Abstract
The effect of prenatal cocaine exposure (PCE) on externalizing behavior and substance use related problems at 15 years of age was examined. Participants consisted of 358 adolescents (183 PCE, 175 non-cocaine exposed (NCE)), primarily African-American and of low socioeconomic status, prospectively enrolled in a longitudinal study from birth. Regression analyses indicated that the amount of PCE was associated with higher externalizing behavioral problems (β=.15, p=.02). Adolescents with PCE were also 2.8 times (95% CI=1.38–5.56) more likely to have substance use related problems than their NCE counterparts. No differences between PCE adolescents in non-kinship adoptive or foster care (n=44) and PCE adolescents in maternal/relative care (n=139) were found in externalizing behavior or in the likelihood of substance use related problems. Findings demonstrate teratologic effects of PCE persisting into adolescence. PCE is a reliable marker for the potential development of problem behaviors in adolescence, including substance use related problems.
Keywords: Prenatal cocaine exposure, externalizing behavior, substance use, parental monitoring, violence exposure
Maternal substance use during pregnancy continues to be a serious public health problem, with approximately 200,000 infants exposed to illicit drugs in utero each year in the United States (SAMHSA, 2012). Prenatal drug exposure during critical windows of embryonic and fetal development may alter brain development due to teratogenic, epigenetic, or cytotoxic effects. Emerging evidence indicates that prenatal cocaine exposure (PCE) disrupts the monoaminergic neurotransmitter system (dopamine, norepinephrine, serotonin) in the prefrontal cortex, a brain region responsible for emotional and behavioral arousal and regulation, attention, and stress response (Mayes, 2002; Thompson, Levitt, & Stanwood, 2009). Prenatal disruptions in the developmental programming of an infant’s behavioral regulatory system due to PCE may have lasting effects on behavioral regulation, manifested differently across development, including greater externalizing behavior problems in childhood and/or early substance use in adolescence (Minnes, Lang, & Singer, 2011). As the prefrontal cortex and its associated networks are still developing in adolescence, the extent of impairment associated with PCE warrants continued evaluation as children with PCE mature.
A review focused on the preschool years (Dixon, Kurtz, & Chin, 2008) provided conflicting evidence of PCE effects on behavior problems, probably reflecting methodological differences among studies in determination of exposure status, operationalization of outcome variables (direct observation vs. parent rating; broad- vs. narrow-band scores), sample composition, confounders controlled for, and inter-individual differences in vulnerability to cocaine effects. However, the effects of PCE on behavioral regulation at early school age and during preadolescence converge with few exceptions despite methodological variability (Ackerman, Riggins, & Black, 2010). In a study of 244 7 year-olds, PCE during the third trimester was related to both caregiver and teacher reported externalizing behavior problems (Richardson, Goldschmidt, Leech, & Willford, 2011). At the same age (7), Accornero and colleagues found no evidence linking PCE and behavior problems via parent report (Accornero, Anthony, Morrow, Xue, & Bandstra, 2006), but increased behavioral problems among PCE children were noted when behavior was rated by examiners blinded to exposure status (Accornero et al., 2011). Our previous studies also indicated PCE effects. At 6 years of age, associations between PCE and symptoms of oppositional defiant disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) were reported using child self-report, although the same study found no differences for caregiver reported problems (Linares et al., 2006). At 9 years, PCE was related to greater odds of aggressive behavior ratings using caregiver report. In addition, girls with PCE were more likely to be rated by their caregivers as having clinically significant delinquent behavior than NCE girls, whereas no differences were found for boys (McLaughlin et al., 2011). Longitudinal studies also lend support for PCE effects on externalizing behavior problems (Bada et al., 2007; Bada et al., 2011) prior to adolescence. Our longitudinal analyses of caregiver reported, clinically elevated, behavioral problems at 4, 6, 9, and 10 years of age indicated PCE to be associated with higher rates of delinquent behaviors among girls (Minnes et al., 2010).
Findings regarding PCE effects on behavioral adjustment and early substance use during adolescence are just beginning to emerge. One recent longitudinal study reported that children with cocaine/poly-drug exposure were rated as having higher levels of caregiver-reported externalizing behaviors through age 15 years than those with no drug exposure (Bada et al., 2012). However, this study did not examine PCE specific effects.
Three studies assessed the effects of PCE on early substance use, producing mixed findings. Increased risk of early substance use (alcohol, tobacco & marijuana) initiation by age 16 (Frank et al., 2011) and cocaine use at age 14 (Delaney-Black et al., 2011) were reported for PCE adolescents, whereas another study (Warner, Behnke, Eyler, & Szabo, 2011) found no relationship between PCE and early adolescent (12.5 years) experimentation with cocaine. Our data also indicates adolescents with PCE use more alcohol, tobacco, and marijuana than their NCE counterparts by 15 years of age (Minnes, Singer, Min, Wu, Lang, & Yoon, In press).
Multiple factors can affect behavioral outcomes of adolescents with PCE including prenatal exposure to other drugs/alcohol (Singer, Arendt, Minnes, Farkas, & Salvator, 2000), elevated blood lead levels (Min et al., 2009), and environmental conditions related to maternal drug use. Women who use cocaine during pregnancy tend to use other substances, including alcohol, cigarettes, and marijuana, all of which have been linked to externalizing behavior problems (Goldschmidt, Day, & Richardson, 2000; Maughan, Taylor, Caspi, & Moffitt, 2004; Sood et al., 2001) and early adolescent substance use (Baer, Sampson, Barr, Connor, & Streissguth, 2003; Cornelius, Leech, Goldschmidt, & Day, 2000; Day, Goldschmidt, & Thomas, 2006; Goldschmidt, Cornelius, & Day, 2012; Monshouwer et al., 2011). Between 12.5%–35% of high risk poly-drug exposed children had elevated blood lead levels (≥ 10 μg/dL) during preschool assessment (Bandstra et al., 2002; Lumeng, Cabral, Gannon, Heeren, & Frank, 2007; Nelson, Lerner, Needlman, Salvator, & Singer, 2004), in contrast to the general population rate of 8.65% for African American children and 2.02% for Caucasian children (Meyer et al., 2003). Elevated lead levels may further predispose PCE children to delinquent behavior (Dietrich, Ris, Succop, Berger, & Bornschein, 2001; Needleman, Riess, Tobin, Biesecker, & Greenhouse, 1996) and tobacco use (Lane et al., 2008). Environmental factors associated with prenatal drug use, such as poor quality of the home environment, caregiver ongoing substance use and psychological distress, violence exposure (Bada et al., 2007; Bada et al., 2011; Frank et al., 2011), non-kinship adoptive/foster care placement (Linares et al., 2006), negative attachment to caregiver (Warner et al., 2011) and low levels of parental monitoring (Bohnert, Anthony, & Breslau, 2012; Laird, Criss, Pettit, Dodge, & Bates, 2008) may heighten the drug exposed child’s vulnerability to maladaptive behavioral development and obscure the long-term effects of PCE. In contrast, positive environmental factors may be protective or compensate for earlier biologic risk factors (Bada et al., 2012; Singer, 2004, 2008)
The purpose of the present study is to extend our previous studies of the effects of PCE into adolescence controlling for confounding factors of prenatal drug exposures, lead, and environmental covariates. Earlier behavioral problems may resolve or may become more pronounced in response to greater developmental demands of adolescence (puberty, academic performance/success, changing relationships with parents and peers). However, there are few prospective studies on adolescents, and this study represents one of the first prospective studies examining PCE effects during adolescence. An advantage of the current cohort is that a significant proportion of PCE children were placed in foster/adoptive care with lower lead levels and better home environments (Singer et al., 2008), allowing us to more specifically assess effects of PCE. We examined PCE effects on externalizing behavior problems in general as well as specific substance use related problems. Adolescents with PCE were hypothesized to report more externalizing behaviors and substance use related problems compared to adolescents who were non-cocaine exposed (NCE). Since our previous findings and those of others (Sood et al., 2005) have implicated gender as an important individual difference factor, we also explored whether the PCE effect on behavioral outcomes was moderated by gender.
Methods
Sample
This study included 358 (183 PCE, 175 NCE) 15 year old adolescents recruited at birth from a metropolitan teaching hospital and their birth mothers or current caregivers (Singer et al., 2002). Pregnant women who lacked prenatal care, exhibited behavior suggesting intoxication, had a history of involvement with the Department of Human Services, or self-admitted drug use, were considered to be high risk for drug use and given drug toxicology screenings at infant birth. Maternal and infant urine samples were obtained shortly before or after infant birth by medical staff and analyzed to detect the presence of cocaine metabolites, cannabinoids, opiates, phencyclidine, and amphetamines, using the Syva Emit method (Syva Co, Palo Alto, CA). Detection of drug use was confirmed with follow-up gas chromatography. In addition to urine analysis, infant meconium was collected and analyzed for cocaine and other drug metabolites at birth, including benzoylecgonine, meta-hydoxybenzoylecgonine, cocaethylene, cannabinoids, opiates, phencyclidine, amphetamines, and benzodiazepines. Women with a psychiatric history (major depression, bipolar disorder, or schizophrenia) (16), low intellectual functioning (1), HIV-positive status (5), or chronic medical illness (4) were excluded, as were infants with Down Syndrome (2), Fetal Alcohol Syndrome (1), or medical illness (3). A total of 415 newborns and their birth mothers were enrolled at birth, of which 218 infants were identified as cocaine exposed based on positive screens of maternal and infant urine, infant meconium, or maternal self-report to hospital or research staff (See Arendt, Singer, Minnes, & Salvator, 1999 for a complete description of drug use assessment).
Since birth, 12 enrolled children died (9 PCE, 3 NCE). Causes of death for the PCE children included sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) (4), cardiopulmonary arrest (1), pneumonia (1), accidental asphyxia (1), respiratory distress syndrome (1), and unknown illness (1). For the NCE children, causes of death were SIDS (2) and respiratory distress syndrome (1). At 15 years of age, 358 children, representing 89% of the living participants in the original study, completed self-assessment. Of the 45 adolescents not seen, the 26 PCE children were more likely to have birth mothers with lower scores on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R) Picture Completion subtest (used as a screening measure of nonverbal intelligence) compared to the study participants, and the 19 NCE children not seen were more likely to be White and exposed to higher prenatal tobacco than the participants.
Procedure
Adolescents and their caregivers were seen at the developmental research laboratory for approximately 5 hours at each follow-up visit. Data in the present study were taken from interviews conducted when the children were ages 2 and 4 (blood lead), 10 (caregiver reported externalizing behavior), 12 (parental attachment, parental monitoring, violence exposure) and 15 years (externalizing behavior and substance use related problems). All participants were given a monetary stipend for participation, along with lunch and assistance with transportation costs if needed. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the participating hospitals. Parental written informed consent and child assent were obtained. A Certificate of Confidentiality (DA-98-91), exempting the study from legislative, judicial, or administrative attempts to obtain confidential information, was obtained from the Department of Health and Human Services.
Measures
Prenatal cocaine and other substance exposures
At the newborn visit birth mothers were asked to recall frequency and amount of drug use for the month prior to and for each trimester of pregnancy. The number of tobacco cigarettes and marijuana joints smoked, and the number of drinks of beer, wine, or hard liquor per week was computed, with each drink equivalent to 0.5 oz. of absolute alcohol. For cocaine, the number of “rocks” consumed and the amount of money spent per day were also noted, in addition to the dichotomous cocaine status (PCE, NCE) variable. Frequency of use was recorded for each drug on a Likert-type scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 7 (daily use) and converted to reflect the average number of days per week a drug was used, except for cigarettes, which was collected as the number smoked per day. Frequency was multiplied by the amount used per day to compute an average use score for the month prior to pregnancy and for each trimester. These scores were then averaged to obtain a total average score. The drug assessment was updated with the child’s current caregiver at each follow-up visit to obtain an assessment of recent (prior 30 day period) postnatal, caregiver drug use. The continuous drug variables were used in analyses.
Externalizing behavior and substance use related problems
Adolescent externalizing behavior was assessed at age 15 using the Youth Self-Report (YSR; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001), a 105-item child self-rating of his or her own behavior designed to assess emotional, behavioral, and social problems in the last 6 months. Items are coded from 0 (not true) to 2 (very often or often true) and written at the 5th grade reading level. Resultant T-scores were standardized for gender and age with higher scores indicating more problem behaviors. For this investigation, T-scores of externalizing behaviors (i.e., aggression and rule-breaking behavior, including alcohol, tobacco, and drug use) were used; with internal consistency reliability (α) = .90 for this administration. T-scores ≥ 60 indicate ratings at or above the borderline/clinical cut-off, and scores below this level are considered nonclinical in nature. At age 10, externalizing behavior was assessed by caregiver-report using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; α = .95), a parallel form to the YSR.
Substance use related problems were measured at age 15 by the 17-item Substance Use and Abuse Scale from the Problem Oriented Screening Instrument for Teenagers (POSIT; Rahdert, 1991), which is designed to detect social and legal problems associated with alcohol and other drug use. The psychometric properties of the POSIT have been widely tested on youth in outpatient substance abuse treatment (McLaney, Boca, & Babor, 1994), arrested youth (Dembo et al., 1996), and youth attending a general adolescent clinic (Knight, Goodman, Pulerwitz, & DuRant, 2001; Shrier, Harris, Kurland, & Knight, 2003), yielding α ranging from .77–.86 (Knight et al., 2001; McLaney et al., 1994) and 1-week test-retest reliability of .77 (Knight et al., 2001). Respondents reporting ≥1 problem(s) associated with substance use were coded 1 (yes) due to skewed distribution.
Child, maternal, and caregiver characteristics
Birth, demographic, and medical characteristics were extracted from hospital birth records including maternal age and marital status at birth, years of education of the biological mother, number of prenatal care visits, parity, child’s race and gender, and infant head circumference. A Hollingshead score of IV or V (Hollingshead, 1957) was used as an indicator of low socioeconomic status. Maternal vocabulary was assessed at birth using the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R; Dunn & Dunn, 1981) and updated using its third edition (PPVT-III; Dunn, Dunn, Williams, Wang, & Booklets, 1997) at age 6 and later assessments. The PPVT-R/PPVT-III are highly correlated with various measures of verbal IQ with α ranging from .73 to .84 and test-retest reliabilities of .76 to .79. The Block Design (BD) and Picture Completion (PC) subtests of the WAIS-R (Wechsler, 1989) were used to estimate maternal non-verbal intelligence at birth, as they correlate highest with Performance IQ (.81 and .75 respectively). Maternal psychological distress was assessed using the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI; Derogatis, 1992), a 53-item self-report questionnaire assessing the experience of nine primary symptoms in the past seven days: somatic complaints, obsessive-compulsive behavior, interpersonal sensitivity, depression, anxiety, hostility, phobic anxiety, paranoid ideation, and psychoticism. Items were rated on a 5-point scale (0=not at all to 4=extremely) with higher scores indicating greater distress. The BSI Global Severity Index (GSI; α =.95), the average rating of all 53 items, was computed at birth and each follow-up visit. At each visit, the child’s placement (with either biological mother/relative or foster/adoptive caregiver) and changes, if any, in placement, defined by a change in both primary caregiver and physical setting lasting greater than one month, were noted and the data on the current caregiver were updated to provide concurrent assessment of caregiver intelligence and psychological distress in addition to drug use. At the 15 year visit, the quality of the caregiving environment was assessed using the Home Observation of the Environment-Early Adolescent version (EA-HOME; Caldwell & Bradley, 2003), and adolescents’ intelligence was assessed using the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children-Fourth Edition (Wechsler, 2003).
Blood lead
At ages 2 and 4 years, all children participated in a separate study of lead exposure (Nelson et al., 2004). Venous blood samples could not be obtained from some children due to lack of parental consent, excessive stress related to the blood draw, child sickness, or time constrains. Valid hematologic measures were available for 143 two-year and 274 four-year old children. Measures were averaged for the 122 children seen at both assessment points, resulting in 298 valid assessments. A greater percentage of African-American and married women and a lower percentage of foster parents consented to blood collection.
Blood collection and analyses of lead were performed by the affiliate University Hospital Laboratory Services Foundation, accredited by the College of American Pathologists and in compliance with Clinical Laboratory Improvement Amendments regulations. The lab was enrolled in the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) proficiency testing program for blood lead and was Occupational Safety and Health Administration approved for blood lead analysis. Approximately 5ml of venous blood was drawn by syringe into a lead free container containing an anticoagulant. Blood lead concentration was determined by atomic absorption spectrophotometry using a graphite furnace and matrix modification to eliminate chemical interferences (Varian). Elevated blood lead values were defined as ≥10 mg/dL as recommended by the CDC for descriptive purposes. Continuous lead values were used in the statistical analyses.
Parental attachment, parental monitoring, and violence exposure
Parental attachment, monitoring, and violence exposure were all assessed at age 12 using The Assessment of Liability and Exposure to Substance Use and Antisocial Behavior (ALEXSA; Ridenour, 2003), an illustration-based, audio, computer-assisted self-interview to assess antisocial behavior, substance involvement and associated risk factors for children ages 9–12. The psychometric characteristics of ALEXSA subscales have been demonstrated with good to excellent test-retest reliability and construct validities (Ridenour, Clark, & Cottler, 2009), criterion validity (Ridenour, Meyer-Chilenski, & Reid, 2012), as well as its cross-cultural equivalence across different races/ethnicities, risk levels, and academic skills (Ridenour et al., 2011).
The ALEXSA parental attachment subscale is a 5-item questionnaire (α =.80) designed to assess the youth’s perceived strength of emotional bond with parents (e.g. “How often does your parent listen to what you say?” “How often do you share your thoughts and feelings with your parent?”). Children rated the frequency using a 4-point Likert scale (0=never to 3=all the time) with higher scores indicating higher parental attachment.
Parental monitoring was measured by the ALEXSA with a 5-item questionnaire (α =.74) designed to assess the youths’ perceptions of whether their parent(s) usually are aware of the youths’ activities and whereabouts with questions such as “Does your parent know none of the things you do in your free time (0), some of the things you do (1), most of the things you do (2), or all of the things you do in your free time (3)?” “Last week, did your parent know where you were going none of the times you left home (0), some of the times you left home (1), most of the time you left home (2), or all of the times you left home (3)?” High scores indicate higher parental monitoring.
The violence exposure subscale on the ALEXSA is an 8-item questionnaire (α =.75) assessing lifetime experiences of violence that have been inflicted directly on the respondent (e.g. “How many times have you been beaten up?” How many times have you been robbed or mugged?) as well as violence that the respondent has witnessed (e.g. “How many times have you seen in person somebody get beat up (remember this does not count T.V. or movies)?” “How many times have you seen in person someone being robbed or mugged?”). Children rated the frequency of violence exposure using a 5-point Likert scale (0=0 times to 5=5 times or more), with higher scores indicating greater exposure.
Statistical analyses
Data that were positively skewed were normalized using a log transformation (drug use, GSI on the BSI, violence exposure, and lead) prior to analyses. Means and standard deviations (SD) were reported by the variables’ original distribution, with transformations used in analyses. Sample characteristics were examined by cocaine status using t-tests for continuous variables or Chi-square analyses for categorical variables. Zero-order Pearson correlations were estimated to examine relationships between study variables. The effects of PCE were evaluated using multiple regression on the YSR externalizing behavior scale and logistic regression on the POSIT substance use related problems.
In order to avoid multicollinearity and saturation of the model, covariates correlated with outcomes at p ≤ .20 were entered into the regression model stepwise (i.e., using a series of steps) and were retained if, on entry, they were significant at p < .10 or caused substantial (> 10%) change in the PCE coefficient (Mickey & Greenland, 1989). PCE was entered first followed by socio-demographic covariates (maternal age and education, parity, number of prenatal visits, maternal/caregiver’s vocabulary ability, non-verbal reasoning, psychological distress, quality of home environment; adolescent age, sex, race), other drug exposure (prenatal tobacco, alcohol, and marijuana exposure), parenting (parental attachment and monitoring), and violence exposure variables. Due to the reduced sample size, blood lead level was entered last. If cocaine status was significant in the final model, average amount of PCE was examined. If results did not differ between a model with cocaine status (yes vs. no) and a model with amount of PCE, a model with amount of PCE was presented. Placement differences (PCE biologic/relative, PCE foster/adoptive care and NCE) were also evaluated using chi-square analyses, analyses of variance (ANOVA), or analyses of covariance (ANCOVA). Multicollinearity was examined using tolerance and variance inflation factor. Child gender was examined as a potential moderator of PCE effects.
Results
Sample characteristics
The birth mothers of adolescents with PCE had less prenatal care, were approximately 4 years older, slightly less educated, and primarily unmarried. They had lower vocabulary scores and reported more psychological distress than mothers of NCE adolescents (Table 1). Adolescents with PCE were exposed to more alcohol, marijuana, and tobacco prenatally, and had lower birth weight, length, and head circumference (Table 2). Caregiver and home environment characteristics at 15 years did not differ except that caregivers of the adolescents with PCE used more tobacco in the previous month. Adolescents with PCE had lower blood lead levels during the preschool years compared to NCE adolescents and were rated as having more externalizing problems by caregivers at age 10. Adolescents with PCE were less likely to be continuously cared for by their birth mothers, with 51% (n=94) of adolescents with PCE, compared to 89% (n=156) of their NCE counterparts, living with their birth parents, and 24% (n=44) of adolescents with PCE, compared to 4.6% (n=8) of their NCE counterparts, living in non-kinship adoptive/foster care (χ2 =24.32, p <.0001) at the 15 year assessment. Adolescents with PCE reported a lower level of parental attachment at 12 years than their NCE counterparts, but no group differences were found in parental monitoring or violence exposure.
Table 1.
PCE (n=183) | NCE (n=175) | P | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||
M | SD | M | SD | ||
Biological maternal | |||||
Mother’s age at birth | 29.70 | 4.99 | 25.52 | 4.74 | <.0001 |
Parity | 3.50 | 1.83 | 2.77 | 1.87 | .0002 |
Number of prenatal visits | 5.18 | 4.59 | 8.69 | 4.85 | <.0001 |
Education, years | 11.55 | 1.65 | 11.93 | 1.38 | .02 |
PPVT-R Standard Score | 73.12 | 14.09 | 77.32 | 14.66 | .007 |
WAIS-R Block Design Scaled Score | 6.86 | 2.12 | 7.18 | 2.02 | .15 |
WAIS-R Picture Completion Scaled Score | 6.70 | 2.14 | 6.88 | 2.31 | .44 |
BSI Global Severity Index | 0.82 | 0.74 | 0.50 | 0.53 | <.0001 |
Married, n (%) | 14 | 7.65 | 28 | 16.00 | .01 |
Low SES, n (%) | 178 | 97.80 | 171 | 97.71 | .96 |
African-American, n (%) | 151 | 82.51 | 145 | 82.86 | .93 |
Amount of substance use during pregnancy | |||||
Tobacco, cigarettes per day | 11.50 | 11.16 | 3.74 | 6.89 | <.0001 |
Alcohol, dose per week | 9.79 | 17.73 | 1.39 | 4.68 | <.0001 |
Marijuana, dose per week | 1.37 | 3.51 | 0.62 | 3.58 | <.0001 |
Cocaine, units per week | 22.73 | 38.24 | --- | --- | --- |
Caregiver at age 15 years | |||||
Education, years | 12.50 | 2.29 | 12.87 | 1.95 | .10 |
PPVT-III Standard Score | 79.95 | 14.89 | 78.73 | 15.08 | .46 |
WAIS-R Block Design Scaled Score | 7.10 | 2.13 | 7.23 | 1.92 | .55 |
WAIS-R Picture Completion Scaled Score | 7.53 | 2.52 | 7.08 | 2.30 | .09 |
BSI Global Severity Index | 0.35 | 0.42 | 0.34 | 0.47 | .45 |
Amount of substance use in the past 30 days | |||||
Tobacco, cigarettes per day | 4.82 | 7.19 | 3.46 | 6.57 | .01 |
Alcohol, dose per week | 1.83 | 4.56 | 2.01 | 4.67 | .49 |
Marijuana, dose per week | 0.54 | 5.48 | 0.46 | 3.10 | .61 |
HOME environment | 47.59 | 6.90 | 48.71 | 6.16 | .11 |
PPVT= Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, PPVT-R (Revised) used at birth; PPVT-III (Third edition) was used at 6 and later years; WAIS-R = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised; BSI=Brief Symptom Inventory.
Table 2.
PCE (n=183) | NCE (n=175) | P | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||
M | SD | M | SD | ||
At birth | |||||
Gestational age, weeks | 37.73 | 2.86 | 38.46 | 2.90 | .02 |
Hobel Neonatal Risk score | 7.71 | 16.77 | 5.90 | 16.11 | .30 |
Birth weight, gramsa | 2700 | 655 | 3100 | 704 | <.0001 |
Birth length, cma | 47.21 | 3.96 | 49.11 | 3.79 | <.0001 |
Head circumference, cma | 32.26 | 2.17 | 33.48 | 2.40 | <.0001 |
Prematurity (<37 wks. gestational age), n (%) | 55 | 30.05 | 33 | 18.86 | .01 |
Low birth weight (<2500 grams), n (%) | 67 | 36.61 | 33 | 18.86 | .0002 |
Very low birth weight (<1500 grams), n (%) | 12 | 6.56 | 7 | 4.00 | .28 |
Small for gestational age, n (%) | 25 | 13.81 | 4 | 2.30 | <.0001 |
Microcephaly, n (%) | 27 | 15.00 | 7 | 4.05 | .0005 |
Male, n (%) | 81 | 44.26 | 84 | 48.00 | .48 |
African-American, n (%) | 150 | 81.97 | 144 | 82.29 | .94 |
Postnatal | |||||
Blood lead level at 2 or/and 4 yearsb | 7.06 | 4.16 | 8.16 | 4.64 | .03 |
Elevated blood lead level (≥ 10 μg/dLb), n (%) | 26 | 18.31 | 37 | 27.21 | .08 |
Age at assessment, years | 15.69 | 0.27 | 15.67 | 0.27 | .50 |
Medicaid, n (%) | 149 | 85.14 | 129 | 75.0 | .02 |
Receiving free lunch at school, n (%) | 145 | 83.33 | 143 | 84.62 | .75 |
WISC-IV Full Scale IQ at 15 | 81.29 | 11.41 | 83.74 | 13.99 | .07 |
Parental attachmentc | 2.10 | .68 | 2.28 | .61 | .008 |
Parental monitoringc | 2.43 | .65 | 2.50 | .58 | .27 |
Violence exposurec | .63 | .77 | .59 | .81 | .54 |
Caregiver reported externalizing behavior at age 10 | 53.5 | 12.6 | 50.1 | 12.0 | .009 |
Always in birth parents’ care up to age 15, n (%) | 60 | 32.8 | 132 | 75.4 | < .0001 |
Current placement at 15, n (%) | < .0001 | ||||
Birth parents’ care | 94 | 51.4 | 156 | 89.1 | |
Relative care | 42 | 23.0 | 7 | 4.0 | |
Non-kinship adoptive care | 38 | 20.8 | 5 | 2.9 | |
Others | 9 | 4.9 | 7 | 4.0 | |
Non-kinship foster care | 5 | 2.7 | 3 | 1.7 | |
Residential treatmentd | 3 | 1.6 | 2 | 1.1 | |
Group home | 1 | 0.5 | 2 | 1.1 |
WISC-IV=Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children-Fourth Edition
Adjusted for gestational age
Sub-sample of 142 PCE and 136 NCE
Assessed at 12 year
One adopted adolescent was in residential treatment
Adolescents with PCE reported more externalizing behavior problems (M=53.0, SD=10.47) than adolescents with NCE (M=50.6, SD=9.42; p <.03) and more substance use related problems (28% for PCE vs. 16% for NCE, p=.008) at 15 years. PCE adolescents were more likely to describe experiencing accidents, feelings of addiction, forgetfulness, and mood swings when using alcohol or drugs. However, no group difference was found in the proportion of adolescents scoring in the probable clinical range (T-scores ≥ 60) of externalizing behavioral problems (25% for PCE vs. 19% for NCE, χ2 =2.05, p=.15). Externalizing behavior and substance use related problems were moderately correlated (r=.29, p < .0001). Zero-order Pearson correlations between key study variables are presented in Table 3.
Table 3.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | 14 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Prenatal cocaine exposure (1 = yes) | --- | .78 | −.04 | .40 | .26 | −.14 | .49 | −.09 | −.14 | −.06 | .03 | .14 | .12 | .14 |
2. Amount of prenatal cocaine exposure, units per week average | --- | −.04 | .29 | .31 | −.10 | .49 | −.02 | −.11 | −.06 | −.05 | .17 | .16 | .10 | |
3. Child sex (1 = male) | --- | −.07 | −.06 | .12 | −.04 | −.10 | −.04 | −.12 | .08 | .09 | −.08 | .18 | ||
4. Birth maternal age | --- | .10 | −.08 | .28 | .05 | .01 | −.04 | .08 | −.05 | −.10 | −.02 | |||
5. Birth maternal GSI at birth | --- | −.11 | .26 | −.11 | −.09 | .00 | −.07 | .20 | .19 | .13 | ||||
6. Maternal PPVT | --- | −.11 | .09 | −.11 | .04 | −.07 | .04 | −.06 | −.11 | |||||
7. Prenatal alcohol dose per week average | --- | −.03 | −.12 | −.07 | .02 | .14 | .08 | .06 | ||||||
8. HOME score | --- | .10 | .11 | −.19 | −.04 | −.19 | −.19 | |||||||
9. Prenatal attachment | --- | .38 | −.24 | −.16 | −.16 | .00 | ||||||||
10. Prenatal monitoring | --- | −.29 | −.12 | −.15 | −.16 | |||||||||
11. Violence exposure | --- | .18 | .26 | .14 | ||||||||||
12. Externalizing behavior (CBCL) at age 10 | --- | .28 | .11 | |||||||||||
13. Externalizing behavior (YSR) at age 15 | --- | .29 | ||||||||||||
14. Substance use related problems (POSIT) at age 15 | --- |
Note. p < .05 when r ≥ |.11|; p < .01 when r ≥ |.14|.
Effects of PCE on YSR externalizing behavior at 15 years
Table 4 presents significant PCE effects on externalizing behavior at 15 years without (Model 1) and with (Model 2) controlling for earlier externalizing behavior. After adjusting for covariates (Model 1; F=6.84, p<.0001), amount of PCE was associated with higher levels of externalizing behavioral problems (β=.15, p=.02). Birth mother’s older age (β= −0.12, p=.04) and better HOME scores at 15 years (β= −0.17, p=.001) were related to fewer externalizing behavioral symptoms. Greater birth mother psychological distress (β= .16, p=.005) and violence exposure (β= .23, p <.001) was related to higher externalizing behavioral symptoms. Blood lead level was not associated with externalizing behaviors (β= .04, p >.50). Further adjustment of caregiver-reported earlier externalizing behavior didn’t change the effect of PCE (Model 2; F=7.56, p<.0001). No gender interaction was found.
Table 4.
Model 1
|
Model 2
|
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
b (se) | β | p | b (se) | β | p | |
Prenatal cocaine exposure1 | 1.02 (.43) | .15 | .02 | .90 (.42) | .14 | .03 |
Child sex, male | −1.96 (1.07) | − .10 | .07 | −2.21 (1.05) | − .11 | .04 |
Birth maternal age | − .21 (.10) | − .12 | .04 | − .17 (.10) | − .10 | .09 |
Biologic mother’s GSI at birth | 4.57 (1.65) | .16 | .005 | 3.63 (1.64) | .13 | .03 |
Prenatal alcohol exposure2 | − .16 (.53) | − .02 | .76 | − .22 (.52) | − .03 | .67 |
HOME score | − .27 (.09) | − .17 | .001 | − .29 (.08) | − .19 | < .001 |
Parental attachment | − .49 (.90) | − .03 | .59 | − .33 (.88) | − .02 | .71 |
Parental monitoring | − .82 (.95) | − .05 | .39 | − .70 (.93) | − .04 | .45 |
Violence exposure | 2.03 (.51) | .23 | < .001 | 1.72 (.51) | .20 | < .001 |
Externalizing behavior at age 103 | .15 (.04) | .19 | < .001 | |||
| ||||||
R2 | .18 | .21 |
Note. Variables that were tested but not included in the final model because they were not significant at p<.10 or didn’t cause substantial (>10%) change in the PCE coefficients are: birth mother education, parity, number of prenatal visits, maternal/caregiver vocabulary ability (PPVT-R), non-verbal reasoning (WAIS-R), adolescent age and race, prenatal tobacco and marijuana exposure, and blood lead level.
Cocaine units per week,
Average alcohol dose per week,
Caregiver report (CBCL)
Effects of PCE on POSIT substance use related problems at 15 years
Adolescents with PCE were at greater risk for substance use related problems than NCE adolescents (OR=2.77, 95% CI=1.38–5.56; Table 5). Adolescents with PCE were 2.8 times more likely to have substance use related problems than their NCE counterparts after adjusting for covariates. Higher parental monitoring decreased the likelihood of substance use related problems (OR=0.63, 95% CI=0.41–0.99) and better HOME scores were associated with a lower likelihood of substance use related problems (OR=0.95, 95% CI=0.91–0.99). Boys were 2.5 (95% CI=1.37–4.71) times more likely to have substance use related problems than girls. Blood lead level was not associated with the likelihood of having substance use related problems (OR=1.08, 95% CI=0.58–1.98) nor was caregiver-reported externalizing behavior at age 10 (OR=1.01, 95% CI=0.98–1.03). No gender interaction was found.
Table 5.
b (se) | Odds Ratio (OR) | 95% CI | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Prenatal cocaine exposure | 1.02 (.36) | 2.77 | 1.38 – 5.56 | .004 |
Child sex, male | .93 (.32) | 2.54 | 1.37 – 4.71 | .003 |
Maternal PPVT score | − .02 (.01) | .98 | .96 – 1.00 | .06 |
Prenatal alcohol exposure, average | − .12 (.14) | .89 | .67 – 1.17 | .39 |
Biologic mother’s GSI at birth | .56 (.44) | 1.75 | .74 – 4.14 | .21 |
HOME score | − .05 (.02) | .95 | .91 – .99 | .04 |
Parental monitoring | − .46 (.23) | .63 | .41 – .99 | .045 |
| ||||
Pseudo R2 | .18 |
Note. Variables that were tested but not included in the final model because they were not significant at p<.10 or didn’t cause substantial (>10%) change in the PCE coefficients are: biological mother’s age, education, parity, number of prenatal visits, maternal/caregiver non-verbal reasoning (WAIS-R), adolescent age and race, prenatal tobacco and marijuana exposure, parental attachment, violence exposure, blood lead level, and caregiver-reported externalizing behavior at age 10
Effects of adoptive/foster care status
PCE adolescents in non-kinship adoptive or foster care (n=44) were exposed to cocaine more heavily (M=28.3, SD=43.6) than PCE adolescents in maternal or relative care (n=139, M=21.97, SD=36.4) although the difference was not statistically significant (F=1.16, p=.28). PCE adolescents in adoptive or foster care did not differ from those in maternal or relative care in maternal age at birth, birth mother’s psychological distress (GSI), and prenatal alcohol exposure. However, their birth mothers had lower receptive vocabulary scores (M=68.4, SD=13.1) than mothers of PCE adolescents in maternal or relative care (M=74.6, SD=14.1) as well as mothers of NCE adolescents (M=77.3, SD=14.7; F=6.8, p=.001). PCE adolescents in adoptive or foster care had a higher HOME score (M=50.9, SD=5.9) than PCE adolescents in maternal or relative care (M=46.6, SD=6.9; F=8.9, p<.0002) and lower blood lead levels (M=5.8, SD=3.6) than NCE children (M=8.2, SD=4.6; F=4.5, p=.01), a reason for overall lower blood lead levels observed among PCE adolescents. PCE adolescents currently residing in non-kinship adoptive or foster care had experienced 2.61 (SD=2.54) placement changes on average compared to PCE adolescents in maternal or relative care who had experienced 1.35 (SD=1.54) placement changes and NCE adolescents who had experienced 0.65 (SD=1.35) placement changes (F=27.8, p< .0001). Of those 44 children with PCE in adoptive or foster care, 46% had only one placement change and 18% had two, indicating approximately two-thirds of them having been in somewhat stable living arrangements and one-third had experienced more than 2 placements. There was no difference between the two PCE groups in parental attachment, parental monitoring, or violence exposure. After controlling for covariates, PCE adolescents in adoptive or foster care reported higher externalizing behavioral problems (β= .17, p=.005) and were more likely to have substance use related problems (OR=2.98, 95% CI=1.04–8.54) than NCE adolescents. There was no difference between the two PCE groups in externalizing behavior, as well as in the likelihood of substance use related problems.
Discussion
The present study demonstrated that PCE is associated with adolescent self-reported externalizing behavior and substance use related problems at 15 years of age. Our results are consistent with our previous findings at 6 years using child self-report (Linares et al., 2006) and 9 years using caregiver-report (McLaughlin et al., 2011). At 6 years, PCE children reported a higher average number of symptoms of oppositional defiant disorder and ADHD compared to NCE children. At 9 years, PCE was related to an increased likelihood of caregiver-reported aggressive behavior. The PCE-associated externalizing behavior problems, noted at 6 and 9 years, continued to be present at age 15 years. To our knowledge, this is one of the first prospective documentations of PCE-related effects on externalizing behaviors during adolescence. Our study is also in line with two prospective studies (Delaney-Black et al., 2011; Frank et al., 2011) indicating a greater risk of early substance use among PCE adolescents. PCE adolescents reported more substance related problems than their NCE counterparts, extending previous studies by demonstrating the extent of impairment in daily functioning for PCE adolescents via increased vulnerability to substance use. However, the gender by cocaine interaction on delinquent behavior, which had been shown at 9 years (McLaughlin et al., 2011) and from our longitudinal analyses of caregivers’ ratings (Minnes et al., 2010), was not noted at age 15 years when self-report was employed. Differences in informants may explain the discrepancy, given the well-known, low cross-informant correlation that has been consistently demonstrated (Achenbach, 1991; De Los Reyes & Kazdin, 2005) with different correlates specific to the informant (Min, Singer, Minnes, Kim, & Short, 2013). Also, different developmental stages combined with confounders affecting gender differently may be responsible for the discrepancy. Compared to adolescent boys, adolescent girls are less exposed to violence (Bacchini, Miranda, & Affuso, 2011), monitored more by their parents (Bacchini et al., 2011; Richards et al., 2004), and likely to be subjected to increased surveillance with the onset of puberty (Leadbeater, Kuperminc, Hertzog, & Blatt, 1999).
Consistent with our previous reports (Linares et al., 2006; McLaughlin et al., 2011; Minnes et al., 2010), PCE adolescents in adoptive or foster care reported higher externalizing behavioral problems than NCE adolescents, but no significant difference was found between PCE adolescents in maternal/relative care and NCE adolescents. These findings on behavioral outcomes in relation to adoptive/foster care placement are in contrast to our previous findings of cognitive and language outcomes during the preadolescent years, where better cognitive and language development was shown for PCE children in adoptive or foster care compared to PCE children in maternal or relative care (Lewis et al., 2011; Singer et al., 2004; Singer et al., 2008). Thus, a more positive postnatal caregiving environment provided by non-kinship adoptive/foster care appears not to have had the same protective impact on the behavioral domain as was shown on cognitive and language outcomes in PCE children. Less stability in the caregiving environment indicated by more placement changes in PCE adolescents in foster/adoptive care may affect behavioral adjustment by compromising children’s ability to build stable relationships, which may again lead to another placement change (Aarons et al., 2010), generating a vicious cycle of disruption and worsening behavioral adjustment.
Other factors also contributed to externalizing behavior and substance use related problems at 15 years. Parental monitoring was associated with a lower likelihood of substance use related problems, while violence exposure was associated with increased externalizing behavior. The protective effects of parental monitoring is noteworthy, as they suggest prevention and/or intervention targets to reduce substance use related problems in adolescents with PCE.
Several limitations in our study should be noted. Children born prenatally exposed to cocaine are often placed outside of their birth mother’s custody, and placement changes may negatively affect behavioral adjustment. Without a comparable number of NCE adolescents living in non-kinship adoptive/foster care, our study is limited in separating the effect of PCE from the effects of placement changes among PCE adolescents. Also, outcome variables of the present study relied on adolescents’ self-report, which might be subject to social desirability bias, as well as adolescents’ ability to accurately self-assess their behaviors. Although biological measures were used for detection of PCE in our study, maternal/caregivers’ self-report was used for assessment of levels of prenatal and postnatal substance use, which may have resulted in underreporting of drug use and, thus, underestimation of the effects of PCE and other drugs.
Despite these limitations, this study has strengths including its prospective design assessing a large number of adolescents and their caregivers since birth with a high rate of retention (89%) at the 15 year follow-up. A comprehensive list of covariates and confounders, including other prenatal substance exposures and lead, were evaluated and controlled statistically when necessary. By incorporating non-kinship adoptive or foster care placement, which disproportionately occurred for adolescents with heavier PCE, our study attempted to tease out the effects of protective environmental factors related to placement among adolescents with PCE. Evaluation of covariates that are more pertinent to adolescence, such as adolescents’ perceived parental attachment, monitoring, and violence exposure, enhanced our understanding of the dynamics of PCE, non-kinship adoptive or foster care, parenting, and adolescent behavioral adjustment.
The present study extends previous studies of PCE by providing evidence of the longitudinal effects of PCE persisting into adolescence. Controlling for confounding factors, PCE adolescents reported more externalizing behavioral problems and more substance use related problems. Our study suggests that PCE is a reliable marker for the potential development of problem behaviors in adolescence, including substance use related problems. Interventions focusing on strengthening parental monitoring and decreasing violence exposure may be promising in reducing behavioral problems among high risk prenatally cocaine/poly-drug exposed adolescents.
Acknowledgments
Thanks are extended to the families who have participated in this study for 15 years and our research staff including Laurie Ellison, Diana Fox, Miaoping Wu, and Terri Lotz-Ganley. This research was supported by a National Institute on Drug Abuse Grant R01-07957. The study sponsor did not have a role in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of this data or in writing of this report or the decision to submit this paper for publication.
Footnotes
Portions of this paper were presented at the 36th Annual Conference of Neurobehavioral Teratology Society (NBTS), Baltimore, MD, in June 2012.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Aarons GA, James S, Monn AR, Raghavan R, Wells RS, Leslie LK. Behavior problems and placement change in a national child welfare sample: A prospective study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2010;49(1):70–80. doi: 10.1097/00004583-201001000-00011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Accornero VH, Anthony JC, Morrow CE, Xue L, Bandstra ES. Prenatal cocaine exposure: An examination of childhood externalizing and internalizing behavior problems at age 7 years. Epidemiologia e Psichiatria Sociale. 2006;15(1):20–29. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Accornero VH, Anthony JC, Morrow CE, Xue L, Mansoor E, Johnson AL, Bandstra ES. Estimated effect of prenatal cocaine exposure on examiner-rated behavior at age 7 years. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2011;33(3):370–378. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2011.02.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM. Integrative guide for the 1991 CBCL/4-18, YSR, and TRF profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Ackerman JP, Riggins T, Black MM. A review of the effects of prenatal cocaine exposure among school-aged children. Pediatrics. 2010;125(3):554–565. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-0637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arendt RE, Singer LT, Minnes S, Salvator A. Accuracy in detecting prenatal drug exposure. Journal of Drug Issues. 1999;29(2):203–214. doi: 10.1177/002204269902900203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bada HS, Bann CM, Bauer CR, Shankaran S, Lester B, LaGasse L, Higgins R. Preadolescent behavior problems after prenatal cocaine exposure: Relationship between teacher and caretaker ratings (maternal lifestyle study) Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2011;33(1):78–87. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2010.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bada HS, Bann CM, Whitaker TM, Bauer CR, Shankaran S, Lagasse L, Higgins R. Protective factors can mitigate behavior problems after prenatal cocaine and other drug exposures. Pediatrics. 2012;130(6):e1479–88. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-3306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bada HS, Das A, Bauer CR, Shankaran S, Lester B, LaGasse L, Higgins R. Impact of prenatal cocaine exposure on child behavior problems through school age. Pediatrics. 2007;119(2):e348–59. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baer JS, Sampson PD, Barr HM, Connor PD, Streissguth AP. A 21-year longitudinal analysis of the effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on young adult drinking. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60(4):377–385. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.4.377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bandstra ES, Morrow CE, Vogel AL, Fifer RC, Ofir AY, Dausa AT, Anthony JC. Longitudinal influence of prenatal cocaine exposure on child language functioning. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2002;24(3):297–308. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(02)00192-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bohnert KM, Anthony JC, Breslau N. Parental monitoring at age 11 and subsequent onset of cannabis use up to age 17: Results from a prospective study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2012;73(2):173–177. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2012.73.173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caldwell BM, Bradley RH. Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment: Administration manual. Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Cornelius MD, Leech SL, Goldschmidt L, Day NL. Prenatal tobacco exposure: Is it a risk factor for early tobacco experimentation? Nicotine & Tobacco Research: Official Journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco. 2000;2(1):45–52. doi: 10.1080/14622200050011295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Day NL, Goldschmidt L, Thomas CA. Prenatal marijuana exposure contributes to the prediction of marijuana use at age 14. Addiction. 2006;101(9):1313–1322. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01523.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Los Reyes A, Kazdin AE. Informant discrepancies in the assessment of childhood psychopathology: A critical review, theoretical framework, and recommendations for further study. Psychological Bulletin. 2005;131(4):483–509. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delaney-Black V, Chiodo LM, Hannigan JH, Greenwald MK, Janisse J, Patterson G, Sokol RJ. Prenatal and postnatal cocaine exposure predict teen cocaine use. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2011;33(1):110–119. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2010.06.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dembo R, Schmeidler J, Borden P, Turner G, Sue CC, Manning D. Examination of the reliability of the problem oriented screening instrument for teenagers (POSIT) among arrested youths entering a juvenile assessment center. Substance use & Misuse. 1996;31(7):785–824. doi: 10.3109/10826089609063958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Derogatis LR. The Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI): Administration, scoring, and procedures manual II. Towson, MD: Clinical Psychometric Research; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Dietrich KN, Ris MD, Succop PA, Berger OG, Bornschein RL. Early exposure to lead and juvenile delinquency. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2001;23(6):511–518. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(01)00184-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dixon DR, Kurtz PF, Chin MD. A systematic review of challenging behaviors in children exposed prenatally to substances of abuse. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2008;29(6):483–502. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2007.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunn L, Dunn L. Peabody picture vocabulary test - revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Dunn L, Dunn L, Williams KT, Wang JJ, Booklets N. Peabody picture vocabulary test, (PPVT-III): Form IIA. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Frank DA, Rose-Jacobs R, Crooks D, Cabral HJ, Gerteis J, Hacker KA, Heeren T. Adolescent initiation of licit and illicit substance use: Impact of intrauterine exposures and post-natal exposure to violence. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2011;33(1):100–109. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2010.06.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldschmidt L, Cornelius MD, Day NL. Prenatal cigarette smoke exposure and early initiation of multiple substance use. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2012;14(6):694–702. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntr280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldschmidt L, Day NL, Richardson GA. Effects of prenatal marijuana exposure on child behavior problems at age 10. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2000;22(3):325–336. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(00)00066-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hollingshead AB. Two factor index of social position. New Haven, CT: Yale University; 1957. [Google Scholar]
- Knight JR, Goodman E, Pulerwitz T, DuRant RH. Reliability of the problem oriented screening instrument for teenagers (POSIT) in adolescent medical practice. The Journal of Adolescent Health. 2001;29(2):125–130. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(00)00206-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laird RD, Criss MM, Pettit GS, Dodge KA, Bates JE. Parents’ monitoring knowledge attenuates the link between antisocial friends and adolescent delinquent behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36(3):299–310. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9178-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lane SD, Webster NJ, Levandowski BA, Rubinstein RA, Keefe RH, Wojtowycz MA, Aubry RH. Environmental injustice: Childhood lead poisoning, teen pregnancy, and tobacco. The Journal of Adolescent Health. 2008;42(1):43–49. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis BA, Minnes S, Short EJ, Weishampel P, Satayathum S, Min MO, Singer LT. The effects of prenatal cocaine on language development at 10 years of age. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2011;33(1):17–24. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2010.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lidsky TI, Schneider JS. Lead neurotoxicity in children: Basic mechanisms and clinical correlates. Brain: A Journal of Neurology. 2003;126(1):5–19. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linares TJ, Singer LT, Kirchner HL, Short EJ, Min MO, Hussey P, Minnes S. Mental health outcomes of cocaine-exposed children at 6 years of age. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2006;31(1):85–97. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsj020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lumeng JC, Cabral HJ, Gannon K, Heeren T, Frank DA. Pre-natal exposures to cocaine and alcohol and physical growth patterns to age 8 years. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2007;29(4):446–457. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2007.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maughan B, Taylor A, Caspi A, Moffitt TE. Prenatal smoking and early childhood conduct problems: Testing genetic and environmental explanations of the association. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004;61(8):836–843. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.8.836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayes LC. A behavioral teratogenic model of the impact of prenatal cocaine exposure on arousal regulatory systems. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2002;24(3):385–395. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(02)00200-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLaney MA, Boca FD, Babor T. A validation study of the problem-oriented screening instrument for teenagers (POSIT) Journal of Mental Health. 1994;3(3):363–376. [Google Scholar]
- McLaughlin AA, Minnes S, Singer LT, Min M, Short EJ, Scott TL, Satayathum S. Caregiver and self-report of mental health symptoms in 9-year old children with prenatal cocaine exposure. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2011;33(5):582–591. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2011.03.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer PA, Pivetz T, Dignam TA, Homa DM, Schoonover J, Brody D Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Surveillance for elevated blood lead levels among children--United States, 1997–2001. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report Surveillance Summaries. 2003;52(10):1–21. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mickey RM, Greenland S. The impact of confounder selection criteria on effect estimation. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1989;129(1):125–137. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a115101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Min MO, Singer LT, Kirchner HL, Minnes S, Short E, Hussain Z, Nelson S. Cognitive development and low-level lead exposure in poly-drug exposed children. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2009;31(4):225–231. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2009.03.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Min MO, Singer LT, Minnes S, Kim H, Short E. Mediating links between maternal childhood trauma and preadolescent behavioral adjustment. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 2013;28(4):831–851. doi: 10.1177/0886260512455868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minnes S, Lang A, Singer L. Prenatal tobacco, marijuana, stimulant, and opiate exposure: Outcomes and practice implications. Addiction Science & Clinical Practice. 2011;6(1):57–70. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minnes S, Singer LT, Min MO, Wu M, Lang A, Yoon S. Effects of prenatal cocaine/polydrug exposure on substance use by age 15. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.09.031. (In Press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minnes S, Singer LT, Kirchner HL, Short E, Lewis B, Satayathum S, Queh D. The effects of prenatal cocaine exposure on problem behavior in children 4–10 years. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2010;32(4):443–451. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2010.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monshouwer K, Huizink AC, Harakeh Z, Raaijmakers QA, Reijneveld SA, Oldehinkel AJ, Vollebergh WA. Prenatal smoking exposure and the risk of behavioral problems and substance use in adolescence: The TRAILS study. European Addiction Research. 2011;17(6):342–350. doi: 10.1159/000334507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Needleman HL, Riess JA, Tobin MJ, Biesecker GE, Greenhouse JB. Bone lead levels and delinquent behavior. JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical Association. 1996;275(5):363–369. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson S, Lerner E, Needlman R, Salvator A, Singer LT. Cocaine, anemia, and neurodevelopmental outcomes in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics: JDBP. 2004;25(1):1–9. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200402000-00001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rahdert ER. DHHS Publication No. ADM 91-1735. Rockville, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse; 1991. The adolescent assessment and referral system manual. [Google Scholar]
- Richardson GA, Goldschmidt L, Leech S, Willford J. Prenatal cocaine exposure: Effects on mother- and teacher-rated behavior problems and growth in school-age children. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2011;33(1):69–77. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2010.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ridenour T. Assessment of liability and exposure to substance use and antisocial behavior©(ALEXSA©) Allison Park, PA: CORE measures; 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ridenour TA, Clark DB, Cottler LB. The illustration-based assessment of liability and EXposure to substance use and antisocial behavior for children. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 2009;35(4):242–252. doi: 10.1080/00952990902998715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ridenour TA, Meyer-Chilenski S, Reid EE. Developmental momentum toward substance dependence: Natural histories and pliability of risk factors in youth experiencing chronic stress. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2012:S87–98. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.12.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ridenour TA, Minnes S, Maldonado-Molina MM, Reynolds MD, Tarter RE, Clark DB. Psychometrics and cross-cultural comparisons of the illustration-based assessment of liability and exposure to substance use and antisocial behavior© for children. The Open Family Studies Journal. 2011;4(Suppl 1-M2):17–26. doi: 10.2174/1874922401104010017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shrier LA, Harris SK, Kurland M, Knight JR. Substance use problems and associated psychiatric symptoms among adolescents in primary care. Pediatrics. 2003;111(6):e699–705. doi: 10.1542/peds.111.6.e699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singer LT, Arendt R, Minnes S, Farkas K, Salvator A. Neurobehavioral outcomes of cocaine-exposed infants. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2000;22(5):653–666. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(00)00092-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singer LT, Arendt R, Minnes S, Farkas K, Salvator A, Kirchner HL, Kliegman R. Cognitive and motor outcomes of cocaine-exposed infants. JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical Association. 2002;287(15):1952–1960. doi: 10.1001/jama.287.15.1952. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singer LT, Minnes S, Short E, Arendt R, Farkas K, Lewis B, Kirchner HL. Cognitive outcomes of preschool children with prenatal cocaine exposure. JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical Association. 2004;291(20):2448–2456. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.20.2448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singer LT, Nelson S, Short E, Min MO, Lewis B, Russ S, Minnes S. Prenatal cocaine exposure: Drug and environmental effects at 9 years. The Journal of Pediatrics. 2008;153(1):105–111. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sood B, Delaney-Black V, Covington C, Nordstrom-Klee B, Ager J, Templin T, Sokol RJ. Prenatal alcohol exposure and childhood behavior at age 6 to 7 years: I. dose-response effect. Pediatrics. 2001;108(2):e34. doi: 10.1542/peds.108.2.e34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sood BG, Nordstrom Bailey B, Covington C, Sokol RJ, Ager J, Janisse J, Delaney-Black V. Gender and alcohol moderate caregiver reported child behavior after prenatal cocaine. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2005;27(2):191–201. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2004.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA) Office of Applied Studies, NSDUH Series H-44, HHS Publication No. SMA 12-4713. Rockville, MD: Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration; 2012. Results from the 2011 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: Summary of National Findings. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson BL, Levitt P, Stanwood GD. Prenatal exposure to drugs: Effects on brain development and implications for policy and education. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10(4):303–312. doi: 10.1038/nrn2598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warner TD, Behnke M, Eyler FD, Szabo NJ. Early adolescent cocaine use as determined by hair analysis in a prenatal cocaine exposure cohort. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2011;33(1):88–99. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2010.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children. 4. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2003. [Google Scholar]