Abstract
Recent work has demonstrated that α1-adrenergic receptor blockade impairs extinction in fear conditioning paradigms in rodents. However, studies of the role of α1-adrenergic receptors in extinction using other conditioning paradigms, such as those examining the conditioned effects of drug of abuse, have provided inconsistent results. In this article, we reanalyze and extend previously-reported findings of the effect of prazosin, an α1-adrenergic receptor antagonist, on the extinction of a cocaine-induced condition place preference in rats, using a median split of performance during the initial test for preference. This new reanalysis, which includes further extinction testing, revealed a paradoxical dose effect. A single post-test administration of a lower dose of prazosin, 0.3 mg/kg IP, impaired extinction in rats that demonstrated a below-median preference during initial testing, but had no effect on extinction in rats that demonstrated an above-median preference during initial testing. In contrast, a single post-test administration of a higher dose of prazosin, 1.0 mg/kg IP, enhanced extinction in rats that demonstrated an above-median preference during initial testing, but had no effect on extinction in rats that demonstrated a below-median preference during initial testing. Consistent with other studies of fear and drug conditioning, these results suggest the involvement of the α1-adrenergic receptor in the formation of extinction memories, but also indicate a potentially important differential effect on extinction based on the dose of prazosin and the strength of the initial learning.
Keywords: extinction, drug conditioning, cocaine, conditioned place preference, memory, reconsolidation
Introduction
Emotional arousal can enhance learning and memory, an effect mediated by norepinephrine signaling acting at adrenergic receptors. Preclinically, adrenergic receptors modulate the consolidation of both aversive and appetitive memories following associative learning between conditioned and unconditioned stimuli [1]. Adrenergic receptors have also been demonstrated to play an important role in the modulation of extinction learning [2–6]. Clinically, drugs acting at noradrenergic receptors are being examined as pharmacotherapies to supplement extinction-based therapies for disorders characterized by extremely maladaptive emotional memories, such as post-traumatic stress disorder [3].
In terms of extinction learning, the involvement of the α1-adrenergic receptor has only recently begun to be characterized. For example, in contextual fear conditioning, Bernardi et al. [7] demonstrated that mice administered prazosin, an α1-adrenergic receptor antagonist, following nonreinforced exposure to the training context impaired the rate of extinction across trials. This impairment of extinction of contextual fear by systemic prazosin has also been seen in rats, in which it was additionally demonstrated that prazosin microinjected into the medial prefrontal cortex-- a key structure involved in the extinction of conditioned fear-- also impaired extinction [8]. Thus, it is becoming apparent that α1-adrenergic receptors are an important contributor to the extinction of contextual fear.
The potential contribution of α1-adrenergic receptors to extinction in other conditioning paradigms, such as those involving drugs of abuse, is less clear. Bernardi et al. [7] found that prazosin administered to mice following several nonreinforced tests in the cocaine conditioned place preference (CPP) paradigm failed to affect the rate of extinction of preference for the cocaine-paired environment. However, prazosin-treated mice showed an increased preference relative to vehicle-treated mice during cocaine reconditioning trials, indicative of an effect on the persistence of extinction. In contrast, a single administration of prazosin either systemically or into the basolateral amygdala in rats following a nonreinforced test for cocaine CPP not only failed to impair extinction on a subsequent test, but actually reduced the preference relative to vehicle-treated animals, indicative of an enhanced extinction or reconsolidation-like impairment [9]. Because of these inconsistencies in the effects of prazosin on extinction learning, especially with respect to the CPP paradigm, further examination of the role of α1-adrenergic receptor antagonists in this paradigm during extinction is warranted.
In the current study, we sought to clarify the role of prazosin in the extinction of cocaine CPP by reanalyzing data from our aforementioned study [9] with additional data not previously examined. Based on ample evidence indicating that the effects of manipulations on extinction may depend critically on the behavioral impact of the extinction session [10–13], we examined the behavioral response to prazosin with respect to differences in the initial response to nonreinforced presentation of drug-associated cues based on a median split [10–12].
Materials & Methods
Subjects
Forty-eight male Sprague Dawley rats, aged 10–12 weeks at the start of experiments, were used as subjects. Rats were housed two per cage, with food and water available ad libitum. Experiments were performed in accordance with the NIH guidelines for the care and use of laboratory animals.
Apparatus & Behavioral Procedures
CPP was assessed using an unbiased design [14] in four automated one-compartment place conditioning chambers (modified from San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA) and involved the following phases performed on consecutive days: habituation, conditioning and testing (extinction sessions), as described in detail previously [9]. Briefly, on alternate days over eight conditioning sessions (four cocaine/four vehicle), rats received cocaine (Sigma, St. Louis, MO; 20 mg/kg IP) prior to 25-min conditioning trials on either a GRID or HOLE floor and vehicle (saline; 1 ml/kg) prior to 25-min trials on the alternate floor. For extinction sessions, rats received a saline injection immediately prior to placement into the apparatus with half GRID/half HOLE floor for a 15-min (Session 1) or 25-min preference test (Sessions 2–6). Rats received a single injection of prazosin (Sigma; 0.3 mg/kg or 1.0 mg/kg IP) or vehicle (sterile water; 2ml/kg) immediately following Session 1. For our reanalysis, which consists of data from a subset of rats that received four additional extinction sessions not reported previously (Sessions 3–6), preference is indicated as the percent time on the cocaine-paired floor during the first 15 min of each session. Rats with an initial preference below 50% for the cocaine-paired floor were dropped from subsequent analyses (n = 10). To further examine the effect of prazosin, rats in the vehicle and prazosin groups were separated into two subgroups (HIGH or LOW preference) based on a median split of the percent time on the drug-paired floor in each group during Session 1.
Statistical analysis
Data were analyzed using repeated measures ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc tests performed where indicated. Significance was set at p < .05.
Results
Rats showed a CPP during the first extinction session that did not differ as a function of group assignment (F(2,35) = 2.5, p = .11; Figure 1A). There was a main effect of session [F(3.7,129.2) = 8.3, p < .001], a main effect of drug [F(2,35) = 5.2, p < .05], and no interaction [F(7.4,129.2) = 0.6, p = .74]. Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed that the 0.3 and 1.0 mg/kg prazosin-treated animals differed over the course of extinction trials (p < .01), while there was no difference between vehicle- and either of the prazosin-treated groups (ps > .20). Activity levels (data not shown) did not differ across extinction trials between groups, as indicated by a significant main effect of session [F(3.9,137.3) = 16.0, p < .001], but no effect of drug [F(2,35) = 1.5, p = .24] and no interaction [F(7.8,137.3) = 0.5, p = .84]. Thus, prazosin had no residual effects on locomotor activity. To examine the effects of prazosin on strong and weak preferences, animals were divided into HIGH and LOW preference groups (above or below the group's median, respectively).
Figure 1.
Post-session prazosin both impaired and enhanced extinction of a cocaine-induced CPP, based on dose and initial preference. Data represent mean (±SEM) percent time on the drug-paired floor during the 15-min Session 1 and the first 15-min of each subsequent session. (A) Overall, rats administered 0.3 mg/kg prazosin (n=12) or 1.0 mg/kg prazosin (n=12) following Session 1 showed no difference in extinction compared to the vehicle-treated group (n=14). (B) Rats in the HIGH initial preference group administered 1.0 mg/kg prazosin (n=6) showed an enhanced extinction relative to vehicle- (n=7) and 0.3 mg/kg prazosin-treated (n=6) animals. (C) Rats in the LOW initial preference group administered 0.3 mg/kg prazosin (n=6) showed an impaired extinction relative to vehicle- (n=7) and 1.0 mg/kg prazosin-treated (n=6) animals.
Rats in the HIGH subgroup showed a CPP during Session 1 that did not differ as a function of group assignment (F(2,16) = 1.9, p = .19; Figure 1B). There was a main effect of session [F(5,80) = 7.6, p < .001], a main effect of drug [F(2,16) = 7.7, p < .01], and no interaction [F(10,80) = 0.5, p = .90]. Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed that the 1.0 mg/kg prazosin-treated animals differed from both the vehicle and 0.3 mg/kg prazosin groups over the course of extinction trials (ps < .05), while there was no difference between vehicle- and 0.3 mg/kg prazosin-treated animals (p = 1.0). These results indicate that the higher dose of prazosin enhanced extinction in the HIGH preference subgroup, while the lower dose of prazosin had no effect. Activity levels (data not shown) did not differ across extinction trials between groups, as indicated by a significant main effect of session [F(3.1,49.9) = 11.6, p < .001], but no effect of drug [F(2,16) = 0.7, p = .52] and no interaction [F(6.2,49.9) = 1.0, p = .46]. Thus, prazosin had no residual effects on locomotor activity.
Rats in the LOW subgroup showed a CPP during Session 1 that differed as a function of group assignment (F(2,16) = 6.7, p < .01; Figure 1C). Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed that the 1.0 mg/kg prazosin-treated animals demonstrated a significantly lower initial preference than the other two groups (ps < .05); however, the vehicle and 0.3 mg/kg prazosin groups did not differ (p = 1.0). There was a main effect of drug [F(2,16) = 4.8, p < .05], but no main effect of session [F(3.1,80) = 2.1, p = .10] or interaction [F(6.2,80) = 1.4, p = .24]. Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed that the 0.3 mg/kg prazosin-treated animals differed from the vehicle group over the course of extinction trials (p < .05), while there was no difference between vehicle- and 1.0 mg/kg prazosin-treated animals (p = 1.0). These results indicate that the lower dose of prazosin impaired extinction in the LOW preference subgroup, while the high dose of prazosin had no effect. Activity levels (data not shown) did not differ across extinction trials between groups, as indicated by a significant main effect of session [F(5,80) = 6.6, p < .001], but no effect of drug [F(2,16) = 0.7, p = .51] and no interaction [F(10,80) = 1.1, p = .37]. Thus, prazosin had no residual effects on locomotor activity.
Discussion
The current reanalysis of our previous finding [9], which includes examination of additional extinction sessions and a median-based behavioral performance approach, indicates that the α1-adrenergic receptor antagonist prazosin can both impair and enhance extinction of a cocaine CPP in rats when administered following an initial test of preference, depending on the dose. Furthermore, these effects of prazosin on CPP were contingent upon the initial preference of the animal, and were maintained over several trials despite the animals receiving only a single administration of prazosin.
The lower dose of prazosin (0.3 mg/kg) impaired extinction only in animals with a lower initial preference, suggesting that weaker learned behaviors may be more vulnerable to the effects of low-dose prazosin. This finding is consistent with recent work demonstrating the ability of α1-adrenergic receptor antagonists to impair the rate of extinction of contextual fear conditioning in both mice and rats [7,8] and the persistence of extinction of cocaine CPP in mice [7]. In contrast, the higher dose of prazosin (1.0 mg/kg) enhanced extinction or produced a reconsolidation-like effect, as evidenced by a decrease in preference following a single post-session administration only in animals with a higher initial preference. Interestingly, this effect-- in which prazosin enhanced, rather than impaired, learning-- is consistent with recent findings in the fear literature. Lazzaro et al. [15] demonstrated that terazosin, another α1-adrenergic receptor antagonist, administered both systemically and intra-lateral amygdala prior to conditioning trials, enhanced fear learning. Similar mechanisms may result in the enhancement of extinction seen in our study. For example, insomuch as the extinction of a behavioral response likely represents the balance between the impairment of an excitatory drive and the enhancement of an inhibitory drive [for an extensive discussion on distinctive circuits mediating excitation and inhibition during extinction learning, see [16]], it is possible that the different doses of prazosin have contrasting effects on excitatory and inhibitory neuronal populations and brain regions that mediate these different processes.
Previous studies have demonstrated that several drugs, especially those involved in mediating the stress response and/or arousal, can both enhance and impair memory. The effect of epinephrine follows an inverted-U dose–response function, such that low-to-intermediate doses enhance memory while high doses impair memory [17,18], implicating arousal level, which is correlated with alterations in brain norepinephrine levels [19], as a key determinant of retention [19,20]. Unfortunately, the role of differential levels of anatagonists on these effects has thus far been unexplored. However, norepinephrine acts on postsynaptic α1-adrenergic receptors to mediate its effects on memory and regulates its own activity via negative feedback involving the activation of presynaptic α2-adrenergic receptors [21]. Thus, the two doses of prazosin used here may differentially alter this negative feedback process based on low vs. high arousal and corresponding norepinephrine levels to enhance or impair further norepinephrine release, resulting in differential extinction learning.
We cannot exclude the possibility that one or both doses of prazosin used in the current study may exert nonspecific effects unrelated to α1-adrenergic receptor blockade. For example, previous studies have suggested that prazosin at high doses may have affinity for the α2-adrenergic receptor [22], and thus may directly regulate norepinephrine release presynaptically, which also could explain the differential effects of dose outlined here. However, prazosin administration at doses similar to those employed in the current study demonstrates minimal affinity for the α2-adrenergic receptor in rat brain [reviewed in 23]. However, several subtypes of the α1-adrenergic receptor have been characterized in the brain, with different affinities for prazosin, and these low and high affinity binding sites showed changes in density based on the chronic administration of low and high prazosin doses similar to or higher than those used here [24]. These subtypes mediate different signal transduction mechanisms [25] that may be responsible for the differences in behavior seen here. Although our behavioral effects were achieved after a single administration, additional studies are needed to better characterize how the cellular and molecular effects of prazosin interact with behavior.
Other studies support the idea that the effects of pharmacological manipulations on extinction may depend critically on the behavioral impact of the extinction session [10–12]. These experiments, as well as the current study, encourage analyzing behavioral responses in different ways, including examining extinction at different parts of the behavioral scale and in subpopulations that show different levels of initial conditioning, especially regarding measures of CPP [22]. One critical feature of drug dependence in humans is the magnitude of the associations between the rewarding effects of the drug and the cues that predict drug availability. The findings presented here further implicate the strength of drug associations as a potential indicator of resistance to the pharmacological supplementation of extinction-based therapies.
Conclusion
Our findings further suggest a role for α1-adrenergic receptors in drug conditioning paradigms, indicating that prazosin can alter extinction learning based on the dose of the drug and the initial level of learning. These results may have important implications for the use of α1-adrenergic receptors as a supplement to therapies designed to extinguish chronic maladaptive memories.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH077111) to K. Matthew Lattal, National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01 DA025922) to K. Matthew Lattal and Marcelo A. Wood and a grant from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (F31 DA022844) to Rick E. Bernardi.
Footnotes
Conflicts of Interest and Source of Funding: No conflicts of interest are declared.
References
- [1].McGaugh JL, Roozendaal B. Drug enhancement of memory consolidation: historical perspective and neurobiological implications. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2009;202:3–14. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1285-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [2].Holmes A, Quirk GJ. Pharmacological facilitation of fear extinction and the search for adjunct treatments for anxiety disorders--the case of yohimbine. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2010;31:2–7. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2009.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [3].Mueller D, Cahill SP. Noradrenergic modulation of extinction learning and exposure therapy. Behav Brain Res. 2010;208:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.11.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [4].Bouton ME, Westbrook RF, Corcoran KA, Maren S. Contextual and temporal modulation of extinction: behavioral and biological mechanisms. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:352–360. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [5].Janak PH, Bowers MS, Corbit LH. Compound stimulus presentation and the norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor atomoxetine enhance long-term extinction of cocaine-seeking behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:975–985. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [6].Janak PH, Corbit LH. Deepened extinction following compound stimulus presentation: noradrenergic modulation. Learn Mem. 2011;18:1–10. doi: 10.1101/lm.1923211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [7].Bernardi RE, Lattal KM. A role for alpha-adrenergic receptors in extinction of conditioned fear and cocaine conditioned place preference. Behav Neurosci. 2010;124:204–210. doi: 10.1037/a0018909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [8].Do-Monte FH, Allensworth M, Carobrez AP. Impairment of contextual conditioned fear extinction after microinjection of alpha-1-adrenergic blocker prazosin into the medial prefrontal cortex. Behav Brain Res. 2010;211:89–95. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [9].Bernardi RE, Ryabinin AE, Berger SP, Lattal KM. Post-retrieval disruption of a cocaine conditioned place preference by systemic and intrabasolateral amygdala beta2- and alpha1-adrenergic antagonists. Learn Mem. 2009;16:777–789. doi: 10.1101/lm.1648509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [10].Bouton ME, Vurbic D, Woods AM. D-cycloserine facilitates context-specific fear extinction learning. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2008;90:504–510. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [11].Weber M, Hart J, Richardson R. Effects of D-cycloserine on extinction of learned fear to an olfactory cue. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2007;87:476–482. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [12].Burgos-Robles A, Vidal-Gonzalez I, Quirk GJ. Sustained conditioned responses in prelimbic prefrontal neurons are correlated with fear expression and extinction failure. J Neurosci. 2009;29:8474–8482. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0378-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [13].McNally GP, Westbrook RF. Predicting danger: the nature, consequences, and neural mechanisms of predictive fear learning. Learn Mem. 2006;13:245–253. doi: 10.1101/lm.196606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [14].Cunningham CL, Gremel CM, Groblewski PA. Drug-induced conditioned place preference and aversion in mice. Nat Protoc. 2006;1:1662–1670. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [15].Lazzaro SC, Hou M, Cunha C, LeDoux JE, Cain CK. Antagonism of lateral amygdala alpha1-adrenergic receptors facilitates fear conditioning and long-term potentiation. Learn Mem. 2010;17:489–493. doi: 10.1101/lm.1918210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [16].Peters J, Kalivas PW, Quirk GJ. Extinction circuits for fear and addiction overlap in prefrontal cortex. Learn Mem. 2009;16:279–288. doi: 10.1101/lm.1041309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [17].Baldi E, Bucherelli C. The inverted “u-shaped” dose-effect relationships in learning and memory: modulation of arousal and consolidation. Nonlinearity Biol Toxicol Med. 2005;3:9–21. doi: 10.2201/nonlin.003.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [18].Calabrese EJ. Adrenergic receptors: biphasic dose responses. Crit Rev Toxicol. 2001;31:523–538. doi: 10.1080/20014091111802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [19].Gold PE, van Buskirk R. Posttraining brain norepinephrine concentrations: correlation with retention performance of avoidance training and with peripheral epinephrine modulation of memory processing. Behav Biol. 1978;23:509–520. doi: 10.1016/s0091-6773(78)91614-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [20].Gold PE, van Buskirk R, Haycock JW. Effects of posttraining epinephrine injections on retention of avoidance training in mice. Behav Biol. 1977;20:197–204. doi: 10.1016/s0091-6773(77)90760-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [21].Gibbs ME, Summers RJ. Role of adrenoceptor subtypes in memory consolidation. Prog Neurobiol. 2002;67:345–391. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(02)00023-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [22].Hancock AA, Buckner SA, Ireland LM, Knepper SM, Kerwin JF., Jr. Actions of terazosin and its enantiomers at subtypes of alpha 1- and alpha 2-adrenoceptors in vitro. J Recept Signal Transduct Res. 1995;15:863–885. doi: 10.3109/10799899509049862. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23].Kyncl JJ. Pharmacology of terazosin. Am J Med. 1986;80:12–19. doi: 10.1016/0002-9343(86)90846-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [24].Takita M, Taniguchi T, Zhu J, Muramatsu I. Chronic treatment with prazosin causes a subtype-specific increase in the alpha 1-adrenoceptor density of the stressed rat cerebral cortex. J Pharm Pharmacol. 1997;49:802–805. doi: 10.1111/j.2042-7158.1997.tb06116.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [25].Minneman KP. Alpha 1-adrenergic receptor subtypes, inositol phosphates, and sources of cell Ca2+ Pharmacol Rev. 1988;40:87–119. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]