Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Am Geriatr Soc. 2014 Mar 17;62(4):673–678. doi: 10.1111/jgs.12734

Association Between Performance on Timed Up and Go Subtasks and Mild Cognitive Impairment: Further Insights into the Links Between Cognitive and Motor Function

Anat Mirelman 1,2, Aner Weiss 1, Aron S Buchman 3, David A Bennett 3, Nir Giladi 1,4, Jefferey M Hausdorff 1,4,5,6
PMCID: PMC3989433  NIHMSID: NIHMS554730  PMID: 24635699

Abstract

Background

Time to complete the Timed Up and Go (TUG), a test of mobility and fall risk, was recently associated with cognitive function.

Objectives

To assess whether different TUG subtasks are preferentially affected among older adults with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and are specific to different cognitive abilities.

Design

Cross sectional study

Setting

Community and home setting

Participants

347 older adults without dementia (mean 83.6±3.5yrs, 75% females, 19.3% MCI) participating in the Rush Memory and Aging Project.

Measurements

Subjects wore a small, light-weight sensor that measured acceleration and angular velocity while they performed the instrumented TUG (iTUG). Measures of iTUG were derived from 4 subtasks: walking, turning, sit-to-stand and stand-to-sit and compared between participants with no cognitive impairment (NCI) versus MCI.

Results

NCI and MCI did not differ in age, sex, years of education (p>0.44) or time to complete the TUG (NCI:7.6±3.7sec vs. MCI:8.4±3.7sec;p=0.12). MCI had less walking consistency (p=0.0091), smaller pitch range during transitions (p=0.005), lower angular velocity during turning, and required more time to complete the turn-to-walk (p=0.042). Gait consistency was correlated with perceptual speed (p=0.012) and turning was correlated with perceptual speed (p=0.024) and visual-spatial abilities (p=0.049).

Conclusions

MCI is associated with impaired performance on iTUG subtasks that cannot be identified when simply measuring overall duration of performance. Distinctive iTUG tasks were related to particular cognitive domains, demonstrating the specificity of motor-cognitive interactions. Using a single body worn sensor for quantify of mobility may facilitate our understanding of late-life gait impairments and their inter-relationship with cognitive decline.

Keywords: aging, mild cognitive impairment, accelerometers, gait, cognition

INTRODUCTION

Gait disorders are common among subjects with cognitive impairments1;2. Alterations in walking may be detected early in the course of dementia and even in the prodromal stage of mild cognitive impairment (MCI)3. Motor slowing may precede and predict the onset of cognitive impairment4;5. Furthermore, participants with cognitive decline and slow gait speed (<1.00 m/s) are twice as likely to develop dementia, specifically vascular dementia, compared to subjects with intact cognition and normal gait speeds6. Interestingly, the combined motor and cognitive assessment provided better predictive value for dementia than cognitive function alone. These findings highlight the importance of gait assessment for augmenting the early identification and natural progression of neurodegenerative disorders associated with dementia6 and support the idea that late-life gait impairments may accompany cognitive decline. Understanding the underpinnings of motor-cognitive changes in MCI is likely to provide important insights into the underlying biology and to provide unique opportunities for interventions that may slow the neurodegenerative process.

The ‘Timed Up and Go’ test (TUG)7 is a quick and widely used performance-based measure of mobility. The TUG has been extensively studied in older adults8;9 and recommended as a simple screening test of fall risk10. TUG duration has also been associated with cognitive function11;12. More specifically, older adults with better executive function and attention performed the TUG more quickly11;12 . The TUG is composed of several different subtasks including transitions, straight-line walking and turning. Successful completion of the transition and turning sub-tasks of the TUG requires the integration of more cognitive resources than straight-line walking11, perhaps due to the processing of different visual and afferent inputs during straight-line walking and curved walking or turns13. If so, different TUG components might be particularly sensitive as predictors of future cognitive decline, more so than just the overall time to complete the TUG. However, the factors that contribute to the association between cognitive function and TUG performance are not well-known.

Previous work has demonstrated the added value of using body-worn sensors to augment the traditional TUG. This approach has been referred to as the instrumented TUG (iTUG)14. The iTUG allows for the quantitative characterization of the specific components of the TUG and their contribution to overall performance15;16. It is also sensitive to changes in performance, disease progression, and disability17;18 and may increase the predictive value for cognitive decline19. Thus, given the associations between the TUG and cognitive function11;12, we tested the hypothesis that different TUG subtasks might be preferentially affected in MCI. In addition, we tested whether specific TUG subtasks were differentially associated with cognitive abilities.

METHODS

Subjects

Subjects participating in the Rush Memory and Aging Project (MAP), a longitudinal cohort study of chronic conditions of aging20, were studied. Participants were recruited from retirement facilities and subsidized housing facilities from around the Chicago metropolitan area. All participants signed an informed consent agreeing to annual clinical evaluation. The study was approved by the institutional review board of Rush University Medical Center. The hybrid body-worn sensor (see below) was added to the study in 2011enabling yearly assessment if motor performance. Persons were eligible for this cross-sectional analyses if they were ambulatory, and without clinical dementia (see online methods) at the time of iTUG testing.

Cognitive and Clinical Testing

Subjects underwent a uniform structured clinical evaluation including assessment of medical history, neurological examination, motor and cognitive performance testing. Details of the clinical evaluation are described elsewhere20 and in the online supplementary material.

Timed Up and Go test

To complete the TUG, participants were instructed to stand up from a chair without using arm rests if they were present, walk 8 feet to a designated location at their normal pace, turn around, walk back and sit back down on the same chair7. The use of walkers and canes was allowed. Participants wore a portable small, light-weight body-fixed sensor (Hybrid, Mcroberts. The Hague) on their lower back secured with a neoprene belt. The sensor included tri-axial a accelerometer and gyroscope. Acceleration signals were derived from three axes: vertical (V), medio-lateral (ML) and anterior posterior (AP). Angular velocities were derived from the gyroscope as: yaw- the rotation around the vertical axis, pitch- the rotation around the medial-lateral axis, and roll- the rotation around the anterior-posterior axis. The device was set to record continuously during two trials of TUG; data presented are from the 2nd trial. After testing was completed, data were transferred to a personal computer for further analysis using Matlab software (version R2012b).

Quantifying the TUG Subtasks

An automated algorithm based on the anterior-posterior axis was used for detecting the start and end times of the TUG as previously described17;21. In addition, quantitative measures for 4 subtasks (sit-to-stand and stand-to-sit transitions, walking and turning) were derived (see the online supplementary material).

Statistical Analyses

All data was assessed for normalcy using Komogorov-Smirnov and Shapiro-Wilk tests, and with Levene's test for homogeneity. Student t-tests and the equivalent Mann Whitney compared iTUG measures between the participants with and without MCI. Univariate regression models examined the associations between iTUG measures and the global cognitive score. For each TUG subtask, the corresponding iTUG measure that significantly distinguished between the groups was used as the dependent variable in the models to evaluate motor-cognitive associations (see Table 2 and Table 3). Cognitive measures significantly associated with iTUG measures in univariate models were entered into the multivariate models adjusted for age, sex and education. All analyses were corrected for multiple comparisons using the Benjamini- Hochberg method22. Group values are reported as mean±standard deviation. Statistical analyses was performed using SPSS version 19.

Table 2.

iTUG measures in MCI and NCI

Measure Axis MCI NCI P-value
Overall TUG
TUG Duration [s] 8.43±3.72 7.61±3.78 0.121
Walking
Walking Duration [s] V 4.98±1.84 4.5±2.06 0.386
Number of steps [#] V 13.49±5.48 12.13±5.00 0.058
Average step duration [s] V 0.61±0.64 0.61±0.70 0.724
Step regularity [g^2] V 0.45±0.13 0.50±0.13 0.009
AP 0.39±0.15 0.46±0.14 0.002
Step symmetry V 1.35±0.43 1.36±0.54 0.934
AP 1.17±0.50 1.33±0.83 0.144
Sit-to-Stand Transition
Duration [s] AP 1.04±0.74 0.96±0.80 0.465
Range [g] AP 0.89±0.27 1.02±0.44 0.036
Jerk [g/s] AP −1.08±0.76 −1.67±0.48 0.330
Range [deg/s] Pitch 151.07±48.43 165.56±54.38 0.005
Jerk [deg/s^2] Pitch 171.23±103.82 197.12±97.81 0.006
Stand-to-Sit Transition
Duration [s] AP 1.01±0.48 0.98±0.51 0.680
Range [g] AP 1.11±0.38 1.11±0.35 0.863
Jerk [g/s] AP 1.08±0.63 1.18±0.87 0.349
Range [deg/s] Pitch 159.65±50.37 161.87±55.38 0.770
Jerk [deg/s^2] Pitch 132.18±89.12 149.74±98.62 0.194
Turn-to-walk
Duration [s] Yaw 2.41±0.67 2.23±0.61 0.042
Yaw Amplitude [deg/s] Yaw 135.56±41.145 146.32±37.78 0.045
Turn-to-sit
Duration [s] Yaw 2.19±0.54 2.17±0.64 0.889
Yaw Amplitude [deg/s] Yaw 134.36±37.99 144.64±41.10 0.050

Acceleration units are represented by g (gravitational), as they represent force per unit mass Angular velocity collected by the gyroscope is represented in deg/sec.

* P-values that are bolded were statistically significant after adjusting for multiple comparisons.

Table 3.

Associations between TUG Subtask Measures and Cognitive Abilities (n=347)

Univariate model Multivariate model
Walking (dependent variable: Step regularity [g^2] in the V axis)
Episodic memory score 0.03 (0.012-0.046) p=0.001 0.02 (−0.003-0.041) p=0.08
Semantic memory score 0.02 (0.002-0.04) p=0.033 −0.02 (−0.047-0.006) p=0.14
Working memory score 0.03 (0.009-0.05) p=0.004 0.009 (−0.013-0.031) p=0.41
Perceptual speed score 0.04 (0.02-0.056) p=0.000 0.04 (0.011-0.059) p=0.004
Visual-spatial score 0.02 (−0.003-0.034) p=0.108
Sit-to-Stand (dependent variable: Pitch range [deg/s] of transition)
Episodic memory score −0.19 (−0.078-0.40) p=0.533
Semantic memory score −0.04 (−0.11-0.02) p=0.203
Working memory score −0.09 (−0.16—0.26) p=0.007 −0.06 (−0.13-0.012) p=0.102
Perceptual speed score −0.07(−0.13—0.008) p=0.027 −0.04 (−0.11-0.026) p=0.215
Visual-spatial score −0.06 (−0.12-0.005) p=0.07
Turning (dependent variable: yaw amplitude [deg/s] during the turn-to-walk transition)
Episodic memory score 4.91 (0.41-9.41) p=0.033 −1.23 (−6.55-4.09) p=0.65
Semantic memory score 4.45 (−0.66-9.56) p=0.088
Working memory score 6.89 (1.81-11.97) p=0.008 0.40 (−5.53-6.34) p=0.89
Perceptual speed score 9.83 (5.03-14.64) p=0.000 6.09 (0.74-11.44) p=0.03
Visual-spatial ability score 8.77 (3.86-13.68) p=0.000 8.68 (2.61-14.74) p=0.05

* Entries are the B values, 95% confidence intervals and the associated p-value. Multivariate model was adjusted for age, sex and years of education.

RESULTS

The clinical characteristics of the participants (n=347, 19.3% MCI) are summarized in Table 1. Individuals with no cognitive impairment (NCI) and MCI did not differ with respect to age, sex, years of education or co-morbidities (p>0.44).

Table 1.

Subject characteristics

MCI NCI P-Value
Demographics
Number of subjects 67 280
Age (yrs) 83.35±3.50 82.75±4.17 0.901
Sex (% female) 75% 74% 0.804
Body-mass index (kg/m2) 27.92±5.36 27.22±5.61 0.484
Education (yrs) 15.18±3.35 15.03±2.83 0.447
Vascular Disease Comorbidities score 0.35±0.57 0.34±0.59 0.888
Vascular Risk score 1.24±0.79 1.22±0.83 0.802
Cognitive Function*
Global cognition score −0.24±0.55 0.33±0.41 P<0.001
Episodic memory score −0.35±0.81 0.51±0.50 P<0.001
Semantic memory score −0.15±0.76 0.32±0.59 P<0.001
Working memory score −0.16±0.73 0.14±0.67 0.002
Perceptual speed score −0.19±0.68 0.20±0.68 P<0.001
Visual-spatial score −0.28±0.89 0.28±0.67 P<0.001
Gait
Gait speed (m/sec) 0.57±0.16 0.48±0.16 0.637
*

Cognitive function is reported using z scores. Higher values reflect better performance.

Vascular disease comorbidity score- This variable measures the participants’ vascular disease burden computed on the basis of self-report questions, clinical examination, and medication inspection. Vascular Risk score- includes the summary scores indicating each individual's vascular risk burden, computed on the basis of self-report questions, clinical examination, and medication inspection (i.e., sum of hypertension, and smoking).

TUG Subtask Measures in MCI and NCI

Time to complete the TUG was not significantly different in the two groups (MCI:8.4±3.7sec vs. NCI:7.6±3.7sec; p=0.12), but iTUG measures differed between MCI and NCI (Table 2).

Walking

Subjects with MCI had lower step regularity (i.e., less consistency of gait) in both the vertical and anterior-posterior directions (Table 2).

Transitions

MCI subjects had lower pitch range and jerk during the transitions from sitting to standing suggesting less forward progression during the movement (Table 2).

Turns

Duration of the turn-to-walk was longer in subjects with MCI and the yaw amplitude was lower in both turns, reflecting lower angular velocity during the turns in the subjects with MCI (Table 2).

TUG Subtask Measures and Cognitive Abilities

The global cognitive score was correlated with gait regularity, pitch range during transitions, and yaw amplitude during turns. In univariate analyses, episodic, semantic, working memory and perceptual speed were correlated with step regularity. Working memory and perceptual speed were correlated with the pitch range during the sit-to-stand transition. Episodic memory, working memory, perceptual speed and visual-spatial ability were correlated with yaw amplitude during the turns (see Table 3). In a multivariate analysis model, perceptual speed remained significantly associated with step regularity during walking and perceptual speed and visual-spatial abilities were correlated with turning (see Table 3).

DISCUSSION

This study demonstrates that utilizing acceleration and angular velocity signals to quantify the TUG and its subtasks can provide insights into gait performance and its association with cognitive function in older adults. The traditional time to complete the TUG was not significantly different between individuals with and without MCI, however, the groups differed in their performance on the subtasks. Application of the sensors enabled acquiring information about performance that is not merely related to the duration of movement but also to the quality of movement generation. Specifically, measures derived from the sensor revealed higher step irregularity in walking in subjects with MCI, less forward trunk movement during transitions, and less axial rotation during turns. The iTUG appears to differentiate gait performance in older adults with MCI from those with normal cognitive function. Moreover, the results suggest that TUG subtasks may be differentially associated with distinct cognitive abilities.

Several intriguing studies demonstrated that motor slowing may predict the onset of cognitive impairment4;5. Thus, one might expect that gait speed would be slower and hence time to complete the TUG would be longer in MCI subjects. In our study, time to complete the TUG was similar in the NCI and MCI groups. Although surprising, this finding is consistent with other studies19;23 which similarly, did not find differences in TUG duration in older adults with and without cognitive decline. A possible explanation could be related to the relatively short duration of walk in the iTUG, which perhaps led to a floor effect or to the increased inter-subject variability in MCI as a result of cognitive reserve24. Nonetheless, specific iTUG components were altered in the subjects with MCI.

Despite its apparent simplicity, the TUG can be viewed as a sequence of complex multiple tasks25. It relies on several distinct aspects of strength, balance, and mobility and coordination of serial movements. Sit-to-stand requires forward movement of the center-of-mass while still seated (in preparation for standing), acceleration in the anterior-posterior and vertical plane, push-off, and finally stabilization once standing is achieved25. The TUG then requires appropriate initiation of stepping, and preparation for turning. Thus, the added value of using the iTUG is the ability to associate the specificity of the task to changes in axial measures and performance. For example, to efficiently transfer from siting to standing, it is necessary to generate a large pitch, forward elevation moment, initiation of movement and planning. Stand-to-sit, on the other hand, is a quicker, more ballistic movement that requires deceleration of movement but less control. Therefore these measures are highly sensitive to change in performance and as observed, were able to differentiate between the groups. The ability to assess different complex tasks perhaps makes the iTUG more useful clinically and when evaluating early changes in function, than assessing more simple tasks such as straight line walking.

The differences observed in the iTUG subtasks in subjects with MCI may reflect mild motor impairment. These changes are not yet clinically significant, but could represent a decline of motor function that may parallel mild cognitive impairments. This finding is in line with an earlier study that showed motor decline in individuals with MCI26. Intriguingly, the detection of these alterations, even while gait speed did not differ from that of the cognitively intact controls, suggests that perhaps these measures may be more sensitive than just time to completion or gait speed. Green et al. used gait measures obtained during the iTUG to predict cognitive decline in older adults without dementia in a longitudinal study. At baseline, all participants were cognitively intact and gait speed was similar between groups. After 2 years, 12% demonstrated cognitive decline, however, their gait speed was still intact but quantitative gait measures from the iTUG turn differed between the groups19. Our study adds to this repot by demonstrating that changes can be observed in the different subtasks and that each of the subtasks is unique in identifying early motor changes associated with specific cognitive decline. Although a wide range of gait tests or metrics that capture additional aspects of balance and mobility may be useful for evaluating “motor” function in individuals with MCI, the iTUG is simple and quick to perform, while also capturing multiple domains of motor function, mobility and cognitive function. Hence, it may represent a simple way of augmenting the clinical assessment, with minimal additional costs or time.

Consistent with earlier reports11;12, we found associations between the iTUG and several cognitive domains. Perceptual speed, reflecting executive function abilities, was associated with walking performance on the iTUG. Prolonged time to plan a movement is particularly relevant for TUG, where fluid transitions between consecutive phases of the task are required. The contribution of visual spatial processing to performance of the turn-to-walk transition (both duration and amplitude) is intriguing. Turning-related neural systems may be more vulnerable to functional impairments than straight line gait. Turning involves more inter-limb coordination, more coupling between posture and gait, modification of locomotor patterns requiring frontal lobe cognitive and executive function as well as perception which plays a role in postural transitions. Turing also places greater demands on visual processing to enable clear directional movement12. A recent factor analysis model of gait posits that domains of pace (speed) and gait variability are related to attention and processing27. The present results support this idea in that slower processing speed was related to TUG performance. Our findings also provide support for the processing speed theory which posits that decline in processing speed reflects a general constraint on all cognitive processing28. This also further reinforces the notion that motor function is associated with cognitive function due to the planning and decision making that is necessary for the successful completion of all movements3.

The mild, but detectable changes in motor function even in a simple test like the iTUG might indicate the beginning of a process that is heading towards more severe disability. These findings may be clinically relevant in older adults because they may provide insight into functional decline in an early stage, when neural substrates still exist and potential preventive interventions can be provided. The iTUG can be used to further enhance the prediction of cognitive impairment6, above and beyond gait speed and, therefore, might be useful for assessing patients with early neurodegenerative diseases (e.g., Parkinson's or Alzheimer's disease) as a measure of disease progression that could also inform on increased risk of falls6. The results also support recommendations for early intervention to preserve mobility that should focus on improving specific physical performance of transitions and turns and cognitive function. As the availability of these assessment methods becomes more commonplace in the clinical and research communities, it will have the potential to transform diagnostic and rehabilitation paradigms by providing clinicians with a larger lexicon for describing gait impairment in older adults. It is, however, important to keep in mind that the association between motor and cognitive function in individuals with MCI as found in this cross sectional study may just be the result of a shared common biology due to some underlying latent variables, i.e., not causal3. While this question cannot be resolved in the present study, the associations between specific cognitive features and specific TUG components is consistent with the possibility that TUG performance actually relies on cognitive function.

There are several limitations to our study. The groups were unbalanced in terms of the size of the cohorts (NCI vs. MCI). Since the study was conducted in the participant's homes, we used a slightly shorter ddistance to assess the TUG and could not fully control the conditions in the home. Nonetheless, we suggest that a difference in length would not change the underlying CNS mechanisms for the planning, initiation and execution of movement. Regardless, the present findings indicate that the iTUG may be a useful tool for detecting early mobility changes in MCI and may also provide clinicians with a sensitive means for assessing functional ability.

Supplementary Material

Supp Fig S1-S2
Supplementary Material

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors would like to acknowledge the contribution of the participants in the study and members of the research team.

Funding source: This work was supported in part by NIH grants R01AG17917 and RO1NS078009.

Grant support is indicated on the title page.

Sponsor's role: The funding sources had no involvement in the design, interpretation, or writing of this article.

Footnotes

Author contributions: Anat Mirelman, Aner Weiss and Jefferey M. Hausdorff prepared the manuscript. Aron S. Buchman was in charge of data acquisition and data transfer. Aron S. Buchman and David A. Bennett were responsible for funding. Anat Mirelman and Jefferey M. Hausdorff have performed the analysis. All authors have read, provided input on the drafts of the manuscript and reviewed the final version of the reported manuscript.

Conflict of interest:

None of the authors report any conflicts of interest.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Buchman AS, Boyle PA, Wilson RS, et al. Physical activity and motor decline in older persons. Muscle Nerve. 2007;35:354–362. doi: 10.1002/mus.20702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Verghese J, Lipton RB, Hall CB, et al. Abnormality of gait as a predictor of non-Alzheimer's dementia. N Engl J Med. 2002;347:1761–1768. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa020441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hausdorff JM, Buchman AS. What links gait speed and MCI with dementia? A fresh look at the association between motor and cognitive function. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2013;68:409–411. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glt002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Buracchio T, Dodge HH, Howieson D, et al. The trajectory of gait speed preceding mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2010;67:980–986. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Waite LM, Grayson DA, Piguet O, et al. Gait slowing as a predictor of incident dementia: 6-year longitudinal data from the Sydney Older Persons Study. J Neurol Sci. 2005:229–230. 89–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2004.11.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Verghese J, Wang C, Lipton RB, et al. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome and the risk of dementia. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2013;68:412–418. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gls191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Podsiadlo D, Richardson S. The timed “Up & Go”: A test of basic functional mobility for frail elderly persons. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1991;39:142–148. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1991.tb01616.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hatch J, Gill-Body KM, Portney LG. Determinants of balance confidence in community-dwelling elderly people. Phys Ther. 2003;83:1072–1079. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Shumway-Cook A, Bauer S, Woollacott M. Predicting the probability for falls in community-dwelling older adults using the “timed up & go” test. Phys Ther. 2000;80:896–903. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Guideline for the prevention of falls in older persons. American Geriatrics Society, British Geriatrics Society, and American Academy of Orthopaedic Surgeons Panel on Falls Prevention. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2001;49:664–672. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Donoghue OA, Horgan NF, Savva GM, et al. Association between timed up-and-go and memory, executive function, and processing speed. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2012;60:1681–1686. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.04120.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Herman T, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM. Properties of the ‘timed up and go’ test: More than meets the eye. Gerontology. 2011;57:203–210. doi: 10.1159/000314963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Odonkor CA, Thomas JC, Holt N, et al. A comparison of straight- and curved-path walking tests among mobility-limited older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2013;68:1532–1539. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glt060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Weiss A, Mirelman A, Buchman AS, et al. Using a body-fixed sensor to identify subclinical gait difficulties in older adults with IADL disability: Maximizing the output of the Timed Up and Go. PLoS One. 2013 Jul 29;8(7) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068885. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Najafi B, Aminian K, Loew F, et al. Measurement of stand-sit and sit-stand transitions using a miniature gyroscope and its application in fall risk evaluation in the elderly. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 2002;49:843–851. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2002.800763. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Zijlstra W, Bisseling RW, Schlumbohm S, et al. A body-fixed-sensor-based analysis of power during sit-to-stand movements. Gait Posture. 2010;31:272–278. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Weiss A, Herman T, Plotnik M, et al. An instrumented timed up and go: The added value of an accelerometer for identifying fall risk in idiopathic fallers. Physiol Meas. 2011;32:2003–2018. doi: 10.1088/0967-3334/32/12/009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Salarian A, Zampieri C, Horak FB, et al. Analyzing 180 degrees turns using an inertial system reveals early signs of progression of Parkinson's disease. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. 2009;2009:224–227. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2009.5333970. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Greene BR, McGrath D, Foran TG, et al. Body-worn sensor based surrogates of minimum ground clearance in elderly fallers and controls. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. 2011;2011:6499–6502. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2011.6091732. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Buchman AS, et al. The Rush Memory and Aging Project: Study design and baseline characteristics of the study cohort. Neuroepidemiology. 2005;25:163–175. doi: 10.1159/000087446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Weiss A, Herman T, Plotnik M, et al. Can an accelerometer enhance the utility of the “timed up & go” test when evaluating patients with Parkinson's disease? Med Eng Phys. 2010;32:119–125. doi: 10.1016/j.medengphy.2009.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J Royal Stat Soc. 1995;57:125–133. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Eggermont LH, Gavett BE, Volkers KM, et al. Lower-extremity function in cognitively healthy aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer's disease. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2010;91:584–588. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2009.11.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Holtzer R, Wang C, Lipton R, et al. The protective effects of executive functions and episodicmemory on gait speed decline in aging defined in the context of cognitive reserve. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2012;60:2093–2098. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.04193.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Janssen WG, Bussmann HB, Stam HJ. Determinants of the sit-to-stand movement: A review. Phys Ther. 2002;82:866–879. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Buchman AS, Boyle PA, Leurgans SE, et al. Cognitive function is associated with the development of mobility impairments in community-dwelling elders. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2011;19:571–580. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181ef7a2e. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Lord S, Galna B, Verghese J, et al. Independent domains of gait in older adults and associated motor and nonmotor attributes: Validation of a factor analysis approach. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2013;68:820–827. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gls255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Salthouse TA. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol Rev. 1996;103:403–428. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.103.3.403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Buchman AS, Boyle PA, Yu L, et al. Total daily physical activity and the risk of AD and cognitive decline in older adults. Neurology. 2012;78:1323–1329. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182535d35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Moe-Nilssen R, Helbostad JL. Estimation of gait cycle characteristics by trunk accelerometry. J Biomech. 2004;37:121–126. doi: 10.1016/s0021-9290(03)00233-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supp Fig S1-S2
Supplementary Material

RESOURCES