman & Hesketh, 2001; Laws & Bishop, 2003), it was hypothesized that the group profiles would reflect special difficulty with syntactically based errors relative to lexically based errors.
Method
Participants
Participants were drawn from a larger longitudinal project on language development in FXS and DS that involved four annual assessments. During each of the four annual visits, participants completed a battery of language and cognitive measures. The current study utilized only a subset of these measures, most of which were obtained at the Time 1 (T1) assessment. Three groups participated in this larger study: 53 children and adolescents with FXS, ages 10;2 to 16;0 at T1, 30 children and adolescents with DS, ages 10;2 to 15;9 at T1, and 56 TD children, ages 3;1 to 8;9 at T1. Ninety percent of participants identified as Caucasian, 4% as African American, 4% as Hispanic, and 2% identified as “other.” Regarding maternal educational level, 42% of participants’ mothers graduated from high school, 43% graduated from college, and 15% graduated with an advanced degree1. Although other papers have been published based on this larger project (Kover, McDuffie, Abbeduto, & Brown, 2012; McDuffie et al., 2010; McDuffie, Kover, Abbeduto, Lewis, & Brown, 2012; Pierpont, Richmond, Abbeduto, Kover, & Brown, 2011), none has focused on the questions of interest in the present study.
Participants with FXS and DS were recruited nationally using a variety of sources, including a university recruitment registry, Internet websites and listservs, newspaper advertisements, and postings/flyers at parent meetings. Participants with TD were largely recruited locally using posters and flyers in public places and a university research registry. Enrollment criteria included English as the native language, use of three-word phrases at least occasionally, and no significant uncorrected vision or hearing impairments, all determined from parent report. Parents of TD children indicated that their child was not receiving special education services at the time of participation, with the exception of limited speech articulation therapy. Parents of adolescents with FXS or DS provided diagnostic confirmation, generally through copies of medical records.
Individuals with more than a mild hearing loss (i.e., pure tone air conduction threshold of 30 dB HL or higher in each ear averaged across 500, 1000, and 2000 Hz) at T1 were excluded from the current study, which resulted in the exclusion of one participant with DS. For one participant with FXS, hearing could not be evaluated at T1 or T2 (one year after T1) because of his level of cooperation and functioning; however, his hearing was successfully evaluated at T3 (two years after T1) and he was found to meet the hearing criterion and thus, he was included in the current study.
Several within-group sibling pairs (n = 8) participated in the larger longitudinal study; however, only one sibling from same-gender sibling pairs was included in the current study. This resulted in the exclusion of five TD participants. Opposite-gender sibling pairs were not excluded from the current study because gender was a factor of interest in some analyses.
Scores from standardized measures of language and cognition, described below, were obtained during T1 for all but two participants with FXS and one participant with TD. For these latter three participants, the tasks of interest were either not completed in a standardized way, not attempted because of non-compliance, or the participant completed less than half of the measure of nonverbal cognition. In these cases, data from the first valid measures (at T2 or T3) were substituted.
Procedures
Written consent was given by all parents before participation. Testing sessions took place in a quiet room and lasted between four and eight hours over the course of two days with breaks taken as needed. A variety of standardized and experimental measures of language and cognition were administered as part of the larger longitudinal study. As previously mentioned, only a subset of the measures formed the basis for the present study.
Measures
Receptive language
Participants were assessed using the TROG-2 (Bishop, 2003), which is a standardized measure used to examine syntactic comprehension skills. According to the manual, internal consistency of the TROG-2 is .877 (N = 896). Correlations between the TROG-2 and subtests from the CELF-P (Linguistics Concepts subtest) and the CELF-3 (Concepts and Directions subtest) reveal moderate levels of correlation (.582 and .525 respectively; Bishop, 2003).
The TROG-2 consists of 20 blocks (A – T), each testing a specific grammatical construction. The order of administration of the blocks reflects their relative order of difficulty for the normative sample. Examples of the grammatical constructions are shown in Table 1. Each block contains four test items. The vocabulary included in the TROG-2 was restricted to simple words to minimize lexical influences on comprehension performance (Bishop, 2003).
For each item in the TROG-2 (Bishop, 2003), the examiner read a sentence that referred to one of four drawings, and the participant’s task was to point to the one drawing that corresponded to the meaning of the sentence. The distracter drawings, or foils, differed by either a lexical or grammatical element. Choosing a drawing that differed from the target by a lexical element (e.g., a noun, verb, or adjective) reflected a lexical error, whereas choosing a drawing that differed by a grammatical element (e.g., a function word, word order, or inflection) resulted in a grammatical error. For example, an item testing the reversible SVO construction took the form, The man is chasing the dog. A lexical distracter for this item depicted a man chasing a ball; a grammatical distracter depicted a dog chasing a man. The foils for a few grammatical constructions (e.g., the four-element construction) contained only lexical distracters, meaning that only lexical errors were possible. Most blocks were constructed so that both lexical and grammatical distracters or only grammatical distracters were included.
Consistent with the manual and standardization of the test, testing began at the first item in Block A and was discontinued after five consecutive failed blocks, with one or more incorrect responses in a block constituting failure. The total number of blocks passed was used to calculate standard and age-equivalent scores. Although using the total number of blocks passed is useful to determine an individual’s overall receptive language abilities, a finer level of analysis of an individual’s performance on the TROG-2 is possible by examining the number of items answered correctly in each block as opposed to whether the block was failed or passed (McDuffie et al., 2012). In the current study, the total number of items answered correctly (rather than blocks passed) was used in the analyses of overall performance and performance on the grammatical constructions of interest.
Nonverbal cognition
Participants were assessed using the Brief IQ subtests of the Leiter International Performance Scale - Revised (Leiter-R; Roid & Miller, 1997): Figure Ground, Form Completion, Sequential Order, and Repeated Patterns. These subtests measure visualization and fluid reasoning skills and yield a nonverbal IQ score, age-equivalent score, and growth score. The Leiter-R is fully nonverbal; examiners use pantomime and nonverbal cues to explain the task and participant responses are given by either pointing or with shapes or cards.
Analyses
Separate analyses addressed the research questions relating to (1) overall performance, (2) performance on specific grammatical constructions, and (3) patterns of error types. Prior to the analyses of interest, participants were compared group-wise on nonverbal cognitive ability using Leiter-R growth scores. Growth scores were used instead of age-equivalent scores because the former provides a measure of the individual’s skills assessed by the items on the Leiter-R rather than by comparing the participants’ abilities to individuals in the same age group (Roid & Miller, 1997). Whereas standardized norm-referenced scales may not always provide a detailed look at the actual skills and growth of an individual, especially for individuals who are functioning lower than their same-aged peers, growth scores reflect the absolute level of ability and may detect small differences in ability, which is particularly useful for individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders. Conceptually, growth scores are similar to age-equivalent scores but without the latter’s psychometric limitations. After initial exclusions based on hearing loss and sibling status, TD children (n = 12) whose nonverbal IQ scores were above 130 (two standard deviations from the mean), as well as TD children (n = 3) and one adolescent with FXS whose total growth scores were high relative to the other participants (i.e., above 500) were excluded from the current study so that a group-wise match could be achieved. This resulted in samples of 52 participants with FXS, 29 participants with DS, and 36 TD participants. Based on these groups, participants with FXS and TD were well matched on Leiter-R growth scores t(86) = .14, p = .890, d = .03, whereas participants with DS did differ from those with FXS and TD, t(79) = 3.5, p = .001, d = .85, and t(63) = 3.2, p = .002, d = .82, respectively. Thus, participants with growth scores higher than 475 were also excluded (n = 31) to allow acceptable overlap in growth scores from the Leiter-R.
Thus, the analysis addressing the first research question included a final sample of 35 participants with FXS, 28 participants with DS, and 23 participants with TD. Characteristics of participants included in this analysis are shown in Table 2. T-tests revealed that there were no significant differences in nonverbal cognitive ability (i.e., Leiter-R growth scores) between participants with FXS and TD, t(56) = −.419, p = .677, d = .11, TD and DS, t(49) = .163, p = .871, d = .05, or FXS and DS, t(61) = .645, p = .522, d = .16. There were 5, 9, and 10 females in the FXS, TD, and DS groups, respectively.
Table 2.
DS |
FXS |
TD |
||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Females | Males | Females | Males | Females | Males | |||||||
(n = 10) | (n = 18) | (n = 5) | (n = 30) | (n = 9) | (n = 14) | |||||||
|
||||||||||||
Characteristic | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD |
Chronological age | 12.7 | 1.4 | 12.9 | 1.9 | 11.0 | 1.0 | 12.8 | 1.8 | 4.7 | 0.5 | 4.6 | 1.1 |
Nonverbal IQa | 45.0 | 8.6 | 41.8 | 5.9 | 54.6 | 7.7 | 42.5 | 5.9 | 110.3 | 7.7 | 107.4 | 12.6 |
Leiter-R growth score | 465.1 | 7.1 | 460.6 | 7.3 | 468.6 | 4.5 | 462.5 | 7.2 | 463.9 | 9.9 | 461.7 | 9.8 |
Leiter-R age-equivalent | 5.3 | 0.7 | 4.7 | 0.7 | 5.6 | 0.5 | 4.9 | 0.7 | 5.1 | 0.4 | 4.8 | 1.0 |
One participant with DS completed only three out of four subtests of the Leiter-R Brief IQ subtests; therefore, scores from the three subtests were averaged to obtain estimated scores.
As a result of the standardized administration of the TROG-2, in which testing is discontinued after five consecutive failed blocks, not all participants were administered the construction containing four lexical elements (Block F) and the construction containing a subject relative clause (Block G). Therefore, only participants who completed each of the blocks of interest (i.e., reversible in and on, reversible SVO, four elements, and relative clause in subject) were included in analyses of those specific grammatical constructions and their accompanying error patterns. Thus, analyses addressing the second and third research questions included 23 participants with FXS, 22 with DS, and 22 with TD, as shown in Tables 4 and 5. The groups in these analyses were well matched on Leiter-R growth scores: FXS and DS, t(43) = −.14, p = .893, d = .04, FXS and TD, t(43) = −.50, p = .620, d = .15, and DS and TD, t(42) = −.60, p = .551, d = .03. As a result of the small number of females in each group, gender was not included in the analyses addressing these research questions.
Table 4.
DS | FXS | TD | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(n = 22) | (n = 23) | (n = 22) | ||||
|
||||||
Characteristic | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD |
Chronological age | 13.0 | 1.7 | 12.8 | 1.8 | 4.7 | 0.9 |
Nonverbal IQ | 43.4 | 7.5 | 43.3 | 6.9 | 108.6 | 11.5 |
Leiter-R growth score | 464.4 | 5.9 | 464.1 | 5.7 | 463.1 | 7.6 |
Leiter-R age-equivalent | 5.1 | 0.6 | 5.1 | 0.6 | 5.0 | 0.8 |
Table 5.
DS | FXS | TD | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(n = 22) | (n = 23) | (n = 22) | ||||
|
||||||
Characteristic | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD |
Standard Score | 55.0 | 0.0 | 55.0 | 0.0 | 101.9 | 12.7 |
Age-equivalent | 4.1 | 0.2 | 4.1 | 0.2 | 5.1 | 1.3 |
Total blocks passed | 3.0 | 1.4 | 3.1 | 1.3 | 6.9 | 3.8 |
Total items passed | 25.5 | 7.8 | 24.7 | 5.8 | 46.4 | 16.3 |
Results
Overall Performance
Research Question 1 was addressed using a 3 (group: FXS, DS, TD) X 2 (gender: male, female) ANOVA, with number of items answered correctly on the TROG-2 as the dependent variable, to test for overall group differences and to establish whether the effect of gender differed across groups. Partial eta squared, a measure of effect size, was calculated and interpreted using the values of .01 representing a small effect, .06 representing a medium effect, and .14 representing a large effect (Cohen, 1988). Scores derived from performance on the TROG-2 are shown in Table 3. There was a significant main effect of group, F(2, 80) = 17.7, p < .001, partial η2 = .31, a significant main effect of gender, F(1, 80) = 7.6, p = .007, partial η2 = .09, and a significant Group X Gender interaction, F(2, 80) = 3.4, p = .038, partial η2 = .08. Participants with TD scored higher than participants with FXS, p < .001, and higher than participants with DS, p < .001. In addition, participants with FXS scored higher than those with DS, p = .019. Although females scored higher than males, the effect of gender was qualified by the interaction with group, which was examined using separate ANOVAs for each group. For participants with FXS, analyses yielded a significant effect of gender, F(1,33) = 16.5, p < .001, partial η2 = .33, but no significant effect of gender was found for participants with TD, F(1, 21) = .011, p = .917, partial η2 < .01, or participants with DS, F(1, 26) = 3.7, p = .066, partial η2 = .12. To examine whether females accounted for the main effect of group, a separate univariate ANOVA with group was conducted only for males, with number correct as the dependent variable. Analyses yielded a significant effect of group, F(2, 59) = 22.8, p < .001, partial η2 = .44. Male participants with TD scored significantly higher than male participants with FXS, p < .001, and male participants with DS, p < .001; however, no significant difference was found between male participants with FXS and DS, p = .648.
Table 3.
DS |
FXS |
TD |
||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Females | Males | Females | Males | Females | Males | |||||||
(n = 10) | (n = 18) | (n = 5) | (n = 30) | (n = 9) | (n = 14) | |||||||
|
||||||||||||
Variable | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD |
Standard score | 55.0 | 0.0 | 55.0 | 0.0 | 59.8 | 6.7 | 55.0 | 0.0 | 100.8 | 12.8 | 100.9 | 14.2 |
Age-equivalent | 4.1 | 0.2 | 4.0 | 0.2 | 4.7 | 0.9 | 4.1 | 0.2 | 4.9 | 0.9 | 5.2 | 1.5 |
Total blocks passed | 3.1 | 1.6 | 2.2 | 1.6 | 5.6 | 3.6 | 2.6 | 1.7 | 6.4 | 3.3 | 6.6 | 4.5 |
Total items passed | 26.8 | 9.5 | 20.1 | 8.6 | 41.6 | 19.4 | 21.7 | 8.1 | 44.3 | 14.9 | 45.1 | 19.7 |
Performance on Specific Constructions
To address Research Question 2, performance on the four grammatical constructions was analyzed using a 3 (group: FXS, DS, TD) X 4 (grammatical construction: reversible in and on, reversible SVO, four elements, and relative clause in subject) repeated measures ANOVA, with grammatical construction as the repeated measure and the number of items answered correctly as the dependent variable. Analyses yielded a main effect of group, F(2,64) = 15.4, p < .001, partial η2 = .33, grammatical construction, F(3,192) = 11.0, p < .001, partial η2 = .15, and a Group X Grammatical construction interaction, F(6,291) = 3.8, p < .001, partial η2 = .11 (see Figure 1). Participants with TD scored higher than participants with FXS and higher than participants with DS, ps < .001. In addition, participants with FXS scored higher than participants with DS, p = .045. Analyzing the effect of grammatical construction, sentences with a subject relative clause were significantly more challenging than all other constructions, ps < .001, except four-element sentences, p =.157. On average, reversible in and on sentences resulted in significantly more correct answers than four-element sentences, p = .001. Reversible SVO sentences were significantly easier than four-element sentences, p = .043. Reversible SVO sentences were somewhat more difficult than reversible in and on sentences but this difference was not significant, p = .161.
To further investigate the interaction between group and grammatical construction, tests of simple effects were conducted for each grammatical construction. For reversible in and on sentences, there was a main effect of group, F(2,64) = 4.1, p = .02, partial η2 = .12. Participants with DS passed significantly fewer items in the reversible in and on sentences than participants with TD or FXS, ps = .015, whereas participants with TD and FXS did not perform significantly differently from one another on average, p = .97. For reversible SVO sentences, there was also a main effect of group, F(2,64) = 15.5, p < .001, partial η2 = .33. Participants with TD passed significantly more items in reversible SVO sentences than either participants with FXS or participants with DS, ps < .001. Performance on reversible SVO sentences for individuals with DS and FXS did not differ, p = .440. For four-element sentences, the analysis yielded no main effect of group, F(2,64) = 2.1, p = .134, partial η2 = .06. Analysis of subject relative clause sentences revealed a main effect of group, F(2,64) = 20.6, p < .001, partial η2 = .39. Participants with TD correctly answered more items in sentences with subject relative clauses than those with FXS or DS, ps < .001, whereas participants with FXS and DS did not perform significantly differently from one another, p = .162.
Patterns of Error Type
To address Research Question 3, patterns of errors were examined by comparing the number of lexical and grammatical errors made for the items testing the four constructions of interest (i.e., reversible in and on sentences, reversible SVO sentences, sentences with four lexical elements, and sentences with subject relative clauses) for participants who completed those blocks. Again, because of the small number of females in each group, gender was not included in this analysis. Across the four grammatical constructions, it was possible to make 27 grammatical errors and 21 lexical errors. To account for this difference and allow for comparison across the two types of errors, the number of errors was prorated, such that the number of errors of each type was divided by the total possible number of errors. The prorated sum of lexical errors and the prorated sum of grammatical errors committed in the context of the four constructions, shown in Table 6, were the dependent variables for each participant. Errors were then analyzed using a 3 (group: FXS, DS, TD) X 2 (error type: lexical, grammatical) repeated measures ANOVA, with error type as the repeated measure. A main effect of group, F(2,64) = 13.7, p < .001, partial η2 = .30, a main effect of error type, F(1,64) = 37.7, p < .001, partial η2 = .37, and a significant Group X Error type interaction, F(2,64) = 7.6, p < .001, partial η2 = .19, emerged (see Figure 2). On average, participants made more grammatical errors than lexical errors. As would be expected based on their overall performance, TD participants made fewer errors than those with FXS or DS, p = .001 and p < .001 respectively. There was no significant difference between the number of errors made by participants with FXS and DS p = .056.
Table 6.
DS | FXS | TD | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(n = 22) | (n = 23) | (n = 22) | ||||
|
||||||
Grammatical Construction | Gram. Errors | Lex. Errors | Gram. Errors | Lex. Errors | Gram. Errors | Lex. Errors |
Reversible In and On | 1.36 | - | 0.74 | - | 0.73 | - |
Reversible SVO | 1.59 | 0.09 | 1.22 | 0.26 | 0.32 | 0.00 |
Four Elements | - | 1.82 | - | 1.57 | - | 1.09 |
Relative Clause in Subject | 2.14 | 0.23 | 2.04 | 0.04 | 0.64 | 0.05 |
Note. A dash indicates that errors of that type were not possible for items testing that grammatical construction.
To investigate the significant Group X Error type interaction, separate 2 (group) X 2 (error type) ANOVAs were conducted. Analyses revealed significant Group X Error type interactions for all pairs of groups. In contrast to TD participants who made very few errors of the grammatical or lexical type, participants with FXS not only made more errors overall but showed increased grammatical errors relative to lexical errors, F(1,43) = 27.4, p < .001, partial η2= .39. Participants with FXS and DS did not perform significantly different in terms of number of errors overall; however, relative to participants with FXS, participants with DS made a significantly greater number of grammatical errors, F(1, 43) = 4.4, p = .041, partial η2 = .09.
In an exploratory descriptive analysis, the mean raw frequency of specific types of grammatical and lexical errors was investigated. Of course, not all error types were possible for each construction and thus, only verb and adjective lexical errors and word order and function word grammatical errors could be examined. No statistical tests were performed because of the uneven opportunities for error types among constructions. On average, participants with FXS and DS made more grammatical errors of the word order type than TD children in reversible SVO sentences (1.22, 1.59, 0.32, respectively) and sentences with subject relative clauses (2.04, 2.14, 0.64, respectively). On reversible in and on sentences, participants with DS made a greater number of grammatical errors of the function word type than TD and FXS participants (0.55, 0.13, 0.09, respectively). For four-element sentences, only lexical errors were possible and the pattern of errors across participant groups was in line with their overall performance (i.e., 1.82 lexical errors by those with DS, compared to 1.57 by participants with FXS, and 1.09 by TD children).
Discussion
The purpose of the present study was to examine the comprehension of specific syntactic constructions and types of errors made by males and females with FXS to gain a more nuanced understanding of the linguistic phenotype of this population. In addition, greater knowledge of the syntactic comprehension skills of individuals with FXS can aid in the development of appropriate interventions.
Overall Performance for Syntactic Comprehension
The first research question focused on overall levels of syntactic comprehension in terms of differences as a function of diagnostic group and gender. TD children achieved higher total TROG-2 scores for items passed than young adolescents with FXS. This result conflicts with findings from Abbeduto et al. (2003) for older adolescents and young adults with FXS, who did not differ significantly from NVMA-matched TD children on the TACL-R. Instead, results of the current study are in line with those from Price et al. (2007), which included a younger group of males with FXS and DS. The current finding, that young adolescents with FXS achieved overall scores that were lower than the scores of the younger NVMA-matched TD children, suggests that the overall syntactic comprehension skills of young adolescent males and females with FXS is lower than expected based on nonverbal cognitive ability. It may be that in late adolescence or even young adulthood, individuals with FXS catch up in terms of receptive language skills; however, longitudinal research is needed to investigate this further.
Young adolescents with FXS achieved overall scores that were higher than the scores of the young adolescents with DS, suggesting that the syntactic comprehension skills of young adolescents with FXS are not as severely affected as in DS (Abbeduto et al., 2003; Price et al., 2007). However, results from the group by gender interactions for each diagnostic group revealed a gender difference only for the FXS group with females outperforming males. This suggests that females with FXS may be accounting for the higher performance of participants with FXS relative to DS. When females were excluded from the analysis, male adolescents with FXS and DS did not perform significantly different from one another. Thus, males with FXS appear to have receptive syntax deficits as severe as those seen in DS, a condition long-recognized for having especially serious syntactic impairments. Clearly syntax must be a target of intervention for males with FXS.
Despite their superior performance relative to males, it is noteworthy that the average TROG-2 standard score obtained by females with FXS was almost 3 standard deviations below average, consistent with the claim that language poses serious challenges for many females with FXS (Sterling & Abbeduto, 2012). The current study chose to include females with FXS because they are a relatively understudied group compared to males with FXS; however, these findings suggest that because of their quantitatively distinct levels of delay, future studies may do well to examine the performance of males and females with FXS separately in order to ensure an accurate picture of the FXS phenotype in relation to other neurodevelopmental disorders. In the following discussions of the second and third research questions, in which gender was not a factor, findings were interpreted for males and females together because diagnostic groups were matched on NVMA, and therefore, had quantitatively similar profiles.
Performance on Specific Grammatical Constructions
The second research question focused on performance differences on specific grammatical constructions as a function of diagnostic group. The constructions examined were reversible sentences with the prepositions in and on (e.g., The duck is on the ball), reversible SVO sentences (e.g., The man is chasing the dog), sentences containing four lexical elements (e.g., There is a yellow star and a big flower), and sentences with subject relative clauses (e.g., The man that is eating looks at the cat). These constructions were chosen based in part on the fact that they were administered to most of the participants in the longitudinal study from which the data were drawn, ensuring an adequate sample size. At the same time, however, these constructions were interesting because they tested dimensions of receptive syntax (i.e., reversibility and length) likely to be especially sensitive to the sequential processing and auditory memory deficits of FXS.
On average across all participants, comprehension of active declarative sentences with four lexical elements was more difficult than reversible sentences including the locative in or on and reversible SVO sentences. Although comprehension of these two reversible constructions depends on processing information about word order, the syntax in these constructions is relatively simple and the sentences are rather short, thereby placing little demand on auditory memory. This difference in performance may have been due to the fact that comprehension of four elements within a sentence places heavy demands on auditory memory independent of syntactic complexity. Interestingly, the four-element construction was tested only with lexical distracters, reinforcing the interpretation that poor performance cannot be attributed to difficulty with the syntax of the construction (Bishop, 2003). Items testing sentences that contained a subject relative clause also were more difficult than the two reversible constructions for all groups. The former sentences containing relative clauses also place heavy demands on auditory memory. Future research should focus on a wider range of constructions and experimentally manipulate auditory memory load to verify the source of difficulty for the four-element and subject relative sentences difficulty for individuals with FXS.
Adolescents with FXS presented a mixed pattern with regards to comprehension of reversible sentences. They were as successful on reversible in and on sentences as were the TD children; however, they were less successful on reversible SVO sentences than were the TD children. This difference may be explained by the way the words in these two sentence types are encoded. In reversible in and on sentences, the relations among the words are encoded by lexical items, whereas for reversible SVO sentences, the relations among the words are encoded by abstract syntactic items. Thus, these data provide evidence that youth with FXS have difficulty with the processing of syntactically encoded information and this difficulty extends beyond cognitive-level expectations.
Similar to their performance on reversible SVO sentences, young adolescents with FXS had lower mean scores than TD children for forms containing a relative clause in the subject; however, they were not different from young adolescents with DS. Because this construction is typically mastered later in development, it may be the case that neither adolescents with FXS nor adolescents with DS have fully grasped the syntax of sentences containing an embedded clause. Additionally, it’s also possible that adolescents with FXS and adolescents with DS display a “linguistic vulnerability” (Schuele & Nicholls, 2000, p. 581) in terms of acquiring complex syntax.
Taken together, these results suggest that aspects of language comprehension are impaired beyond nonverbal cognitive ability expectations for male and female adolescents with FXS and DS. For adolescents with FXS, syntactic comprehension deficits may be related to specific grammatical constructions, particularly those that have high demands for auditory memory or for syntactic processing that does not depend solely on lexical knowledge (i.e., lexical bootstrapping). Experience with, and knowledge of, particular vocabulary might support syntactic comprehension for adolescents with FXS, as evidenced by comprehension of locatives and four-element sentences that apparently keeps pace with nonverbal cognitive development. In contrast, comprehension that relies less upon recognition of lexical units and more upon syntactic information per se appears to create a challenge for young adolescents with FXS. For adolescents with DS, lexical comprehension might not be well-developed enough to serve as a support for syntactic aspects of comprehension, resulting in a more generalized pattern of deficits in language comprehension.
Patterns of Error Types
The third research question focused on differences in the pattern of lexical and grammatical errors across diagnostic groups. In addressing this question, the frequency of errors committed in the context of reversible in and on sentences, reversible SVO sentences, sentences containing four lexical elements, and sentences with subject relative clauses were examined. Across the four grammatical constructions, young adolescents with FXS and DS made a greater number of grammatical errors than lexical errors relative to TD children. This finding is consistent with previous research on adolescents with DS (Laws & Bishop, 2003) and individuals with intellectual disabilities (Abbeduto et al., 1989), suggesting that poor performance on the TROG-2 was related to difficulty with grammar and not necessarily vocabulary. The current findings suggest that for both young adolescents with FXS and DS, grammatical difficulties may be the main factor driving the lower performance on these constructions.
In addition to the broad categories of errors, specific types of grammatical and lexical errors were examined in the four grammatical constructions of interest. For sentences testing the locatives in and on, each group made more grammatical errors of the word order type than of the function word type; however, young adolescents with DS made a greater number of grammatical errors of the function word type than both the FXS and TD groups. This finding suggests that there may be differences in the profile of syntactic comprehension deficits of young adolescents with FXS and DS. This could be essential information for language interventionists working on receptive grammar with young adolescents with FXS.
For sentences testing reversible SVO and relative clauses, participants made more grammatical errors than lexical errors. Although participants with FXS and DS did not greatly differ from each other in their accuracy for reversible active sentences, they made a greater number of grammatical errors of the word order type than TD children. This finding suggests that poor performance on such constructions may be due to a difficulty comprehending the word order of reversible and embedded syntactic forms.
Clinical Implications
The results of the current study have implications for interventions for young adolescent males and females with FXS. Findings regarding overall syntactic comprehension abilities reveal the importance of improving receptive syntax of these individuals. Their poor processing of sequential patterns and weakness in auditory memory appear to be major sources of comprehension problems and may make certain grammatical constructions more difficult than others. Thus, interventions for young adolescents with FXS may need to target grammatical constructions that are high in syntactic processing demands and constructions that rely less on lexical knowledge. It would also be useful to try and improve general sequential processing skills and auditory memory or to circumvent those areas of weakness when imparting new syntactic knowledge.
Limitations and Future Research
There are several limitations of the current study. First, the current study included a small number of females with FXS. Future studies including a larger sample of females with FXS are needed. Second, the current study did not employ a measure of auditory memory or of sequential processing. Future studies should utilize such measures to determine the extent to which the mere processing load of certain grammatical constructions or general sequential processing deficits account for the receptive language impairments found in these individuals. Third, the classification of foils for the error type analysis was limited to types of errors that could be consistently characterized across TROG-2 items and constructions. Our understanding of the FXS phenotype would benefit from research that addressed other types of comprehension errors.
Conclusions
The current study reveals important findings regarding the receptive syntactic difficulties for young adolescents with FXS and highlights the need for examining language in finer detail than is often done for individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders. The profile of language comprehension identified for adolescents with FXS suggest that research examining the relationships among lexical and syntactic ability in relation to the broader profile of cognitive impairments over the course of development for youth with genetic sources of intellectual disability will be informative.
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by NIH grants R01 HD024356 and P30 HD003352, as well as F31 DC010959 awarded to the second author. The study reported in this paper was completed to fulfill the requirements for the first author’s master’s degree from the University of Wisconsin-Madison. We offer special thanks to Jan Edwards and Susan Ellis Weismer, and to the families who participated. A portion of the results from the current study were presented at the 2011 Symposium on Research in Child Language Disorders.
Footnotes
Missing for one participant with FXS.
References
- Abbeduto L, Brady N, Kover ST. Language development and fragile X syndrome: Profiles, syndrome-specificity, and within-syndrome differences. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2007;13(1):36–46. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abbeduto L, Furman L, Davies B. Relation between the receptive language and mental age of persons with mental retardation. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 1989;95(5):535–543. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abbeduto L, McDuffie A. Genetic syndromes associated with intellectual disabilities. In: Armstrong CL, Morrow L, editors. Handbook of Medical Neuropsychology: Applications of Cognitive Neuroscience. Springer Science + Business Media; New York, NY: 2010. pp. 193–221. [Google Scholar]
- Abbeduto L, Murphy MM, Cawthon SW, Richmond EK, Weissman MD, Karadottir S, O’Brien A. Receptive language skills of adolescents and young adults with Down or fragile X syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2003;108(3):149–160. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2003)108<0149:RLSOAA>2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker S, Hooper S, Skinner M, Hatton D, Schaaf J, Ornstein P, Bailey D. Working memory subsystems and task complexity in young boys with fragile X syndrome. Disability Research. 2011;55(1):19–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01343.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop DVM. Uncommon Understanding: Development and Disorders in Language Comprehension in Children. Psychology Press; Hove: 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Bishop D. Test for Reception of Grammar, Version 2. Pearson Assessment; London, England: 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Burack JA, Shulman C, Katzir E, Schaap T, Brennan JM, Iarocci G, Amir N. Cognitive and behavioural development of Israeli males with fragile X and Down syndrome. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 1999;23(2):519–531. [Google Scholar]
- Carrow-Woolfolk E. Test for Auditory Comprehension of Language, Revised. PRO-ED; Austin, Texas: 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Fragile X syndrome. 2010 Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/Features/FragileX/
- Chapman RS, Hesketh LJ. Language, cognition, and short-term memory in individuals with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome: Research & Practice. 2001;7(1):1–7. doi: 10.3104/reviews.108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman RS, Schwartz SE, Kay-Raining Bird E. Language skills of children and adolescents with Down syndrome: I. Comprehension. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1991;34(5):1106–1120. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3405.1106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ: 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Crawford D, Acuna J, Sherman S. FMR1 and the fragile X syndrome: Human genome epidemiology review. Genetics in Medicine. 2001;3(5):359–371. doi: 10.1097/00125817-200109000-00006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dykens EM, Hodapp RM, Finucane BM. Genetics and mental retardation syndromes: A new look at behavior and interventions. Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co; Baltimore, MD: 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Dykens EM, Hodapp RM, Leckman JF. Strengths and weaknesses in the intellectual functioning of males with fragile X syndrome. Mental Deficiency. 1987;92(2):234–236. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finestack LH, Abbeduto L. Expressive language profiles of verbally expressive adolescents and young adults with Down syndrome or fragile X syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2010;53(5):1334–1348. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0125). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finestack LH, Palmer M, Abbeduto A. Macrostructural narrative language of adolescents and young adults with Down syndrome or fragile X syndrome. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2012;21(1):29–46. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2011/10-0095). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finestack LH, Sterling AM, Abbeduto L. Discriminating Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome based on language ability. Journal of Child Language. 2013;40(1):244–265. doi: 10.1017/S0305000912000207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glenn S, Cunningham C. Performance of young people with Down syndrome on the Leiter-R and British picture vocabulary scales. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2005;49(4):239–244. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2005.00643.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kover ST, McDuffie A, Abbeduto L, Brown WT. Effects of sampling context on spontaneous expressive language in males with fragile X syndrome of Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2012;55(4):1022–1038. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2011/11-0075). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karmiloff-Smith A, Grant J, Berthoud I, Davies M, Howlin P, Udwin O. Language and Williams syndrome: How intact is “intact”? Child Development. 1997;68(2):246–262. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laws G, Bishop D. A comparison of language abilities in adolescents with Down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2003;46(6):1324–1339. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2003/103). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy Y, Gottesman R, Borochowitz Z, Frydman M, Sagi M. Language in boys with fragile X syndrome. Journal of Child Language. 2006;33(1):125–144. doi: 10.1017/s030500090500718x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazzocco MMM, Thompson L, Sudhalter V, Belser R, Lesniak-Karpiak K, Ross J. Language use in females with fragile X or Turner syndrome during brief initial social interactions. Journal of Development and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2006;27(4):319–328. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200608000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDuffie A, Abbeduto L, Lewis P, Kim JS, Kover ST, Weber A, Brown WT. Autism spectrum disorder in children and adolescents with fragile X syndrome: Within-syndrome differences and age-related changes. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. 2010;115(4):307–326. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-115.4.307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDuffie A, Kover ST, Abbeduto L, Lewis P, Brown WT. Profiles of receptive and expressive language abilities in boys with comorbid fragile X syndrome and autism. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. 2012;117(1):18–32. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-117.1.18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller JF. The developmental asynchrony of language development in children with Down syndrome. The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA; Cambridge, MA, US: 1988. pp. 167–198. [Google Scholar]
- Murphy MM, Abbeduto L. Gender differences in repetitive language in fragile X syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2007;51(5):387–400. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00888.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ornstein PA, Schaaf JM, Hooper SR, Hatton DD, Mirrett P, Bailey DB. Memory skills of boys with fragile X syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2008;113(6):453–465. doi: 10.1352/2008.113:453-465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pierpont EL, Richmond EK, Abbeduto L, Kover ST, Brown WT. Contributions of phonological and verbal working memory to language development in adolescents with fragile X syndrome. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. 2011;3(4):335–347. doi: 10.1007/s11689-011-9095-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price J, Roberts J, Hennon E, Berni M, Anderson K, Sideris J. Syntactic complexity during conversation of boys with fragile X syndrome and Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2008;51(1):3–15. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price J, Roberts J, Vandergrift N, Martin G. Language comprehension in boys with fragile X syndrome and boys with Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2007;51(4):318–326. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00881.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roid GH, Miller LJ. Leiter International Performance Scale-Revised. Stoelting; Wood Dale, IL: 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Schuele CM, Nicholls LM. Relative clause: Evidence of continued linguistic vulnerability in children with specific language impairment. Clinician Linguistics and Phonetics. 2000;14(8):563–585. [Google Scholar]
- Sterling A, Abbeduto L. Language development in school-age girls with fragile X syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2012;56(10):974–983. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2012.01578.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Lely H, Harris M. Comprehension of reversible sentences in specifically language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1990;55(1):101–117. doi: 10.1044/jshd.5501.101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volden J, Smith IM, Szatmari P, Bryson S, Fombonne E, Mirenda P, Thompson A. Using the preschool language scale, fourth edition to characterize language in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2011;20(3):200–208. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2011/10-0035). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]