Abstract
Recent evidence suggests that impulsivity and sensation seeking are not stable risk factors for substance use among adolescents and early adults but rather that they undergo significant developmental maturation and change. Further, developmental trends of both personality facets may vary across individuals. In the current investigation, we used longitudinal data from ages 15-26 on 5,632 individuals drawn from the offspring generation of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (CNLSY) to examine whether inter-individual differences in intra-individual change in impulsivity and sensation seeking predicted the escalation of alcohol, marijuana, and cigarette use in adolescence and early adulthood. Latent growth curve models revealed significant individual differences in rates of change in both personality and substance use. Most importantly, age-related changes in personality were positively associated with individual differences in substance use change. Individuals who declined more slowly in impulsivity increased in alcohol, marijuana, and cigarette use more rapidly, whereas individuals who declined more slowly in sensation seeking increased more rapidly in alcohol use only. Although risk for substance use across the population may peak during adolescence and early adulthood, this risk may be highest among those who decline more gradually in impulsivity.
Keywords: Impulsivity, Sensation Seeking, Substance Use, Correlated Changes, Latent Growth Curve Modeling
The facets of personality underlying undercontrolled or disinhibited behavior have long been identified as correlates of adolescent and young adult substance use, among other forms of externalizing psychopathology (Cooper, Wood, Orcutt, & Albino, 2003; Krueger et al., 2002; Sher & Trull, 1994; Sher, Trull, Bartholow, & Vieth, 1999). Although the exact nature and structure of undercontrolled personality continues to be a source of debate (e.g., Block & Block, 2006; Cloninger, Przybeck, & Svrakic, 1991; Cross, Copping, & Campbell, 2011; Kirby & Finch, 2010; Whiteside & Lynam, 2001; Zuckerman, Kuhlman, Joireman, Teta, & Kraft, 1993), a broad literature nevertheless reveals a consistent distinction between two important, empirically and conceptually distinct facets: impulsivity, defined as a tendency to act without considering consequences, and sensation seeking, defined as a preference for varied, novel, and exciting experiences (Dawe, Gullo, & Loxton, 2004; Duckworth & Kern, 2011; Gullo, Ward, Dawe, Powell, & Jackson, 2011; Kirby & Finch, 2010; Magid & Colder, 2007; Magid, MacLean, & Colder, 2007; Smith et al., 2007). More impulsive and sensation-seeking adolescents are at greater risk for early use and abuse of a variety of substances (Iacono, Malone, & McGue, 2008). A breadth of research has found support for the role of impulsivity in adolescent and young adult alcohol use (Dick et al., 2010), in addition to cigarette and illicit drug use (Elkins, King, McGue, & Iacono, 2006). Similarly, sensation seeking has most commonly been studied in relation to alcohol use (Hittner & Swickert, 2006), but some recent evidence also links sensation seeking to marijuana and cigarette use (Crawford, Pentz, Chou, Li, & Dwyer, 2003; Martin et al., 2002; Romer & Hennessy, 2007).
Reflecting the long-held view that personality is broadly immutable and essentially fixed in young adulthood (McCrae & Costa, 1994), etiological models of alcohol and other substance use have often treated impulsivity and sensation seeking as relatively stable individual difference risk factors (e.g., Sher, Bartholow, & Wood, 2000). These developmental models (i.e., “vulnerability models”; Roberts, Jackson, Burger, & Trautwein, 2009) have typically conceptualized personality as helping to dictate risk for substance use early in life, with higher levels of impulsivity or sensation seeking leading to an earlier onset and more problematic, persistent course (Chassin, Pitts, & Prost, 2002; Flory, Lynam, Milich, Leukefeld, & Clayton, 2004; Sher & Gotham, 1999; Zucker, Cicchetti, & Cohen, 2006). Complicating this view, however, contemporary research in personality has shown that virtually all aspects of personality change across the lifespan, in some cases quite dramatically (Caspi, Roberts, & Shiner, 2005). Indeed, there are population-level trends in personality change that reflect normative patterns of development through adolescence, adulthood, and even old age (Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006). Consistent with Caspi and colleagues' (2005) maturity principle, these patterns, including increased social dominance, agreeableness, and conscientiousness and decreased neuroticism, tend to promote psychosocial maturity and improved functioning with age.
Recent research on developmental changes in personality has suggested that impulsivity and sensation seeking show diverging patterns of age-related change across adolescence and early adulthood. Behavioral and self-report evidence from cross-sectional (Steinberg et al., 2008; Steinberg et al., 2009; Vaidya, Latzman, Markon, & Watson, 2010) and longitudinal (Harden & Tucker-Drob, 2011) research shows that impulsivity declines linearly from childhood until the third decade of life. In contrast, some of the same studies show that sensation seeking rises to a peak in mid-adolescence (approximately age 16) before declining into adulthood (Cauffman et al., 2010; Harden & Tucker-Drob, 2011; Romer & Hennessy, 2007; Steinberg et al., 2008). As Steinberg (2008, 2010) and others (Casey, Getz, & Galvan, 2008; Somerville, Jones, & Casey, 2010) have argued, mean increases in sensation seeking during early and middle adolescence, in conjunction with the gradual decrease in impulsivity, may help explain why substance use and other risk-taking behaviors emerge during adolescence. Whereas only 8% of 10th graders binge drink, that rate increases to 25% by the end of high school and 40% through age 24 (Bachman, Wadsworth, O'Malley, & Johnston, 1997; Johnston, O'Malley, Bachman, & Schulenberg, 2009b). Further, risk for the onset of alcohol dependence peaks at age 18, and more than 9% of those aged 18-29 meet criteria for an alcohol use disorder (Grant et al., 2004; Li, Hewitt, & Grant, 2004). The use and abuse of other substances follows similar patterns. Among those aged 18-29, 11% used marijuana in the past year, with 4% meeting criteria for a marijuana use disorder (Compton, Grant, Colliver, Glantz, & Stinson, 2004), and the largest mean-level increases in tobacco use occur during late adolescence (Bachman et al., 1997; Johnston, O'Malley, Bachman, & Schulenberg, 2009a; Johnston et al., 2009b). Thus, the emergence of substance use appears to coincide with the apex of mean-level personality risk.
Individual Differences in Personality Change and Substance Use
Missing from etiological theories derived from mean-level age-trends, however, is an acknowledgement of individual differences in both substance use and personality. The adolescent emergence of substance use is far from universal across the population. As Romer (2010) has observed, although the prevalence of substance use—in addition to other forms of risk-taking such as physical aggression—may be higher among adolescents relative to adults, only a fraction of adolescents actually engage in such behaviors. Data from recent Monitoring the Future surveys confirm that a sizeable majority of adolescents and early adults do not smoke cigarettes or marijuana (Johnston et al., 2009a, 2009b). Even among college students, who drink more heavily than their peers who do not attend college, one in five abstains from alcohol use, and two in five drink at levels below the threshold for binge drinking (Wechsler, Dowdall, Maenner, Gledhill-Hoyt, & Lee, 1998).
Similarly, against the backdrop of normative maturational changes, there is substantial inter-individual instability in personality over time (Roberts & DelVecchio, 2000), and virtually all dimensions of personality are characterized by individual differences in change across adolescence and early adulthood (Donnellan, Conger, & Burzette, 2007; Neyer & Lehnart, 2007; Vaidya, Gray, Haig, Mroczek, & Watson, 2008). These differential personality changes may be driven by both intrinsic differences and by life experiences. Adolescence and early adulthood are rich with significant social-role transitions (e.g., leaving the parental home, joining the workforce, establishing romantic partnerships), and recent evidence suggests that personality changes and role demands are mutually reinforcing through the dual processes of social environment selection and socialization (Neyer & Lehnart, 2007; Roberts & Bogg, 2004; Roberts, Walton, Bogg, & Caspi, 2006; Scollon & Diener, 2006; Vaidya, Gray, Haig, & Watson, 2002). Relevant to the current investigation, the mean-level developmental trends in impulsivity and sensation seeking emphasized by recent etiological models belie meaningful individual differences in intra-individual change. Harden and Tucker-Drob (2011) found significant variability in trajectories of impulsivity and sensation seeking between the ages of 12 and 24 years old: Some adolescents demonstrate quite rapid declines in impulsivity and sensation seeking, whereas others demonstrate very slow declines.
An additional challenge for etiological theories of adolescent substance use derived from mean-level age-trends in personality (e.g., Steinberg, 2008, 2010) has been that mean levels of substance use continue to increase into early adulthood, while decreases in both sensation seeking and impulsivity are typical after mid-adolescence (Harden & Tucker-Drob, 2011; Romer & Hennessy, 2007; Steinberg et al., 2008). That is, the initiation of substance use may often occur at the normative apex of sensation seeking, but the continuation and escalation of use in the population coincides with declines in both sensation seeking and impulsivity. Although these mean-level patterns appear contradictory, the recognition that individuals differ not only in their initial levels of impulsivity and sensation seeking, but also in their trajectories of personality change, permits a more comprehensive understanding of how personality may impact changes in substance use. Just as greater impulsivity and sensation seeking at a given age predict greater substance use, individual-level deviations from normative age-trends may put some adolescents and early adults at particularly elevated risk for changes in substance use. That is, it is possible that individual-level personality and substance use trajectories covary, with those adolescents who decline in impulsivity and sensation seeking most gradually also experiencing the steepest increases in substance use. In sum, whereas adolescence and early adulthood may be, on average, a period of increased propensity for substance use, this risk may be highest among those who decline in impulsivity and sensation seeking most gradually.
Differences in personality change trajectories have been implicated in a number of important life outcomes (Mroczek & Spiro, 2007; Roberts & Mroczek, 2008), but relatively little research has tested the impact of this inter-individual variability on substance use. Littlefield and colleagues (2009, 2010a, 2010b) have found that more rapid declines in impulsivity are associated with declining alcohol involvement through young adulthood. In a separate study, greater increases in sensation seeking during middle school predicted increased marijuana use during high school (Crawford et al., 2003). Although substance use peaks during late adolescence and early adulthood, evidence for the roles of changing impulsivity and sensation seeking during this crucial developmental period is lacking. To our knowledge, no study has directly tested associations between changes in the use of multiple substances and these two personality constructs through adolescence and early adulthood.
Goals of the Present Study
In the current investigation, we used 12 years of longitudinal data from the offspring generation of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (CNLSY) to test the role of personality change in individual differences in substance use from adolescence to early adulthood. In particular, we tested whether variability in the development of impulsivity and sensation seeking related to variability in the development of alcohol, marijuana, and cigarette use across ages 15-26. Our analyses focused on three key research questions: (1) are there individual differences in change in impulsivity and sensation seeking?; (2) are there individual differences in rates of change in alcohol, marijuana, and cigarette use?; and (3) are individual differences in rates of change in substance use associated with change in impulsivity and sensation seeking?
Method
Participants and Procedures
Mother generation: The National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY79)
The Bureau of Labor Statistics designed and funded the NLSY79 survey in order to study workforce participation in the U.S. A complex survey design was used to select a nationally representative sample of 3,000 households containing 6,111 youth aged 14-21 years as of December 31, 1978. An additional oversample of 3,652 African-American and Hispanic youth was selected to over-represent these racial/ethnic groups.1 The response rate for the initial NLSY79 survey was over 90% of the eligible sample, and participants have been interviewed annually from 1979 to 1994 and biennially since 1994. Retention rates for follow-up assessments of the NLSY79 sample were greater than 90% for the first 16 waves and greater than 80% for subsequent waves.
Offspring generation: The NLSY79 children and young adults (CNLSY)
Beginning in 1986, the biological children of NLSY79 women were assessed biennially (Chase-Lansdale, Mott, Brooks-Gunn, & Phillips, 1991). The initial participation rate was 95%, and the average retention rate through 2006 was approximately 90%. Beginning in 1994, adolescent offspring who were age 15 by the end of the survey calendar year were administered a separate interview (the CNLSY “young adult” interview), which included measures of personality and substance use. As of 2006, 11,466 children were identified as having been born to 6,283 NLSY79 women. After weighting for sample selection, the average NLSY79 woman has had 1.9 children, which is more than 90% of their ultimate predicted childbearing.
The current project uses data from a subsample of 5,632 adolescents and young adults who reported on their impulsivity, sensation seeking, and alcohol, marijuana, and cigarette use at least once between the ages of 15 and 26. Although some participants may have initiated substance use prior to age 15, the young adult substance use assessments differed from the measures administered to participants under age 15, rendering the inclusion of younger children impossible. This CNLSY subsample is demographically diverse: 1,215 youth (21.6%) were Hispanic/Latino, 1,970 (35.0%) were African-American, and the remaining 2,447 (43.5%) were non-Hispanic White, and 49.1% were female. Because children were assessed biennially, all data were analyzed using six two-year age groups: 15-16 year olds, 17-18 years olds, 19-20 year olds, 21-22 years olds, 23-24 year olds, and 25-26 years olds.
As displayed in greater detail in Table 1, of the 5,632 participants included in the present analyses, only a subset provided data at each age group. There are four sources of missingness in the CNLSY young adult interview data. First, the interviews began in 1994, excluding from the age 15-16 interviews the relatively small number of offspring born prior to 1978. Second, some of those eligible did not complete the age 15-16 interview; these individuals differed modestly from participants on maternal demographic variables.2 Third, there was attrition following the age 15-16 interview, although attrition was largely independent of study variables.3 Finally, less data is available for later cohorts of participants, who have had fewer opportunities to be assessed since the age of 15; 4,515 adolescents provided data at age 15-16, whereas 639 provided data at age 25-26.
Table 1. Number of CNLSY Participants by Age Group and Birth Cohort.
Birth Cohort | Total N | Age Group | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||||
15-16 | 17-18 | 19-20 | 21-22 | 23-24 | 25-26 | ||
1972-73 | 24 | - | - | - | 18 | 13 | - |
1974-75 | 126 | - | - | 91 | 87 | - | 113 |
1976-77 | 311 | - | 249 | 219 | 87 | 167 | 25 |
1978-79 | 515 | 447 | 416 | 359 | 78 | 31 | 432 |
1980-81 | 743 | 581 | 595 | 53 | 155 | 630 | 69a |
1982-83 | 845 | 697 | 42 | 164 | 734 | 57a | - |
1984-85 | 862 | 680 | 138 | 779 | 47a | - | - |
1986-87 | 798 | 727 | 742 | 43a | - | - | - |
1988-89 | 789 | 764 | 25a | - | - | - | - |
1990-91 | 619 | 619 | - | - | - | - | - |
| |||||||
All Cohorts | 5632 | 4515 | 2207 | 1708 | 1206 | 898 | 639 |
Note. Empty cells signify that a particular birth cohort was not assessed during that age range.
Small number of participants represents “spillover” of birth cohort into subsequent age group because interview date occurred after the participant's birthday.
This final source is a well-documented potential contributor of bias to the CNLSY sample: Because not all NLSY79 mothers have completed their childbearing and not all offspring have completed adolescence, the current CNLSY data over-represent the earliest cohort of participants, who were born to relatively young mothers (Turley, 2003). Younger mothers, in turn, systematically differ from women who delay childbearing on socioeconomic and behavioral variables that may be relevant for personality and substance use in their offspring (Harden et al., 2007). To correct for this source of sampling bias, all analyses controlled for maternal age at first birth, as well as externalizing and internalizing symptoms and socio-demographic factors (including maternal education, socioeconomic status, and race/ethnicity) that differ between older and younger mothers. This approach has been used to correct for sampling bias in previous studies of the CNLSY sample (D'Onofrio et al., 2008; Harden et al., 2009; Mendle et al., 2009).
Measures
Maternal demographics
Socioeconomic status (SES) was measured using mother-reported total family income, including government support and food stamps but excluding income received by unmarried cohabitating partners, when the mother was 30 years old. The median annual family income was approximately $22,600, and ranged from $0 to $375,000. Maternal cognitive ability was measured in the 1980 assessment using the Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery (ASVAB), test of knowledge and skill in 10 subject areas. Composite scores on the ASVAB (based on the word knowledge, paragraph comprehension, math knowledge, and arithmetic reasoning subtests) were standardized and converted to a percentile score. Maternal education was measured using maternal report of the number of years of school completed (M = 13.32 years, SD = 2.40 years; approximately 9% of the sample reported 11 years or less). Finally, maternal age at first birth was calculated using the date of birth for the mother and her first child (M = 21.12, SD = 3.86, range = 13.17 – 34.60 years).
Maternal externalizing and internalizing symptoms
Mothers completed a version of the Self-Reported Delinquency interview in the 1980 NLSY79 assessment, at which time they were 15-23 years old (Elliott & Huizinga, 1983). This commonly used, reliable, and valid measure included 12 items assessing the frequency with which respondents engaged in delinquent acts ranging from destroying property to attacking another person to selling hard drugs. As a measure of maternal internalizing symptoms, mothers completed the Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression scale in 1992 at ages 27-35 (Radloff, 1977). This widely used measure comprises 20 items such as I felt hopeless, I felt sad, and I thought my life had been a failure. Respondents endorse each item on a scale ranging from 0 = rarely or none of the time to 3 = most or all of the time.
Personality
We used a latent variable approach to the measurement of impulsivity and sensation seeking. Impulsivity was assessed by youth self-report on three indicator items: I often get in a jam because I do things without thinking; I think that planning takes the fun out of things; and I have to use a lot of self-control to keep out of trouble. Similarly, sensation-seeking was assessed by youth self-report on the following three indicator items: I enjoy taking risks; I enjoy new and exciting experiences, even if they are a little frightening or unusual; and life with no danger in it would be too dull for me. All impulsivity and sensation-seeking items were rated on 4-point scales ranging from 0 = strongly disagree to 3 = strongly agree. Across all time-points, average scores on the three impulsivity items were Ms = 1.40, 1.17, and 1.33, respectively (SDs = 0.66, 0.57, 0.73; all ranges = 0.00 – 3.00). Average scores on the three sensation seeking items were Ms = 1.53, 1.96, and 1.52, respectively (SDs = 0.64, 0.54, 0.67; all ranges = 0.00 – 3.00). Although impulsivity and sensation seeking were positively associated across all assessments (rs ranging from .24 to .26, p < .001), they were distinct from each other. In confirmatory factor analysis, a single-factor model of all six personality items fit the data poorly, χ2 (8) = 869.63, p < .001, CFI = .80, RMSEA = .17, whereas a two-factor model fit the data significantly better, Δχ2 = 702.91, p < .001, CFI = .96, RMSEA = .07. As has been previously reported, the CNLSY measures of impulsivity and sensation seeking demonstrate strong validity. Consistent with previous research on these traits (Whiteside & Lynam, 2001), the impulsivity measure is moderately and negatively associated with the Big Five dimensions of Conscientiousness and Emotional Stability, whereas sensation seeking is moderately and positively associated with Extraversion and Openness (Harden & Tucker-Drob, 2011).
Substance use
Substance use was measured using self-reported frequencies of alcohol, marijuana, and cigarette use, each of which was assessed at every assessment wave. Participants reported the frequency with which they drank alcohol in the past year on a 9-point scale ranging from 1 = did not drink to 9 = drank daily. Across all time-points, alcohol use scores averaged 3.00 (SD = 1.81, range = 1.00 – 9.00), corresponding to a total of 3-5 drinking days in the past 12 months. Alcohol abstainers comprised between 21.5% (age 23-24) and 64.3% (age 15-16) of included participants. Abstainers were coded as 1 (did not drink). Past-30-day cigarette and marijuana use was assessed using 6-point scales, with responses ranging from 0 = never to 5 = every day. Across all time-points, marijuana use scores averaged 0.25 (SD = 0.69, range = 1.00 – 5.00), and cigarette use scores averaged 0.88 (SD = 1.50, range = 1.00 – 5.00), indicating that across ages 15-26, participants used marijuana and cigarettes on average less than once per week. Past-30-day abstainers comprised between 87.1% (age 19-20) and 93.0% (age 15-16) of the sample for marijuana use and between 63.4% (age 25-26) and 83.7% (age 15-16) for cigarette use. Marijuana and cigarette abstainers were coded as 0 (Never used). See Table 2 for correlations among substance use variables and Table 3 for correlations between personality and substance use variables.
Table 2. Zero-Order Correlations among Alcohol, Marijuana, and Cigarette Use.
Variable | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | 14 | 15 | 16 | 17 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Alcohol use | |||||||||||||||||
1. Age 15-16 | - | ||||||||||||||||
2. Age 17-18 | .39* | - | |||||||||||||||
3. Age 19-20 | .27* | .43* | - | ||||||||||||||
4. Age 21-22 | .16* | .32* | .49* | - | |||||||||||||
5. Age 23-24 | .15* | .28* | .36* | .55* | - | ||||||||||||
6. Age 25-26 | .13* | .30* | .34* | .42* | .60* | - | |||||||||||
Marijuana use | |||||||||||||||||
7. Age 15-16 | .40* | .20* | .12* | .07* | .05 | .07 | - | ||||||||||
8. Age 17-18 | .28* | .36* | .21* | .14* | .12* | .11* | .30* | - | |||||||||
9. Age 19-20 | .20* | .25* | .33* | .17* | .13* | .15* | .23* | .37* | - | ||||||||
10. Age 21-22 | .15* | .18* | .22* | .23* | .20* | .12* | .18* | .26* | .43* | - | |||||||
11. Age 23-24 | .12* | .15* | .16* | .16* | .21* | .16* | .16* | .20* | .31* | .44* | - | ||||||
12. Age 25-26 | .14* | .19* | .18* | .13* | .15* | .19* | .19* | .25* | .37* | .38* | .47* | - | |||||
Cigarette use | |||||||||||||||||
13. Age 15-16 | .44* | .26* | .14* | .09* | .07* | .11* | .44* | .23* | .19* | .16* | .13* | .10* | - | ||||
14. Age 17-18 | .33* | .39* | .22* | .16* | .10* | .11* | .24* | .32* | .25* | .22* | .16* | .14* | .54* | - | |||
15. Age 19-20 | .20* | .29* | .29* | .17* | .15* | .14* | .18* | .19* | .27* | .22* | .19* | .18* | .42* | .58* | - | ||
16. Age 21-22 | .20* | .24* | .22* | .25* | .17* | .15* | .13* | .19* | .22* | .25* | .18* | .16* | .37* | .50* | .66* | - | |
17. Age 23-24 | .20* | .28* | .22* | .20* | .25* | .21* | .12* | .15* | .19* | .17* | .19* | .15* | .36* | .48* | .62* | .69* | - |
18. Age 25-26 | .15* | .25* | .22* | .24* | .19* | .22* | .15* | .16* | .22* | .21* | .17* | .20* | .27* | .45* | .64* | .67* | .69* |
p < .05.
Table 3. Zero-Order Correlations between Personality and Substance Use.
Substance Use Variable | Impulsivity | Sensation Seeking | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
|
|||||||||||
Age 15-16 | Age 17-18 | Age 19-20 | Age 21-22 | Age 23-24 | Age 25-26 | Age 15-16 | Age 17-18 | Age 19-20 | Age 21-22 | Age 23-24 | Age 25-26 | |
Alcohol use | ||||||||||||
Age 15-16 | .15* | .11* | .11* | .08 | .09 | .07 | .19* | .09* | .12* | .00 | .04 | .16* |
Age 17-18 | .14* | .16* | .13* | .10* | .11* | .09 | .18* | .21* | .13* | .12* | .14* | .11 |
Age 19-20 | .12* | .12* | .14* | .08* | .10* | .13* | .22* | .21* | .27* | .11* | .18* | .12* |
Age 21-22 | .04* | .01 | .10* | .11* | .07 | .12* | .20* | .23* | .26* | .21* | .18* | .18* |
Age 23-24 | .00 | .03 | .10* | .08* | .10* | .15* | .16* | .16* | .22* | .15* | .22* | .24* |
Age 25-26 | .06 | .04 | .03 | .07 | .09* | .09* | .11* | .12* | .12* | .17* | .16* | .14* |
Marijuana use | ||||||||||||
Age 15-16 | .13* | .13* | .15* | .14* | .14* | .13* | .10* | .05 | .04 | .04 | .02 | .05 |
Age 17-18 | .11* | .14* | .12* | .13* | .13* | .18* | .08* | .07* | .07* | .01 | -.05 | .06 |
Age 19-20 | .09* | .12* | .17* | .09* | .05 | .19* | .11* | .08* | .14* | .09* | .03 | .12* |
Age 21-22 | .10* | .13* | .13* | .11* | .03 | .17* | .10* | .12* | .13* | .12* | .01 | .05 |
Age 23-24 | .05 | .05 | .09* | .06 | .14* | .13* | .09* | .03 | .02 | .09* | .10* | .07 |
Age 25-26 | .08* | .06 | .07 | .21* | .11* | .25* | .07* | -.01 | .04 | .09 | .05 | .07 |
Cigarette use | ||||||||||||
Age 15-16 | .15* | .09* | .13* | .14* | .04 | .05 | .13* | .04 | .08* | .06 | .07 | .15* |
Age 17-18 | .16* | .14* | .11* | .15* | .03 | .15* | .15* | .10* | .06* | .07* | .06 | .12* |
Age 19-20 | .16* | .15* | .14* | .17* | .03 | .16* | .13* | .11* | .10* | .09* | .09* | .13* |
Age 21-22 | .12* | .11* | .16* | .17* | .08* | .19* | .12* | .12* | .11* | .11* | .10* | .13* |
Age 23-24 | .10* | .09* | .16* | .14* | .12* | .18* | .13* | .07* | .07 | .10* | .10* | .14* |
Age 25-26 | .12* | .08* | .07 | .14* | .06 | .22* | .11* | .07* | .08 | .14* | .05 | .04 |
Note. Personality scores are means across the three items each for impulsivity and sensation seeking.
p < .05.
Analytic Approach
In order to analyze both intra-individual change across time and inter-individual differences in change, we estimated a series of latent growth curve models (LGMs; McArdle & Nesselroade, 2003; Meredith & Tisak, 1990) in Mplus version 5 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998-2007). We analyzed all models using full information maximum likelihood to account for missing data (Schafer & Graham, 2002) and additionally adjusted standard errors and model fit statistics for non-independence of data from participants born to the same mother (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2006). Following several recent studies (Brown, Catalano, Fleming, Haggerty, & Abbott, 2005; Flory et al., 2004; Littlefield et al., 2009), we modeled substance use frequency as continuous but entered the impulsivity and sensation seeking indicators as ordered categorical variables.
Data were analyzed in three steps. First, we tested for measurement invariance of impulsivity and sensation seeking across ages 15-26 (Meredith, Horn, Collins, & Sayer, 2001). Second, we fit linear and non-linear univariate models of change in each of the substance use outcomes and personality constructs in order to determine which model best represented the shape of change in each variable over time. Third, we tested for correlated changes among impulsivity, sensation seeking, and substance use in a series of three multivariate LGMs (one each for alcohol, marijuana, and cigarette use). To avoid redundancy, we report model parameters for the multivariate models only. Because the χ2 model fit test can be overly sensitive with large samples, we followed the recommendations of Kline (2005) in additionally using the CFI, TLI, and RMSEA to evaluate model fit. CFI and TLI values greater than .95 and RMSEA values less than .05 indicate good model fit. All models included adolescent gender and ethnicity, along with maternal SES, years of education, cognitive ability, age at first birth, delinquency, and depression, as exogenous covariates.
Results
Personality Measurement Invariance
An impulsivity model in which factor loadings and thresholds for each item were constrained to be equal across time (i.e., strong measurement invariance) fit the data well, χ2 (55) = 241.14, p < .001, CFI = .98, TLI = .94, RMSEA = .02. Permitting loadings, thresholds, and residual variances to vary across time separately or in conjunction significantly improved model fit as assessed by χ2 tests of difference (ps < .001). Given the sensitivity of this test in large sample sizes, however, we also examined change in other model fit indices. The CFI, TLI, and RMSEA values improved only marginally when parameters were unconstrained (maximum ΔCFI = .02, ΔTLI = .06, ΔRMSEA = .02). Further, inspection of parameter values suggested that measurement appeared relatively invariant across time. Standardized factor loadings for each item, for example, differed by less than 0.14. We therefore modeled impulsivity with strong measurement invariance in all subsequent analyses.4
The strong measurement invariance model for sensation seeking similarly fit the data well, χ2 (55) = 356.11, p < .001, CFI = .99, TLI = .97, RMSEA = .03. Although permitting parameters to vary across time improved χ2 model fit significantly (ps < .001), CFI, TLI, and RMSEA values again improved only marginally (maximum ΔCFI = .01, ΔTLI = .02, ΔRMSEA = .01). Inspection of the unconstrained models suggested that measurement was largely similar across time, with standardized factor loadings varying by less than .07. We again selected the strong measurement invariance model for all subsequent sensation seeking analyses.
Univariate Growth Curve Models and Demographic Predictors of Change
Our next analytic step was to model change over time in personality and substance use using univariate Latent Growth Curve Models (LGMs). LGMs serve the dual purpose of determining average patterns of change across time and estimating how much individuals' trajectories deviate from mean trends. A generic LGM equation for Y[t]n, the personality or substance use score for person n at age t can be expressed algebraically as follows (McArdle & Nesselroade, 2003):
In the LGM, yIn, the latent intercept score representing the initial level (i.e., at age 15-16), and ySn, the latent change score representing the magnitude of linear change over time, are assumed to be multivariate normal. A[t] is a vector of time-specific ‘basis’ coefficients, which represent the shape of change over time, and e[t]n is a vector of time-specific residual errors. Hypotheses regarding the shape of change across time can be tested by constraining the values in vector A[t]. In order to determine which growth curve best explained change in personality and substance use, we compared two models for each variable: a linear change model and a non-linear, ‘latent basis’ model. The non-linear model does not constrain the model to a specific trajectory shape. Rather, after setting the first two vector values to 0 and 1 for scaling and identification purposes, the model estimates the values of A[t] (i.e., the shape of change across time) from the observed data, permitting a non-linear estimated change trajectory that closely matches the shape of the observed data. In all models, we accounted for the effects of demographics by regressing the latent intercept and change factors onto exogenous covariates.
As shown in Figure 1, impulsivity, on average, descended linearly after age 15-16 before stabilizing in the early 20s. Sensation seeking, in contrast, descended most sharply following age 15-16, but then decreased gradually through age 25-26. These observed trends are consistent with previous literature suggesting that impulsivity declines linearly through adolescence before stabilizing in the early 20s (Casey et al., 2008; Steinberg, 2008; Steinberg et al., 2008). Consequently, we constrained the non-linear model of impulsivity to linear change through age 23-24 but permitted it to freely estimate the shape of change from 23-24 to 25-26. This model fit the data better than the linear model, χ2 (284) = 628.37, p < .001, CFI = .96, TLI = .96, RMSEA = .02, Δχ2 (1) = 4.51, p < .05. Similarly, we tested a non-linear LGM for developmental change in sensation seeking, which fit the data better than did the linear model, χ2 (281) = 504.17, p < .001, CFI = .99, TLI = .99, RMSEA = .01, Δχ2 (4) = 11.19, p < .05.
We next compared linear growth models to non-linear growth models for substance use. The non-linear models fit well for all three substances and were superior to the linear models for alcohol, χ2 (48) = 265.10, p < .001, CFI = .95, TLI = .92, RMSEA = .03, Δχ2 (4) = 248.33, p < .001, marijuana, χ2 (48) = 153.05, p < .001, CFI = .93, TLI = .90, RMSEA = .02, Δχ2 (4) = 26.80, p < .001, and cigarette use, χ2 (48) = 173.64, p < .001, CFI = .98, TLI = .97, RMSEA = .02, Δχ2 (4) = 256.21, p < .001. Figure 2 compares the observed means for substance use at each age (“Observed”) with the means implied by the non-linear model (“Predicted”). The non-linear models accurately estimated the mean-level trajectories for each substance. At the mean level, alcohol use increased sharply in adolescence before tapering off and peaking at age 23-24, whereas marijuana use increased gradually through age 23-24. On average, cigarette use increased sharply in the late teens and continued to increase through age 25-26.
These trends, however, belied variability in both levels of use at age 15-16 and trajectories of change through adolescence and early adulthood. Some individuals reported greater substance use than others at age 15-16, and some individuals experienced steeper non-linear increases in substance use from age 15-16 through early adulthood. Indeed, all change factor variances were significantly greater than zero, ps < .001. Figure 3 displays the variability in estimated substance use trajectories across a randomly selected subset of 500 individual participants.
Demographic predictors from univariate models
There were significant demographic differences in intercept and change factors from the univariate models (see Table 4). Female participants demonstrated faster decreases in impulsivity and sensation seeking, slower increases in alcohol, marijuana, and cigarette use, and lower initial levels of impulsivity, sensation seeking, and marijuana use. Relative to White participants, African-Americans increased their cigarette use more slowly and had lower initial levels of sensation seeking and alcohol and cigarette use, whereas Hispanic/Latino participants increased their cigarette use more slowly and demonstrated less frequent initial cigarette use but more frequent initial marijuana use.
Table 4. Associations between Demographic Covariates and Latent Growth Curve Factors from Univariate Models.
Demographic Variable | Impulsivity | Sensation Seeking | Alcohol Use | Marijuana Use | Cigarette Use | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||||||
Intercept | Change | Intercept | Change | Intercept | Change | Intercept | Change | Intercept | Change | |
Adolescent | ||||||||||
Female gender | -.12* | -.21* | -.12* | -.16* | .02 | -.32* | -.05* | -.17* | .02 | -.15* |
Black | -.01 | -.01 | -.27* | -.01 | -.23* | .00 | -.05 | .01 | -.26* | -.11* |
Hispanic | .02 | -.07 | -.01 | -.06 | .00 | -.03 | .07* | -.06 | -.11* | -.11* |
Maternal | ||||||||||
Income | -.03 | -.14 | .02 | .04 | .01 | .10* | -.06* | .03 | -.05* | -.07* |
Years of education | -.02 | .05 | .06* | -.02 | -.01 | .07* | .00 | .02 | .01 | .02 |
Cognitive ability | -.11* | -.04 | .03 | .00 | .05 | .09* | .04 | .04 | .00 | -.02 |
Earlier age at first birth | .08* | -.07 | -.03 | -.21* | .23* | -.22* | .16* | -.08* | .18* | .06* |
Delinquency | .07* | -.01 | .08* | -.05 | .05* | .02 | .08* | .02 | .05* | .03 |
Depression | .09* | .07 | .02 | .00 | .02 | .02 | .01 | .04 | .04* | .04 |
Note. Values are standardized regression coefficients.
p < .05.
Maternal socio-economic status predicted faster increases in alcohol use, slower increases in cigarette use, and lower initial levels of marijuana and cigarette use. Maternal education predicted faster increases in alcohol use and higher initial levels of sensation seeking. Finally, maternal cognitive ability predicted greater increases in alcohol use and lower initial impulsivity.
Maternal behavioral problems were generally associated with less-adaptive patterns of both personality and substance use. Early maternal age at first birth predicted faster increases in cigarette use, along with higher initial levels of impulsivity and alcohol, marijuana, and cigarette use. However, adolescents born to younger mothers decreased in sensation seeking more and increased in alcohol and marijuana use less. Initially, adolescents of more delinquent mothers were more impulsive and sensation seeking, and they used alcohol, marijuana and cigarettes more. Adolescents with more depressed mothers initially were more impulsive and smoked cigarettes more.
Multivariate Models of Change in Impulsivity, Sensation Seeking, and Substance Use
We estimated three multivariate LGMs to test whether changes in impulsivity and sensation seeking were associated with changes in use of each substance. In preliminary models, illustrated in Figure 4 Panel A, we permitted the intercept and change factors of impulsivity and sensation seeking to covary freely with each other and with the intercept and change factors of each of the three substance use outcomes. These associations tested whether impulsivity, sensation seeking, and each type of substance use were correlated at the beginning of the assessment period (correlated intercepts) and whether change in impulsivity, sensation seeking, and substance use was correlated across the assessment period (correlated change factors). Additionally, we permitted all change factors to covary with all intercept factors. These covariances tested whether initial values in one variable were associated with differing patterns of growth in the other.
A model in which all covariances among intercept and change factors are freely estimated is limited, however, because correlated changes are confounded by correlated intercept factors (Littlefield et al., 2010b). That is, given the common finding of an association between individuals' initial levels and their rates of change over time, the pattern of associations among initial levels of personality and substance use could obscure an independent association between change in personality and change in substance use. Therefore, after estimating the preliminary multivariate models, we estimated a set of models in which all change factors were regressed onto all intercept factors, permitting a test of correlated changes controlling for their associations with intercept factors. See Figure 4, Panel B.
Preliminary models
The impulsivity, sensation seeking, and alcohol use model fit well, χ2 (1123) = 2,478.94, p < .001, CFI = .97, TLI = .96, RMSEA = .02. As shown in Table 5, after accounting for the effects of demographic covariates, there was significant residual variance in the intercept and change factors for impulsivity, sensation seeking, and alcohol use. Table 6 presents correlations among intercept and change factors for impulsivity, sensation seeking, and alcohol use. Impulsivity and sensation seeking trajectories were closely aligned; the personality intercepts were positively and strongly associated, as were the personality change factors. The alcohol use intercept was also moderately associated with the impulsivity and sensation seeking intercepts. Most importantly, we found evidence for correlated changes in impulsivity and alcohol use, with a moderate association between the change factors. In contrast, change in sensation seeking was not significantly associated with change in alcohol use.
Table 5. Unstandardized Parameter Estimates from Multivariate Latent Growth Curve Models of Impulsivity, Sensation Seeking, and Substance Use.
Parameter | Alcohol Model | Marijuana Model | Cigarette Model |
---|---|---|---|
Estimate [95% C.I.] | Estimate [95% C.I.] | Estimate [95% C.I.] | |
Impulsivity | |||
Intercept-mean | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 |
Intercept-variance | 0.27* [0.22, 0.33] | 0.27* [0.22, 0.32] | 0.28* [0.22, 0.34] |
Change-mean | -0.04 [-0.15, 0.08] | -0.04 [-0.15, 0.08] | -0.04 [-0.15, 0.08] |
Change-variance | 0.01* [0.003, 0.02] | 0.01* [0.002, 0.02] | 0.01* [0.002, 0.02] |
Sensation seeking | |||
Intercept-mean | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 |
Intercept-variance | 1.11* [0.85, 1.38] | 1.09* [0.83, 1.36] | 1.10* [0.82, 1.39] |
Change-mean | -0.27 [-0.60, 0.06] | -0.27 [-0.59, 0.06] | -0.29 [-0.62, 0.05] |
Change-variance | 0.22* [0.04, 0.41] | 0.21* [0.03, 0.39] | 0.24* [0.04, 0.45] |
Substance use | |||
Intercept-mean | 3.70* [3.23, 4.18] | 0.72* [0.51, 0.94] | 1.75* [1.40, 2.10] |
Intercept-variance | 1.70* [1.43, 1.96] | 0.22* [0.20, 0.24] | 1.18* [0.96, 1.39] |
Change-mean | 0.32* [0.01, 0.63] | 0.04 [-0.07, 0.16] | 1.08* [0.75, 1.41] |
Change-variance | 0.41* [0.23, 0.59] | 0.04* [0.03, 0.06] | 0.49* [0.27, 0.72] |
Note. Because all models include demographic variables as exogenous covariates, reported variances are residual variances.
p < .05.
Table 6. Correlations among Intercept and Change Factors from Preliminary Multivariate Latent Growth Curve Models.
Factor | Impulsivity | Sensation Seeking | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
Intercept | Change | Intercept | Change | |
Impulsivity | ||||
Intercept | - | |||
Change | -.47* | - | ||
Sensation seeking | ||||
Intercept | .56* | -.39* | - | |
Change | -.37* | .67* | -.58* | - |
Alcohol use | ||||
Intercept | .40* | -.26* | .34* | -.23* |
Change | -.15* | .30* | -.03 | .14 |
Marijuana use | ||||
Intercept | .32* | .01 | .18* | -.13 |
Change | -.04 | .09 | .06 | .02 |
Cigarette use | ||||
Intercept | .25* | -.13 | .15* | -.11 |
Change | .09* | .11 | .08 | -.01 |
Note. Correlations between impulsivity and sensation seeking factors are taken from alcohol use models. Correlations from other substance use models did not differ from those reported here in significance and differed in magnitude by at most .03. Intercept-change correlations for alcohol use, marijuana use, and cigarette use were r = -.48, p < .001, r = -.33, p < .001, and r = -.27, p < .05, respectively.
p < .05.
The preliminary marijuana use model also fit the data well, χ2 (1123) = 2,581.52, p < .001, CFI = .96, TLI = .96, RMSEA = .02, and there were significant individual differences in the impulsivity, sensation seeking, and marijuana use intercept and change factors. There were small-to-moderate positive associations between the marijuana use intercept and the impulsivity and sensation seeking intercepts. However, change in marijuana use was not significantly associated with change in either impulsivity or sensation seeking.
Finally, the preliminary cigarette use model fit the data well, χ2 (1123) = 2,338.05, p < .001, CFI = .97, TLI = .96, RMSEA = .01, and intercepts and changes in impulsivity, sensation seeking, and cigarette use varied significantly between individuals after accounting for demographics. We again found small-to-moderate-sized correlations among impulsivity, sensation seeking, and cigarette use intercepts. However, change in cigarette use was not associated with change in either personality facet.
Models controlling for initial levels of personality and substance use
We next repeated the above three multivariate LGMs with all change factors regressed onto all intercept factors (see Figure 4, Panel B). Results of these models replicated key findings from the preliminary models, including significant residual variances in intercept and change factors for all constructs. Similar to the models above, we found significant, large correlations between the impulsivity and sensation seeking intercept and change factors. Most importantly, the alcohol use model provided evidence of correlated changes in alcohol use and personality; individuals who decreased less steeply in impulsivity and sensation seeking increased more steeply in alcohol use. We also found moderate-to-large associations among intercept factors for alcohol use, impulsivity, and sensation seeking. Beyond these correlations, greater initial levels of sensation seeking predicted steeper increases in alcohol use, but there were no other associations across personality and alcohol use intercept and change factors. See Table 7.
Table 7. Factor Correlations and Regression Coefficients from Multivariate Latent Growth Curve Models Controlling for Initial Levels of Impulsivity, Sensation Seeking, and Substance Use.
Factor | Impulsivity | Sensation Seeking | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
Intercept | Change | Intercept | Change | |
Impulsivity | ||||
Intercept | - | - | ||
Change | -.34* | - | ||
Sensation seeking | ||||
Intercept | .56* | -.19 | - | - |
Change | -.06 | .61* | -.55* | - |
Alcohol use | ||||
Intercept | .40* | -.06 | .34* | -.02 |
Change | -.04 | .28* | .16* | .15* |
Marijuana use | ||||
Intercept | .32* | .17 | .18* | -.02 |
Change | .02 | .17* | .12* | .05 |
Cigarette use | ||||
Intercept | .25* | -.02 | .15* | -.01 |
Change | .14* | .18* | .05 | .05 |
Note. Values are correlation coefficients (for correlated intercepts and changes) and standardized regression coefficients (for all other associations). Associations between impulsivity and sensation seeking factors are taken from alcohol use models; associations from other substance use models did not differ from those reported here in significance and differed in magnitude by .07 at most. Intercept-change associations for alcohol use, marijuana use, and cigarette use were β = -.48, p < .001, β = -.36, p < .001, and β = -.32, p < .001, respectively.
p < .05.
In the marijuana use model, we found a small-to-moderate correlation between change in marijuana use and change in impulsivity but not change in sensation seeking. Both personality facet intercepts were also positively associated with the marijuana use intercept. Additionally, more sensation-seeking adolescents at the intercept increased their marijuana use to a greater extent, but there were no other associations across personality and marijuana use intercept and change factors.
Finally, in the cigarette use model, we found evidence of correlated changes in cigarette use and impulsivity but not sensation seeking. In addition, there were small-to-moderate associations between the cigarette intercept and the impulsivity and sensation seeking intercepts. Adolescents higher in impulsivity at the intercept additionally increased their cigarette use more, but there were no other associations across personality and cigarette use intercept and change factors.
Additional Analyses: Frequency of Substance Use among Non-Abstainers
Given the large number of abstainers included in the analyses presented here, it is possible that our findings reflect an effect on the timing of substance use initiation use rather than its progression. That is, rather than demonstrating correlated change with emerging and increasing use, change in impulsivity and sensation seeking may correspond with when adolescents first initiate substance use. In order to examine this possibility, we repeated our analyses with abstainers coded as missing. In these models, we largely replicated our substantive findings in both significance and magnitude. Briefly, among non-abstainers, there was significant variability in rates of change in alcohol, marijuana, and cigarette use, and we found correlated intercepts for all substances and both personality constructs. It is notable, however, that we found correlated changes between impulsivity and sensation seeking and between impulsivity and alcohol use but not in any other personality-substance use pair. In sum, whereas slower decreases in impulsivity may predict greater increases in alcohol use, they may be more strongly associated with the timing of marijuana and cigarette initiation than with the escalation of use. The complete results of the additional analyses are available upon request from the first author.
Discussion
This paper presented four key findings. First, consistent with an emerging body of research on personality change, there were significant individual differences in rates of age-related change in impulsivity and sensation seeking across adolescence and early adulthood. Second, consistent with previous longitudinal research (Harden & Tucker-Drob, 2011), we found evidence that impulsivity and sensation seeking develop as separate but related facets of personality. Although the two facets were distinct, declines in each were positively associated. Third, we found variability in rates of change in alcohol, marijuana, and cigarette use. This variability was partly explained by demographic differences, but significant individual differences in change remained after accounting for demographics and maternal behavioral problems. Finally and most important, even after accounting for demographics, mothers' behavior problems, and cross-sectional associations at age 15-16, we found substantial support for correlated changes in personality and substance use: Slower decreases in impulsivity were associated with greater increases in alcohol, marijuana, and cigarette use after accounting for initial levels of impulsivity and substance use. Similarly, a slower decrease in sensation seeking was associated with a greater increase in alcohol use. These correlations in rates of change were all small-to-moderate in magnitude. Additional analyses suggested that whereas impulsivity change was associated with change in the frequency of alcohol use, the other personality-substance use correlated changes may have reflected associations with the timing of substance use initiation.
The current findings expand upon recent models of the role of personality change in the etiology of substance use (Casey et al., 2008; Steinberg, 2008). Whereas historically personality has been considered as a stable individual difference risk factor (e.g., Flory et al., 2004; Roberts et al., 2009; Sher et al., 2000; Sher & Trull, 1994), contemporary models have incorporated mean differences between adolescents and adults in levels of impulsivity and sensation seeking. Consistent with this largely cross-sectional literature, we found that, on average, impulsivity and sensation seeking declined from adolescence to adulthood. Moreover, individual differences in change in impulsivity and—to a lesser extent—sensation seeking were moderately associated with individual differences in escalating substance use from mid-adolescence to early adulthood. These results suggest that there is meaningful heterogeneity in rates of change in these personality facets and that this heterogeneity may help explain variability in trajectories of increasing substance use. By integrating inter-individual differences in intra-individual developmental changes, these results expand upon etiological models derived from mean-level changes. Although adolescence and early adulthood are generally times of increased risk for the initiation and progression of substance use, this risk may be most evident among those who decline slowly in impulsivity.
It is also important to note that, although the period during which sensation seeking peaked (i.e., mid-adolescence) aligned with rising levels of substance use, the highest rates of use in this sample occurred later in early adulthood. That is, substance use was most common in the years following what has been proposed as the mean-level peak of personality risk (Steinberg, 2008, 2010). The current results suggest that a failure to decline in impulsivity and—to a lesser extent—sensation seeking may be responsible for this continued growth in substance use. Maintaining high levels of impulsivity through the end of adolescence may be particularly problematic in light of the many environmental transitions that characterize the transition to adulthood. For many adolescents, the completion of high school is followed by a departure from the parental home, reducing parental monitoring and support (Wetherill & Fromme, 2007). Over half of high school graduates enter college (Johnston et al., 2009a), and many enter the workforce and establish romantic partnerships. Across these transitions, a common theme is the increasing need for self-regulation in the pursuit of long-term goals. We speculate that as parental and familial influences fade in early adulthood (Arnett, 2000), those who decline more gradually with regards to impulsivity or sensation seeking may become increasingly susceptible to peer influences and personal temptations, and they may initiate or escalate their substance use.
The current results demonstrate that models that explain the emergence of substance use and other risk-taking behaviors in terms of personality developmental would benefit from further consideration of the role of individual differences in personality change. Additionally, disentangling change in impulsivity and sensation seeking from post-adolescent changes in social roles will be an important goal for future research on these models. Previous research has implicated, in particular, the transitions to college and marriage as important for the progression of substance use, and it is likely that personality change is relevant to these role changes as well (Bachman et al., 2002; Bachman et al., 1997; Neyer & Lehnart, 2007; Roberts & Bogg, 2004; Scollon & Diener, 2006). There are at least three pathways by which the association between personality change and substance use change may dovetail with post-adolescent social role transitions. First, role transitions may mediate the association between personality change and substance use via environment selection. Those early adults who decline in impulsivity more rapidly may be more likely, for example, to successfully maintain romantic relationships and establish long-term partnerships, which may then promote pro-social behavior and decreased substance use. Second, role socialization may affect change in personality, which might in turn influence substance use. Third, social role transitions may serve as third-variable confounds. The transition to adulthood may impact both impulsivity and substance use, meaning that the observed association between changes in impulsivity and substance use would be spurious. Distinguishing among these partially competing hypotheses will be an important undertaking and may require the use of quasi-experimental research designs. In particular, within-family studies, which can help rule out selection on the basis of family background factors, would be ideal for helping to distinguish among the selection, socialization, and confounding pathways (Johnson, Turkheimer, Gottesman, & Bouchard, 2009; Rutter, Pickles, Murray, & Eaves, 2001).
In interpreting the current findings, an important consideration is that our analyses cannot definitely establish the direction of the longitudinal associations. Alternative explanation for our findings is that there may be transactional relations between substance use and personality change (Cheong, MacKinnon, & Khoo, 2003). Just as changes in impulsivity and sensation seeking might influence the emergence of substance use, the use of substances might also influence trajectories of change in personality. Quinn and colleagues (2011) recently demonstrated that heavier drinking during the college years predicted increases in both impulsivity and sensation seeking. Similarly, Roberts and Bogg (2004) found that marijuana use among adult women predicted change in the Big Five conscientiousness facet of social responsibility (but see also Littlefield, Vergés, Wood, & Sher, in press). There is a need for future longitudinal research that examines reciprocal relations between personality and substance use.
Limitations and Future Directions
This investigation shares a number of strengths and weaknesses with other studies conducted using large, publicly available datasets. A key strength of the current study was the accelerated longitudinal design, which permitted the inclusion of longitudinal assessments spanning over a decade of adolescence and early adulthood. Previous investigations (e.g., Littlefield et al., 2009) have identified the possibility of correlated changes in personality and substance use, but our findings capture the critical developmental period in which substance use emerges and peaks in prevalence. Furthermore, our analyses included a large, demographically and geographically diverse sample, which permitted us to assess the influence of demographic variables and maternal behavioral problems.
As a consequence of its scope, however, the CNLSY includes non-trivial missing data due to assessment timing, participation refusal, and study attrition. To address this limitation, we used full information maximum likelihood estimation procedures and included a breadth of maternal demographic and psychosocial covariates that predict study non-participation. Missing data and the oversample of African-American and Hispanic/Latino mothers in the NLSY79 mean that the CNLSY sample cannot be considered perfectly representative of the U.S. population. Nevertheless, it is a larger and more diverse sample than is represented in many published analyses of personality change.
A related limitation of the breadth of the CNSLY survey was the relatively brief measurement of key variables. Impulsivity and sensation seeking were assessed using three items each, and we assessed alcohol, marijuana, and cigarettes using single Likert-type frequency scales. Additionally, we found some evidence suggesting possible measurement variance in personality. Variability in measurement may limit conclusions about change over time, although the alternative fit indices suggested that any measurement variance was minor. Nevertheless, replication of these findings with stronger measures is needed.
Some recent factor-analytic studies suggest that dispositional impulsivity can be disaggregated into four distinct—but interrelated—constructs: lack of premeditation, lack of perseverance, and positive and negative urgency (Cyders, Flory, Rainer, & Smith, 2009; Cyders & Smith, 2008; Smith et al., 2007; Whiteside & Lynam, 2001). The measure of impulsivity used here shares features with the two facets most strongly associated with substance use and abuse (Lynam & Miller, 2004; Magid & Colder, 2007; Miller, Flory, Lynam, & Leukefeld, 2003; Smith et al., 2007): lack of premeditation (i.e., acting without thinking; Whiteside & Lynam, 2003) and urgency (i.e., “the tendency to commit rash or regrettable actions as a result of intense negative affect”; Whiteside & Lynam, 2001, p. 677). However, we could not distinguish among the four impulsivity facets in this study. Similarly, some models have distinguished among four facets (experience seeking, thrill and adventure seeking, boredom susceptibility, and disinhibition) of sensation seeking (Roberti, Storch, & Bravata, 2003; Rowland & Franken, 1986; Zuckerman, 1994). Our measure of sensation seeking appears most similar to measures of the disinhibition and boredom susceptibility facets, although we note that other factor-analytic studies have found evidence for a unidimensional sensation seeking construct (Whiteside & Lynam, 2001).
Importantly, the mean age-trends in impulsivity, sensation seeking, and alcohol, marijuana, and cigarette use reported here are quite similar, if not identical, to those in other samples using more comprehensive measures (e.g., Bachman et al., 1997; Johnston et al., 2009a; Steinberg et al., 2008 but see Romer & Hennessy, 2007, for a somewhat later peak in sensation seeking among men). Furthermore, demographic differences, such as the lower levels of impulsivity and sensation seeking among female participants and lower levels of alcohol use among African-Americans, are also consistent with previous research (Bachman et al., 1997; Duckworth & Seligman, 2006; Flory et al., 2004; Paschall, Bersamin, & Flewelling, 2005; Romer & Hennessy, 2007). These similarities increase our confidence in both the validity of the measures and the generalizability of the sample. However, further research with comprehensive measures of personality and substance use and related problems and a nationally representative sample would provide even stronger support for our conclusions. Although beyond the scope of the present investigation, future research should also attempt to determine whether the current results are moderated by demographic variables, including gender, and ethnicity.
Finally, we modeled abstinence as the lower end of a continuous substance use dimension. Previous research has identified differing risk factors for initiation and progression of substance use, suggesting that each may result from distinct etiological processes, with initiation in particular reflecting shared environmental rather than genetic influences (Heath, Meyer, Jardine, & Martin, 1991). Abstainers comprised a large proportion of our sample, and we replicated our results for impulsivity and alcohol but not marijuana or cigarette use when examining changes in frequency of use, suggesting that correlated changes in impulsivity and marijuana and cigarette use may reflect the timing of initiation. Because our investigation began at age 15-16 and therefore could not capture substance use initiation for all participants, however, further research is needed to determine the relative role of personality change in initiation versus progression of substance use.
Conclusions
Evidence accumulating over the past several decades has identified alcohol and other substance use as primarily a problem of youth and young adults (e.g., Sher & Gotham, 1999). More recently, emerging theories of change in personality and neurobiology have begun identifying the developmental processes that may underlie adolescents' increased propensity for substance use and other forms of risk-taking. The present findings are among the first to provide longitudinal evidence that age-related changes in impulsivity and—to a lesser extent—sensation seeking may help account for variability in the etiology of substance use: Youth who decline later or more slowly in impulsivity escalate their substance use more rapidly. Although further evidence is needed to identify the specific mechanisms through which intra-individual changes in impulsivity and substance use are associated, this investigation demonstrated the value of using longitudinal methods to understand the role of personality development in the etiology of substance use.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Grant T32-AA007471 and the Waggoner Center for Alcohol and Addiction Research. K. Paige Harden is a Faculty Research Associate of the Population Research Center at the University of Texas at Austin, which is supported by a center grant from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (R24-HD042849).
Footnotes
Additional samples of youth in the military and economically disadvantaged Whites were also initiated in 1979 but were discontinued between 1984 and 1990 because of budget limitations.
At baseline (ages 15-16), based on birth cohorts to which baseline interviews were administered (i.e., the 1978-1991 birth cohorts only), participants with missing data differed with regards to maternal demographic variables, including younger maternal age at first birth (20.3 years versus 21.6 years, t = 7.86, p < .001); lower maternal cognitive ability (34.3 versus 26.0. t = 7.58, p <.01); lower family income (137.0 versus 156.8, t = 7.22, p <.01); and lower maternal education (13.0 versus 13.4 years, t = 4.31, p < .01), but did not differ with regards to maternal depressive symptoms or maternal delinquency.
Attrition from baseline to ages 17-18 through ages 25-26 (based on the 1978-1981 birth cohorts, who were administered interviews across all age groups) was not associated with baseline impulsivity; baseline sensation seeking; or maternal age at first birth, cognitive ability, family income, depressive symptoms, or delinquency. Attrited participants at ages 17-18 were less likely to be African-American (30% versus 47%); child race/ethnicity was not associated with subsequent attrition at later ages. Attrition at ages 19-20 was associated with lower maternal education (12.2 versus 12.8 years, t = 2.70, p < .01); maternal education was not associated with subsequent attrition at later ages.
Tests of measurement invariance in impulsivity revealed relatively modest but statistically significant variance in each measurement component. Freeing residual variances, χ2 (15) = 107.68, p < .001, factor loadings, χ2 (15) = 69.64, p < .001, and item thresholds, χ2 (30) = 157.46, p < .001, each incrementally improved model fit, although there was no readily identifiable pattern of change in the measurement model as a function of participant age. Similarly, freeing residual variances, χ2 (15) = 171.22, p < .001, factor loadings, χ2 (15) = 63.51, p < .001, and item thresholds, χ2 (30) = 314.32, p < .001, each incrementally but modestly improved the fit of the sensation seeking measurement model. Again, however, measurement did not appear to change over time in a discernible pattern.
References
- Arnett JJ. Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist. 2000;55:469–480. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asparouhov T, Muthén B. Comparison of estimatation methods for complex survey data analysis. Mplus Technical Appendices. 2006 Available from http://www.statmodel.com/resrchpap.shtml.
- Bachman JG, O'Malley PM, Schulenberg JE, Johnston LD, Bryant AL, Merline AC. The decline of substance use in young adulthood: Changes in social activities, roles, and beliefs. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Bachman JG, Wadsworth KN, O'Malley PM, Johnston LD. Smoking, drinking, and drug use in young adulthood: The impacts of new freedoms and new responsibilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Block J, Block JH. Venturing a 30-year longitudinal study. American Psychologist. 2006;61:315–327. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.61.4.315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown EC, Catalano RF, Fleming CB, Haggerty KP, Abbott RD. Adolescent substance use outcomes in the Raising Healthy Children Project: A two-part latent growth curve analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2005;73:699–710. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.4.699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casey BJ, Getz S, Galvan A. The adolescent brain. Developmental Review. 2008;28:62–77. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caspi A, Roberts BW, Shiner RL. Personality development: Stability and change. Annual Review of Psychology. 2005;56:453–484. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cauffman E, Shulman EP, Steinberg L, Claus E, Banich MT, Graham S, et al. Age differences in affective decision making as indexed by performance on the Iowa Gambling Task. Developmental Psychology. 2010;46:193–207. doi: 10.1037/a0016128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chase-Lansdale PL, Mott FL, Brooks-Gunn J, Phillips DA. Children of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth: A unique research opportunity. Developmental Psychology. 1991;27:918–931. [Google Scholar]
- Chassin L, Pitts SC, Prost J. Binge drinking trajectories from adolescence to emerging adulthood in a high-risk sample: Predictors and substance abuse outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70:67–78. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheong J, MacKinnon DP, Khoo ST. Investigation of mediational processes using parallel process latent growth curve modeling. Structural Equation Modeling. 2003;10:238–262. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM1002_5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cloninger CR, Przybeck TR, Svrakic DM. The Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire: U.S. normative data. Psychological Reports. 1991;69:1047–1057. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1991.69.3.1047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Compton WM, Grant BF, Colliver JD, Glantz MD, Stinson FS. Prevalence of marijuana use disorders in the United States: 1991-1992 and 2001-2002. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2004;291:2114–2121. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.17.2114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper ML, Wood PK, Orcutt HK, Albino A. Personality and the predisposition to engage in risky or problem behaviors during adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;84:390–410. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.84.2.390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crawford AM, Pentz MA, Chou CP, Li C, Dwyer JH. Parallel developmental trajectories of sensation seeking and regular substance use in adolescents. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2003;17:179–192. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.17.3.179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cross CP, Copping LT, Campbell A. Sex differences in impulsivity: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2011;137:97–130. doi: 10.1037/a0021591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cyders MA, Flory K, Rainer S, Smith GT. The role of personality dispositions to risky behavior in predicting first-year college drinking. Addiction. 2009;104:193–202. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02434.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cyders MA, Smith GT. Emotion-based dispositions to rash action: Positive and negative urgency. Psychological Bulletin. 2008;134:807–828. doi: 10.1037/a0013341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D'Onofrio BM, Van Hulle CA, Waldman ID, Rodgers JL, Harden KP, Rathouz PJ, et al. Smoking during pregnancy and offspring externalizing problems: An exploration of genetic and environmental confounds. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20:139–164. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dawe S, Gullo MJ, Loxton NJ. Reward drive and rash impulsiveness as dimensions of impulsivity: Implications for substance misuse. Addictive Behaviors. 2004;29:1389–1405. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dick DM, Smith GT, Olausson P, Mitchell SH, Leeman RF, O'Malley SS, et al. Understanding the construct of impulsivity and its relationship to alcohol use disorders. Addiction Biology. 2010;15:217–226. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2009.00190.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donnellan MB, Conger RD, Burzette RG. Personality development from late adolescence to young adulthood: Differential stability, normative maturity, and evidence for the maturity-stability hypothesis. Journal of Personality. 2007;75:237–263. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00438.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duckworth AL, Kern ML. A meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control measures. Journal of Research in Personality. 2011;45:259–268. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duckworth AL, Seligman MEP. Self-discipline gives girls the edge: Gender in self-discipline, grades, and achievement test scores. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2006;98:198–208. [Google Scholar]
- Elkins IJ, King SM, McGue M, Iacono WG. Personality traits and the development of nicotine, alcohol, and illicit drug disorders: Prospective links from adolescence to young adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2006;115:26–39. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.1.26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott DS, Huizinga D. Social class and delinquent behavior in a national youth panel. Criminology: An Interdisciplinary Journal. 1983;21:149–177. [Google Scholar]
- Flory K, Lynam D, Milich R, Leukefeld C, Clayton R. Early adolescent through young adult alcohol and marijuana use trajectories: Early predictors, young adult outcomes, and predictive utility. Development and Psychopathology. 2004;16:193–213. doi: 10.1017/s0954579404044475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant BF, Dawson DA, Stinson FS, Chou SP, Dufour MC, Pickering RP. The 12-month prevalence and trends in DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence: United States, 1991-1992 and 2001-2002. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2004;74:223–234. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gullo MJ, Ward E, Dawe S, Powell J, Jackson CJ. Support for a two-factor model of impulsivity and hazardous substance use in British and Australian young adults. Journal of Research in Personality. 2011;45:10–18. [Google Scholar]
- Harden KP, D'Onofrio BM, Van Hulle C, Turkheimer E, Rodgers JL, Waldman ID, et al. Population density and youth antisocial behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2009;50:999–1008. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02044.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harden KP, Lynch SK, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, D'Onofrio BM, Slutske WS, et al. A behavior genetic investigation of adolescent motherhood and offspring mental health problems. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2007;116:667–683. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.4.667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harden KP, Tucker-Drob EM. Individual differences in the development of sensation seeking and impulsivity during adolescence: Further evidence for a dual systems model. Developmental Psychology. 2011;47:739–746. doi: 10.1037/a0023279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heath AC, Meyer J, Jardine R, Martin NG. The inheritance of alcohol consumption patterns in a general population twin sample: II. Determinants of consumption frequency and quantity consumed. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1991;52:425–433. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1991.52.425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hittner JB, Swickert R. Sensation seeking and alcohol use: A meta-analytic review. Addictive Behaviors. 2006;31:1383–1401. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iacono WG, Malone SM, McGue M. Behavioral disinhibition and the development of early-onset addiction: Common and specific influences. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2008;4:325–348. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.4.022007.141157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson W, Turkheimer E, Gottesman II, Bouchard TJ., Jr Beyond heritability: Twin studies in behavioral research. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2009;18:217–220. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01639.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnston LD, O'Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg J. Monitoring the Future: National survey results on drug use, 1975-2008. Vol. 2. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse; 2009a. College students and adults ages 19-50. [Google Scholar]
- Johnston LD, O'Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg J. Monitoring the Future: National survey results on drug use, 1975-2008. Vol. 1. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse; 2009b. Secondary School Students. [Google Scholar]
- Kirby KN, Finch JC. The hierarchical structure of self-reported impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences. 2010;48:704–713. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.01.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Krueger RF, Hicks BM, Patrick CJ, Carlson SR, Iacono WG, McGue M. Etiologic connections among substance dependence, antisocial behavior and personality: Modeling the externalizing spectrum. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;111:411–424. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li TK, Hewitt BG, Grant BF. Alcohol use disorders and mood disorders: A National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism perspective. Biological Psychiatry. 2004;56:718–720. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Littlefield AK, Sher KJ, Wood PK. Is “maturing out” of problematic alcohol involvement related to personality change? Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2009;118:360–374. doi: 10.1037/a0015125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Littlefield AK, Sher KJ, Wood PK. Do changes in drinking motives mediate the relation between personality change and “maturing out” of problem drinking? Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2010a;119:93–105. doi: 10.1037/a0017512. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Littlefield AK, Sher KJ, Wood PK. A personality-based description of maturing out of alcohol problems: Extension with a Five-Factor model and robustness to modeling challenges. Addictive Behaviors. 2010b;35:948–954. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.06.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Littlefield AK, Vergés A, Wood PK, Sher KJ. Transactional models between personality and alcohol involvement: A further examination. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. doi: 10.1037/a0026912. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynam DR, Miller JD. Personality pathways to impulsive behavior and their relations to deviance: Results from three samples. Journal of Quantitative Criminology. 2004;20:319–341. [Google Scholar]
- Magid V, Colder CR. The UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale: Factor structure and associations with college drinking. Personality and Individual Differences. 2007;43:1927–1937. [Google Scholar]
- Magid V, MacLean MG, Colder CR. Differentiating between sensation seeking and impulsivity through their mediated relations with alcohol use and problems. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32:2046–2061. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.01.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin CA, Kelly TH, Rayens MK, Brogli BR, Brenzel A, Smith WJ, et al. Sensation seeking, puberty and nicotine, alcohol and marijuana use in adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2002;41:1495–1502. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200212000-00022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McArdle JJ, Nesselroade JR. Growth curve analysis in contemporary psychological research. In: Schinka J, Velicer W, editors. Comprehensive handbook of psychology, volume two: Research methods in psychology. New York: Wiley; 2003. pp. 447–480. [Google Scholar]
- McCrae RR, Costa PT. The stability of personality: Observation and evaluations. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 1994;3:173–175. [Google Scholar]
- Mendle J, Harden KP, Turkheimer E, Van Hulle CA, D'Onofrio BM, Brooks-Gunn J, et al. Associations between father absence and age of first sexual intercourse. Child Development. 2009;80:1463–1480. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01345.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meredith W, Horn J, Collins LM, Sayer AG. New methods for the analysis of change. Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association; 2001. The role of factorial invariance in modeling growth and change; pp. 203–240. [Google Scholar]
- Meredith W, Tisak J. Latent curve analysis. Psychometrika. 1990;55:107–122. [Google Scholar]
- Miller J, Flory K, Lynam D, Leukefeld C. A test of the four-factor model of impulsivity-related traits. Personality and Individual Differences. 2003;34:1403–1418. [Google Scholar]
- Mroczek DK, Spiro A. Personality change influences mortality in older men. Psychological Science. 2007;18:371–376. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01907.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén LK, Muthén BO. MPlus user's guide. 5th. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén; 1998-2007. [Google Scholar]
- Neyer FJ, Lehnart J. Relationships matter in personality development: Evidence from an 8-year longitudinal study across young adulthood. Journal of Personality. 2007;75:535–568. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00448.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paschall MJ, Bersamin M, Flewelling RL. Racial/Ethnic differences in the association between college attendance and heavy alcohol use: A national study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2005;66:266–274. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quinn PD, Stappenbeck CA, Fromme K. Collegiate heavy drinking prospectively predicts change in sensation seeking and impulsivity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2011;120:543–556. doi: 10.1037/a0023159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
- Roberti JW, Storch EA, Bravata E. Further psychometric support for the Sensation Seeking Scale-Form V. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2003;81:291–292. doi: 10.1207/S15327752JPA8103_12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts BW, Bogg T. A longitudinal study of the relationships between conscientiousness and the social- environmental factors and substance-use behaviors that influence health. Journal of Personality. 2004;72:325–353. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00264.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts BW, DelVecchio WF. The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: A quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin. 2000;126:3–25. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts BW, Jackson JJ, Burger J, Trautwein U. Conscientiousness and externalizing psychopathology: Overlap, developmental patterns, and etiology of two related constructs. Development and Psychopathology. 2009;21:871–888. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts BW, Mroczek D. Personality trait change in adulthood. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2008;17:31–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00543.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts BW, Walton K, Bogg T, Caspi A. De-investment in work and non-normative personality trait change in young adulthood. European Journal of Personality. 2006;20:461–474. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts BW, Walton KE, Viechtbauer W. Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin. 2006;132:1–25. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romer D. Adolescent risk taking, impulsivity, and brain development: Implications for prevention. Developmental Psychobiology. 2010;52:263–276. doi: 10.1002/dev.20442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romer D, Hennessy M. A biosocial-affect model of adolescent sensation seeking: The role of affect evaluation and peer-group influence in adolescent drug use. Prevention Science. 2007;8:89–101. doi: 10.1007/s11121-007-0064-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowland GL, Franken RE. The four dimensions of sensation seeking: a confirmatory factor analysis. Personality and Individual Differences. 1986;7:237–240. [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M, Pickles A, Murray R, Eaves L. Testing hypotheses on specific environmental causal effects on behavior. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127:291–324. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.3.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schafer JL, Graham JW. Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods. 2002;7:147–177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scollon CN, Diener E. Love, work, and changes in extraversion and neuroticism over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2006;91:1152–1165. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.6.1152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sher KJ, Bartholow BD, Wood MD. Personality and substance use disorders: A prospective study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2000;68:818–829. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sher KJ, Gotham HJ. Pathological alcohol involvement: A developmental disorder of young adulthood. Development and Psychopathology. 1999;11:933–956. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499002394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sher KJ, Trull TJ. Personality and disinhibitory psychopathology: Alcoholism and antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1994;103:92–102. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.103.1.92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sher KJ, Trull TJ, Bartholow BD, Vieth A. Personality and alcoholism: Issues, methods, and etiological processes. In: Leonard KE, Blane HT, editors. Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism. 2nd. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. pp. 54–105. [Google Scholar]
- Smith GT, Fischer S, Cyders MA, Annus AM, Spillane NS, McCarthy DM. On the validity and utility of discriminating among impulsivity-like traits. Assessment. 2007;14:155–170. doi: 10.1177/1073191106295527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Somerville LH, Jones RM, Casey BJ. A time of change: Behavioral and neural correlates of adolescent sensitivity to appetitive and aversive environmental cues. Brain and Cognition. 2010;72:124–133. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg L. A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review. 2008;28:78–106. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg L. A dual systems model of adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Psychobiology. 2010;52:216–224. doi: 10.1002/dev.20445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg L, Albert D, Cauffman E, Banich M, Graham S, Woolard J. Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: Evidence for a dual systems model. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44:1764–1778. doi: 10.1037/a0012955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg L, Graham S, O'Brien L, Woolard J, Cauffman E, Banich M. Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Development. 2009;80:28–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01244.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turley RNL. Are children of young mothers disadvantaged because of their mother's age or family background? Child Development. 2003;74:465–474. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.7402010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaidya JG, Gray EK, Haig J, Watson D. On the temporal stability of personality: Evidence for differential stability and the role of life experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2002;83:1469–1484. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaidya JG, Gray EK, Haig JR, Mroczek DK, Watson D. Differential stability and individual growth trajectories of big five and affective traits during young adulthood. Journal of Personality. 2008;76:267–304. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00486.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaidya JG, Latzman RD, Markon KE, Watson D. Age differences on measures of disinhibition during young adulthood. Personality and Individual Differences. 2010;48:815–820. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler H, Dowdall GW, Maenner G, Gledhill-Hoyt J, Lee H. Changes in binge drinking and related problems among American college students between 1993 and 1997. Journal of American College Health. 1998;47:57–68. doi: 10.1080/07448489809595621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wetherill RR, Fromme K. Perceived awareness and caring influences alcohol use by high school and college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2007;21:147–154. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.21.2.147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whiteside SP, Lynam DR. The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001;30:669–689. [Google Scholar]
- Whiteside SP, Lynam DR. Understanding the role of impulsivity and externalizing psychopathology in alcohol abuse: Application of the UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2003;11:210–217. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.11.3.210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zucker RA, Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ. Developmental psychopathology, Vol 3: Risk, disorder, and adaptation. 2nd. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons Inc; 2006. Alcohol use and the alcohol use disorders: A developmental-biopsychosocial systems formulation covering the life course; pp. 620–656. [Google Scholar]
- Zuckerman M. Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Zuckerman M, Kuhlman DM, Joireman J, Teta P, Kraft M. A comparison of three structural models for personality: The Big Three, the Big Five, and the Alternative Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1993;65:757–768. [Google Scholar]