Numerous studies of animal models have clearly demonstrated that abnormal visual experience can lead to abnormal visual development.1–5 Although there are a number of logical parallels in clinical patients, for example, infants with congenital cataracts or ptosis, patients with bilateral hyperopia present a more subtle challenge.6 Individuals with the same amount of hyperopia, siblings even, develop along different paths, exhibiting no noticeable abnormality, refractive esotropia and amblyopia, or bilateral amblyopia with no strabismus.
If a child with hyperopia develops refractive strabismus, the onset is typically months or years after birth,7 and it is assumed that cases of bilateral amblyopia also develop experience-dependent vision loss postnatally—the fact that amblyopia can be treated with manipulations of visual experience supports the idea that the amblyopia’s existence was also related to visual experience.8–10 If these conditions arise postnatally, was the child who accommodated to overcome their hyperopia the child who drove their accommodative convergence into a strabismus? Was the child who did not accommodate the child who experienced poor retinal image quality in both eyes leading to bilateral amblyopia?
These are both logical behaviors that might explain the different clinical outcomes. If so, one might predict that those who accommodate consistently would be the ones at risk for strabismus and those who do not would be at risk for bilateral amblyopia. Is there evidence to support this? In the only prospective longitudinal study to observe accommodation in hyperopia in the context of clinical outcome, the authors determined that infants who went on to develop strabismus tended to have larger accommodative errors before the onset of the deviation.11 Thus, it appears that the most common behavior might actually be to avoid accommodating in all cases in which the accommodation and vergence systems are under stress. For some reason, months or years after birth, some patients appear to exert enough accommodation to develop esotropia whereas others, with a preference for single vision perhaps, maintain poor accommodation and develop bilateral amblyopia.
Logic suggests that preventative optical correction of hyperopia should relieve stress on the relationship between accommodation and vergence to prevent the strabismus and amblyopia. Two groups have attempted these trials, with mixed results.12–16 Prescribing glasses for higher hyperopes during the first year after birth did not lead to a dramatic or systematic reduction in the incidence of strabismus, although it did consistently result in reduced prevalence of significant amblyopia at the outcome visit.
Where are we now? We know that visual experience can affect development17; we know that higher hyperopic children who do not emmetropize during infancy are at risk for strabismus and amblyopia; we know that simple optical correction starting during the first year does not routinely eliminate strabismus and amblyopia; and we know that patients who do not accommodate well may be at risk for poor emmetropization,18 strabismus, and amblyopia.11
In parallel, there is increasing interest in conducting vision screening at earlier ages.19 These programs are identifying young children with apparently asymptomatic moderate and high hyperopia who are still at risk for developing strabismus or amblyopia. Which children might need to be monitored? How should these children be managed? We also now know that the most dramatic period of emmetropization occurs during the first year of life in typically developing children and therefore that older children with hyperopia are likely to remain at significant risk even if they undergo some later emmetropization.18,20
On the basis of the literature described, one evidence-based logical screening approach is to target the children with poor accommodation. If they are the least likely to emmetropize and the most likely to develop strabismus and amblyopia, these would be the individuals on whom to concentrate limited resources. On the basis of the treatment trials, it is not clear that we can prevent strabismus with optical correction, but it is likely that the prevalence of significant amblyopia can be reduced and any strabismus can be detected rapidly with recognition of the individual’s risk. A model also incorporating family history is likely to have even greater sensitivity and specificity.21
The study by Dr. Tarczy-Hornoch22 in this issue of the Journal of AAPOS provides insight into the patients that might fail such a test or screening. She examined accommodative accuracy as a function of refractive error in 5- to 24-month-old patients. Her work suggests that a criterion of greater than 1.25 D of accommodative lag measured with modified Bell retinoscopy would identify the outer 5% of the distribution of performance and that individuals with lags greater than this value frequently have hyperopia greater than 4 D. These individuals with large lags have visual experience that is abnormal relative to their peers and appear to be at the most risk for strabismus and amblyopia,11 Now that we have consensus-based clinical guidelines for prescribing for hyperopia,23 studying these children with higher hyperopia in a prospective longitudinal design is complex and this study is important in identifying a lower refractive error bound for those at the greatest risk based on retinal image quality.
Our work supports Dr. Tarczy-Hornoch’s findings. We have recently published a related project24 in which we examined subjects 4 to 90 months of age who had never had any form of clinical intervention or treatment in their eye care. When we considered the group as a whole, those with less than approximately 4 D of hyperopia on cycloplegic refraction demonstrated small and similar lags of accommodation, whereas those with higher hyperopia, amblyopia, or strabismus had more variable lags. A receiver operating characteristic analysis designed to detect hyperopia greater than 5 D in any meridian, plus amblyopia and/ or strabismus, had an area under the curve of 0.90 (95% confidence interval 0.82–0.95), and for a lag criterion of 1.3 D had a sensitivity of 83.3% and a specificity of 96.5%. In agreement with Dr. Tarczy-Hornoch, this study also suggested that hyperopic subjects of more than approximately 4 D were at the greatest risk for increased accommodative lag, which, using Nott retinoscopy as opposed to modified Bell retinoscopy, was also shown to start at a little over a diopter of lag.
These data provided by Dr. Tarczy-Hornoch22 (and our work in agreement) provide clinicians with a clearer quantitative picture of the patients at risk for abnormal visual experience and abnormal visual development. The data do not address prediction of strabismus and amblyopia, but they do identify those patients apparently at greater risk. In moving forward, there are a number of important questions that need to be targeted. For example, we need to know how stable an individual’s accommodative performance is and to determine whether a hyperopic child might exhibit a large lag at one visit with a lag within the normal range at another visit. The variability in performance of higher hyperopes noted in both cross-sectional studies might reflect the range that any single individual would exhibit over time. Also, the bigger questions of when to identify these patients and the optimal management strategy remain. Data collected early in the first year might predict which infants will undergo typical emmetropization,18,20,25 whereas data collected later might help identify the hyperopic children at most risk and who might benefit from an intervention.11 Managing this risk and any remaining potential for some emmetropization in a preventative approach will be another challenge, which we will need to compare with the effort required and success achieved in a treatment approach once strabismus and amblyopia have developed. In addition this, of course, does not address the impact of accommodative performance on function during sustained near activity as is required in school.
Acknowledgments
Financial support: NEI RO1 EY014460.
References
- 1.Mitchell DE, Timney B. Postnatal development of function in the mammalian visual system. In: Brookhart JM, Mountcastle VD, editors. Handbook of physiology: The Nervous System. Bethesda, MD: American Physiological Society; 1984. pp. 507–55. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hendrickson AE, Movshon JA, Eggers HM, Gizzi MS, Boothe RG, Kiorpes L. Effects of early unilateral blur on the macaque’s visual system. II. Anatomical observations. J Neurosci. 1987;7:1327–39. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.07-05-01327.1987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Movshon JA, Eggers HM, Gizzi MS, Hendrickson AE, Kiorpes L, Boothe RG. Effects of early unilateral blur on the macaque’s visual system. III. Physiological observations. J Neurosci. 1987;7:1340–51. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.07-05-01340.1987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kiorpes L, Boothe RG, Hendrickson AE, Movshon JA, Eggers HM, Gizzi MS. Effects of early unilateral blur on the macaque’s visual system. I. Behavioral observations. J Neurosci. 1987;7:1318–26. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.07-05-01318.1987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Crawford ML, Harwerth RS. Ocular dominance column width and contrast sensitivity in monkeys reared with strabismus or anisometropia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2004;45:3036–42. doi: 10.1167/iovs.04-0029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Tarczy-Hornoch K. The epidemiology of early childhood hyperopia. Optom Vis Sci. 2007;84:115–23. doi: 10.1097/OPX.0b013e318031b674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Parks MM. Abnormal accommodative convergence in squint. AMA Arch Ophthalmol. 1958;59:364–80. doi: 10.1001/archopht.1958.00940040070008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Wallace DK, Chandler DL, Beck RW, et al. Treatment of bilateral refractive amblyopia in children three to less than 10 years of age. Am J Ophthalmol. 2007;144:487–96. doi: 10.1016/j.ajo.2007.05.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Klimek DL, Cruz OA, Scott WE, Davitt BV. Isoametropic amblyopia due to high hyperopia in children. J AAPOS. 2004;8:310–13. doi: 10.1016/j.jaapos.2004.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Fern KD. Visual acuity outcome in isometropic hyperopia. Optom Vis Sci. 1989;66:649–58. doi: 10.1097/00006324-198910000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Ingram RM, Gill LE, Goldacre MJ. Emmetropisation and accommodation in hypermetropic children before they show signs of squint—a preliminary analysis. Bull Soc Belge Ophtalmol. 1994;253:41–56. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ingram RM, Walker C, Wilson JM, Arnold PE, Lucas J, Dally S. A first attempt to prevent amblyopia and squint by spectacle correction of abnormal refractions from age 1 year. Br J Ophthalmol. 1985;69:851–3. doi: 10.1136/bjo.69.11.851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Ingram RM, Arnold PE, Dally S, Lucas J. Results of a randomised trial of treating abnormal hypermetropia from the age of 6 months. Br J Ophthalmol. 1990;74:158–9. doi: 10.1136/bjo.74.3.158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Ingram RM, Arnold PE, Dally S, Lucas J. Emmetropisation, squint, and reduced visual acuity after treatment. Br J Ophthalmol. 1991;75:414–16. doi: 10.1136/bjo.75.7.414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Atkinson J. Infant vision screening: Prediction and prevention of strabismus and amblyopia from refractive screening in the Cambridge Photorefraction Program. In: Simons K, editor. Early visual development, normal and abnormal. New York: Oxford University Press; 1993. pp. 335–48. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Anker S, Atkinson J, Braddick O, Nardini M, Ehrlich D. Non-cycloplegic refractive screening can identify infants whose visual outcome at 4 years is improved by spectacle correction. Strabismus. 2004;12:227–45. doi: 10.1080/09273970490517935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Candy TR, Wang J, Ravikumar S. Retinal image quality and postnatal visual experience during infancy. Optom Vis Sci. 2009;86:E556–71. doi: 10.1097/OPX.0b013e3181a76e6f. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Mutti DO, Mitchell GL, Jones LA, et al. Accommodation, acuity, and their relationship to emmetropization in infants. Optom Vis Sci. 2009;86:666–76. doi: 10.1097/OPX.0b013e3181a6174f. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Screening for Visual Impairment in Children Ages 1 to 5 Years, Topic Page. U.S. Preventive Services Task Force; Jan, 2011. [Accessed March 5, 2012]. Available at: http://www.uspreventiveservicestaskforce.org/uspstf/uspsvsch.htm. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Saunders KJ, Woodhouse JM, Westall CA. Emmetropisation in human infancy: rate of change is related to initial refractive error. Vision Res. 1995;35:1325–8. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)00222-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Aurell E, Norrsell K. A longitudinal study of children with a family history of strabismus: Factors determining the incidence of strabismus. Br J Ophthalmol. 1990;74:589–94. doi: 10.1136/bjo.74.10.589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Tarczy-Hornoch K. Accommodative lag and refractive error in infants and toddlers. J AAPOS. 2012;16:112–17. doi: 10.1016/j.jaapos.2011.10.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.American Academy of Ophthalmology Pediatric Ophthalmology/ Strabismus Panel. Pediatric Eye Evaluations. San Francisco, CA: American Academy of Ophthalmology; 2007. [Accessed March 5, 2012]. Preferred Practice Pattern® Guidelines. Available at: http://www.aao.org/ppp. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Candy TR, Gray KH, Hohenbary CC, Lyon DW. The accommodative lag of the young hyperopic patient. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012;53:143–9. doi: 10.1167/iovs.11-8174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Ehrlich DL, Braddick OJ, Atkinson J, et al. Infant emmetropization: Longitudinal changes in refraction components from nine to twenty months of age. Optom Vis Sci. 1997;74:822–43. doi: 10.1097/00006324-199710000-00022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]