Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Cognit Ther Res. 2013 Jun 1;37(3):483–497. doi: 10.1007/s10608-012-9490-3

Major Depression and Risky Sexual Behavior among Substance Dependent Patients: The Moderating Roles of Distress Tolerance and Gender

Matthew T Tull 1,2, Kim L Gratz 1
PMCID: PMC3953030  NIHMSID: NIHMS415984  PMID: 24634552

Abstract

The goal of the present study was to examine the main and interactive effects of major depressive disorder (MDD), distress tolerance (DT), and gender on multiple indices of risky sexual behavior (RSB) within a sample of 185 substance dependent patients in residential substance abuse treatment. Participants were interviewed to establish current MDD (as well as other Axis I and II diagnoses) and completed a behavioral measure of DT, the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task – Computerized Version. Results provided evidence of a MDD × DT interaction for number of different past year commercial and casual sexual partners with which penetrative sex occurred. Post-hoc analyses demonstrated that participants with current MDD and low DT reported the greatest number of commercial and casual sexual partners. Results highlight one psychological vulnerability (DT) that may improve our understanding of the complex relationship between depression and RSB. Treatment implications of findings are discussed.

Keywords: Emotion Dysregulation, Mood Disorders, Risk-Taking, Sexual Risk-Taking, Substance Use Disorders


Despite significant advances in prevention efforts, more than 1,000,000 individuals in the United States are living with HIV/AIDS, and an estimated 42,959 more continue to contract HIV each year (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [CDC], 2011). Throughout the United States, risky sexual behavior (RSB) is the primary mode of HIV transmission (CDC, 2011). Engagement in both commercial sex (i.e., the exchange of sex for drugs or money) and casual sex (i.e., sex with a partner one does not know well) have been found to be associated with heightened risk for HIV/AIDS and other sexually transmitted diseases (STD; Levin et al., 1995; McKeganey, 1994; Rosenheck, Ngilangwa, Manongi, & Kapiga, 2010; Wood et al., 2007). Indeed, studies have found that unprotected sex is common within both commercial (Bellis, 1990; McGowan et al., 2004; Patterson et al., 2009) and casual (Catania et al., 1995; Levin et al., 1995; Mitchell & Latimer, 2009b) sexual relationships.

The substantial economic, societal, and personal costs associated with HIV/AIDS (e.g., Hellinger, 1998; Holtgrave & Pinkerton, 1997; Hutchinson et al., 2006) have contributed to a rapidly growing body of research focused on identifying the factors that may be associated with heightened risk for engaging in RSB, with the goal of developing targeted intervention and prevention programs. One factor that has received considerable attention in this regard is depression. Individuals suffering from depression exhibit deficits in executive functioning, such as decision-making, concentration, planning, and problem-solving (American Psychiatric Association, 2000; Rogers et al., 2004). Moreover, research has found that individuals with a diagnosis of major depressive disorder (MDD) have an altered sensitivity to reward and punishment, exhibiting a tendency to engage in behaviors associated with high immediate reward despite risk for greater future punishment (Must et al., 2006). Consequently, individuals who are depressed may be more likely to engage in behaviors that provide the opportunity for immediate reinforcement despite the potential for longer-term negative consequences, such as RSB. Consistent with this, studies have demonstrated that depression is associated with greater levels of impulsivity and maladaptive behaviors in general (Gonzalez, Reynolds, & Skewes, 2011; Hanson, Luciana, & Sullwold, 2008). Likewise, the high levels of negative affect, hopelessness, and helplessness associated with depression (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) may motivate RSB in an effort to avoid or distract from unpleasant internal states. In support, Alvy et al. (2011) found that avoidant coping mediated the relationship between depression and RSB.

Importantly, however, despite growing evidence for an association between depression and multiple forms of RSB (including unprotected sex, commercial sex, casual sex, number of sexual partners, and sex while under the influence of alcohol or drugs; Alvy et al., 2011; Hutton, Lyketsos, Zenilman, Thompson, & Erbelding, 2004; Klein, Elifson, & Sterk, 2008; Perdue, Hagan, Thiede, & Valleroy, 2003; Reisner et al., 2009; Risser, Timpson, McCurdy, Ross, & Williams, 2006; Seth, Raiji, DiClemente, Wingood, & Rose, 2009), other studies have failed to find a significant association between depression and RSB (e.g., Koblin et al., 2006; McCusker, Goldstein, Bigelow, & Zorn, 1995; Stall et al., 2003). Thus, findings suggest that the relation between depression and RSB may be complex and accounted for by other psychological vulnerabilities (see Alvy et al., 2011).

One particular psychological vulnerability that warrants investigation is distress tolerance (DT), defined as the willingness to withstand or persist in the face of aversive psychological states (Simons & Gaher, 2005). Low DT has recently received increasing attention as a psychological vulnerability for diverse forms of psychopathology and maladaptive behaviors (Leyro, Zvolensky, & Bernstein, 2010; Zvolensky, Bernstein, & Vujanovic, 2011). Depression in particular is characterized by a variety of deficits in emotional responding (e.g., appraisal of negative emotions as threatening, low self-efficacy for coping with negative emotional situations, and the unwillingness to experience negative emotions) characteristic of low DT (for a review, see Clen, Mennin, & Fresco, 2011). Although empirical research on the association between depression and DT is limited, a small body of research has found evidence of a negative association between depression and DT (Cougle, Timpano, & Goetz, 2012; Dennhardt & Murphy, 2011; Iverson, Follette, Pistorello, & Fruzzetti, in press).

As for the relevance of DT to RSB, previous research has demonstrated that negative affective states may contribute to risky decision making (Leith & Baumeister, 1996) and engagement in RSB in particular (Crepaz & Marks, 2001; Lucenko, Malow, Sanchez-Martinez, Jennings, & Dévieux, 2003). Consequently, it has been suggested that RSB may function to avoid or escape negative emotional states (Crepaz & Marks, 2001). In support of this notion, avoidant coping has been found to be associated with RSB (Alvy et al., 2011; Folkman, Chesney, Pollack, & Phillips, 1992; Semple, Patterson, & Grant, 2000). Furthermore, emotion regulation difficulties (defined as maladaptive ways of responding to emotional distress, including the unwillingness to experience emotional distress) have been found to be significantly associated with RSB above and beyond other RSB-relevant risk factors (Tull, Weiss, Adams, & Gratz, 2012). It is possible that individuals with depression and low DT may experience considerable difficulty withstanding the negative affect associated with a depressed mood, thus increasing motivation to engage in behaviors that function to escape negative affective states, such as RSB. In support of this notion, research has demonstrated that low DT is associated with a variety of other risky behaviors that have been proposed to serve an emotionally avoidant function (see Chapman, Gratz, & Brown, 2006; Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette, & Strosahl, 1996), such as substance use (e.g., Buckner, Keough, & Schmidt, 2007; Gorka, Ali, & Daughters, in press; Howell, Leyro, Hogan, Buckner, & Zvolensky, 2010); deliberate self-harm (Gratz et al., 2011), and bulimic behaviors (Anestis, Lavender, et al., in press; Anestis, Selby, Fink, & Joiner, 2007).

Another factor worth considering in the relation between MDD and RSB is gender. Although studies have consistently demonstrated higher rates of MDD among women compared to men (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2001; Piccinelli & Wilkinson, 2000), few consistent gender differences in the presentation of MDD have been found (Carter, Joyce, Mulder, Luty, & McKenzie, 2000). However, there is support for gender differences in RSB. First, there is evidence that substance dependent women are more likely than substance dependent men to engage in commercial sex in order to obtain money or drugs (Tyndall et al., 2002). Consistent with this, Mitchell and Latimer (2009a) found that female (vs. male) injection drug users exhibited greater engagement in commercial sex, whereas male (vs. female) injection drug users engaged in more casual sex. In addition, Evans et al. (2003) demonstrated that female injection drug users were more likely than male injection drug users to report having sexual partners who were also injection drug users (Evans et al., 2003).

Consequently, the goal of the present study was to examine the effects of MDD, DT, and gender on multiple indices of RSB (i.e., frequency of past year penetrative sex with a commercial or casual partner and likelihood of using a condom when having penetrative sex with a commercial or casual partner). To ensure that our findings were specific to RSB versus sexual behavior in general, we also explored the effects of MDD, DT, and gender on frequency of past year penetrative sex with a regular (i.e., committed) partner and the likelihood of using a condom when having penetrative sex with a regular partner. These associations were examined within a particularly high-risk sample of patients with substance use disorders (SUD) in residential substance abuse treatment. SUD patients have been shown to be at high risk for both RSB (Bornovalova, Daughters, & Lejuez, 2010; Reynolds et al., 2010; Subramaniam, Stitzer, Woody, Fishman, & Kolodner, 2009) and MDD (Grant et al., 2004). Furthermore, the co-occurrence of MDD with SUD has been associated with RSB (Carey et al., 2004) and other HIV-risk behaviors (injection risk behaviors; Stein, Solomon, Herman, Anderson, & Miller, 2003). SUD patients also exhibit heightened difficulties in emotion regulation (Fox, Hong, & Sinha, 2008; McDermott, Tull, Gratz, Daughters, & Lejuez, 2009), including low DT (Richards, Daughters, Bornovalova, Brown, & Lejuez, 2011). Although the effect of low DT on RSB among SUD patients has yet to be explored, low DT has been found to be associated with a number of negative clinical outcomes in SUD patients, including duration of most recent abstinence attempt (Daughters, Lejuez, Kahler, Strong, & Brown, 2005) and substance abuse treatment dropout (Daughters, Lejuez, Bornovalova et al., 2005; Tull, Gratz, Coffey, & McDermott, in press). Low DT has also been found to be associated with antisocial personality disorder among SUD patients (Daughters, Sargeant, Bornovalova, Gratz, & Lejuez, 2008). Consistent with previous research, we expected to find main effects of MDD, DT, and gender on RSB. Further, we hypothesized that female participants with MDD and low DT would exhibit the highest rates of RSB compared to all other participants.

Method

Participants

Participants were 185 substance dependent patients (60% male) consecutively admitted to a residential substance abuse treatment facility in the Jackson, Mississippi area. Participants ranged in age from 18 to 61, with an average age of 36 (SD = 10.10). With regard to racial/ethnic background, 59.5% were White, 31.9% Black/African-American, 4.3% Native American, 2.2% bi-racial, 1.6% Latino, and 0.5% Asian/Asian-American. The majority of participants (60.5%) reported obtaining a high school education or less, being single (71.4%), and unemployed (70.3%). In addition, approximately half of participants (46.5%) reporting a past year income of less than $10,000. Clinical data for participants is presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Clinical descriptive data (N = 185).

Men (n=111) Women (n=74)
MDD (n=18)
No MDD (n=93)
MDD (n=20)
No MDD (n=54)
Low DT (n=12) High DT (n=6) Low DT (n=64) High DT (n=29) Low DT (n=14) High DT (n=6) Low DT (n=41) High DT (n=13)
Panic disorder 25.0% 16.7% 14.1% 13.8% 50.0% 50% 17.1% 38.5%
Social anxiety disorder 16.7% 16.7% 3.1% 13.8% 21.4% 0% 12.2% 23.1%
Specific phobia 16.7% 16.7% 1.6% 17.2% 28.6% 50% 4.9% 7.7%
OCD 0% 0% 0% 3.4% 7.1% 33.3% 2.4% 7.7%
GAD 66.7% 66.7% 17.2% 20.7% 78.6% 83.3% 29.3% 38.5%
PTSD 33.3% 0% 10.9% 20.7% 64.3% 83.3% 39.0% 30.8%
Alcohol dependence 41.7% 50.0% 32.8% 48.3% 35.7% 50.0% 39.0% 15.4%
Cocaine dependence 50.0% 66.7% 28.1% 24.1% 21.4% 0% 41.5% 38.5%
Opioid dependence 8.3% 33.3% 15.6% 0% 14.3% 16.7% 14.6% 23.1%
Marijuana dependence 33.3% 50.0% 20.3% 24.1% 14.3% 0% 9.8% 7.7%
Sedative dependence 16.7% 0% 7.8% 6.9% 14.3% 0% 9.8% 15.4%
Stimulant dependence 8.3% 0% 7.8% 3.4% 28.6% 33.3% 17.1% 7.7%
Hallucinogen dependence 0% 0% 1.6% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0%
BPD 41.7% 50.0% 10.9% 5.0% 92.9% 50.0% 46.3% 46.2%
Number of regular sex partners 2.33 (4.21) 1.50 (1.05) 2.48 (4.86) 1.10 (1.82) 2.50 (4.24) 1.17 (0.98) 1.24 (1.14) 1.92 (1.55)
Condom use with regular partners 4.67 (1.72) 3.33 (2.25) 3.98 (1.77) 4.07 (1.87) 4.00 (1.84) 4.83 (0.98) 4.10 (1.79) 3.92 (1.71)
Number of commercial sex partners 6.08 (13.93) 0.67 (0.82) 0.59 (2.18) 0.21 (0.49) 3.71 (6.97) 1.00 (2.45) 0.37 (0.83) 1.38 (3.60)
Condom use with commercial partners 1.83a (1.84) 2.60b (2.51) 0.94d (1.77) 0.86f (0.69) 2.00h (2.07) 5.00c (0.00) 1.33j (1.82) 1.80b (2.49)
Number of casual sex partners 7.75 (14.02) 0.33 (0.82) 1.78 (2.99) 0.93 (1.60) 4.00 (6.85) 0.83 (0.75) 0.66 (1.20) 2.38 (3.57)
Condom use with casual partners 3.67a (1.21) 4.00c (0.00) 3.26e (1.68) 2.67g (1.72) 3.43f (1.99) 4.00i (2.00) 2.75d (1.88) 2.57f (1.81)

Note. Non-transformed means are presented for the risky sexual behavior variables. Standard deviations are presented in parentheses for risky sexual behavior variables. OCD = obsessive compulsive disorder; GAD = generalized anxiety disorder; PTSD = Posttraumatic stress disorder; BPD = borderline personality disorder;

a

n = 6;

b

n = 5;

c

n = 1;

d

n = 16;

e

n = 27;

f

n = 7;

g

n= 12;

h

n = 8;

i

n = 4.

j

n = 18.

Measures

Assessment of demographic information

All participants completed a general demographics form assessing age, racial/ethnic background, marital status, education, and income in the past year.

Diagnostic assessment measures

In order to assess for current (i.e., past month) MDD and other Axis I disorders (with the exception of posttraumatic stress disorder [PTSD]), participants were interviewed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I disorders (SCID-IV; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1996). PTSD was assessed with the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS; Blake et al., 1990). Finally, participants were also interviewed using the borderline personality disorder (BPD) module of the Diagnostic Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders (DIPD-IV; Zanarini, Frankenburg, Sickel, & Young, 1996) to assess for the presence of current (i.e., past two years) BPD (Zanarini et al., 2000). Interviews were conducted by bachelors- or masters-level clinical assessors trained to reliability with the principal investigator (first author) and co-investigator (second author). All interviews were reviewed by the principal investigator, with diagnoses confirmed in consensus meetings.

Assessment of DT

The Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task - Computerized Version (PASAT-C; Lejuez, Kahler, & Brown, 2003) was used to assess DT. During this task, numbers are sequentially flashed on a computer screen, and participants are instructed to sum the most recent number with the previous number (using the computer mouse to click on the correct answer). After providing each sum, the participant must ignore the sum and add the following number to the most recently presented number. When a correct answer is provided, a point is obtained. If an incorrect answer is provided, or if the participant fails to provide an answer before the next number is presented, an “explosion” sound is played and the score does not change.

The version of the PASAT-C used in this study consisted of 3 levels with increasingly shorter latencies between number presentations. Because the correct answer must be provided before the presentation of the next number to obtain a point, difficulty increases as latencies decrease. Level 1 (low difficulty) had a 3-second latency between number presentations, Level 2 (medium difficulty) had a 2-second latency, and Level 3 (high difficulty) had a 1-second latency. The first level lasted 3 minutes, the second level lasted 5 minutes, and the third level lasted 7 minutes and included an option to terminate the task at any time. However, participants were under the impression that their performance on this task (including the length of time they persisted on it) would determine the amount of money they would receive as reimbursement for their participation (providing an incentive to perform well on the task, although all participants were reimbursed the full amount regardless of their performance).

To determine whether DT is best represented as a continuous or dichotomous variable, we examined whether latency in seconds to task termination was normally distributed. According to the Shapiro-Wilk test (Shapiro & Wilk, 1965), latency to terminate the PASAT-C was not normally distributed (p < .001), supporting dichotomization of this variable (see MacCallum, Zhang, Preacher, & Rucker, 2002). Providing further support for the use of a dichotomous DT variable, the vast majority (86%) of participants who terminated the PASAT-C did so within the first 2 minutes of the final level, and the mean latency to terminate the PASAT-C among participants who quit the PASAT-C was 60.13 sec (SD = 91.53). Consequently, and consistent with previous studies (e.g., Anestis, Gratz, et al., in press; Gorka et al., in press; Gratz, Rosenthal, Tull, Lejuez, & Gunderson, 2006; Schloss & Haaga, 2011), DT was indexed by quit status, with individuals who persisted until the end of the task categorized as having high DT and those who quit the task categorized as having low DT.

As a manipulation check to ensure that the task induced emotional distress, participants rated their level of anxiety, frustration, and irritation both before the task (pre-task) and immediately prior to receiving the option to terminate the task (post-task). Each item was rated from 0 to 100, with the average rating across all forms of distress used to examine changes in distress in response to the task (see Manipulation Check below).

The PASAT-C has been used extensively with patients in residential substance abuse treatment (e.g., Anestis, Tull, Bagge, & Gratz, 2012; Bornovalova et al., 2008; Daughters et al., 2008; Daughters, Lejuez, Bornovalova et al., 2005; Gratz, Bornovalova, Delany-Brumsey, Nick, & Lejuez, 2007). In support of the construct validity of the PASAT-C, this task has been shown to induce emotional distress in the form of anxiety, anger, frustration, and irritability among clinical and non-clinical samples (e.g., Bornovalova et al., 2008; Gratz et al., 2011; Lejuez et al., 2003), and to be significantly correlated with self-report measures of emotion dysregulation and the unwillingness to experience emotional distress (Gratz et al., 2006). Providing evidence for its convergent and predictive validity, substance users with (vs. without) both BPD (Bornovalova et al., 2008) and antisocial personality disorder (Daughters et al., 2008) have been found to exhibit low DT. Furthermore, low DT has been found to predict early treatment dropout among substance users in residential substance abuse treatment (Daughters, Lejuez, Bornovalova et al., 2005; Tull et al., in press) and to interact with depressive symptoms in predicting problematic alcohol use (Gorka et al., in press). Performance on the PASAT-C has also been found to be stable across time among SUD patients in residential substance abuse treatment (Bornovalova, Gratz, Daughters, Hunt, & Lejuez, 2012). Finally, providing evidence that persistence on this task is not simply a measure of skill level or emotional distress in response to the task, neither emotional distress in response to the task nor task performance has been found to be significantly associated with task termination (Bornovalova et al., 2008; Daughters et al., 2008; Gratz et al. , 2007).

Assessment of RSB

Participants completed a modified version of the HIV Risk-taking Behavior Scale (HRBS; Darke, Hall, Heather, Ward, & Wodak, 1991) to assess RSB. The HRBS has been found to have good construct and convergent validity, with high rates of agreement between respondents and their sexual partners on sexual behavior items (average percentage of 96%), as well as strong test-retest reliability (r = .86) over a period of 1 week (Darke et al., 1991).

Six items pertaining to penetrative sex with regular, commercial, or casual partners were of interest in this study. Three items asked participants to indicate the number of different regular, commercial, and casual sexual partners with whom they had engaged in penetrative sex in the past year (with individuals who did not have penetrative sex with a regular, commercial, or casual partner in the past year asked to enter a zero). This is a modification of the original HRBS (which utilizes a Likert-type scale to assess number of sexual partners). Participants were then presented with three items asking how often a condom was used during penetrative sex with regular, commercial, or casual partners. Participants were asked to respond to these three items using a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = Never; 2 = Rarely; 3 = Sometimes; 4 = Often; 5 = Everytime), with a “does not apply” response option available to participants who reported never engaging in penetrative sex with a regular, commercial, or casual partner in the past year. Participants who indicated that this question did not apply to them were excluded from analyses involving these items. Consistent with past research (Lejuez, Bornovalova, Daughters, & Curtin, 2005), and to obtain a better representation of sexual behavior patterns, we lengthened the timeframe of the items to the past year (vs. the past 6 months specified in the original HRBS). Although these modifications create a non-standardized version of the HRBS, they allowed us to obtain a more accurate representation of RSB patterns.

Procedure

All procedures were reviewed and approved by the University of Mississippi Medical Center's Institutional Review Board. To be eligible for inclusion in the study, participants were required to have: 1) obtained a Mini-Mental Status Exam (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975) score of ≥ 24; and 2) exhibited no current psychotic disorders (as determined by the SCID-IV). Eligible participants were recruited for this study no sooner than 72 hours after entry in the facility (to limit the possible interference of withdrawal symptoms on study engagement). Those who met inclusion criteria were provided with information about study procedures and associated risks, following which written informed consent was obtained. All participants took part in this study within their first two weeks of treatment. To reduce participant burden, the study protocol was conducted across two different sessions and participants were paid $15 per session.

Results

Preliminary Analyses

Descriptive data for all participants across MDD, DT, and gender categories are presented in Table 1. Based on the SCID-IV, 20.5% (n = 38) of participants had a current diagnosis of MDD, and 70.8% (n = 131) were classified as having low DT on the PASAT-C. With regard to RSB and other sexual behaviors, 19.5% (n = 36) of participants reported having penetrative sex with a commercial partner in the past year, 47.6% (n = 88) reported having penetrative sex with a casual partner in the past year, and 73.5% (n = 136) reported having penetrative sex with a regular partner in the past year. Among those who engaged in past-year penetrative sex with a commercial, casual, or regular partner, the average number of past-year penetrative sexual partners was 5.89 (SD = 8.85) for commercial partners, 4.03 (SD = 6.31) for casual partners, and 1.87 (SD = 3.4) for regular partners. Correlations between the primary variables of interest (i.e., RSB and other sexual behaviors, DT, MDD, and gender), as well as those examined as potential covariates, are presented in Table 2.

Table 2.

Correlations between primary variables of interest and potential covariates.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14
1. Regular sex partners --- −.22** 27** .28* .41** −.07 −.23** −.02 −.10 −.11 −.02 −.01 .00 .02
2. Condom use – regular --- −.06 −.16 −.22** .66** .18* −.02 −.20** .04 .07 .01 .01 .04
3. Commercial sex partners --- .73** .62** .01 .15* −.06 −.05 .16* .02 −.12 −.08 .27**
4. Condom use – commerciala --- .61** −.61** .20 −.08 .08 .03 .19 −.09 −.16 .26*
5. Casual sex partners --- −.07 −.09 .06 −.18* −.01 .03 −.04 .02 .14
6. Condom use – casualb --- .08 .08 .11 .02 .17 .08 .06 .18
7. Age --- .07 .07 .24** .14 −.11 .03 −.01
8. Gender (0=female) --- −.17* .27** −.18* .10 .22** −.13
9. Marital status (0=single) --- −.11 .03 .18* −.05 .09
10. Race/ethnicity (0=White) --- −.17* −.22** −.03 −.01
11. Education (0=≤high school) --- .18** .15* .06
12. Income (0=<$ 10,000) --- .51** .02
13. Employed (0=unemployed) --- −.16*
14. Major depression ---
15. PASAT-Quit
16. Alcohol dependence
17. Sedative dependence
18. Marijuana dependence
19. Stimulant dependence
20. Opioid dependence
21. Cocaine dependence
22. Hallucinogen dependence
23. Panic disorder
24. Social anxiety disorder
25. Specific phobia
26. OCD
27. GAD
28. PTSD
29. BPD

Mean/% 1.92 4.09 1.15 1.50 1.92 3.10 36.00 60% 28.6% 40.5% 39.5% 53.5% 29.7% 20.5%
SD 3.47 1.76 4.51 1.89 4.78 1.73 10.10 --- --- --- --- --- --- ---
15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29
1. Regular sex partners .05 −.07 −.04 .15* .04 .17* .00 .10 .12 −.02 .02 .05 −.03 −.06 .04
2. Condom use – regulara .01 −.09 .00 .00 .19** −.02 −.07 .04 .06 .06 .06 .08 .16* .12 −.03
3. Commercial sex partners .05 −.04 −.10 .02 −.07 .00 .26** .09 .06 .11 .16* .16* .04 .14 .24**
4. Condom use – commerciala −.11 .04 −.17 .13 −.01 −.05 .25* .01 .12 .18 .31* .17 .09 .20 .29*
5. Casual sex partners .07 .06 −.01 .16* −.03 .05 .18* .07 .15* .01 .10 .13 .04 −.01 .08
6. Condom use – casualb .07 −.03 .05 .06 .26* −.11 −.05 .12 .02 −.08 .10 .05 .26* .01 .00
7. Age .24** .12 −.10 −.19* −.10 −.11 .17* .06 −.06 −.01 .02 −.08 −.04 .03 −.03
8. Gender (0=female) −.06 .04 −.05 .19* −.20** −.07 −.02 .06 −.17* −.11 −.09 −.16* −.19** −.34** −.39**
9. Marital status (0=single) .07 −.04 −.04 −.02 .15* .06 −.11 −.05 .05 .05 .02 .02 .03 .09 .12
10. Race/ethnicity (0=White) −.08 .07 −.23** .04 −.30** −.29** .25** −.06 −.29** −.04 .08 −.15* −.14 −.04 −.18*
11. Education (0=≤high school) −.07 .13 .05 −.21** .03 .07 −.04 .09 .02 −.07 −.02 −.02 .20** .12 .09
12. Income (0=<$ 10,000) −.19** .00 .18* .08 −.04 .12 −.10 .07 .03 −.03 −.04 −.01 .09 −.06 −.12
13. Employed (0=unemployed) −.03 .11 .04 .06 −.16* .05 −.07 .11 −.02 −.04 −.03 −.05 −.04 −.14 −.21**
14. Major depression −.03 .05 .02 .07 .11 .03 .02 −.04 .20** .08 .27** .13 .43** .23** .33**
15. PASAT-Quit --- −.05 .04 −.03 .08 .05 .04 .05 −.05 −.08 −.17* −.15* −.05 .00 .02
16. Alcohol dependence --- .06 −.02 −.03 −.01 .06 .10 .01 .02 .00 −.08 .12 .02 .08
17. Sedative dependence --- .14 .06 .20** .06 −.02 .20** .01 −.05 .05 .13 −.03 .14
18. Marijuana dependence --- .18* .02 .12 .16* .06 .10 .12 −.09 .14 .08 .11
19. Stimulant dependence --- .06 −.10 .21** .32** −.02 .05 .13 .18* .24** .26**
20. Opioid dependence --- −.04 −.03 .26** .02 −.08 −.07 .09 .08 .19**
21. Cocaine dependence --- .11 −.08 .02 .03 −.06 .02 −.12 .15*
22. Hallucinogen dependence --- −.04 −.03 −.03 −.01 −.05 −.05 −.05
23. Panic disorder --- .25** .26** .28** .28** .22** .26**
24. Social anxiety disorder --- .34** .23** .20** .29** .24**
25. Specific phobia --- .34** .25** .23** .07
26. OCD --- .19** .23** .13
27. GAD --- .33** .31**
28. PTSD --- .34**
29. BPD ---

Mean/% 70.8% 37.3% 9.2% 18.4% 11.4% 13.5% 32.4% 0.5% 21.1% 10.8% 10.3% 3.2% 33.5% 27.6% 33.0%
SD --- --- --- --- --- --- --- --- --- --- --- --- --- --- ---

Note. Transformed variables are used for the risky sexual behavior variables. Percentages reflect a score of 1 for dichotomous variables or the presence of a diagnosis. OCD = obsessive compulsive disorder; GAD = generalized anxiety disorder; PTSD = posttraumatic stress disorder; BPD = borderline personality disorder;

a

n = 66;

b

n = 80.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

RSB and other sexual behavior variables evidenced skew and/or kurtosis; thus, a logarithmic transformation was performed on the RSB variables, following which they approximated normal distributions. For ease of presentation and interpretation, the original means and standard deviations of these variables prior to transformation are presented in Table 2. However, all analyses utilized the transformed scores.

Manipulation check

Providing support for the use of quit status on the PASAT-C as a measure of the willingness to tolerate distress (DT), results of a 2 (current MDD vs. no current MDD) × 2 (female vs. male) × 2 (pre- vs. post-task) repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) for emotional distress revealed a significant main effect of time, F (1, 181) = 91.06, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.34, with all participants reporting an increase in distress from pre- (Mean = 68.88, SD = 71.50) to post-task (Mean = 146.96, SD = 93.37). Further, the MDD × gender × time interaction was not significant, F (1, 181) = 0.02, p > 0.05, ηp2 = 0.00, indicating that the task resulted in a comparable increase in distress across MDD status and gender.

Identification of covariates

Prior to conducting primary analyses, in order to identify potential covariates (see Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007), a series of Pearson product-moment and point-biserial correlation analyses was conducted to explore associations between the different indices of RSB and demographic factors (i.e., age, racial/ethnic background, income, education level, marital status, and employment) and diagnostic variables (anxiety disorders, specific substance use disorders, and BPD). Given the small number of participants in many of the demographic categories, these variables were collapsed into dichotomous variables of: (1) White (59.5%) versus non-White (40.5%); (2) past year income of < $10,000 (46.5%) versus ≥ $10,000 (53.5%); (3) a high school education or less (60.5%) versus some post-high school education (39.5%); (4) single (71.4%) versus married (28.6%); and (5) unemployed (70.3%) versus employed (29.7%). Variables found to be significantly associated with each dependent variable (see Table 2) were included in analyses involving that dependent variable.

Primary Analyses

To test hypotheses, we conducted a series of 2 (current MDD vs. no current MDD) × 2 (high-DT vs. low-DT) × 2 (female vs. male) analysis of covariance (ANCOVA; controlling for covariates relevant to each outcome), with number of penetrative regular sexual partners, likelihood of using a condom when having penetrative sex with a regular partner, number of penetrative commercial sexual partners, likelihood of using a condom when having penetrative sex with a commercial partner, number of penetrative casual sexual partners, and likelihood of using a condom when having penetrative sex with a casual partner (all transformed) serving as the dependent variables.

With regard to the number of commercial sexual partners in the past year, a significant MDD × DT interaction was found, F (1, 171) = 4.07, p < .05, ηp2 = .02 (controlling for age, racial/ethnic background, cocaine dependence, specific phobia, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and BPD; see Table 3). Post-hoc planned comparisons revealed that participants with MDD and low DT reported the greatest number of commercial sexual partners in the past year compared to all other groups, ts (df = 36–129) ≥ 1.99, ps < .05. However, no significant main effects or interactions were found for the likelihood of using a condom when having penetrative sex with a commercial partner (controlling for cocaine dependence, specific phobia, and BPD), Fs (1, 55) < 2.50, ps > .05, ηp2s ≤ .04.

Table 3.

ANCOVA results examining the interactive effect of major depressive disorder, distress tolerance, and gender on number of past-year penetrative commercial sexual partners.

F (1,171) P η p 2
Age 1.73 .19 .01
Race/ethnicity 3.80 .05 .02
Cocaine dependence 5.59 .02 .03
Specific phobia 0.24 .62 .00
Obsessive compulsive disorder 4.68 .03 .03
Borderline personality disorder 3.70 .06 .02
Gender 0.01 .93 .00
Distress tolerance 2.46 .12 .01
Major depression 2.86 .09 .02
Gender × Distress tolerance 0.74 .39 .00
Gender × Major depression 0.42 .52 .00
Major depression × Distress tolerance 4.07 .045 .02
Major depression × Distress tolerance × Gender 0.03 .86 .00

As for the number of different casual sexual partners in the past year, significant MDD × DT, F (1, 173) = 6.02, p < .05, ηp2 = .03, and gender × DT, F (1, 173) = 6.69, p < .05, ηp2 = .04, interactions were found (controlling for marijuana dependence, cocaine dependence, panic disorder, and marital status; see Table 4). Post-hoc planned comparisons revealed that participants with MDD and low DT reported the greatest number of casual sexual partners in the past year compared to all other groups, ts (df = 36–129) ≥ 2.01, ps < .05. For the gender × DT interaction, post-hoc planned comparisons demonstrated that men with high DT reported significantly fewer past-year casual sexual partners than all other groups, ts (df = 52–109) ≥ 1.95, ps < .05. Similar to the findings regarding commercial sexual partners, however, no significant main effects or interactions were found for the likelihood of using a condom when having penetrative sex with a casual sexual partner (controlling for stimulant dependence and generalized anxiety disorder), Fs (1, 70) < 1.80, ps > .05, ηp2s < .03.

Table 4.

ANCOVA results examining the interactive effect of major depressive disorder, distress tolerance, and gender on number of past-year penetrative casual sexual partners.

F (1,173) p η p 2
Marital status 5.89 .02 .03
Panic disorder 2.72 .10 .02
Marijuana dependence 3.37 .07 .02
Cocaine dependence 5.71 .02 .03
Gender 0.78 .38 .01
Distress tolerance 3.87 .05 .02
Major depression 0.53 .47 .00
Gender × Distress tolerance 6.69 .01 .04
Gender × Major depression 0.66 .42 .00
Major depression × Distress tolerance 6.02 .02 .03
Major depression × Distress tolerance × Gender 0.38 .54 .00

Finally, with regard to the number of regular sexual partners in the past year, as well as the likelihood of using a condom when having penetrative sex with a regular partner, no significant main effects or interactions were found, Fs (1,173–174) < 2.28, ps > .05, ηp2 < .015.

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to examine the main and interactive effects of current MDD, DT, and gender on multiple indices of RSB (i.e., frequency of past year penetrative sex with a commercial or casual partner and likelihood of using a condom when having penetrative sex with a commercial or casual partner) within a sample of SUD patients in residential substance abuse treatment. Hypotheses were partially supported. Contrary to past research (e.g., Alvy et al., 2011; Evans et al., 2003; Hutton et al., 2004; Klein et al., 2008; Mitchell & Latimer, 2009a; Perdue et al., 2003; Reisner et al., 2009; Risser et al., 2006; Seth et al., 2009; Tyndall et al., 2002), results did not provide support for the hypothesized main effects of MDD or gender on any RSB outcome. Likewise, and contrary to past research demonstrating significant associations between low DT and a myriad of risky behaviors (Anestis et al., 2007; Buckner et al., 2007; Gratz et al., 2011; Gorka et al., in press; Howell et al., 2010), findings did not provide evidence of a main effect of DT on RSB. Nonetheless, in partial support of hypotheses, findings of a significant gender × DT interaction revealed that men with high DT (compared to all other groups) reported the fewest number of casual sexual partners in the past year. Given evidence that substance dependent men may be at high risk for engaging in casual sex (Mitchell & Latimer, 2009a), this finding suggests that high levels of DT may serve as a protective factor for some forms of RSB among men. Future research in this area may help inform the development of interventions focused on increasing DT among substance dependent men in order to reduce the likelihood of casual sex and associated risks.

Consistent with the proposal that the association between depression and RSB may be complex, findings indicated that DT moderated the effect of MDD on two indices of RSB. Specifically, participants with current MDD and low DT reported engaging in penetrative sex with significantly greater numbers of casual and commercial sexual partners than all other groups of participants. These findings are in-line with theoretical (Crepaz & Marks, 2001) and empirical (Lucenko et al., 2003) literature suggesting that RSB may be motivated by negative affective states, as well as emerging research demonstrating that difficulties in emotion regulation may underlie RSB (e.g., Messman-Moore, Walsh, & DiLillo, 2010; Tull et al., 2012). Results of the present study suggest that the presence of MDD among SUD patients may increase the risk for RSB among patients with low DT. SUD patients with MDD who experience difficulties tolerating emotional distress may be more likely to engage in behaviors that function to alleviate or distract from negative affective states. Findings of significant MDD × DT interactions for two forms of RSB (combined with the absence of significant main effects of MDD or DT on any form of RSB) highlight the importance of exploring the different contexts in which patients with MDD or low DT may be at heightened risk for negative clinical outcomes. Such findings are also consistent with other research examining the roles of depression and DT in maladaptive behaviors. For example, although Gorka et al. (in press) found that DT was not significantly associated with problematic alcohol use at a zero-order level, the combination of low DT and more severe depressive symptoms was predictive of problematic alcohol use.

Although results provided support for the combined role of MDD and DT in the number of past-year commercial and casual sexual partners, we did not find evidence for the role of MDD and/or DT in the likelihood of using a condom when engaging in penetrative sex with a commercial or casual partner. Whereas low DT among SUD patients with MDD may motivate engagement in penetrative sex with different commercial and sexual partners, other psychological vulnerabilities may be more relevant to the decision to use a condom when having penetrative sex with these partners. In particular, given that the unwillingness to experience emotional distress is considered to be one facet of the larger construct of emotion dysregulation (Gratz et al., 2006), it is possible that other dimensions of emotion dysregulation may influence the use of protection when having sex with a commercial or casual partner. For example, Tull et al. (2012) found that the specific emotion dysregulation dimension of lack of emotional clarity predicted the likelihood of using a condom when having penetrative sex with a commercial partner while under the influence of substances. Factors other than those associated with emotion dysregulation may also be driving unsafe sexual practices. For example, Alvy et al. (2011) demonstrated that self-efficacy for sexual safety and the use of sex and drugs to escape cognitions mediated the association between depression and sexual risk-taking (assessed as a composite variable that included unprotected sex) among men who have sex with men (MSM). In addition, Safren et al. (2010) found that negative expectancies about condom use were associated with unprotected receptive or insertive anal intercourse among MSM.

Results must be considered in light of the limitations present. First and foremost, the outcome being examined in this study, past year RSB, was assessed through self-report. Although few alternatives are available when it comes to assessing RSB, there are limitations associated with this approach that warrant mention. First, the veracity of self-reported RSB may be limited by the perceived negative consequences of reporting this behavior. In addition, despite multiple studies examining self-reported RSB in the past year among substance dependent individuals (Carey, Carey, Maisto, Gordon, & Vanable, 2001; Lejuez et al., 2005; for a review, see also Meade & Sikkema, 2005), the length of this time period (relative to the original HRBS) may have negatively influenced recall of RSB. Given that we are utilizing a substance dependent sample, some participants may also have been engaging in RSB in the context of substance use, further limiting their ability to provide an accurate report of their frequency of RSB in the past year. We also did not collect data on whether participants were involved in the criminal justice system or another residential treatment program in the past year, which would have limited opportunities for RSB. All of these factors may have resulted in the lower rates of RSB within this sample, compared to other studies of clinical populations that used a briefer assessment period (i.e., past 3 months; Carey et al., 2004; Meade, 2006). Nonetheless, it warrants mention that the percentage of participants in this study reporting penetrative sex with a commercial partner was higher (19.5%) than that reported by Meade and Sikkema (2005) in a review of studies examining past-year commercial sex among adults with severe mental illness, including substance dependence (weighted mean = 12.73%). Moreover, there is evidence that self-report measures may result in more valid reports of HIV-risk behaviors in general (e.g., drug use; Weatherby et al., 1994) and RSB in particular (Johnson et al., 2000; for a review, see Fenton, Johnson, McManus, & Erens, 2001), compared to other assessment methods (e.g., interviews). Nonetheless, future studies would benefit from the inclusion of other assessment methods of RSB that may result in more valid data, such as timeline follow-back procedures (Weinhardt, Carey, & Carey, 2000). Prospective or longitudinal studies that examine the effects of MDD and DT on future engagement in RSB would also help to address this limitation.

Furthermore, although the use of a laboratory-based DT task is a strength of this study (as it assesses DT in the context of actual distress and is not susceptible to some of the limitations of self-report measures of DT), future research would benefit from the multimodal assessment of DT. Indeed, a growing body of research suggests that self-report and behavioral measures of DT are often not significantly intercorrelated (Anestis, Lavender et al., in press; Bernstein, Marshall, & Zvolensky, 2011; McHugh et al., 2011) and are differentially associated with a variety of clinical outcomes (Anestis, Lavender et al., in press; Bernstein et al., 2011; see also Zvolensky, Leyro, Bernstein, & Vujanovic, 2011). Consequently, it is possible that self-report and behavioral DT measures are capturing different (unique) aspects of a single construct or assessing two separate constructs. Future research exploring the unique effects of perceived and actual DT on RSB among SUD patients with MDD may help inform the development of more comprehensive and effective RSB reduction programs for these patients.

Likewise, although the PASAT-C is an empirically supported laboratory stressor (see Zvolensky, Leyro et al., 2011) and resulted in a significant increase in emotional distress among the participants in this sample, it is a general stressor. Thus, it may not induce distress comparable to that experienced in real world contexts among SUD patients with MDD. As such, studies that utilize more personally-relevant or MDD-relevant stressors (e.g., examination of the willingness to tolerate sadness, or the assessment of DT following a sadness induction) may produce results with greater generalizability, as well as findings with larger effect sizes than those obtained here. In addition, future studies would benefit from exploring whether the tolerance of specific emotional states (e.g., guilt, anger, shame) improves our understanding of the effect of MDD on RSB.

Research is also needed to examine the mechanisms underlying the interrelations of MDD, low DT, and RSB among SUD patients. For example, consistent with the findings of Gorka et al. (in press), the combination of low DT and MDD may contribute to substance abuse which, in turn, may then increase the risk for RSB. In addition, low DT has been found to be related to negative urgency (i.e., the tendency to act impulsively when experiencing negative affect; Kaiser, Milich, Lynam, & Charnigo, 2012), which is also associated with RSB (Simons, Maisto, & Ray, 2010). Future research should also explore other factors that may contribute to RSB within this population. Despite finding a significant interaction between MDD and low DT in the number of past-year commercial and casual sexual partners, these effects were accompanied by a small effect size. Consequently, in addition to exploring these relationships within larger samples of SUD patients with MDD, studies are needed to examine the effect of other diagnoses associated with low DT and RSB within SUD patients, such as antisocial personality disorder (Compton, Cottler, Shillington, & Price, 1995; Daughters et al., 2008).

Finally, this study was conducted within a population previously found to be at high risk for HIV and other STD infections – SUD patients (Klinkenberg & Sacks, 2004). However, it is not clear if the results of this study generalize to other populations at heightened risk for RSB and HIV infection, such as MSM (CDC, 2011; Safren et al., 2010) and individuals with other psychiatric disorders besides SUDs, including PTSD (Brief et al., 2004; Munroe, Kibler, Ma, Dollar, & Coleman, 2010) and BPD (Klinkenberg & Sacks, 2004; Tull, Gratz, & Weiss, 2011). Consequently, it will be important for future studies to attempt to replicate our findings within these populations.

Despite limitations, the results of this study add to the growing body of literature on the deleterious effects of low DT and highlight a potential avenue for reducing RSB within a high-risk population. Indeed, given the high rates of sexually transmitted diseases found among SUD patients (Klinkenberg & Sacks, 2004), research focused on identifying the factors associated with RSB within this population has great clinical and public health significance, highlighting potential targets for intervention. Findings from this study suggest that low DT may be one such treatment target among SUD patients (particularly those with co-occurring MDD). The past three decades have seen the development and evaluation of multiple interventions focused on reducing HIV-risk behaviors, including RSB. However, although a number of these HIV-risk reduction interventions include information on coping skills for reducing stress in general or symptoms associated with specific psychiatric conditions, or the discussion of safe sexual practices and/or HIV status disclosure (Gordon, Forsyth, Stall, & Cheever, 2005; Kalichman, Rompa, & Cage, 2005; Kelly & Kalichman, 2002; Knauz et al., 2007), none target DT or other dimensions of emotion regulation directly. Consequently, it may be worthwhile to examine the effect of interventions that specifically focus on improving DT and emotion regulation in reducing RSB. One intervention that may be particularly useful in targeting DT within this population is Skills for Improving Distress Intolerance, a six-session individual treatment for low DT that has been found to significantly improve both DT and depression among patients in residential SUD treatment (Bornovalova et al., 2012). Furthermore, given the emphasis within dialectical behavior therapy (DBT; Linehan, 1993) on both DT and emotion regulation in general, future research should examine the utility of DBT in the treatment of RSB among SUD patients (given evidence that DBT has been found to improve emotion regulation abilities within SUD populations; Axelrod, Perepletchikova, Holtzman, & Sinha, 2011).

Acknowledgments

Support for this study was provided in part by R21 DA022383 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health awarded to the first author. The authors would like to thank Dr. Carl W. Lejuez, Dr. Scott F. Coffey, Bettye Van Norman, Nicole H. Weiss, Michael J. McDermott, Melissa Soenke, Sarah Anne Moore, Rachel Brooks, and Jessica Fulton for their assistance with this project.

References

  1. Alvy LM, McKirnan DJ, Mansergh G, Koblin B, Colfax GN, Flores SA, et al. Depression is associated with sexual risk among men who have sex with men, but is mediated by cognitive escape and self-efficacy. AIDS and Behavior. 2011;15:1171–1179. doi: 10.1007/s10461-010-9678-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th edition. Author; Washington, DC: 2000. text revision. [Google Scholar]
  3. Anestis MD, Gratz KL, Bagge CL, Tull MT. The interactive role of distress tolerance and borderline personality disorder in suicide attempts among substance users in residential treatment. Comprehensive Psychiatry. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.04.004. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Anestis MD, Lavender JM, Marshall-Berenz EC, Gratz KL, Tull MT, Joiner TE. Multi-method evaluation of distress tolerance measures: Interrelations and associations with impulsive behaviors. Cognitive Therapy and Research. in press. [Google Scholar]
  5. Anestis MD, Selby EA, Fink EL, Joiner TE. The multifaceted role of distress tolerance in dysregulation eating behaviors. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2007;40:718–726. doi: 10.1002/eat.20471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Anestis MD, Tull MT, Bagge CL, Gratz KL. The moderating role of distress tolerance in the relationship between posttraumatic stress disorder symptom clusters and suicidal behavior among trauma-exposed substance users in residential treatment. Archives of Suicide Research. 2012;16:198–211. doi: 10.1080/13811118.2012.695269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Axelrod SR, Perepletchikova F, Holtzman K, Sinha R. Emotion regulation and substance use frequency in women with substance dependence and borderline personality disorder receiving dialectical behavior therapy. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 2011;37:37–42. doi: 10.3109/00952990.2010.535582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bellis DJ. Fear of AIDS and risk reduction among heroin-addicted female street prostitutes: Personal interviews with 72 Southern California subjects. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education. 1990;35:26–37. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bernstein A, Marshall EC, Zvolensky MJ. Multi-method evaluation of distress tolerance measures and construct(s): Concurrent relations to mood and anxiety psychopathology and quality of life. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology. 2011;2:386–399. [Google Scholar]
  10. Blake DD, Weathers FW, Nagy L, Kaloupek DG, Klauminzer G, Charney DS, Keane TM. The Clinician Administered PTSD Scale. National Center for PTSD- Behavioral Science Division; Boston: 1990. [Google Scholar]
  11. Bornovalova MA, Daughters SB, Lejuez CW. Motivations for sexual risk behavior across commercial and casual partners among male urban drug users: Contextual features and clinical correlates. Behavior Modification. 2010;34:219–246. doi: 10.1177/0145445510364414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bornovalova MA, Gratz KL, Daughters SB, Hunt ED, Lejuez CW. Initial RCT of a distress tolerance treatment for individuals with substance use disorders. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2012;122:70–76. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.09.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bornovalova MA, Gratz KL, Daughters SB, Nick B, Delany-Brumsey A, Lynch TR, et al. A multimodal assessment of the relationship between emotion dysregulation and borderline personality disorder among inner-city substance users in residential treatment. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2008;42:717–726. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2007.07.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brief DJ, Bollinger AR, Vielhauer MJ, Berger-Greenstein JA, Morgan EE, Brady SM, Keane TM. Understanding the interface of HIV, trauma, post-traumatic stress disorder, and substance use and its implications for health outcomes. AIDS Care. 2004;16(Supplement 1):S97–120. doi: 10.1080/09540120412301315259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Buckner JD, Keough ME, Schmidt NB. Problematic alcohol and cannabis use among young adults: The roles of depression and discomfort and distress tolerance. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32:1957–1963. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.12.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Carey MP, Carey KB, Maisto SA, Gordon CM, Vanable PA. Prevalence and correlates of sexual activity and HIV-related risk behavior among psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2001;69:846–850. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.69.5.846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Carey MP, Carey KB, Maisto SA, Schroder KEE, Vanable PA, Gordon CM. HIV risk behavior among psychiatric outpatients: Association with psychiatric disorder, substance use disorder, and gender. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2004;192:289–296. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000120888.45094.38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Carter JD, Joyce PR, Mulder RT, Luty SE, McKenzie J. Gender differences in the presentation of depressed outpatients: A comparison of descriptive variables. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2000;61:59–67. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(00)00151-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Catania JA, Binson D, Dolcini MM, Stall R, Choi KH, Pollack LM, et al. Risk factors for HIV and other sexually transmitted diseases and prevention practices among US heterosexual adults: Changes from 1990 to 1992. American Journal of Public Health. 1995;85:1492–1499. doi: 10.2105/ajph.85.11.1492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [CDC] HIV surveillance report: Diagnoses of HIV infection and AIDS in the United States and dependent areas, 2009. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 2011. [Accessed June 22, 2012]. Web site: http://www.cdc.gov/hiv/topics/surveillance/basic.htm. [Google Scholar]
  21. Chapman AL, Gratz KL, Brown MZ. Solving the puzzle of deliberate self-harm: The experiential avoidance model. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2006;44:371–394. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Clen SL, Mennin DS, Fresco DM. Major depressive disorder. In: Zvolensky MJ, Bernstein A, Vujanovic AA, editors. Distress Tolerance. Guilford Press; New York, NY: 2011. pp. 149–170. [Google Scholar]
  23. Compton WM, Cottler LB, Shillington AM, Price RK. Is antisocial personality disorder associated with increased HIV risk behaviors in cocaine users? Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 1995;37:37–43. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(94)01056-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Cougle JR, Timpano KR, Goetz AR. Exploring the unique and interactive roles of distress tolerance and negative urgency in obsessions. Personality and Individual Differences. 2012;52:515–520. [Google Scholar]
  25. Crepaz N, Marks G. Are negative affective states associated with HIV sexual risk behaviors? A meta-analytic review. Journal of Health Psychology. 2001;20:291–299. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.20.4.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Darke S, Hall W, Heather N, Ward J, Wodak A. The reliability and validity of a scale to measure HIV risk-taking behaviour among intravenous drug users. AIDS. 1991;5:181–185. doi: 10.1097/00002030-199102000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Daughters SB, Lejuez CW, Bornovalova MA, Kahler CW, Strong DR, Brown RA. Distress tolerance as a predictor of early treatment dropout in a residential substance abuse treatment facility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2005;114:729–734. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Daughters SB, Lejuez CW, Kahler CW, Strong DR, Brown RA. Psychological distress tolerance and duration of most recent abstinence attempt among residential treatment-seeking substance abusers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2005;19:208–211. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.19.2.208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Daughters SB, Sargeant MN, Bornovalova MA, Gratz KL, Lejuez CW. The relationship between distress tolerance and antisocial personality disorder among male inner-city treatment seeking substance users. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2008;22:509–524. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2008.22.5.509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Dennhardt AA, Murphy JG. Associations between depression, distress tolerance, delay discounting, and alcohol-related problems in European American and African American college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2011;25:595–604. doi: 10.1037/a0025807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Evans JL, Hahn JA, Page-Shafer K, Lum PJ, Stein ES, Davidson PJ, Moss AR. Gender differences in sexual and injection risk behavior among active young injection drug users in San Francisco (the UFO Study) Journal of Urban Health. 2003;80:137–146. doi: 10.1093/jurban/jtg137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Fenton KA, Johnson AM, McManus S, Erens B. Measuring sexual behavior: Methodological challenges in survey research. Sexually Transmitted Infections. 2001;77:84–92. doi: 10.1136/sti.77.2.84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders – Patient edition (SCIP-I/P, Version 2.0) New York State Psychiatric Institute; New York, NY: 1996. [Google Scholar]
  34. Folkman S, Chesney MA, Pollack L, Phillips C. Stress, coping, and high-risk sexual behavior. Health Psychology. 1992;11:218–222. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.11.4.218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Fox HC, Hong KA, Sinha R. Difficulties in emotion regulation and impulse control in recently abstinent alcoholics compared with social drinkers. Addictive Behaviors. 2008;33:388–394. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Gonzalez VM, Reynolds B, Skewes MC. Role of impulsivity in the relationship between depression and alcohol problems among emerging adult college drinkers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2011;19:303–313. doi: 10.1037/a0022720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Gordon CM, Forsyth AD, Stall R, Cheever LW. Prevention interventions with persons living with HIV/AIDS: State of the science and future directions. AIDS Education and Prevention. 2005;17:6–20. doi: 10.1521/aeap.17.2.6.58697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gorka SM, Ali B, Daughters SB. The role of distress tolerance in the relationship between depressive symptoms and problematic alcohol use. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. doi: 10.1037/a0026386. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Grant BF, Stinson FS, Dawson DA, Chou P, Dufour MC, Compton W, Pickering RP, Kaplan K. Prevalence and co-occurrence of substance use disorders and independent mood and anxiety disorders: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004;61:807–816. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.8.807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gratz KL, Bornovalova MA, Delany-Brumsey A, Nick B, Lejuez CW. A laboratory-based study of the relationship between childhood abuse and experiential avoidance among inner-city substance users: The role of emotional nonacceptance. Behavior Therapy. 2007;38:256–268. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2006.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gratz KL, Hepworth C, Tull MT, Paulson A, Clarke S, Remington B, Lejuez CW. An experimental investigation of emotional willingness and physical pain tolerance in deliberate self-harm: The moderating role of interpersonal distress. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 2011;52:63–74. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2010.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gratz KL, Rosenthal M, Tull MT, Lejuez CW, Gunderson JG. An experimental investigation of emotion dysregulation in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2006;115:850–855. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.4.850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hanson KL, Luciana M, Sullwold K. Reward-related decision-making deficits and elevated impulsivity among MDMA and other drug users. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2008;96:99–110. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hayes SC, Wilson KG, Gifford EV, Follette VM, Strosahl K. Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1996;64:1152–1168. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.64.6.1152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hellinger FJ. Cost and financing of care for persons with HIV disease: An overview. Health Care Financing Review. 1998;19:1–14. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Holtgrave DR, Pinkerton SD. Updates of illness and quality of life estimates for use in economic evaluations of HIV prevention programs. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes and Human Retrovirology. 1997;16:54–62. doi: 10.1097/00042560-199709010-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Howell AN, Leyro TM, Hogan J, Buckner JD, Zvolensky MJ. Anxiety sensitivity, distress tolerance, and discomfort intolerance in relation to coping and conformity motives for alcohol use and alcohol use problems among young adult drinkers. Addictive Behaviors. 2010;35:1144–1147. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hutchinson AB, Farnham PG, Dean HD, Ekwueme DU, del Rio C, Kamimoto L, et al. The economic burden of HIV in the United States in the era of highly active antiretroviral therapy: Evidence of continuing racial and ethnic differences. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes. 2006;43:451–457. doi: 10.1097/01.qai.0000243090.32866.4e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hutton HE, Lyketsos CG, Zenilman JM, Thompson RE, Erbelding RJ. Depression and HIV risk behaviors among patients in a sexually transmitted disease clinic. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2004;161:912–914. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.5.912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Iverson KM, Follette VM, Pistorello J, Fruzetti AE. An investigation of experiential avoidance, emotion dysregulation, and distress tolerance in young adult outpatients with borderline personality disorder symptoms. Personality Disorder: Theory, Research, and Treatment. doi: 10.1037/a0023703. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Johnson ME, Fisher DG, Montoya I, Booth R, Rhodes F, Andersen M, Zhuo Z, Williams M. Reliability and validity of not-in-treatment drug users' follow-up self-reports. AIDS and Behavior. 2000;4:373–380. [Google Scholar]
  53. Kaiser AJ, Milich R, Lynam DR, Charnigo RJ. Negative urgency, distress tolerance, and substance abuse among college students. Addictive Behaviors. 2012;37:1075–1083. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.04.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kalichman SC, Rompa D, Cage M. Group interventions to reduce HIV transmission risk behavior among persons living with HIV/AIDS. Behavior Modification. 2005;29:256–285. doi: 10.1177/0145445504272603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kelly JA, Kalichman SC. Behavioral research in HIV/AIDS primary and secondary prevention: Recent advances and future directions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70:626–639. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Klein H, Elifson KW, Sterk CE. Depression and HIV risk behavior practices among “at risk” women. Women & Health. 2008;48:167–188. doi: 10.1080/03630240802313605. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Klinkenberg WD, Sacks S. Mental disorders and drug abuse in persons living with HIV/AIDS. AIDS Care. 2004;16(Suppl. 1):S22–S42. doi: 10.1080/09540120412331315303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Knauz RO, Safren SA, O'Cleirigh C, Capistrant BD, Driskell JR, Aguilar D, Salomon L, Hobson J, Mayer KH. Developing an HIV-prevention intervention for HIV-infected men who have sex with men in HIV care: Project enhance. AIDS & Behavior. 2007;11:S117–S126. doi: 10.1007/s10461-007-9257-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Koblin BA, Husnik MJ, Colfax G, Huang Y, Madison M, Mayer K, Buchbinder S. Risk factors for HIV infection among men who have sex with men. AIDS. 2006;21:731–739. doi: 10.1097/01.aids.0000216374.61442.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Lejuez CW, Kahler CW, Brown RA. A modified computer version of the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT) as a laboratory-based stressor. The Behavior Therapist. 2003;26:290–293. [Google Scholar]
  61. Leith KP, Baumeister RF. Why do bad moods increase self-defeating behavior? Emotion, risk tasking, and self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1996;71:1250–1267. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.71.6.1250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Lejuez CW, Bornovalova MA, Daughters SB, Curtin JJ. Differences in impulsivity and sexual risk behavior among inner-city crack/cocaine users and heroin users. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2005;77:169–175. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.08.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Levin LI, Peterman TA, Renzullo PO, Lasley-Bibbs V, Shu XO, Brundage JF, McNeil JG. HIV-1 seroconversion and risk behaviors among young men in the US Army. The Seroconversion Risk Factor Study Group. American Journal of Public Health. 1995;85:1500–1506. doi: 10.2105/ajph.85.11.1500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Leyro TM, Zvolensky MJ, Bernstein A. Distress tolerance and psychopathological symptoms and disorders: A review of the empirical literature among adults. Psychological Bulletin. 2010;136:576–600. doi: 10.1037/a0019712. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Linehan MM. Cognitive Behavioral Treatment of Borderline Personality Disorder. Guilford Press; New York: 1993. [Google Scholar]
  66. Lucenko BA, Malow RM, Sanchez-Martinez M, Jennings T, Dévieux JG. Negative affect and HIV risk in alcohol and other drug (AOD) abusing adolescent offenders. Journal of Child and Adolescent Substance Abuse. 2003;13:1–17. doi: 10.1300/J029v13n01_01. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. MacCallum RC, Zhang S, Preacher KJ, Rucker DD. On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychological Methods. 2002;7:19–40. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. McCusker J, Goldstein R, Bigelow C, Zorn M. Psychiatric status and HIV risk reduction among residential drug abuse treatment clients. Addiction. 1995;90:1377–1387. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1995.901013779.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. McDermott MD, Tull MT, Gratz KL, Daughters SB, Lejuez CW. The role of anxiety sensitivity and difficulties in emotion regulation in posttraumatic stress disorder among crack/cocaine dependent patients in residential substance abuse treatment. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2009;23:591–599. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.01.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. McHugh RK, Daughters SB, Lejuez CW, Murray HW, Hearon BA, Gorka SM, Otto MW. Shared variance among self-report and behavioral measures of distress intolerance. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2011;35:266–275. doi: 10.1007/s10608-010-9295-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. McKeganey NP. Prostitution and HIV: What do we know and where might research be targeted in the future? AIDS. 1994;8:1215–1226. doi: 10.1097/00002030-199409000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. McGowan JP, Shah SS, Ganea CE, Blum S, Ernst JA, Irwin KL, et al. Risk behavior for transmission of human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) among HIV-seropositive individuals in an urban setting. Clinical Infectious Diseases. 2004;38:122–127. doi: 10.1086/380128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Meade CS. Sexual risk behavior among persons dually diagnosed with severe mental illness and substance use disorder. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2006;30:147–157. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2005.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Meade CS, Sikkema KJ. HIV risk behavior among adults with severe mental illness: A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review. 2005;25:433–457. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Messman-Moore TL, Walsh KL, DiLillo D. Emotion dysregulation and risky sexual behavior in revictimization. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2010;34:967–976. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2010.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Mitchell MM, Latimer WW. Gender differences in high risk sexual behaviors and injection practices associated with perceived HIV risk among injection drug users. AIDS Education and Prevention. 2009a;21:384–394. doi: 10.1521/aeap.2009.21.4.384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Mitchell MM, Latimer WW. Unprotected casual sex and perceived risk of contracting HIV among drug users in Baltimore, Maryland: Evaluating the influence of non-injection versus injection drug user status. AIDS Care. 2009b;21:221–230. doi: 10.1080/09540120801982897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Munroe CD, Kibler JL, Ma M, Dollar KM, Coleman M. The relationship between posttraumatic stress symptoms and sexual risk: Examining potential mechanisms. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy. 2010;2:49–53. [Google Scholar]
  79. Must A, Szabó Z, Bódi N, Szász A, Janka Z, Kéri S. Sensitivity to reward and punishment and the prefrontal cortex in major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2006;90:209–215. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Nolen-Hoeksema S. Gender differences in depression. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2001;10:173–176. [Google Scholar]
  81. Patterson TL, Goldenberg S, Gallardo M, Lozado R, Orozovich P, Abramovitz D, Strathdee SA. Correlates of HIV, sexually transmitted infections, and associated high-risk behaviors among male clients of female sex workers in Tijuana, Mexico. AIDS. 2009;24:1765–1771. doi: 10.1097/QAD.0b013e32832f08a1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Perdue T, Hagan H, Thiede H, Valleroy L. Depression and HIV risk behavior among Seattle-area injection drug users and young men who have sex with men. AIDS Education and Prevention. 2003;15:81–92. doi: 10.1521/aeap.15.1.81.23842. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Piccinelli M, Wilkinson G. Gender differences in depression: Critical review. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2000;177:486–492. doi: 10.1192/bjp.177.6.486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Reisner SL, Mimiaga MJ, Skeer M, Bright D, Cranston K, Isenberg D, Bland S, Barker TA, Mayer KH. Clinically significant depressive symptoms as a risk factor for HIV infection among Black MSM in Massachusetts. AIDS and Behavior. 2009;13:798–810. doi: 10.1007/s10461-009-9571-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Reynolds EK, Magidson JF, Bornovalova MA, Gwadz M, Ewart CK, Daughters SB, Lejuez CW. Application of the social action theory to understand factors associated with risky sexual behavior among individuals in residential substance abuse treatment. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2010;24:311–321. doi: 10.1037/a0018929. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Richards JM, Daughters SB, Bornovalova MA, Brown RA, Lejuez CW. Substance use disorders. In: Zvolensky MJ, Bernstein A, Vujanovic AA, editors. Distress Tolerance. Guilford Press; New York, NY: 2011. pp. 171–197. [Google Scholar]
  87. Risser JMH, Timpson SC, McCurdy SA, Ross MW, Williams ML. Psychological correlates of trading sex for money among African American crack cocaine smokers. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 2006;32:645–653. doi: 10.1080/00952990600919062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Rogers MA, Kasai K, Koji M, Fukuda R, Iwanami A, Nakagome K, et al. Executive and prefrontal dysfunction in unipolar depression: A review of neuropsychological and imaging evidence. Neuroscience Research. 2004;50:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2004.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Rosenheck R, Ngilangwa D, Manongi R, Kapiga S. Treatment-seeking behavior for sexually transmitted infections in a high-risk population. AIDS Care. 2010;22:1350–1358. doi: 10.1080/09540121003758507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Safren SA, Traeger L, Skeer MR, O'Cleirigh C, Meade CS, Covahey C, Mayer KH. Testing a social-cognitive model of HIV transmission risk behaviors in HIV-infected MSM with and without depression. Health Psychology. 2010;29:215–221. doi: 10.1037/a0017859. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Schloss HM, Haaga DAF. Interrelating behavioral measures of distress tolerance with self-reported experiential avoidance. Journal of Rational-Emotive Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy. 2011;29:53–63. doi: 10.1007/s10942-011-0127-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Semple SJ, Patterson TL, Grant I. Psychosocial predictors of unprotected anal intercourse in a sample of HIV positive gay men who volunteer for a sexual risk reduction intervention. AIDS Education and Prevention. 2000;12:416–430. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Seth P, Raiji PT, DiClemente RJ, Wingood GM, Rose E. Psychological distress as a correlate of a biologically confirmed STI, risky sexual practices, self-efficacy and communication with male sex partners in African-American female adolescents. Psychology, Health, and Medicine. 2009;13:291–300. doi: 10.1080/13548500902730119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Shapiro SS, Wilk MB. An analysis of variance test for normality (complete samples) Biometrika. 1965;52:591–611. [Google Scholar]
  95. Simons JS, Gaher RM. The Distress Tolerance Scale: Development and validation of a self-report measure. Motivation and Emotion. 2005;29:83–102. [Google Scholar]
  96. Simons JS, Maisto SA, Wray TB. Sexual risk taking among young adult alcohol and marijuana users. Addictive Behaviors. 2010;35:533–536. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.12.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Stall R, Mills TC, Williamson J, Hart T, Greenwood G, Paul J, et al. Association of co-occurring psychosocial health problems and increased vulnerability to HIV/AIDS among urban men who have sex with men. American Journal of Public Health. 2003;93:939–942. doi: 10.2105/ajph.93.6.939. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Stein MD, Solomon DA, Herman DS, Anderson BJ, Miller I. Depression severity and drug injection HIV risk behaviors. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:1659–1662. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.9.1659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Subramaniam GA, Stitzer ML, Woody G, Fishman MJ, Kolodner K. Clinical characteristics of treatment-seeking adolescents with opioid versus cannabis/alcohol use disorders. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2009;99:141–149. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.07.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using multivariate statistics. 5th ed. Allyn & Bacon/Pearson Education; Boston, MA: 2007. [Google Scholar]
  101. Tull MT, Gratz KL, Coffey SF, Weiss NH, McDermott MJ. Examining the interactive effect of posttraumatic stress disorder, distress tolerance, and gender on residential substance use disorder treatment retention. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. doi: 10.1037/a0029911. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Tull MT, Gratz KL, Weiss NH. Exploring associations between borderline personality disorder, crack/cocaine dependence, gender, and risky sexual behavior among substance dependent inpatients. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment. 2011;2:209–219. doi: 10.1037/a0021878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Tull MT, Weiss NH, Adams CE, Gratz KL. The contribution of emotion dysregulation to risky sexual behavior within a sample of patients in residential substance abuse treatment. Addictive Behaviors. 2012;37:1084–1092. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Tyndall MW, Patrick D, Spittal P, Li K, O'Schaughnessy MV, Schecter MT. Risky sexual behaviors among injection drug users with high HIV prevalence: Implications for STD control. Sexually Transmitted Infections. 2002;78(Suppl. 1):i170–i175. doi: 10.1136/sti.78.suppl_1.i170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Weatherby NL, Needle R, Cesari H, Booth RE, McCoy CB, Watters JK, Williams M, Chitwood DD. Validity of self-reported drug use among injection drug users and crack cocaine users recruited through street outreach. Evaluation and Program Planning. 1994;17:347–355. [Google Scholar]
  106. Weinhardt LS, Carey KB, Carey MP. HIV risk sensitization following a detailed sexual behavior interview: A preliminary investigation. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2000;23:393–398. doi: 10.1023/a:1005505018784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Wood E, Schachar J, Li K, Stoltz J, Shannon K, Miller C, et al. Sex trade involvement is associated with elevated HIV incidence among injection drug users in Vancouver. Addiction Research and Theory. 2007;15:321–325. [Google Scholar]
  108. Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Sickel AE, Young L. Diagnostic interview for DSM-IV personality disorders. Unpublished measure. McLean Hospital; Boston, MA: 1996. [Google Scholar]
  109. Zanarini MC, Skodol AE, Bender D, Dolan R, Sanislow C, Schaefer E, Gunderson JG. The Collaborative Longitudinal Personality Disorders Study: Reliability of Axis I and II diagnoses. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2000;14:291–299. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2000.14.4.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Zvolensky MJ, Bernstein A, Vujanovic AA. Distress tolerance. Guilford Press; New York, NY: 2011. [Google Scholar]
  111. Zvolensky MJ, Leyro TM, Bernstein A, Vujanovic AA. Historical perspectives, theory, and measurement of distress tolerance. In: Zvolensky MJ, Bernstein A, Vujanovic AA, editors. Distress Tolerance. Guilford Press; New York, NY: 2011. pp. 3–27. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES