Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Apr 16.
Published in final edited form as: Med Decis Making. 2009 Sep 2;30(2):246–257. doi: 10.1177/0272989X09341751

Are Providers More Likely to Contribute to Healthcare Disparities Under High Levels of Cognitive Load? How Features of the Healthcare Setting May Lead to Biases in Medical Decision Making

Diana J Burgess 1
PMCID: PMC3988900  NIHMSID: NIHMS570086  PMID: 19726783

Abstract

Systematic reviews of healthcare disparities suggest that clinicians’ diagnostic and therapeutic decision making varies by clinically irrelevant characteristics, such as patient race, and that this variation may contribute to healthcare disparities. However, there is little understanding of the particular features of the healthcare setting under which clinicians are most likely to be inappropriately influenced by these characteristics. This study delineates several hypotheses to stimulate future research in this area. It is posited that healthcare settings in which providers experience high levels of cognitive load will increase the likelihood of racial disparities via 2 pathways. First, providers who experience higher levels of cognitive load are hypothesized to make poorer medical decisions and provide poorer care for all patients, due to lower levels of controlled processing (H1). Second, under greater levels of cognitive load, it is hypothesized that healthcare providers’ medical decisions and interpersonal behaviors will be more likely to be influenced by racial stereotypes, leading to poorer processes and outcomes of care for racial minority patients (H2). It is further hypothesized that certain characteristics of healthcare settings will result in higher levels of cognitive load experienced by providers (H3). Finally, it is hypothesized that minority patients will be disproportionately likely to be treated in healthcare settings in which providers experience greater levels of cognitive load (H4a), which will result in racial disparities due to lower levels of controlled processing by providers (H4b) and the influence of racial stereotypes (H4c).The study concludes with implications for research and practice that flow from this framework.

Keywords: healthcare disparities, stereotyping, organizations, race/ethnicity, social cognition, cognitive load


This study introduces a conceptual framework for exploring how features of healthcare settings that increase cognitive load may contribute to racial/ethnic disparities in healthcare. In this study, cognitive load can be thought of as the amount of mental activity imposed on working memory, which may come from competing mental tasks, environmental factors, our own psychological or physiological state (e.g., fatigue), as well as from the demands inherent in the task at hand (i.e., intrinsic cognitive load). According to this formulation, cognitive load is best conceptualized as a quantity that varies rather than a state that is either present or absent.

The conceptual framework presented below is grounded in dual process models of social cognition, in which controlled and automatic processes handle different types of mental activities.1,2 Controlled processes involve intentional, conscious, and effortful thought. Importantly, high levels of cognitive load can interrupt, impair, or prevent the successful execution of controlled processes by “taking up” the necessary cognitive resources for those processes. By contrast, automatic processes are relatively effortless, can occur outside of consciousness and without intent, and importantly, are not disrupted under high levels of cognitive load. Recent studies have identified a neurocognitive basis for this distinction, identifying different patterns of neural activation for automatic versus controlled processes.3

PRIMARY HYPOTHESES

It is posited that healthcare settings in which providers experience high levels of cognitive load will increase the likelihood of racial disparities via 2 pathways. First, providers who experience higher levels of cognitive load are hypothesized to make poorer medical decisions and provide poorer care for all patients, due to lower levels of controlled processing (H1). Second, under greater levels of cognitive load, it is hypothesized that healthcare providers’ medical decisions and interpersonal behaviors will be more likely to be influenced by racial stereotypes, leading to poorer processes and outcomes of care for racial minority patients (H2). It is further hypothesized that certain healthcare settings will result in higher levels of cognitive load experienced by providers (H3). Finally, it is hypothesized that minority patients will be disproportionately likely to be treated in healthcare settings in which providers experience greater levels of cognitive load (H4a), which will result in racial disparities due to lower levels of controlled processing by providers (H4b) and the influence of racial stereotypes (H4c).

Hypothesis 1: Providers Who Experience Higher Levels of Cognitive Load Will Make Poorer Medical Decisions and Provide Poorer Care for all Patients, due to Lower Levels of Controlled Processing

To understand the impact of cognitive load on medical decisions and patient care, it is important to understand the role of automatic and controlled processes in learning and performance. In learning a new task, we expand precious cognitive resources engaging in controlled processes. When we are learning how to drive, for example, we spend a great deal of conscious effort thinking about negotiating the brakes, clutch, gas pedal and paying attention to steering—tasks that constitute intrinsic cognitive load—and consequently have very little attention to spare for other tasks. After we have learned and practiced the behavior it becomes automatized and we have spare cognitive resources to devote to other tasks. However, even after we have become experts, there can be external circumstances where we need to pay attention, snap out of our automatic behavior, and shift our attention to the situation at hand. Usually, we are adept at conserving cognitive resources by strategically reallocating those resources to where they are most needed. However, our ability to reallocate attention is hindered under high levels of cognitive load (e.g., fatigue, poor road conditions) and we are not as efficient at shifting from automatic to controlled processing.4,5

As scholars and practitioners are well aware, the art of medicine involves numerous mental processes, many of which become automatized with training. For example, experienced clinicians tend to generate diagnoses automatically when faced with patients whose pattern of symptoms “matches” previously seen patterns of symptom configurations.6 Clinicians also rely on a number of medical heuristics or “shortcuts” that facilitate decision making.7 In the ideal scenario, clinicians are able to strategically shift from automatic to controlled processes when the situation is warranted, such as when faced with complex or unfamiliar cases, or in instances when it is important to critically reflect upon their decisions.8 However, under cognitive load, clinicians’ ability to switch from controlled to automatic processes is expected to become compromised, leading to poorer care.

The detrimental effect of various sources of cognitive load, such as fatigue, stress, mental distress, excessive workload, inadequate staffing, and time pressure on medical errors and impaired cognitive performance by clinicians is well documented (see hypothesis 3 for a more comprehensive discussion).915 However, there is a dearth of studies that have rigorously examined the effect of cognitive load on providers using methods from other disciplines, such as aviation, and human factors and educational research.1618

Hypothesis 2: Under Greater Levels of Cognitive Load, Providers’ Medical Decisions and Interpersonal Behaviors Are More Likely to Be Influenced by Racial Stereotypes, Leading to Poorer Processes and Outcomes of Care for Racial Minority Patients

The processes by which provider stereotyping is posited to contribute to racial disparities in care have received significant attention in the recent Institute of Medicine (IOM) report.19 Salient patient characteristics, such as race, gender, and age, may activate stereotypes that might lead to biases in providers’ interpretation of behaviors and symptoms, expectations about patient behaviors (e.g., adherence), and medical decisions, and may also adversely influence the quality of the medical encounter.2023

Several lines of research support the hypothesis that medical decision making will be more likely to be influenced by racial/ethnic stereotypes under high levels of cognitive load.

Numerous studies have shown that activation and application of stereotypes can be automatic processes that occur unconsciously and unintentionally—a phenomenon known as implicit stereotyping.20,22 Indeed, there is evidence that white physicians hold negative implicit (unconscious) stereotypes about African Americans (as do whites in the general population), which can result in medical decision-making biases.24 However, it is possible to override or correct for these automatic processes of categorization and stereotyping, via controlled processes, if we have sufficient cognitive resources and we are motivated to do so.25,26 With sufficient cognitive resources and motivation, it is more likely that we will engage in individuation—an effortful, controlled process in which we focus on the unique features of the person with whom we are interacting, which may differ from the stereotype that was automatically activated.27 If we are aware that we may engage in bias due to stereotypes and we are motivated to overcome this, we also may engage in “de-biasing” or “correction” strategies where we actively try to correct for judgments that we suspect may be “tainted” by stereotypes.28 Conversely, under greater levels of cognitive load, individuation and correction for activated stereotypes are less likely.

One line of research supporting H2 uses an experimental paradigm in which subjects are randomly assigned to evaluate or make judgments about “targets” from different social categories (e.g., male v. female; black v. white) under conditions of high v. low levels of cognitive load. Cognitive load is typically manipulated by having subjects perform a concurrent mental task, such as rehearsing an 8-digit string of numbers29,30 or attending to a news broadcast.31 Cognitive load manipulation has also involved increasing the complexity of the judgment task,32 putting subjects under time pressure,33 and having subjects perform tasks during their “off peak” phase of circadian arousal (during the daytime for “night owls” and during the nighttime for “morning people”).34 These studies have consistently found that biased decisions (e.g., gender bias in performance ratings, age discrimination in hiring decisions) are more likely for subjects who are making the decisions under high levels of cognitive load.30,3541

Converging evidence for H2 comes from neuroimaging studies demonstrating that that the desire and ability to regulate attitudes, in general, and prejudiced attitudes, in particular, is associated with greater activity of the right lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC), an area of the brain that supports controlled processing.3,42,43 Likewise, activation of the LPFC is associated with inhibition of biased beliefs within the context of solving logic problems44—a process analogous to what providers would engage in if they were to correct for the influence of automatically activated stereotypes. There is also evidence that the process of self-reflection on emotional stimuli—a controlled process whose activity is disrupted by cognitive load manipulations—can reduce activity in the amygdala45—part of the automatic processing system that is associated with implicit prejudice.42

An important issue is why cognitive load might increase the use of clinically irrelevant characteristics such as race as opposed to clinically relevant characteristics that activate the appropriate scripts for diagnostic reasoning.6 One explanation is that certain patient characteristics such as African American race are more likely than other characteristics to automatically activate stereotypes.46 Specifically, neuroimaging studies have shown differential activity in the amygdale, the area of the brain that is responsive to perceived threat, in response to African American compared with white men.42 Evolutionary psychologists have posited that our brains are wired to be highly responsive to characteristics associated with survival (i.e., those associated with physical threat) and reproductive fitness (e.g., age, gender), suggesting that certain patient characteristics may predominate even when they are not clinically relevant.46,47 Cognitive load may also increase the extent that automatically activated, clinically irrelevant stereotypes are applied, by inhibiting their suppression, an explanation consistent with a study in which higher levels of cognitive load predicted poorer ability to control inhibit stereotypic responses on a stereotype inhibition task.48

The single published study explicitly designed to test the hypothesis that cognitive load increases provider contribution to disparities due to stereotyping operationalized cognitive load in terms of low versus high patient load.49 This study employed a retrospective chart review based on the records of 1236 adult psychiatric patients treated in psychiatric emergency services. The patient load variable was derived by combining the average number of patients typically seen during specified shifts (“institutional peak times”) and the actual volume of patients seen by the individual clinician during his or her shift (“stratified invidual peak times”). Patient records were sampled according to this measure of patient load, in which random samples were taken from all adult patients evaluated by each clinician during low peak and high peak shifts. The researchers found that, under conditions of high cognitive load, being female compared with being male increased the odds of receiving a diagnosis of depressive disorder, a disorder that has been shown to be overdiagnosed among women and that has been viewed as a stereotypically female condition.50 This provides indirect evidence that provider bias may be greater under higher levels of cognitive load, although it is limited by the fact that stereotyping was not measured, but merely inferred.

Hypothesis 3: Certain Characteristics of Healthcare Settings Will Result in Higher Levels of Cognitive Load Experienced by Providers

Hypothesis 3a: Providers Will Experience Greater Levels of Cognitive Load in Settings in which They Face Greater “Competing Demands”

Health service researchers are well aware of the multiple demands that compete for providers’ attention within clinical encounters, particularly for complex patients with multiple chronic conditions.5156 From a social cognitive perspective, competing demands include not only clinical tasks, but other tasks that require effortful cognitive “work,” such as communicating with patients with low English language proficiency or diminished cognitive capacity. Hence, patient case mix is likely to increase cognitive load to the extent that it is comprised of patient populations that impose a greater number of demands on providers.

Hypothesis 3b: Providers Will Experience Greater Levels of Cognitive Load in Settings in which They Experience Higher Levels of Fatigue, Stress, Anxiety, Distress, and Time Pressure

Numerous studies have shown that fatigue, sleep deprivation, chronic and acute stress, and anxiety significantly deplete working memory and diminish mental processing ability.5774 There also is evidence that physiological and psychological impairments diminish providers’ performance in healthcare settings. For example, studies of physicians, residents, nurses, and paramedics have found that higher levels of acute and chronic stress, fatigue, psychological distress, depression, and burnout are associated with a greater likelihood of making medical errors and providing suboptimal or poorer patient care.10,11,13,7579

Research has shown a linkage between characteristics of practice settings and physician work stress. For example, physician work stress has been shown to be greater for individuals in solo practice and for physicians treating a greater number of complex patients with multiple clinical conditions.80 Greater levels of physician stress have also been associated with low levels of perceived control over workplace and administrative issues, low levels of perceived organizational support for balancing work and family,80,81 and sexual or racial harassment,80,8284 and greater perceived time pressure, overload, and numbers of hours worked.79,80,8587

Hypothesis 3c: Facilities with High Proportions of Trainees Will Be Particularly Vulnerable to the Adverse Effects of Cognitive Load

Central to theories of learning is the concept that the initial acquisition of tasks or skills is effortful and hence takes up valuable working memory capacity. With practice, skills become increasingly automatized and impose less cognitive burden on providers. Related to this, experts use less cognitive capacity than novices when solving problems, because their mental representations are more elaborate and able to integrate more knowledge into a single representation or chunk. Hence, the expert is able to allocate more resources to other cognitive activities. For instance, a physician with greater experience in a particular domain will develop a richer schema and so is able to better integrate separate pieces of clinical information into a larger chunk than a physician with less experience, leaving him or her with greater cognitive resources to attend to other mental tasks. Conversely, the less expert physician may be more likely to rely on stereotyping because he or she has less cognitive capacity to integrate individual-level information.

Teaching hospitals that rely on residents are also likely to experience high levels of cognitive load among its workforce, since fatigue, work stress, and emotional exhaustion continue to remain high among residents, even after the enactment (in 2003) of legislation that limited residents’ work week to 80 hours and restricted residents to a maximum of 30 hours of continuous duty. While some studies have shown reductions in residents’ fatigue, stress, and emotional exhaustion since this legislation was enacted,88,89 other studies found no change in total work or sleep hours following enactment of this legislation.90

Hypothesis 3d: Providers Will Experience Lower Levels of Cognitive Load in Settings with Greater Structures and Practices that Support Medical-Decision Making

Structures and practices that support medical decisions may alleviate the cognitive burden associated with particular clinical tasks.9193 However, the design of the technological structures is critical in determining whether the technology will reduce or contribute to cognitive load. This is illustrated by 2 recent studies conducted with medical students and board certified physicians that examined the extent to which presentation of clinical information affects cognitive load.94 Cognitive load was found to be lower when patient information was presented via a combination of symbols and texts (i.e., “knowledge-enhanced graphical symbols”) compared with when it was presented via traditional charts and records, underscoring the importance of designing technological systems that minimize provider cognitive load and the need for additional research to inform these design considerations. In addition, although new technologies may ultimately reduce provider cognitive load, during the learning phase new technological systems are likely to contribute to cognitive load until their use becomes automatized.

Indirect evidence for H3d comes from studies in which increases in quality improvement are associated with reductions in racial disparities (in hemodialysis, breast cancer screening, diabetes care processes, cardiovascular care, congestive heart failure, and depression).9598 Although the mechanisms that underlie these reductions in disparities are unknown, it is plausible that structures and processes designed to improve quality, such as clinical support systems, may reduce the effect of racial stereotypes on decision making by decreasing cognitive load experienced by providers.

Hypothesis 3e: Providers Will Experience Higher Levels of Cognitive Load When Interacting with Patients of a Different Race or Ethnicity

Interactions with someone of another race has been shown to be another source of cognitive load that can impair performance (H1) and promote the use of stereotypes (H2), because these interactions lead to increased anxiety and an increase in self-regulatory behaviors, such as monitoring or regulating one’s thoughts, feelings, or behaviors to avoid appearing prejudiced.99104 A drawback to this research is that it consists entirely of laboratory studies with undergraduate students. However, a study showing that physicians experience anxiety when interacting with black or Latino patients, and that self-reported ratings of interracial anxiety are associated with lower patient ratings of encounter quality among nonwhite patients,105 suggests that examining the effects of interracial anxiety on a broader range of outcomes (e.g., medical decision making) would be a fruitful area for future research.

Hypothesis 4: Minority Patients Are Disproportionately Likely To Be Treated in Healthcare Settings in which Providers Experience Greater Levels of Cognitive Load (H4a) and Are Therefore Expected To Experience Poorer Care than Whites due to Lower Levels of Controlled Processing by Providers (H4b) and the Influence of Racial Stereotypes (H4c)

There is evidence that racial and ethnic minorities are more likely than whites to be treated in settings in which providers experience high levels of cognitive load.106 For example, physicians in clinics with at least 30%minority patients (N = 27) were more likely than physicians in other clinics (N = 69) to lack access to referral specialists, to have more difficult patients (e.g., patients who have chronic pain and substance abuse; patients who are medically and psychosocially complex) and to report lower levels of job satisfaction and work control and were 4 times more likely to report a chaotic workplace.107 Racial minorities are also more likely to be treated in settings that lack technologies, such as computerized record systems, which have the potential to reduce cognitive load. There is also evidence that African Americans are less likely to be treated by physicians with the highest levels of expertise (board certified physicians, “high volume” surgical teams),108111 who require fewer cognitive resources than less experienced providers. International medical graduates, who make up a large percentage of the work force in settings in which minorities receive care, experience a great deal of stress, including perceived discrimination, cultural differences, and a lack of social support.19

Because the majority of African Americans in the United States currently receive care within a relatively small percentage of hospitals,106,108,112,113 it is particularly important to examine whether providers working in those settings experience greater levels of cognitive load compared with providers in predominantly white settings. Although there is evidence that the site of care may contribute to disparities,106,108,113118 with nonwhites more likely to be treated in facilities in which they receive lower quality care than whites, researchers have generally conceptualized patient race and site of care as separate and competing factors and have not examined how the two may interact,106 so that it is not possible to determine whether certain settings render provider bias more likely, an “either/or perspective” illustrated by a question posed in one journal article: “Are racial disparities the result of who you are or where you seek care?”119 An exception is 2 recent studies in which racial disparities in treatment were greater in facilities that had larger minority populations.120,121 Nonetheless, additional research that examines the interactive as well as the main effects of patient race and racial composition of the healthcare facility is needed.

IMPLICATIONS FOR RESEARCH

Much of the existing research supporting the proposition that provider stereotypes and bias contributes to disparities in healthcare comes from vignette studies in which patient race is experimentally manipulated in the form of a clinical vignette and the provider is asked to make a series of medical decisions. However, many vignette studies fail to find strong main effects of race: either race unexpectedly interacts with another factor or the effect of race is not significant122125; see Aberegg and Terry126 for a discussion. For instance, in contrast to the numerous studies documenting racial disparities in treatment of cardiovascular disease,127,128 several factorial experiments found no effect of patient race on physicians’ treatment decisions for coronary heart disease129,130,131 and coronary revascularization,132 and the well-publicized study by Schulman and others.133 found lower likelihood of cardiac catheterization only for black women, but not for black men. Similarly, despite documented disparities in depression diagnosis and treatment in African Americans, several experiments using videotaped vignettes failed to find an effect of race on diagnosis and treatment.134136

This lack of simple main effects of race using experimental vignettes for conditions in which there are documented racial disparities in care points to the difficulty and importance of creating vignette experiments that fully capture the complexity of the clinical encounter. Although the experimental method eliminates threats to internal validity inherent in observational studies, allowing researchers to isolate the effect of race from potentially confounding factors (e.g., socioeconomic status, expression of pain), in its current form it creates a condition in which providers have time to rely on more effortful strategies of individuation, thus reducing the impact of unconscious stereotypes.27 It is plausible that this failure to account for providers’ cognitive load in vignette studies may help explain why these studies do not provide consistent and strong support for the hypothesis that stereotyping contributes to disparities in healthcare.

I suggest that researchers who seek to study the provider contribution to disparities incorporate cognitive load into their research design. To fully understand the effect of cognitive load on disparities in processes and outcomes of care it will be necessary to take a multimethodological approach. Efforts should be made to conduct vignette studies (in which race and other patient characteristics are systematically manipulated) with standardized patients, in contexts that contain the types of cognitive demands that providers face in the clinical setting (e.g., fatigue, stress, competing demands). Other experiments might manipulate and measure the effects of cognitive load on providers’ task performance using standardized patients or using other types of simulations. Observational studies might operationalize cognitive load via organizational characteristics posited to be associated with cognitive load (“distal measures” such as patient volume, presence of international medical graduates; provider work flow) as well as proximal measures of cognitive load (e.g., burnout, stress, perceived overload), to examine the extent to which cognitive load mediates the relationship between organizational characteristics and outcomes such as racial/ethnic disparities in care.79

IMPLICATIONS FOR POLICY AND PRACTICE

Although a great deal of research is clearly needed, this framework points to the importance of developing initiatives, at the provider, organizational, and policy level, to reduce the burden of provider cognitive load. At the provider level, one approach could involve interventions such as mindfulness training, which has been demonstrated to reduce stress and help providers regulate negative emotions,137139 as well as training in other forms of stress management,140 ideally as part of medical training. Another approach is to foster the automaticity of desired provider behaviors, including routine tasks as well as new scripts specifically designed to counteract or prevent known bias, including racial and ethnic bias. For example, providers might mentally simulate and rehearse new scripts to correct for common cognitive biases, including biases due to the inappropriate application of stereotypes.7

It is also possible to practice self-regulation of automatic prejudiced responses, so that self-regulation becomes automatized, by becoming aware of “prejudiced cues” and developing new responses to replace those initial responses with nonprejudiced responses.141 As discussed previously, automatic processes conserve cognitive load that can be used for controlled processing.142 Developing new scripts (or routines) requires rehearsal and practice of the desired behaviors in low cognitive load settings, while the operation of the script becomes automatized, before moving into settings with high levels of cognitive load, such as a busy clinic. Programs or courses designed to promote “reflective practice” or self-awareness offer the potential for providers to reflect on his or her automatic, habitual practices and, afterward (in a low cognitive load setting), identify areas for improvement and change.7,8,143,144

This framework also points to key changes in the work environment to reduce chaos, improve work-flow, and increase providers’ sense of control over their work environment. This may require additional resources for increased staffing but likely will also require more thoughtful consideration of how new processes are implemented. For example, electronic medical record systems should be designed to minimize extraneous cognitive load.94 Likewise, it is important to ensure that new initiatives (e.g., performance measurement) employ techniques used in other fields, such as cognitive task analysis to measure cognitive load.18,145 It is also important to avoid disparities-reduction interventions that, both of which may increase cognitive load. Increase interracial anxiety or that promote stereotype suppression.146 On the flip side, “cognitive aids” (e.g., algorithms, checklists, clinical decision support tools) can reduce the burden of cognitive load.7 For instance, checklists can relieve providers of the mental burden of following newly introduced processes before they become automatized.147,148

In the face of scarce resources it is also critical that these types of interventions are targeted at environments in which there is likely to be a great deal of cognitive load, such as systems that serve minority populations.107 This would require broader changes in policies, such as the allocation of more resources to such systems as well as better reimbursement for primary care.107 It is also important to consider the burden of cognitive load on trainees, whose performance is more likely to be adversely affected by high levels of load than providers with greater expertise.142

CONCLUSION

There is scant research investigating the effect of organizational characteristics on disparities in healthcare.149 I argue that incorporating the concept of cognitive load into health disparities research can advance the science by positing a potentially important mechanism through which a diverse range of features in the healthcare setting may affect provider bias. According to dual process models of cognition, cognitive load is a key determinant of the extent to which individuals will engage in automatic or controlled processes. Among healthcare providers, a greater reliance on automatic processes, such as stereotyping, is expected to be particularly disadvantageous for members of socially stigmatized groups and to contribute to disparities in care.20,23,24,150,151 The disproportionate burden of cognitive load experienced by providers who treat racial/ethnic minorities is also expected to result in poorer performance in tasks that require higher levels of executive functioning, such as medical decision making. Understanding the particular organizational characteristics that increase or decrease provider cognitive load would allow for the development of organizational-level interventions and policy changes to reduce racial and ethnic disparities in care. At a more general level, by positing a mechanism to link organizational, interpersonal, and intrapsychic phenomena, this model also provides a framework for more precisely examining the effect of a diverse range of organizational-level characteristics on medical decision making, potentially leading to valuable insights and advances.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This project was supported by a Merit Review Entry Program (MREP) award to Dr. Burgess from the VA Health Services Research & Development (HSR&D).The author thanks Dr. Melissa Partin, Dr. Brian Martinson, Dr. Thomas Flottemesch, and Dr. Joan Griffin for their insightful comments on previous drafts of the manuscript.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Smith ER, DeCoster J. Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2000;4:108–131. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Deutsch R, Strack F. Duality models in social psychology: from dual processes to interacting systems. Psychol Rev. 2006;17:166–172. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Lieberman MD. Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58:259–289. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Anttila V, Juha L. Surrogate in-vehicle information systems and driver behavior in an urban environment: a field study on the effects of visual and cognitive load. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour. 2005;8:121–133. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Horrey WJ, Simons DJ. Examining cognitive interference and adaptive safety behaviours in tactical vehicle control. Ergonomics. 2007;50:1340–1350. doi: 10.1080/00140130701318889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Elstein AS, Schwartz A. Clinical problem solving and diagnostic decision making: selective review of the cognitive literature. BMJ. 2002;324:729–732. doi: 10.1136/bmj.324.7339.729. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Croskerry P. Achieving quality in clinical decision making: cognitive strategies and detection of bias. Acad Emerg Med. 2002;9:1184–1204. doi: 10.1111/j.1553-2712.2002.tb01574.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Mamede S, Schmidt HG, Rikers R. Diagnostic errors and reflective practice in medicine. J Eval Clin Pract. 2007;13:138–145. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2753.2006.00638.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Garrett C. The effect of nurse staffing patterns on medical errors and nurse burnout. Aorn J. 2008;87:1191–1204. doi: 10.1016/j.aorn.2008.01.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.West CP, Huschka MM, Novotny PJ, et al. Association of perceived medical errors with resident distress and empathy: a prospective longitudinal study. JAMA. 2006;296:1071–1078. doi: 10.1001/jama.296.9.1071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.LeBlanc VR, MacDonald RD, McArthur B, King K, Lepine T. Paramedic performance in calculating drug dosages following stressful scenarios in a human patient simulator. Prehosp Emerg Care. 2005;9:439–444. doi: 10.1080/10903120500255255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Weissman JS, Rothschild JM, Bendavid E, et al. Hospital workload and adverse events. Med Care. 2007;45:448–455. doi: 10.1097/01.mlr.0000257231.86368.09. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Landrigan CP, Rothschild JM, Cronin JW, et al. Effect of reducing interns’ work hours on serious medical errors in intensive care units. N Engl J Med. 2004;351:1838–1848. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa041406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Tamblyn R, Berkson L, Dauphinee WD, et al. Unnecessary prescribing of NSAIDs and the management of NSAID-related gastropathy in medical practice. Ann Intern Med. 1997;127:429–438. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-127-6-199709150-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Levinson W, Roter DL, Mullooly JP, Dull VT, Frankel RM. Physician-patient communication. The relationship with malpractice claims among primary care physicians and surgeons. JAMA. 1997;277:553–559. doi: 10.1001/jama.277.7.553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Young G, Zavelina L, Hooper V. Assessment of workload using NASA Task Load Index in perianesthesia nursing. J Perianesth Nurs. 2008;23:102–110. doi: 10.1016/j.jopan.2008.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hashimoto M, Kuwahara N, Naya F, Noma H, Kogure K, Osuga M. The proposal of measurement of RNs at acute care setting cognitive process when they make cognitive error. Stud Health Technol Inform. 2006;122:455–457. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Paas F, Tuovinen JE, Tabbers H, Van Gerven PWM. Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance cognitive load theory. Educational Psychologist. 2003;38:63–71. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Smedley BD, Stith AY, Nelson AR, editors. Unequal Treatment: Confronting Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Healthcare. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2002. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Burgess DJ, Fu SS, van Ryn M. Why do providers contribute to disparities and what can be done about it? J Gen Intern Med. 2004;19:1154–1159. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2004.30227.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Burgess DJ, van Ryn M, Malat J, Matoka M. Understanding the provider contribution to race/ethnicity disparities in pain treatment: insights from social cognitive research on stereotyping. Pain Med. 2006;7:119–134. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4637.2006.00105.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.van Ryn M. Research on the provider contribution to race/ethnicity disparities in medical care. Med Care. 2002;40(1 Suppl):I140–I151. doi: 10.1097/00005650-200201001-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.van Ryn M, Fu SS. Paved with good intentions: do public health and human service providers contribute to racial/ethnic disparities in health? Am J Public Health. 2003;93:248–255. doi: 10.2105/ajph.93.2.248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Green AR, Carney DR, Pallin DJ, et al. Implicit bias among physicians and its prediction of thrombolysis decisions for black and white patients. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22:1231–1238. doi: 10.1007/s11606-007-0258-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Fiske ST, Lin M, Neuberg SL. The continuum model: Ten years later. In: Chaiken S, Trope Y, editors. Dual process theories in social psychology. New York: Guilford; 1999. pp. 211–254. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Brewer MB. A dual process model of impression formation. In: Wyer TSSRS, editor. Advances in Social Cognition. Hillsdale (NJ): Lawrence Erlbaum; 1988. pp. 1–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Fiske ST. Stereotyping, Prejudice, and Discrimination. 4th ed. Boston: McGraw-Hill; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Wegener DT, Dunn M, Tokusato D. The flexible correction model: Phenomenology and the use of naive theories in avoiding or removing bias. In: Moskowitz GB, editor. Cognitive Social Psychology: The Princeton Symposium on the Legacy and Future of Social Cognition. Matwah (NJ): Lawrence Erlbaum; 2001. pp. 277–290. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Gilbert DT, Hixon J. The trouble of thinking: activation and application of stereotypic beliefs. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991;60:509–517. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Macrae C, Hewstone M, Griffiths R. Processing load and memory for stereotype-based information. Eur J Soc Psychol. 1993;23:77–87. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Stangor C, Duan C. Effects of multiple task demands upon memory for information about social goups. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1991;27:357–378. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bodenhausen GV, Lichtenstein M. Social stereotypes and information processing strategies: The impact of task complexity. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1987;52:871–880. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.52.5.871. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Kreuglanski AW, Freund T. The freezing and unfreezing of lay-inferences: Effects on impressional primacy, ethnic stereotyping, and numerical anchoring. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1983;19:448–468. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Bodenhausen GV. Stereotypes as judgemental heuristics: evidence of circadian variations in discrimination. Psychol Sci. 1990;1:319–322. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Martell RF. Sex bias at work: The effects of attentional and memory demands on performance ratings of men and women. J Appl Psychol. 1991;21:1939–1960. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Perry EL, Kulik CT, Bourhis AC. Moderating effects of personal and contextual factors in age discrimination. J Appl Psychol. 1996;81:628–647. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.81.6.628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Spears R, Haslam AS. Stereotyping and the burden of cognitive load. In: Spears R, Oakes PJ, Ellemers N, Haslam SA, editors. The Social Psychology of Stereotyping and Group Life. Malden (MA): Blackwell Publishing; 1997. pp. 171–207. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Dijker AJ, Koomen W. Stereotyping and attitudinal effects under time pressure. Eur J Soc Psychol. 1996;26:61–74. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Sczesny Meta-cognition about biological sex and gender-stereotyping physical appearance: consequences for the assessment of leadership competence. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2004;30:13–21. doi: 10.1177/0146167203258831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Wigboldus DH, Sherman JW, Franzese JW, van Knippenberg A. Capacity and comprehension: spontaneous stereotyping under cognitive load. Soc Cogn. 2004;22:292–309. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Pendry LF, Macrae CN. What the disinterested perceiver overlooks: Goal-directed social categorization. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 1996;22:249–256. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Cunningham WA, Johnson MK, Raye CL, Chris Gatenby J, Gore JC, Banaji MR. Separable neural components in the processing of black and white faces. Psychol Sci. 2004;15:806–813. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00760.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Richeson JA, Baird AA, Gordon HL, et al. An fMRI investigation of the impact of interracial contact on executive function. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:1323–1328. doi: 10.1038/nn1156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Goel V, Dolan RJ. Explaining modulation of reasoning by belief. Cognition. 2003;87:B11–B22. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00185-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100:253–258. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0135058100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Richeson JA, Trawalter S. The threat of appearing prejudiced and race-based attentional biases. Psychol Sci. 2008;19:98–102. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02052.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Ackerman JM, Shapiro JR, Neuberg SL, et al. They all look the same to me (unless they’re angry): from out-group homogeneity to out-group heterogeneity. Psychol Sci. 2006;17:836–840. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01790.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Amodio DM. Intergroup anxiety effects on the control of racial stereotypes: a psychoneuroendocrine analysis. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2009;45:60–67. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Muroff JR, Jackson JS, Mowbray CT, Himle JA. The influence of gender, patient volume and time on clinical diagnostic decision making in psychiatric emergency services. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2007;29:481–488. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2007.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Rosenfield S. Race differences in involuntary hospitalization: psychiatric vs. labeling perspectives. J Health Soc Behav. 1984;25:14–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Jaen C, Stange K, Nutting P. Competing demands of primary care: a model for the delivery of clinical preventive services. J Fam Pract. 1994;38:859–863. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Redelmeier D, Tan S, Booth G. The treatment of unrelated disorders in patients with chronic medical diseases. N Engl J Med. 1998;338:1516–1520. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199805213382106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Parchman ML, Pugh JA, Romero RL, Bowers KW. Competing demands or clinical inertia: the case of elevated glycosylated hemoglobin. Ann Fam Med. 2007;5:196–201. doi: 10.1370/afm.679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Jaen C, McIlvain H, Pol L, Phillips RL, Jr, Flocke S, Crabtree BF. Tailoring tobacco counseling to the competing demands in the clinical encounter. J Fam Pract. 2001;50:859–863. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Nutting P, Rost K, Smith J, Werner J, Elliot C. Competing demands from physical problems: effect on initiating and completing depression care over 6 months. Arch Fam Med. 2000;9:1059–1064. doi: 10.1001/archfami.9.10.1059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Nutting P, Baier M, Werner J, Cutter G, Conry C, Stewart L. Competing demands in the office visit: what influences mammography recommendations. J Am Board Fam Pract. 2001;14:352–361. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Lieberman HR, Tharion WJ, Shukitt-Hale B, Speckman KL, Tulley R. Effects of caffeine, sleep loss, and stress on cognitive performance and mood during U.S. Navy SEAL training. Sea-Air-Land. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002;164:250–261. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1217-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Sagaspe P, Charles A, Taillard J, Bioulac B, Philip P. Inhibition and working memory: effect of acute sleep deprivation on a random generation task. Can J Exp Psychol. 2003;57:265–273. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Nilsson JP, Soderstrom M, Karlsson AU, et al. Less effective executive functioning after one night’s sleep deprivation. J Sleep Res. 2005;14:1–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2005.00442.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Banks S, Dinges DF. Behavioral and physiological consequences of sleep restriction. J Clin Sleep Med. 2007;3:519–528. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Philip P, Sagaspe P, Taillard J, et al. Fatigue, sleep restriction, and performance in automobile drivers: a controlled study in a natural environment. Sleep. 2003;26:277–280. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.3.277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Klein K, Boals A. The relationship of life event stress and working memory capacity. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2001;15:565–579. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Sandstrom A, Rhodin I, Lundberg M, Olsson T, Nyberg L. Impaired cognitive performance in patients with chronic burnout syndrome. Biol Psychol. 2005;69:271–279. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Caswell L, Vitaliano P, Croyle K, Scanland J, Zhang J, Darwula A. Negative associations of chronic stress and cognitive performance in older adult spouse caregivers. Exp Aging Res. 2003;29:303–318. doi: 10.1080/03610730303721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Brand N, Hanson E, Godaert G. Chronic stress affects blood pressure and speed of short-term memory. Percept Mot Skills. 2000;91:291–298. doi: 10.2466/pms.2000.91.1.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Morgan C, Doran A, Steffian G, Hazlett G, Southwick S. Stress-induced deficits in working memory and visuo-constructive abilities in special operations soldiers. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:722–729. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Dibartolo PM, Brown TA, Barlow DH. Effects of anxiety on attentional allocation and task performance: an information processing analysis. Behav Res Ther. 1997;35:1101–1111. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Ashcraft MH, Kirk EP. The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance. J Exp Psychol. 2001;130:224–237. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.130.2.224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Stephan WG, Finlay K. The role of empathy in improving intergroup relations. J Soc Issues. 1999;55:729–743. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Darke S. Effects of anxiety on inferential reasoning task performance. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;55:499–505. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.55.3.499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Oei NY, Everaerd WT, Elzinga BM, van Well S, Bermond B. Psychosocial stress impairs working memory at high loads: an association with cortisol levels and memory retrieval. Stress. 2006;9:133–141. doi: 10.1080/10253890600965773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Robinson SJ, Sunram-Lea SI, Leach J, Owen-Lynch PJ. The effects of exposure to an acute naturalistic stressor on working memory, state anxiety and salivary cortisol concentrations. Stress. 2008;11:115–124. doi: 10.1080/10253890701559970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Braunstein-Bercovitz H. Does stress enhance or impair selective attention? The effects of stress and perceptual load on negative priming. Anxiety Stress Coping. 2003;16:345–357. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Braunstein-Bercovitz H, Dimentman-Ashenazi I, Lubow R. Stress affects the selection of relevant from irrelevant stimuli. Emotion. 2001;1:182–192. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.1.2.182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Williams ES, Manwell LB, Konrad TR, Linzer M. The relationship of organizational culture, stress, satisfaction, and burnout with physician-reported error and suboptimal patient care: results from the MEMO study. Health Care Manage Rev. 2007;32:203–212. doi: 10.1097/01.HMR.0000281626.28363.59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Shanafelt T, Bradley K, Wipf J, Back A. Burnout and self-reported patient care in an internal medicine residency program. Ann Intern Med. 2002;136:358–367. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-136-5-200203050-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Firth-Cozens J, Greenhalgh J. Doctors’ perceptions of the links between stress and lowered clinical care. Soc Sci Med. 1997;44:1017–1022. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(96)00227-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Dugan J, Lauer E, Bouquot Z, Dutro BK, Smith M, Widmeyer G. Stressful nurses: the effect on patient outcomes. J Nurs Care Qual. 1996;10:46–58. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Shirom A, Nirel N, Vinokur AD. Overload, autonomy, and burnout as predictors of physicians’ quality of care. J Occup Health Psychol. 2006;11:328–342. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.11.4.328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Linzer M, Gerrity M, Douglas JA, McMurray JE, Willaims ES, Konrad TR. Physician stress: results from the physician worklife study. Stress and Health. 2002;18:37–42. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Williams ES, Konrad TR, Linzer M, et al. Physician, practice, and patient characteristics related to primary care physician physical and mental health: results from the Physician Worklife Study. Health Serv Res. 2002;37:121–143. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Kassebaum DG, Cutler ER. On the culture of student abuse in medical school. Acad Med. 1998;73:1149–1158. doi: 10.1097/00001888-199811000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Nora LM, McLaughlin MA, Fosson SE, et al. Gender discrimination and sexual harassment in medical education: perspectives gained by a 14-school study. Acad Med. 2002;77(12 Pt 1):1226–1234. doi: 10.1097/00001888-200212000-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Frank E, Brogan D, Schiffman M. Prevalence and correlates of harassment among US women physicians. Arch Intern Med. 1998;158:352–358. doi: 10.1001/archinte.158.4.352. 23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Kirkcaldy BD, Trimpop R, Cooper CL. Working hours, job stress, work satisfaction, and accident rates among medical practitioners and allied personnel. Int J Stress Manag. 1997;4:79–87. [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Gelsema TI, van der Doef M, Maes S, Janssen M, Akerboom S, Verhoeven C. A longitudinal study of job stress in the nursing profession: causes and consequences. J Nurs Manag. 2006;14:289–299. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2934.2006.00635.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Lindholm M. Working conditions, psychosocial resources and work stress in nurses and physicians in chief managers’ positions. J Nurs Manag. 2006;14:300–309. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2934.2006.00636.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Goitein L, Shanafelt TD, Wipf JE, Slatore CG, Back AL. The effects of work-hour limitations on resident well-being, patient care, and education in an internal medicine residency program. Arch Intern Med. 2005;165:2601–2606. doi: 10.1001/archinte.165.22.2601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Gopal R, Glasheen JJ, Miyoshi TJ, Prochazka AV. Burnout and internal medicine resident work-hour restrictions. Arch Internal Med. 2005;165:2595–2600. doi: 10.1001/archinte.165.22.2595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Landrigan CP, Fahrenkopf AM, Lewin D, et al. Effects of the accreditation council for graduate medical education duty hour limits on sleep, work hours, and safety. Pediatrics. 2008;122:250–258. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-2306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Patel VL, Arocha JF, Kaufman DR. A primer on aspects of cognition for medical informatics. JAMA. 2001;65:193–211. doi: 10.1136/jamia.2001.0080324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Galanter CA, Patel VL. Medical decision making: a selective review for child psychiatrists and psychologists. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46:675–689. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01452.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Patel VL, Kaufman DR, Arocha JF. Emerging paradigms of cognition in medical decision making. J Biomed Inform. 2002;35:52–75. doi: 10.1016/s1532-0464(02)00009-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Workman M, Lesser MF, Kim J. An exploratory study of cognitive load in diagnosing patient conditions. Int J Qual Health Care. 2007;19:127–133. doi: 10.1093/intqhc/mzm007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Sequist TD, Adams A, Zhang F, Ross-Degnan D, Ayanian JZ. Effect of quality improvement on racial disparities in diabetes care. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166:675–681. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.6.675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Wells K, Sherbourne C, Schoenbaum M, et al. Five-year impact of quality improvement for depression: results of a group-level randomized controlled trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:378–386. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.4.378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Sehgal AR. Impact of quality improvement efforts on race and sex disparities in hemodialysis. JAMA. 2003;289:996–1000. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.8.996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Miranda J, Duan N, Sherbourne C, et al. Improving care for minorities: can quality improvement interventions improve care and outcomes for depressed minorities? Results of a randomized, controlled trial. Health Serv Res. 2003;38:613–630. doi: 10.1111/1475-6773.00136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Vorauer JD, Hunter AJ, Main KJ, Roy SA. Meta-stereotype activation: evidence from indirect measures for specific evaluative concerns experienced by members of dominant groups in intergroup interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;78:690–707. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.4.690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Plant EA, Devine PG. Internal and external motivation to respond without prejudice. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;75:811–832. doi: 10.1177/0146167205275304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Richeson JA, Trawalter S. Why do interracial interactions impair executive function? A resource depletion account. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2005;88:934–947. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.6.934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Richeson JA, Shelton JN. When prejudice does not pay: effects of interracial contact on executive function. Psychol Sci. 2003;14:287–290. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.03437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Blascovich J, Mendes WB, Hunter SB, Lickel B, Kowai-Bell N. Perceiver threat in social interactions with stigmatized others. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;80:253–267. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Mendes WB, Blascovich J, Lickel B, Hunter S. Challenge and threat during social interaction with white and black men. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2002;28:939–952. [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Saha S, Korthuis P, Cohn A, Sharp V, Moore R, Beach M. Physician Interracial Anxiety, Patient Trust, and Satisfaction with HIV Care. Presentation at the International Conference on Communication in Healthcare; September 2008; Oslo, Norway. 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Bach PB. Racial disparities and site of care. Ethn Dis. 2005;15(2) Suppl 2:S31–S33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Varkey AB, Manwell LB, Williams ES, et al. Separate and unequal: clinics where minority and nonminority patients receive primary care. Arch Intern Med. 2009;169:243–250. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2008.559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Bach PB, Pham HH, Schrag D, Tate RC, Hargraves JL. Primary care physicians who treat blacks and whites. N Engl J Med. 2004;351:575–584. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa040609. 5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Birkmeyer JD, Siewers AE, Finlayson EV, et al. Hospital volume and surgical mortality in the United States. N Engl J Med. 2002;346:1128–1137. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa012337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Birkmeyer JD, Stukel TA, Siewers AE, Goodney PP, Wennberg DE, Lucas FL. Surgeon volume and operative mortality in the United States. N Engl J Med. 2003;349:2117–2127. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa035205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Liu JH, Zingmond DS, McGory ML, et al. Disparities in the utilization of high-volume hospitals for complex surgery. JAMA. 2006;296:1973–1980. doi: 10.1001/jama.296.16.1973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Jha AK, Shlipak MG, Hosmer W, Frances CD, Browner WS. Racial differences in mortality among men hospitalized in the Veterans Affairs health care system. JAMA. 2001;285:297–303. doi: 10.1001/jama.285.3.297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Skinner J, Chandra A, Staiger D, Lee J, McClellan M. Mortality after acute myocardial infarction in hospitals that disproportionately treat black patients. Circulation. 2005;112:2634–2641. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.105.543231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Epstein AM. Health care in America–still too separate, not yet equal. N Engl J Med. 2004;351:603–605. doi: 10.1056/NEJMe048181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Clarke SP, Davis BL, Nailon RE. Racial segregation and differential outcomes in hospital care. West J Nurs Res. 2007;29:739–757. doi: 10.1177/0193945907303167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Lucas FL, Stukel TA, Morris AM, Siewers AE, Birkmeyer JD. Race and surgical mortality in the United States. Ann Surg. 2006;243:281–286. doi: 10.1097/01.sla.0000197560.92456.32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Konety SH, Vaughan Sarrazin MS, Rosenthal GE. Patient and hospital differences underlying racial variation in outcomes after coronary artery bypass graft surgery. Circulation. 2005;111:1210–1216. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.0000157728.49918.9F. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Barnato AE, Lucas FL, Staiger D, Wennberg DE, Chandra A. Hospital-level racial disparities in acute myocardial infarction treatment and outcomes. Med Care. 2005;43:308–319. doi: 10.1097/01.mlr.0000156848.62086.06. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Hasnain-Wynia R, Baker DW, Nerenz D, et al. Disparities in health care are driven by where minority patients seek care: examination of the hospital quality alliance measures. Arch Intern Med. 2007;167:1233–1239. doi: 10.1001/archinte.167.12.1233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Silber JH, Rosenbaum PR, Zhang X, Even-Shoshan O. Influence of patient and hospital characteristics on anesthesia time in medicare patients undergoing general and orthopedic surgery. Anesthesiology. 2007;106:356–364. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200702000-00025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Groeneveld PW, Kruse GB, Chen Z, Asch DA. Variation in cardiac procedure use and racial disparity among Veterans Affairs Hospitals. Am Heart J. 2007;153:320–327. doi: 10.1016/j.ahj.2006.10.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Tamayo-Sarver JH, Dawson NV, Hinze SW, et al. The effect of race/ethnicity and desirable social characteristics on physicians’ decisions to prescribe opioid analgesics. Acad Emerg Med. 2003;10:1239–1248. doi: 10.1111/j.1553-2712.2003.tb00608.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Battaglia TA, Ash A, Prout MN, Freund KM. Cancer prevention trials and primary care physicians: factors associated with recommending trial enrollment. Cancer Detect Prev. 2006;30:34–37. doi: 10.1016/j.cdp.2005.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Baig A, Shadigian E, Heisler M. Hidden from plain sight: residents’ domestic violence screening attitudes and reported practices. J Gen Intern Med. 2006;21:949–954. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2006.00494.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Santry HP, Lauderdale DS, Cagney KA, Rathouz PJ, Alverdy JC, Chin MH. Predictors of patient selection in bariatric surgery. Ann Surg. 2007;245:59–67. doi: 10.1097/01.sla.0000232551.55712.b3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Aberegg SK, Terry PB. Medical decision-making and healthcare disparities: the physician’s role. J Lab Clin Med. 2004;144:11–17. doi: 10.1016/j.lab.2004.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Mensah GA, Mokdad AH, Ford ES, Greenlund KJ, Croft JB. State of disparities in cardiovascular health in the United States. Circulation. 2005;111:1233–1241. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.0000158136.76824.04. 15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Kressin NR, Petersen LA. Racial differences in the use of invasive cardiovascular procedures: review of the literature and prescription for future research. Ann Intern Med. 2001;135:352–366. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-135-5-200109040-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Arber S, McKinlay J, Adams A, Marceau L, Link C, O’Donnell A. Influence of patient characteristics on doctors’ questioning and lifestyle advice for coronary heart disease: a UK/US video experiment. Br J Gen Pract. 2004;54:673–678. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Arber S, McKinlay J, Adams A, Marceau L, Link C, O’Donnell A. Patient characteristics and inequalities in doctors’ diagnostic and management strategies relating to CHD: a video-simulation experiment. Soc Sci Med. 2006;62:103–115. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.05.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.McKinlay JB, Link CL, Freund KM, Marceau LD, O’Donnell AB, Lutfey KL. Sources of variation in physician adherence with clinical guidelines: results from a factorial experiment. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22:289–296. doi: 10.1007/s11606-006-0075-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Barnhart JM, Wassertheil-Smoller S. The effect of race/ethnicity, sex, and social circumstances on coronary revascularization preferences: a vignette comparison. Cardiol Rev. 2006;14:215–222. doi: 10.1097/01.crd.0000214180.24372.d5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Schulman KA, Berlin JA, Harless W, et al. The effect of race and sex on physicians’ recommendations for cardiac catheterization. N Engl J Med. 1999;340:618–626. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199902253400806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Kales HC, Neighbors HW, Blow FC, et al. Race, gender, and psychiatrists’ diagnosis and treatment of major depression among elderly patients. Psychiatr Serv. 2005;56:721–728. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.56.6.721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Kales HC, Neighbors HW, Valenstein M, et al. Effect of race and sex on primary care physicians’ diagnosis and treatment of late-life depression. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53:777–784. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53255.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.McKinlay JB, Lin T, Freund K, Moskowitz M. The unexpected influence of physician attributes on clinical decisions: results of an experiment. J Health Soc Behav. 2002;43:92–106. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Schenstrom A, Ronnberg S, Bodlund O. Mindfulness-based cognitive attitude training for primary care staff: a pilot study. Complement Health Pract Rev. 2006;11:144–152. [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Mackenzie CS, Poulin PA, Seidman-Carlson R. A brief mindfulness-based stress reduction intervention for nurses and nurse aides. Appl Nurs Res. 2006;19:105–109. doi: 10.1016/j.apnr.2005.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Shapiro SL, Astin JA, Bishop SR, Cordova M. Mindfulness-based stress reduction for health care professionals: Results from a randomized trial. Int J Stress Manag. 2005;12:164–176. [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Shapiro SL, Shapiro DE, Schwartz GE. Stress management in medical education: a review of the literature. Acad Med. 2000;75:748–759. doi: 10.1097/00001888-200007000-00023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Monteith MJ, Ashburn-Nardo L, Voils CI, Czopp AM. Putting the brakes on prejudice: on the development and operation of cues for control. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002;83:1029–1050. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.83.5.1029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Feldon DF. Cognitive load and classroom teaching: The double-edged sword of automaticity. Educational Psychologist. 2007;42:123–137. [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Epstein RM. Mindful practice. JAMA. 1999;282:833–839. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.9.833. 1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Smith RC, Dwamena FC, Fortin AHt. Teaching personal awareness. J Gen Intern Med. 2005;20:201–207. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2005.40212.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Fackler J, Watts C, Grome A, Miller T, Crandall B, Pronovost P. Critical care physician cognitive task analysis: an exploratory study. Crit Care. 2009;13:R33. doi: 10.1186/cc7740. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Burgess D, van Ryn M, Dovidio J, Saha S. Reducing racial bias among health care providers: lessons from social-cognitive psychology. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22:882–887. doi: 10.1007/s11606-007-0160-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Gawande A. The checklist. The New Yorker. 2007 Dec 10; [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Pronovost PJ, Berenholtz SM, Goeschel C, et al. Improving patient safety in intensive care units in Michigan. J Crit Care. 2008;23:207–221. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrc.2007.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Dreachslin JL, Weech-Maldonado R, Dansky KH. Racial and ethnic diversity and organizational behavior: a focused research agenda for health services management. Soc Sci Med. 2004;59:961–971. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2003.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.van Ryn M, Burke J. The effect of patient race and socioeconomic status on physicians’ perceptions of patients. Soc Sci Med. 2000;50:813–828. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(99)00338-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.van Ryn M, Burgess D, Malat J, Griffin J. Physicians’ perceptions of patients’ social and behavioral characteristics and race disparities in treatment recommendations for men with coronary artery disease. Am J Public Health. 2006;96:351–357. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2004.041806. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES