DEFINITIONS AND SCOPE OF THE HEALTH PROBLEM
Spatial neglect is defined as a failure to report, respond, or orient to stimuli in contralesional space after brain injury that is not explained by primary sensory or motor deficits (Heilman, 1979). Conservative estimates in the US population reveal that spatial neglect is present in at least 30% of stroke survivors leading to an estimated total annual incidence of 239 000 people with neglect in the acute phase. At least 10% of patients with acute neglect will experience symptoms in the chronic phase (Barrett et al., 2006); thus a conservative estimate of chronic neglect is about 3% of US stroke survivors, or 195 000 people. These numbers are comparable with the prevalence of spinal cord injury of 259 000 cases (National Spinal Cord Injury Statistical Center, 2009) and brain and central nervous system cancer of 111 000 cases in the USA. (National Cancer Institute, Surveillance Epidemiology and End Results, 2009). Neglect occurs in approximately 50% of right hemisphere stroke survivors (Buxbaumet al., 2004) and up to 75% of patients may persist with some symptoms in the chronic phase (Farne et al., 2004). Neglect can also occur after left hemisphere injury, but it is less common and persistent than when it occurs from right-sided lesions (Beis et al., 2004).
The estimated direct and indirect cost of stroke in the USA for 2009 is 68.9 billion dollars (American Heart Association, 2009). The number of stroke survivors may increase with the aging population, as aging is associated with increased stroke risk (Hier et al., 1983; Dooneief and Mayeux, 1989; Ringman et al., 2004). Disability associated with spatial neglect may be overlooked, and those most at risk, i.e., those with right hemispheric stroke, may be less likely to receive acute medical attention than those with a left hemisphere injury (Gainotti, 1972; Ringman et al., 2004; Fink, 2005; Foerch et al., 2005). Spatial neglect is associated with longer average length of hospital stay (Katz et al., 1999; Gillen et al., 2005), increased family burden (Buxbaum et al., 2004), and higher requirements for assistance and skilled nursing placement (Rundek et al., 2000). Spatial neglect rehabilitation thus represents a unique opportunity for promoting recovery and preserving precious economic resources.
CLINICAL CHARACTERISTICS OF SPATIAL NEGLECT
Spatial neglect, unlike many other cognitive disorders, is demonstrated in different species of mammals (Payne and Rushmore, 2004). In humans, recognition of the right hemisphere dominance for spatial attention is fundamental to understanding this disorder (Heilman and Van Den Abell, 1979, 1980). Accordingly, the right hemisphere, unlike the left, attends to both hemispaces and plays a critical role generating spatial-based perceptual-attention to external stimuli, producing and maintaining internal spatial representations and directing motor-intentional responses. Since the right hemispace is represented by both hemispheres while the left hemispace is only represented by the right, injury of the right hemisphere may disproportionately affect perceptual-attention, representation, and motor-intention functions related to the left side.
Cognitive neuroscience basis
Successful rehabilitation of a patient with spatial neglect requires understanding the neuropsychological mechanisms underlying its behavioral manifestations. Contrasting a subject’s behavior between tasks with different demands allows the dissociation of a variety of functional processes involved in spatial attention, perception, and its related motor functions. The behavioral and neurophysiologic study of patients and animals with focal brain lesions and of normal individuals has provided the main methodology for the recognition of spatial neglect as a distinct entity and for the localization of the brain regions most commonly associated with its clinical characteristics. Functional neuroimaging allows the visualization of regions of increased activity during the execution of a task. Sometimes functional imaging studies reveal brain activation in more extensive or distant areas than might be expected from lesion studies. The findings of these different methodologies are consistent with the brain’s organization as a functional network where, depending on their connectivity, some areas of the network are more critical in the processing of information than others. Therefore, lesion studies identify the critical areas of a network while functional imaging identifies the participating components of the network without necessarily demonstrating which of these components are crucial (Mesulam, 2000). An extensive analysis of the neuroscientific foundations of spatial neglect is not the focus of this chapter and has been reviewed elsewhere (Adair and Barrett, 2008). Here we summarize some of the essential concepts that are relevant for the application of neuroscience knowledge to theoretically based models of rehabilitation.
Two broadly defined stages of neuropsychological processing have been dissociated in spatial neglect: a perceptual-attentional component, necessary for spatial operations or “where” constructs, and a premotor-intentional component necessary for “aiming” or directing movements in three-dimensional, and especially contralesional, space. Posterior brain regions of dorsal and lateral parieto-temporal polymodal association cortex and their subcortical connections, including associated white matter tracts, are thought to be involved in the perceptual-attentional component (Mort et al., 2003; Hillis et al., 2005). This component includes a variety of “downstream” operations, from the detection of a stimulus to its representation and manipulation. It may also affect tactile, visual, and auditory sensory modalities. The phenomenon of extinction, in which the patient fails to perceive a contralesional stimulus only when it is presented simultaneously with a stimulus on the ipsilesional side, may be best explained by limited capacity of perceptual-attentional resources (Heilman, 1979) since the “extinguished” stimulus is actually processed through sensory systems (Marzi et al., 2001; Beversdorf et al., 2008) and its perception may vary depending on modality (Hillis et al., 2006) or specific task demands (Riestra et al., 2001, 2002). Anterior brain regions of prefrontal and premotor cortex and their subcortical basal ganglia connections are thought to critically support an “aiming” premotor-intentional component. This component is often referred to as directional hypokinesia but it is better conceptualized as a disorder of intentional movement directed toward or performed in the contralesional hemispace, or involving the contralesional hemibody (Coslett et al., 1990; Heilman, 2004; Nys et al., 2006; Sapir et al., 2007). Of note, some patients with dorsal, parietal cortical lesions who lack putamenal or frontal subcortical injury may also demonstrate prominent “aiming” dysfunction (Triggs et al., 1994; Na et al., 1999; Barrett and Burkholder, 2006). Therefore, the critical brain substrate of “where” versus “aiming” spatial bias is still underspecified. Finally, ascending regulation from the reticular activation system and long dopaminergic pathways is crucial to maintain the level of arousal and motor activation related to spatial tasks.
Clinical behavioral abnormalities associated with spatial dysfunction
In addition to the level of arousal/activation and stage of processing, other factors influence behavior in spatial neglect. A subject may engage different frames of reference in the performance of a task: an egocentric frame in which the subject uses his own body and midline as a reference for spatial computations and an allocentric frame in which the subject performs these computations based on the object’s spatial features regardless of its location relative to the subject’s body (Hillis et al., 1998; Bartolomeo and Chokron, 1999). Stimulus distance location also plays an important role depending on whether the subject operates in personal space related to bodily surface, peripersonal space related to near, reaching distance, or far extrapersonal space (Mennemeier et al., 1992; Halligan et al., 2003; Committeri et al., 2007). Other manifestations of right hemisphere injury including mood disorders and disorders of emotional communication (Gainotti, 1972; Starkstein et al., 1989), unawareness of deficit (anosognosia) (Bisiach et al., 1986), changes in body schema (Coslett, 1998), and primary sensory and motor deficits (Bottini et al., 1995, 2005; Vallar et al., 1995) are important for planning rehabilitation strategies, as they impact the therapeutic outcome.
Clinical impact of neglect-associated abnormalities
Specific spatial neglect-related problems, such as those described above, can be observed to affect patients not only in the laboratory, but also in their daily activities. Deficits in orienting to or exploring stimuli in contralesional space related to abnormal “where” input, internal imagery or representation, and “aiming” motor-intentional output appear to the careful observer to be distinctly manifest in natural performance errors. For example, “where” errors may occur when the patient does not notice family or clinicians approaching in the neglected space; imagery errors may occur when patients attempt to direct a helper in fetching personal effects from the room or navigating the hospital; “aiming” errors may affect posture or transfers. Behavior may also vary depending on whether the subject is expected to bathe, shave or dress (personal space), read or explore a food tray (peripersonal space), or walk to the bathroom or a therapy room down the hall (extrapersonal space). Rehabilitation interventions may have specific effects on “where,” imagery, or “aiming” processing and depend on frame of reference or operational space. Therefore, an incomplete understanding of the patient’s symptomatology and assigning all spatial neglect patients to a single rehabilitation approach or outcome measure may present a major problem with validity, as it would be expected to produce disparate or incongruent results across studies.
TREATMENT THEORY AND TREATMENT FOR SPATIAL NEGLECT
Approaches to the spatial neglect syndrome
The different personnel who make up the interdisciplinary rehabilitation team may actually employ different treatment approaches. A restorative or restitutive approach attempts to reinstate premorbid capacity of injured brain-behavior systems via visual, tactile or auditory stimulation cuing, which is gradually reduced and then eliminated, and with integration of engaging activities. A vicariative strategy activates a system closely related to or sharing key components with spatial cognitive systems in order to increase spatial activation. For example, patients might be asked to walk and encouraged to advance the hemiparetic left leg, while they are simultaneously instructed to monitor their body spatial position or take note of physical details of their surroundings. A compensatory approach may involve counseling the family about safety issues, and arranging for the patient’s immediate environment to be visually simplified. Physician prescription of medication treatment to increase arousal or attention may be viewed as either restitutive or compensatory. It is not clear what combination of restitutive, vicariative, and compensatory approaches is ideal to improve spatial neglect symptoms, and at present we prefer to use restitutive and vicariative approaches, with compensation limited to environmental management for physical safety.
Evidence-based medicine approach to spatial neglect treatment
Different treatment and rehabilitation approaches for spatial neglect have been described (see Pierce and Buxbaum, 2002; Proto et al., 2009 for reviews). We believe that using these different treatments to selectively target “where” and “aiming” components might greatly improve rehabilitation. Current literature, however, does not reflect attempts to examine this aim directly and few studies discuss how patient characteristics influence treatment candidacy. There is not a definite answer to whether there are any spatial neglect treatments generally applicable to improve functional behavior and recent reviews emphasized that few studies employ functional-based outcomes (Bowen et al., 2002; Lincoln and Bowen, 2006; Bowen and Lincoln, 2007). We reviewed these three articles and seven other evidence-based published resources (Cicerone et al., 2000, 2005; Bowen et al., 2002; Jutai et al., 2003; Cappa et al., 2005; Luauté et al., 2006; Teasell et al., 2008; Menon et al., 2009) and concluded that evidence-based reviews unfortunately do not provide either direct clinical or scientific guidance in spatial neglect treatment because they are internally inconsistent in several ways. Not all the reviews support a treatment or treatments as potentially effective nor do they agree on the level of evidence supporting these treatments. Only some of the above papers acknowledged that efficacy might differ by intervention, or considered efficacy of different types of treatment separately. The evidence-based reviews also do not consider the reasons for failure to obtain treatment effect in reviewed studies. Whether failed treatments were potentially effective, but inappropriately targeted, or how subject characteristics influenced treatment efficacy at the level of impairment, behavior, or function was not considered and may be critically important.
Pathways and protocols of interest
As we stated, there is disagreement among the evidence-based sources, and the information they present cannot be regarded as definitive. However, based on our best assessment of the available information we favour three treatment approaches which received Level 1a or “strong” support in evidence-based reviews and that may be used for research treatment studies and for standardizing clinical patient care. These approaches are: visual scanning treatment (Weinberg et al., 1977, 1979), limb activation therapy (Robertson and North, 1993; see also Kalra et al., 1997; Eskes et al., 2003), and “general treatment” (Jutai et al., 2003), which we interpret as similar to “perceptual training” (Teasell et al., 2008). Prism adaptation training (Rossi et al., 1990; Rossetti et al., 1998) is an emerging therapy for which there is positive supportive evidence (see review of studies in Menon et al., 2009), at this point; however, we are reluctant to recommend it as standard because the critical determinants of treatment effect, the functional abilities likely to improve, and characteristics of patients most likely to benefit are still not established.
We recognize that therapists may be enthusiastic about treatments with which they have personal, hands-on successful experience and that have been reported to be useful and therefore we believe it is reasonable for a team to develop rehabilitation programs that combine evidence-based and empirically supported approaches. Unfortunately, manualized treatments of definite functional benefit for a variety of patients with spatial neglect and different symptom complexes are not currently available. Clinical teams should develop standardized approaches of assessment and treatment based on good quality clinical reasoning. We recommend taking treatment procedures directly from research sources in which treatment efficacy was reported. Commonly, research procedures are too time-consuming, difficult to understand, and hard to administer reliably. It is often helpful to involve a colleague experienced in clinical research to help with this stage. Such a colleague can recommend methods of shortening treatment procedures, can translate research terminology into treatment terms, and can assist with hands-on instruction of the therapy team. A number of proposed “where” interventions and “aiming” interventions are listed in Table 29.1.
Table 29.1.
“Where” interventions | “Aiming” interventions |
---|---|
Devices, medications increasing arousal
|
Adaptation to right-shifting prisms |
Limb activation therapy | |
Constraint induced movement therapy | |
Tool use movement therapy | |
Scanning training, if administered as motor habit training | |
Induced asymmetry/selective sensory deprivation
| |
Physical therapy/mobilization of the neglected body or both sides of the body (e.g., standing) | |
Medication? | |
Internal or external illusions
|
|
Medication? |
Theoretically proposed mechanisms of action of rehabilitative treatments (framework for future research). “Where” interventions may affect perceptual/attentional input, or internal sensory representations or spatial imagery. “Aiming” interventions may affect motorintentional output or premotor imagery.
We would also recommend that instead of having one set of practitioners targeting one task (for example, reading), while another set of practitioners work on another set of behaviors (for example, toileting), the rehabilitation team leader train the therapy team in identifying two or three behaviors or functions that the team agrees are priorities for interdisciplinary care. If the entire team can prioritize one set of behaviors, for example, toileting, appropriate targets for treatments for other modalities can be derived, for example, reliable identification of visual-spatial cues to locate the bathroom from different perspectives, or thematic language exercises focused on grooming and hygiene. The assessment pathway may then include deciding upon which targeted behaviors involve either “where” or “aiming” deficits or a combination of both. The team may also designate strategies for a coexistent major cognitive deficit (memory loss, communication disorders, and executive dysfunction) whose contribution may imply that a prioritized task will need to be treated slightly differently. For those prioritized target behaviors that the team agrees are primarily either “where” or “aiming” behaviors, it is then appropriate to implement a treatment that primarily addresses either the “where” or “aiming” system. Therapists should be provided with the choice of at least two behavioral treatments in each modality as can be seen in Table 29.1. As we have stated, one of the shortcomings of the treatment evidence information available is that outcomes of different rehabilitation approaches have not been related to underlying neglect mechanisms. The information included in the table is based on “proof of principle” evidence (Vallar et al., 1996; Barrett et al., 1999, 2001; Barrett and Burkholder, 2006; Fortis et al., 2009) and it is intended to suggest a structure for future systematic clinical research and clinical trials that include patient stratification according to both the type and level of deficit.
Recording outcomes and periodic self-audit
Responsible use of behavioral spatial neglect treatment protocols or pathways requires periodically examining the treatment outcomes and picking measures appropriately close to the proposed mechanism of the treatment rather than generic functional measures, which may be more distant from direct treatment effect. Recording severity of spatial neglect as measured by a standard instrument such as the Catherine Bergego Scale (Azouvi et al., 2003) and Functional Independence Measure (FIM) score at treatment initiation is necessary. In many settings the FIM score (Uniform Data System for Medical Rehabilitation, 1997) is the only periodic reassessment tool utilized, but we find that recording neglect-specific scores are much more accurate, as many factors which do not pertain to visual-spatial function can affect the FIM. Key staff members identified as having knowledge and interest in directing the spatial neglect program should review outcomes for each of the protocols being implemented at the institution to see which appear most promising for further development. Staff can also be instructed to utilize a treatment that emerges as clearly most feasible or superior.
PHARMACOLOGICALTREATMENT OF SPATIAL NEGLECT
Dopaminergic modulation
Theoretically, several pharmacological agents could be useful for the treatment of neglect; however, there are few controlled studies addressing specifically the effect of drug therapy in this condition. Among the pharmacological approaches, the best documented are those directed to monoamines, particularly the dopaminergic system. In animal models dopamine pharmacotherapy restores contralesional spatial attention and orienting (see Schwarting and Huston, 1996, for a review). Dopaminergic pharmacotherapy may improve spatial neglect in humans (Fleet et al., 1987;Mukand et al., 2001) and neglect symptoms sensitive to dopaminergic supplementation may be identified with intentional premotor exploratory function (Bisiach et al., 1990; Coslett et al., 1990; Tegnér and Levander, 1991; Barrett et al., 1999; Mapstone et al., 2003; Heilman, 2004). However, the influence of these agents may be selective to certain symptoms or may even worsen neglect in some patients, particularly those with basal ganglia lesions affecting ipsilesional postsynaptic dopaminergic transmission (Geminiani et al., 1998; Grujic et al., 1998; Barrett et al., 1999).
Combined dopaminergic and adrenergic modulation
Other agents with aminergic properties have been tried with varied success. Amantadine, a medication with dopaminergic activity (but that also has anticholinergic and antiglutamatergic effects) was ineffective for improving several neglect measures in a double-blind placebo controlled study involving four patients with neglect (Buxbaum et al., 2007). Methylphenidate, which affects both norepinephrine and dopamine, had favorable results in a case report but its effects were inferior and shorter acting than those of bromocriptine (Hurford et al., 1998). Malhotra et al. (2006) found that guanfecine, a noradrenergic agonist, improved leftward exploration in two patients with temporo-parietal lesions but not in another patient with a frontal lesion, suggesting that increasing dorsolateral prefrontal cortex-mediated vigilance may improve neglect symptoms even in patients with posterior injuries. Woods et al. (2006) reported improvement in magnitude estimation using modafinil, a psychostimulant with probable dopaminergic effects (Volkow et al., 2009) in a patient with neglect symptoms associated with a left hemisphere lesion. In a study of healthy volunteers, modafinil, but not methylphenidate, decreased the rightward bias in a perceptual task apparently mediated by an increase in right hemisphere mediated alertness (Dodds et al., 2009).
Serotonergic modulation
Right hemisphere lesions are associated with negative affective symptoms and depression (Starkstein et al., 1989) thus patients with neglect may often be treated with antidepressants. These drugs may have overall positive effects in affective symptoms, but how they affect neglect has not been investigated. Physicians treating neglect patients should consider that serotonin reuptake inhibitors have multiple mechanisms of action involving different monoamines and, in some cases, anticholinergic effects. Serotonin may modulate dopaminergic activity by means of multiple mechanisms (see Alex and Pehek, 2007 for review) and produce extrapyramidal and behavioral symptoms including parkinsonism and apathy (Leo, 1996; Lane, 1998; Barnhart et al., 2004; Wongpakaran et al., 2007). Hypokinesia and apathy are likely to be overlooked as part of the depressive symptomatology or right hemisphere injury, therefore, physicians should be observant of possible “paradoxical” effects of these substances and consider the differences in pharmacodynamic profiles, favoring more activating agents with noradrenergic and dopaminergic properties.
Cholinergic modulation
Experiments using nicotine have shown that cholinergic activity plays an important role in a fronto-parietal-thalamic network that regulates arousal, motor activation, and visual attention in humans (Lawrence et al., 2002; Nelson et al., 2005). Nicotine modulates reorienting of visuospatial attention through regulation of neural activity in human parietal cortex (Thiel et al., 2005; Vossel et al., 2008). One study found that nicotine improved performance in a visual location task in chronic neglect subjects, provided that the lesions spared right parietal and temporal cortex (Vossel et al., 2009).
The studies reviewed above suggest that a variety of drugs with dopaminergic, noradrenergic, and cholinergic activity may have beneficial effects in some manifestations of neglect, however these effects may vary depending on lesion location, individual patient susceptibility, and the pharmacodynamic profiles and doses of specific agents.
“Accidental” treatment effects
In many care settings, treatments that may be offered for other related medical or neurological conditions may impair recovery of spatial neglect. For example, a compression glove to decrease dependent edema may interfere with tactile input from one limb increasing stimulation asymmetry. Therefore, it is the authors’ opinion that stimulus-depriving interventions should be carried out symmetrically as much as possible. The use of splints or orthoses when these are not orthopedically indicated in order to “increase attention” to the neglected side or limb, also increases tactile sensory inhibition directly decreasing motor awareness in that body region and must be avoided. Physicians also must be aware of the different mechanisms of action of medication being prescribed. Drugs with anticholinergic, GABAergic, anti-dopaminergic, or sedative effect should be avoided whenever possible. These definitely include benzodiazepines, which in some patients may cause exacerbation or re-emergence of previously recovered spatial neglect or aphasia (Lazar et al., 2002). The reported paradoxical effect of zolpidem increasing alertness in patients with vegetative state (Cohen and Duong, 2008; Shames and Ring, 2008) does not justify its routine use in spatial neglect, as we feel adverse effects on arousal and attention of this medication are much more likely than is paradoxical improvement. Unnecessary antiepileptic medication, standing narcotic analgesics, and medications commonly used for gastrointestinal conditions, which may induce delirium and impair attention, should be avoided as well.
CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS
An enormous amount of research dating back to the mid 20th century has advanced our knowledge of the brain mechanisms involved in spatial attention and behavior and has revealed how failure of these mechanisms may result in the clinical picture of spatial neglect. In order to achieve the goal of applying neuroscience theory to patient care in spatial neglect it is necessary to integrate essential physiologic subcomponents of attentional-perceptual and motor-intentional mechanisms within a comprehensive model that defines specific targets for therapeutic intervention. The lack of consensus regarding the most effective therapy for neglect illustrates the high complexity of the clinical problem and suggests that it is unlikely that a single form of intervention will prevail as the sole rehabilitative treatment. Future challenges thus also involve developing outcome measures with appropriate construct and external validity that effectively measure clinically significant change due to treatment as well as the interaction of recovery components with specific treatment effects. This will allow us to evaluate the effectiveness of theoretically supported rehabilitation treatments and choose the best combination of these treatments for the individual patient. Success in identifying dysfunctional brain-behavior mechanisms, predicting their effects on spatial neglect associated behavior, validly assessing for symptoms, and developing appropriate treatments will result in high benefits to both society and the individual stroke survivor.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
The support of the Kessler Foundation and the NIH (K24 HD062647 and NS055808) is acknowledged.
References
- Adair JC, Barrett AM. Spatial neglect: clinical and neuroscience review: a wealth of information on the poverty of spatial attention. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1142:21–43. doi: 10.1196/annals.1444.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alex KD, Pehek EA. Pharmacologic mechanisms of serotonergic regulation of dopamine neurotransmission. Pharmacol Ther. 2007;113:296–320. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2006.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Heart Association. Heart Disease and Stroke Statistics 2009 Update. 2009 http://www.americanheart.org. [Google Scholar]
- Azouvi P, Olivier S, de Montety G, et al. Behavioral assessment of unilateral neglect: study of the psychometric properties of the Catherine Bergego Scale. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2003;84:51–57. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2003.50062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnhart WJ, Makela EH, Latocha MJ. SSRI-induced apathy syndrome: a clinical review. J Psychiatr Pract. 2004;10:196–199. doi: 10.1097/00131746-200405000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett AM, Burkholder S. Monocular patching in subjects with right-hemisphere stroke affects perceptual-attentional bias. J Rehabil Res Dev. 2006;43:337–346. doi: 10.1682/jrrd.2005.01.0015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett AM, Crucian GP, Schwartz RL, et al. Adverse effect of dopamine agonist therapy in a patient with motor-intentional neglect. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1999;80:600–603. doi: 10.1016/s0003-9993(99)90205-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett AM, Crucian GP, Beversdorf DQ, et al. Monocular patching may worsen sensory-attentional neglect: a case report. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2001;82:516–518. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2001.21973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett AM, Buxbaum LJ, Coslett HB, et al. Cognitive rehabilitation interventions for neglect and related disorders: moving from bench to bedside in stroke patients. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18:1223–1236. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.7.1223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartolomeo P, Chokron S. Egocentric frame of reference: its role in spatial bias after right hemisphere lesions. Neuropsychologia. 1999;37:881–894. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(98)00150-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beis JM, Keller C, Morin N, et al. Right spatial neglect after left hemisphere stroke: qualitative and quantitative study. Neurology. 2004;63:1600–1605. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000142967.60579.32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beversdorf DQ, Hughes JD, Heilman KM. Functional MRI of the primary somatosensory cortex in extinction to simultaneous bilateral tactile stimuli due to right temporal lobe stroke. Neurocase. 2008;14:419–424. doi: 10.1080/13554790802422112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bisiach E, Vallar G, Perani D, et al. Unawareness of disease following lesions of the right hemisphere: anosognosia for hemiplegia and anosognosia for hemianopia. Neuropsychologia. 1986;24:471–482. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(86)90092-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bisiach E, Geminiani G, Berti A, et al. Perceptual and pre-motor factors of unilateral neglect. Neurology. 1990;40:1278–1281. doi: 10.1212/wnl.40.8.1278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bottini G, Paulesu E, Sterzi R, et al. Modulation of conscious experience by peripheral sensory stimuli. Nature. 1995;376:778–781. doi: 10.1038/376778a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bottini G, Paulesu E, Gandola M, et al. Left caloric vestibular stimulation ameliorates right hemianesthesia. Neurology. 2005;65:1278–1283. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000182398.14088.e8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowen A, Lincoln NB. Cognitive rehabilitation for spatial neglect following stroke. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2007;2:CD003586. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003586.pub2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowen A, Lincoln NB, Dewey M. Cognitive rehabilitation for spatial neglect following stroke. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2002;2:CD003586. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buxbaum LJ, Ferraro MK, Veramonti T, et al. Hemi-spatial neglect: subtypes, neuroanatomy, and disability. Neurology. 2004;62:749–756. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000113730.73031.f4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buxbaum LJ, Ferraro M, Whyte J, et al. Amantadine treatment of hemispatial neglect: a double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Am J Phys Med Rehabil. 2007;86:527–537. doi: 10.1097/PHM.0b013e31806e3392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cappa SF, Benke T, Clarke S, et al. EFNS guidelines on cognitive rehabilitation: Report of an EFNS task force. Eur J Neurol. 2005;12:665–680. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2005.01330.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cicerone KD, Dahlberg C, Kalmar K, et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: recommendations for clinical practice. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2000;81:1596–1615. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2000.19240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cicerone KD, Dahlberg C, Malec JF, et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: updated review of the literature from 1998 through 2002. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2005;86:1681–1692. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2005.03.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen SI, Duong TT. Increased arousal in a patient with anoxic brain injury after administration of zolpidem. Am J Phys Med Rehabil. 2008;87:229–231. doi: 10.1097/PHM.0b013e318161971b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Committeri G, Pitazlis S, Galati G, et al. Neural bases of personal and extrapersonal neglect in humans. Brain. 2007;130:431–441. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coslett HB. Evidence for a disturbance of the body schema in neglect. Brain Cogn. 1998;37:527–544. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1998.1011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coslett HB, Bowers D, Fitzpatrick EF, et al. Directional hypokinesia and hemispatial inattention in neglect. Brain. 1990;113:475–486. doi: 10.1093/brain/113.2.475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodds C, Müller U, Manly T. Effects of psychostimulants on alertness and spatial bias in healthy participants. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009;21:529–537. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dooneief G, Mayeux R. Hemispatial neglect [letter] Neurology. 1989;39:458. doi: 10.1212/wnl.39.3.458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eskes GA, Butler B, McDonald A, et al. Limb activation effects in hemispatial neglect. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2003;84:323–328. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2003.50012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farne A, Buxbaum LJ, Ferraro M, et al. Patterns of spontaneous recovery of neglect and associated disorders in acute right brain-damaged patients. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2004;75:1401–1410. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2002.003095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fink JN. Underdiagnosis of right-brain stroke. Lancet. 2005;366:349–350. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67004-3. [comment on Foersch et al., 2005] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleet WS, Valenstein E, Watson RT, et al. Dopamine agonist therapy for neglect in humans. Neurology. 1987;37:1765–1770. doi: 10.1212/wnl.37.11.1765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foerch C, Misselwitz B, Sitzer M, et al. Difference in recognition of right and left hemispheric stroke. Lancet. 2005;366:392–393. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67024-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fortis P, Kornitzer J, Goedert KM, et al. Effect of prism adaptation on “aiming” spatial bias and functional abilities. Neurology. 2009;72:A332. Abstract. [Google Scholar]
- Gainotti G. Emotional behavior and hemispheric side of the lesion. Cortex. 1972;8:41–55. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(72)80026-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geminiani G, Bottini G, Sterzi R. Dopaminergic stimulation in unilateral neglect. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1998;65:344–347. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.65.3.344. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gillen R, Tennen H, McKee T. Unilateral spatial neglect: relation to rehabilitation outcomes in patients with right hemisphere stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2005;86:763–767. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2004.10.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grujic Z, Mapstone M, Gitelman DR, et al. Dopamine agonists reorient visual exploration away from the neglected hemispace. Neurology. 1998;51:1395–1398. doi: 10.1212/wnl.51.5.1395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halligan PW, Fink GR, Marshall JC, et al. Spatial cognition: evidence from visual neglect. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003;7:125–133. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00032-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heilman KM. Neglect and related disorders. In: Heilman KM, Valenstein E, editors. Clinical Neuropsychology. 1st edn. New York: Oxford University Press; 1979. pp. 268–307. [Google Scholar]
- Heilman KM. Intentional neglect. Front Biosci. 2004;9:694–705. doi: 10.2741/1261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heilman K, Van Den Abell T. Right hemisphere dominance for mediating cerebral activation. Neuropsychologia. 1979;17:315–321. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(79)90077-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heilman KM, Van Den Abell T. Right hemisphere dominance for attention: the mechanism underlying hemispheric asymmetries of inattention (neglect) Neurology. 1980;30:327–330. doi: 10.1212/wnl.30.3.327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hier DB, Mondlock J, Caplan LR. Behavioral abnormalities after right hemisphere stroke. Neurology. 1983;33:337–344. doi: 10.1212/wnl.33.3.337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hillis AE, Rapp B, Benzing L, et al. Dissociable coordinate frames of unilateral spatial neglect: “viewer-centered” neglect. Brain Cogn. 1998;37:491–526. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1998.1010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hillis AE, Newhart M, Heidler J, et al. Anatomy of spatial attention: insights from perfusion imaging and hemi-spatial neglect in acute stroke. J Neurosci. 2005;25:3161–3167. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4468-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hillis AE, Chang S, Heidler-Gary J, et al. Neural correlates of modality-specific spatial extinction. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18:1889–1898. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.11.1889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hurford P, Stringer AY, Jann B. Neuropharmacologic treatment of hemineglect: a case report comparing bromocriptine and methylphenidate. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1998;79:346–349. doi: 10.1016/s0003-9993(98)90019-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jutai JW, Bhogal SK, Foley NC, et al. Treatment of visual perceptual disorders post stroke. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2003;10:77–106. doi: 10.1310/07BE-5E1N-735J-1C6U. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalra L, Perez I, Gupta S, et al. The influence of visual neglect on stroke rehabilitation. Stroke. 1997;28:1386–1391. doi: 10.1161/01.str.28.7.1386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katz N, Hartman-Maeir A, Ring H, et al. Functional disability and rehabilitation outcome in right hemisphere damaged patients with and without unilateral spatial neglect. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1999;80:379–384. doi: 10.1016/s0003-9993(99)90273-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence NS, Ross TJ, Stein EA. Cognitive mechanisms of nicotine on visual attention. Neuron. 2002;36:539–548. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01004-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lazar RM, Fitzsimmons BF, Marshall RS, et al. Reemergence of stroke deficits with midazolam challenge. Stroke. 2002;33:283–285. doi: 10.1161/hs0102.101222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lane RM. SSRI-induced extrapyramidal side-effects and akathisia: implications for treatment. J Psychopharmacol. 1998;12:192–214. doi: 10.1177/026988119801200212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leo RJ. Movement disorders associated with the serotonin selective reuptake inhibitors. J Clin Psychiatry. 1996;57:449–454. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v57n1002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lincoln NB, Bowen A. The need for randomised treatment studies in neglect research. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2006;24:401–408. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luauté J, Halligan P, Rode G, et al. Visuo-spatial neglect: a systematic review of current interventions and their effectiveness. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30:961–982. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malhotra PA, Parton AD, Greenwood R, et al. Noradrenergic modulation of space exploration in visual neglect. Ann Neurol. 2006;59:186–190. doi: 10.1002/ana.20701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mapstone M, Weintraub S, Nowinski C, et al. Cerebral hemispheric specialization for spatial attention: spatial distribution of search-related eye fixations in the absence of neglect. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:1396–1409. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(03)00043-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marzi CA, Girelli M, Natale E, et al. What exactly is extinguished in unilateral visual extinction? Neurophysiological evidence. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39:1354–1366. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00123-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mennemeier M, Wertman E, Heilman KM. Neglect of near personal space. Brain. 1992;115:37–50. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.1.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menon A, Petzold A, Korner-Bitensky N. Unilateral Spatial Neglect Interventions. Strokengine. 2009 http://www.medicine.mcgill.ca/Strokengine. [Google Scholar]
- Mesulam MM. Behavioral neuroanatomy. In: Mesulam M-M, editor. Principles of Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology. 2nd edn. New York: Oxford University Press; 2000. p. 2. [Google Scholar]
- Mort DJ, Malhotra P, Mannan SK, et al. The anatomy of visual neglect. Brain. 2003;126:1986–1997. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mukand JA, Guilmette TJ, Allen DG, et al. Dopaminergic therapy with carbidopa L-dopa for left neglect after stroke: a case series. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2001;82:1279–1282. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2001.25149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Na DL, Adair JC, Kang Y, et al. Motor perseverative behavior on a line cancellation task. Neurology. 1999;52:1569–1576. doi: 10.1212/wnl.52.8.1569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Cancer Institute. Surveillance Epidemiology and End Results. [accessed July 30, 2009]; http://seer.cancer.gov/csr/1975_2006/results_single/sect_01_table.01.pdf.
- National Spinal Cord Injury Statistical Cente. Facts and Figures at a Glance. [accessed July 30, 2009];2009 Updated April 2009. http://www.spinalcord.uab.edu/ [Google Scholar]
- Nelson CL, Sarter M, Bruno JP. Prefrontal cortical modulation of acetylcholine release in posterior parietal cortex. Neuroscience. 2005;132:347–359. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nys GM, van Zandvoort MJ, van der Worp HB, et al. Neuropsychological and neuroanatomical correlates of perseverative responses in subacute stroke. Brain. 2006;129:2148–2157. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Payne BR, Rushmore RJ. Functional circuitry underlying natural and interventional cancellation of visual neglect. Exp Brain Res. 2004;154:127–153. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1660-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pierce SR, Buxbaum LJ. Treatments of unilateral neglect: a review. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2002;83:256–268. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2002.27333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Proto D, Pella RD, Hill BD, et al. Assessment and rehabilitation of acquired visuospatial and proprioceptive deficits associated with visuospatial neglect. NeuroRehabilitation. 2009;24:145–157. doi: 10.3233/NRE-2009-0463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riestra AR, Crucian GP, Burks DW, et al. Extinction, working memory, and line bisection in spatial neglect. Neurology. 2001;57:147–149. doi: 10.1212/wnl.57.1.147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riestra AR, Womack KB, Crucian GP, et al. Is the middle between both halves?: midpoint location and segment size estimation in neglect. Neurology. 2002;59:1580–1584. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000034762.27776.67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ringman JM, Saver JL, Woolson RF, et al. Frequency, risk factors, anatomy, and course of unilateral neglect in an acute stroke cohort. Neurology. 2004;63:468–474. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000133011.10689.ce. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robertson IH, North N. Active and passive activation of left limbs: influence on visual and sensory neglect. Neuropsychologia. 1993;31:293–300. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(93)90093-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossetti Y, Rode G, Pisella L. Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect. Nature. 1998;395:166–169. doi: 10.1038/25988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossi PW, Kheyfets S, Reding MJ. Frensel prisms improve visual perception in stroke patients with homonymous hemianopia or unilateral visual neglect. Neurology. 1990;40:1597–1599. doi: 10.1212/wnl.40.10.1597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rundek T, Mast H, Hartmann A, et al. Predictors of resource use after acute hospitalization: the Northern Manhattan Stroke Study. Neurology. 2000;55:1180–1187. doi: 10.1212/wnl.55.8.1180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sapir A, Kaplan JB, He BJ, et al. Anatomical correlates of directional hypokinesia in patients with hemispatial neglect. J Neurosci. 2007;27:4045–4051. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0041-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwarting RK, Huston JP. The unilateral 6-hydroxydopamine lesion model in behavioral brain research: analysis of functional deficits, recovery and treatments. Prog Neurobiol. 1996;50:275–331. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(96)00040-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shames JL, Ring H. Transient reversal of anoxic brain injury-related minimally conscious state after zolpidem administration: a case report. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2008;89:386–388. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.08.137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Starkstein SE, Robinson RG, Honig MA, et al. Mood changes after right-hemisphere lesions. Br J Psychiatry. 1989;155:79–85. doi: 10.1192/bjp.155.1.79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teasell R, Salter K, Bitensky J, et al. Evidence-Based Review of Stroke Rehabilitation: Module 13, Perceptual Disorders. Heart and Stroke Foundation Ontario and Ministry of Health and Long-Term Care of Ontario. 2008 http://www.ebrsr.com. [Google Scholar]
- Tegnér R, Levander M. Through a looking glass: a new technique to demonstrate directional hypokinesia in unilateral neglect. Brain. 1991;114:1943–1951. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.4.1943. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thiel CM, Zilles K, Fink GR. Nicotine modulates reorienting of visuospatial attention and neural activity in human parietal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:810–820. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Triggs WJ, Gold M, Gerstle G, et al. Motor neglect associated with a discrete parietal lesion. Neurology. 1994;44:1164–1166. doi: 10.1212/wnl.44.6.1164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uniform Data System for Medical Rehabilitation. Version 5.1. Buffalo: 1997. The Guide for the Uniform Data Set for Medical Rehabilitation (Including the FIM ® Instrument) [Google Scholar]
- Vallar G, Rusconi ML, Barozzi S, et al. Improvement of left visuo-spatial hemineglect by left-sided transcutaneous electrical stimulation. Neuropsychologia. 1995;33:73–82. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)00088-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vallar G, Rusconi ML, Bernardini B. Modulation of neglect hemianesthesia by transcutaneous electrical stimulation. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1996;2:452–459. doi: 10.1017/s1355617700001533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Logan J, et al. Effects of modafinil on dopamine and dopamine transporters in the male human brain: clinical implications. JAMA. 2009;301:1148–1154. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vossel S, Thiel CM, Fink GR. Behavioral and neural effects of nicotine on visuospatial attentional reorienting in non-smoking subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:731–738. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vossel S, Kukolja J, Thimm M, et al. The effect of nicotine on visuospatial attention in chronic spatial neglect depends upon lesion location. J Psychopharmacol. 2010;24:1357–1365. doi: 10.1177/0269881109105397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinberg J, Diller L, Gordon WA, et al. Visual scanning training effect on reading-related tasks in acquired right brain damage. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1977;58:479–486. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinberg J, Diller L, Gordon WA. Training sensory awareness and spatial organization in people with right brain damage. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1979;60:491–496. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wongpakaran N, van Reekum R, Wongpakaran T. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor use associates with apathy among depressed elderly: a case-control study. Ann Gen Psychiatry. 2007;6:7. doi: 10.1186/1744-859X-6-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woods AJ, Mennemeier M, Garcia-Rill E, et al. Bias in magnitude estimation following left hemisphere injury. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:1406–1412. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.12.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]