Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Apr 21.
Published in final edited form as: Nat Commun. 2011 Dec 20;2:592. doi: 10.1038/ncomms1603

Rapid Adaptation to Food Availability by a Dopamine-Mediated Morphogenetic Response

Diane K Adams 1, Mary A Sewell 2, Robert C Angerer 1, Lynne M Angerer 1
PMCID: PMC3992878  NIHMSID: NIHMS425478  PMID: 22186888

Abstract

Food can act as a powerful stimulus, eliciting metabolic, behavioral and developmental responses. These phenotypic changes can alter ecological and evolutionary processes; yet, the molecular mechanisms underlying many plastic phenotypic responses remain unknown. Here we show that dopamine signaling through a type-D2 receptor mediates developmental plasticity by regulating arm length in pre-feeding sea urchin larvae in response to food availability. While prey-induced traits are often thought to improve food acquisition, the mechanism underlying this plastic response acts to reduce feeding structure size and subsequent feeding rate. Consequently, the developmental program and/or maternal provisioning predetermine the maximum possible feeding rate, and food-induced dopamine signaling reduces food acquisition potential during periods of abundant resources to preserve maternal energetic reserves. Sea urchin larvae may have co-opted the widespread use of food-induced dopamine signaling from behavioral responses to instead alter their development.

Introduction

Predator-prey interactions can produce developmental and morphological responses with significant ecological and evolutionary implications1-5. Predator-induced defenses reduce the prey's risk of consumption, while prey-induced offenses improve consumption or competitive ability in the predators1-3, 6-8. These phenotypic changes can thus affect trophic interactions, predator and prey population dynamics, community dynamics, and can drive speciation1, 2, 6. A mechanistic understanding of these responses is necessary to understand their evolution, limitations, adaptive value, and functional consequences4, 9, 10, but molecular mechanisms remain elusive for prey-induced offenses and all but a few models of predator-induced defenses11, 12.

Echinoid larvae exhibit phenotypic plasticity in feeding structure size after feeding is initiated13, 14 and also in advance of feeding15, providing a tractable developmental model to investigate prey-induced phenotypic plasticity. When food is plentiful, larvae develop smaller feeding arms, whereas when food is scarce, they develop longer feeding arms. Like many other prey-induced responses6, 7, the sea urchin larval response has been described as an offensive response, developing a larger feeding structure when food is scarce in order to improve food acquisition15. Such a response, if adaptive, would allow larvae to adjust to the inherent spatial and temporal variability in their food (algae) supply. It might also allow them to buffer trophic mismatches due to climate change16, 17, as the seasonal peak abundance of some echinoderm larvae in the water column has shifted forward in time16. However, the adaptive value and offensive nature of this prey-induced phenotypic response has not been established. Sea urchin larvae may be able to induce the development of longer arms when food is scarce, the assumed offensive response; alternatively, larvae may only be able to inhibit development from a pre-determined maximum to make shorter arms when food is abundant. The alternative hypotheses result in the same observed phenotypes but through different directional mechanisms with very different organismal (e.g. offensive vs. protective/defensive) and ecological (increasing vs. decreasing grazing/predation pressure) consequences and thus different limitations and implications for selection and evolution.

In order to distinguish whether long arms are induced in low food density or, conversely, whether short arms are induced at high food density, it is necessary to determine the underlying food-response mechanism. In pre-feeding Strongylocentrotus purpuratus larvae, this developmental response occurs over only a few days and requires direct chemosensation of algae15; thus it is likely to occur through a neurosensory mechanism. We report that dopamine is the neuro-signaling pathway by which algal sensation alters development. Dopamine signaling promotes shortening of larval arm length from a developmentally-determined maximum. Thus, unexpectedly, this prey-induced plasticity is not an offensive-response to improve resource acquisition. By experimentally manipulating dopamine signaling and arm length, we assess the costs, benefits, and limitations of this phenotypic response and provide evidence to suggest that sea urchin larva may have co-opted a widely used signaling pathway of food intake for developmental responses.

Results

Algae-Induced Dopamine Signaling Inhibits Arm Elongation

By screening pharmaceuticals targeted to known sea urchin neurotransmitters, we discovered that dopamine signaling is involved in regulating post-oral arm growth in pre-feeding larvae. We used the length of the skeletal element of the post-oral arms as a proxy for the size of the feeding apparatus (Fig. 1a,b), as this skeletal structure supports the ciliary band with which the larvae capture food particles. As in previous studies15, the response to food was manifested as plasticity in post-oral arm length (Fig. 1c) rather than global changes in body size (Fig. 1d). Thus, body rod length was used here as a control for whole-embryo or non-specific responses to perturbations, such as sickness. Pharmacological activation of dopamine type-D2 receptor (DRD2) function with the agonist, quinpirole, inhibited arm elongation, recapitulating the phenotype observed in larvae reared in the presence of abundant food. Post-oral arm length decreased preferentially compared to body rod length with increasing dosage of quinpirole (Fig. 2a) providing evidence that the direction of the response may be to inhibit growth of the feeding arms. Furthermore, the short-arm phenotypic response to food was suppressed by antagonizing DRD2 with amisulpride (Fig. 2b, Supplementary Table S1), suggesting that dopamine functions downstream of algal sensation.

Figure 1. Larvae alter the length of their post-oral arms in response to food.

Figure 1

DIC images of larvae at the end of the pre-feeding period, day 5, cultured without (a) and with (b) algae starting at gastrula stage. The lengths of the post-oral and body rod skeletal elements are indicated by the black lines. Quantification of (c) post-oral arm length, representing a 20.2% (± 3.9 SEM) change, and (d) body rod length, representing a 3.9% (± 1.5 SEM) change for larvae cultured with (green, n = 20) and without (blue, n = 18) algae. Student's two-tailed t-test, *** P < 0.001. Error bars ± SEM. PO, post-oral arm; BR, body rod. Scale bars, 50 μm.

Figure 2. Dopamine signaling through type-D2 receptors mediates inhibition of skeletal growth by algal sensation.

Figure 2

(a) Change in post-oral arm and body rod length for 5-day larvae treated with DRD2 agonist, quinpirole, at the indicated concentrations vs. control. One-way ANOVA for post-oral arm length, F3,463 = 30.586, P < 0.0001. Bonferroni-corrected comparisons vs. control; *** P < 0.001. The decrease in arm length was not due to a global response or sick embryos as body rod length and other aspects of larval morphology did not change with treatment. (b) Change in post-oral arm and body rod length for 5-day larvae cultured with vs. without algae for control (blue) and DRD2 antagonist amisulpride 25 μM (light gray) treatments. ANOVA (Supplementary Table S1), treatment×algae for post-oral arms, F1,363 = 7.005, P = 0.008 and for body rods, F1,345 = 1.584, P = 0.209; Bonferroni-corrected comparisons without vs. with algae (i.e. algae-induced change), *** P < 0.001. Error bars ± SEM. DRD2, dopamine receptor type-D2.

For an independent test of involvement of the dopamine pathway in algal sensation, we disrupted tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) production and blocked TH activity to prevent dopamine biosynthesis (Fig. 3, Supplementary Table S2, S3). TH is required for the conversion of L-tyrosine to L-DOPA, the dopamine precursor, and is the rate-limiting enzyme in catecholamine biosynthesis. Microinjection of a translation-blocking morpholino antisense oligonucleotide (MASO) targeted to TH (TH MASO1), suppressed the post-oral arm response to algae (Fig. 3a). TH MASO1 morphants developed normally (Fig. 3a, Supplementary Fig. S1, Supplementary Table S2), but lacked detectable TH protein (Fig. 3b). A second TH morpholino (Fig. 3c) as well as pharmaceutical inhibition of TH with α-methyl-p-tyrosine (Fig. 3d) also suppressed the short-arm response to food, confirming the TH MASO1 morphant phenotype.

Figure 3. Dopamine biosynthesis is necessary for the response to algae.

Figure 3

(a) Change in post-oral arm and body rod length for 4-day larvae cultured with vs. without algae for control (blue) and a translation-blocking MASO at 0.4 mM (TH MASO1, light gray) treatments. ANOVA (Supplementary Table S2), treatment×algae for post-oral arms, F1,286 = 19.604, P < 0.001 and for body rods, F1,285 = 0.359, P = 0.550. Bonferroni-corrected comparisons without vs. with algae (i.e. algae-induced change), *** P < 0.001. (b) TH protein was not detectible in TH MASO1 injected embryos 4 days post-fertilization (right panel, n = 22/22 TH negative) as compared to controls (white arrow in left panel, n = 3/26 TH negative). Nuclei stained with DAPI (blue). Scale bars, 50 μm. (c) Change in post-oral arm and body rod length for 4-day larvae cultured with vs. without algae for control (blue) and a splice-blocking MASO at 0.15 mM (TH MASO2, light gray) treatments. Student's two-tailed t-test without vs. with algae, *** P < 0.001. (d) Change in post-oral arm and body rod length for 5-day larvae cultured with vs. without algae in control (blue) and TH inhibitor (light gray) treatments. ANOVA (Supplementary Table S3), treatment×algae for post-oral arms, F1,300 = 13.708, P < 0.001 and for body rods, F1,262 = 0.060, P = 0.807. Bonferroni-corrected comparisons without vs. with algae, *** P < 0.001. Error bars ± SEM. TH, tyrosine hydroxylase; MASO, morpholino anti-sense oligonucleotide.

The responses to pharmaceutical and gene knock-down perturbations of DRD2 and TH were specific, affecting the length of the post-oral arms, but not that of body rods, indicating a local response rather than a non-specific, whole-embryo response (Fig. 2, Fig. 3, Supplementary Fig. S1, Supplementary Table S1-S3). These data suggest that dopamine signaling through a dopamine type-D2 receptor is both sufficient to mimic the response to high algal concentrations and necessary to mediate it.

Dopaminergic neurons develop in the tips of the post-oral arms at the appropriate time to mediate the short-arm response to algae. Cells expressing TH were first detected by immunostaining at the prism stage (Fig. 4a,b) at the onset of elongation of the post-oral arms (Fig. 4c, black arrow), and persisted through embryonic skeletal elongation to the start of feeding (Fig. 4d,e). Dopamine, TH protein, and dopa decarboxylase mRNA were each detected in cells near the post-oral arm tips of plutei, in close proximity to the overlying ectoderm and to the primary mesenchyme cells (PMCs) that fabricate the larval skeleton (Fig. 4d-f). TH-positive cells also express the neural marker, Synaptotagmin B18 (Supplementary Fig. S2), and extend processes along the length of the ciliary band (Fig. 4e), which could be involved in algal sensation. These data are consistent with the distribution of dopamine-positive cells in S. droebachiensis larvae19 and a neurosensory function. Higher magnification (40x) of embryos double-stained for dopamine and a PMC marker revealed that dopamine-positive cells were immediately adjacent to the PMCs (Fig. 4g-j). Although plastic phenotypic responses often involve neuro-stimulation of a secondary endocrine signal9, the close proximity of dopamine-positive cells to the PMCs and overlying ectoderm in the post-oral arms raises the possibility that dopamine may signal directly to one or both cell types to inhibit arm growth.

Figure 4. Dopamine and dopamine biosynthesis enzymes are spatially and temporally expressed correctly to mediate the response to algae.

Figure 4

Fluorescent immunohistochemical detection of the rate-limiting dopamine biosynthesis enzyme, TH, and a PMC marker (SM30) for reference, at 54 hours post-fertilization (hpf) (a) and 60 hpf (b). White arrows in these and subsequent panels indicate the post-oral arm tip. (c) Post-oral arm length vs. hpf showing coincident initiation of arm elongation and first detection of TH-positive cells (black arrow). Gray dashed line indicates approximate length of an initial triradiate spicule. Error bars ± SEM. Fluorescent immunohistochemical detection of dopamine (d) and TH (e), and PMC markers, Msp130 (d) and SM30 (e), at pluteus stage (92 hpf). Fluorescent whole-mount in situ hybridization detecting ddc mRNA (f) in the post-oral arm tip and in the serotonergic ganglia (at the top of the image) at early pluteus (72 hpf, oral view). (g-j) Higher-magnification images of dopamine and PMC (Msp130) immunostains at the arm tip. Nuclei stained with DAPI (blue). DA, dopamine; TH, tyrosine hydroxylase; PMC, primary mesenchymal cell; ddc, dopa decarboxylase. Scale bars, (a-f) 50 μm, (g-j) 10 μm.

Gene knock-down (Fig. 3) and pharmaceutical perturbation experiments (Fig. 2, 3) targeting two independent points in the signaling pathway, together with the temporal and spatial expression patterns of dopamine and dopamine biosynthetic enzymes (Fig. 4), identify dopamine as a key signal mediating the phenotypic response to food availability. In the absence of dopamine signaling, plasticity in arm length is lost. Thus the direction of the phenotypic plasticity is clear; short arms are induced at high food density when dopamine signaling inhibits skeletal elongation and thus feeding structure growth.

Consequences for Food Acquisition and Maternal Lipid Stores

Showing the involvement of dopamine signaling established that the response is directed toward reducing the size of feeding arms and also allowed us to artificially manipulate the size of the feeding structure to determine the adaptive capacity. We tested the functional consequences for feeding and energy expenditure by pharmaceutically varying feeding apparatus size. This method was advantageous over varying algal concentrations because sea urchin larvae can incorporate exogenous dissolved organic material20, 21 that could change their metabolic state, even though they are not yet feeding.

Previous studies have shown that the size of the feeding apparatus correlates well with the maximum food clearance rate over developmental time13, 22 and among different species23 of echinoderm larvae. The differences in size are greater between species or development stages (e.g. 4-arm vs. 8-arm echinoplutei) than we have observed here at a single developmental stage in a single species. To determine whether the relatively small changes in feeding structure could significantly affect algal capture rates, we pharmaceutically manipulated feeding structure size and then allowed the larvae to feed on a suspension of algal mimics. Variation in post-oral arm length was significantly correlated with (P < 0.001) and explained approximately twenty percent of the variation in bead ingestion rate at low densities (1,500 beads ml−1; Fig. 5a) and more than twenty-five percent of the variation at higher densities (4,000 beads ml−1; Supplementary Fig. S3). The clearance rate increased 8.7% (± 1.2% S.D.) to 11.2% (± 3.4% S.D.) per 10% increase in post-oral arm length. Thus, differential arm elongation during the pre-feeding stage can alter individual fitness by varying the ciliary feeding capacity, and thus potential food intake rate. These, together with the dopamine perturbation results, lead to the conclusion that food induces a rapid morphogenetic response in pre-feeding sea urchin larvae that reduces food intake potential.

Figure 5. Altering post-oral arm length confers a fitness advantage.

Figure 5

(a) Regression of clearance rate on post-oral arm length at 5 days pf in replicate trials (circles, triangles and diamonds) at 1,500 beads ml−1. Both variables were normalized relative to the observed maxima. Pooled Model I Regression, R2 = 0.197, β1 = 0.866, P < 0.001, n = 203. (b) Regression of the mean percent of maternal triglyceride remaining on mean post-oral arm length, 5 days pf. The percent triglyceride was calculated as the amount of triglyceride on day 5 relative to the initial maternal triglyceride load in fertilized eggs, 0 day. Post-oral arm length was normalized to the maximum. Model II Geometric Mean Regression, R2 = 0.877, β1 = 0.287 ± 0.14 (95% CI), n = 6. Error bars ± SEM.

The existence of a mechanism to suppress arm growth in high food densities despite the feeding advantage of longer arms seems counterintuitive and suggests that there must be a substantial cost to the long armed phenotype of the pre-feeding larva. To test this idea, we next analyzed the expenditure of maternal triglyceride stores over a range of pharmaceutically induced post-oral arm lengths. Triglycerides are the primary energetic lipids used during pre-feeding development to fuel construction of the feeding larva24, 25. The amount of stored triglycerides, as a percentage of the initial maternal triglyceride investment, was significantly negatively correlated with post-oral arm length at the end of the pre-feeding stage, 5 days post-fertilization (pf) (Fig. 5b). The larval culture with the shortest arms at 5 days pf had 62.0% (± 0.7% SEM) of the initial triglycerides remaining compared to 44.6% (± 2.2% SEM) for the larval culture with the longest arms at 5 days pf (Fig. 5b). Thus, embryos could variably conserve ~15% of the maternal triglyceride load when dopamine signaling was strongly stimulated and larval arms were short. Assuming an average usage of approximately 1 ng triglyceride per individual per day (Supplementary Fig. S4), a savings of 15% represents more than one day of energy. This is a substantial cost early in larval life, as triglycerides are depleted by day 10 (Supplementary Fig. S4) and are not replenished by feeding25. Developmental plasticity through dopamine signaling thus allows larvae to preserve a considerable proportion of maternal energetic reserves.

Discussion

The food response of sea urchin larvae, like many prey-induced traits, has been thought to be offensive and act to increase predator food acquisition2, 15. In contrast, our data demonstrate that the molecular mechanism actually acts to reduce the size of the feeding structure when algae are detected. The algae-induced dopamine signaling suppresses the developmental ‘default’ program operating in pre-feeding larvae to produce shorter arms with lower food acquisition potential.

For phenotypic plasticity to be maintained, phenotypic changes should maximize fitness for the given environment. We show that the long-armed phenotype maximizes fitness when food is scarce through increased food acquisition potential at the expense of maternal lipids; whereas the short-armed phenotype maximizes fitness when food is abundant through conserving maternal lipids at the expense of maximum food acquisition potential. There may be additional fitness trade-offs, for example between arm length and stomach size26 and/or swimming performance27 associated with this developmental response.

While the functional tradeoffs are bidirectional, the mechanism that establishes the phenotypes is a unidirectional response. Maximum food acquisition rates are the ‘default’ state constrained by the developmental program and/or maternal inputs. Thus, increasing the size of the feeding apparatus beyond its current maximum, to compensate for potential decreases in food resources due to climate change, would require additional maternal investment and/or evolution of the developmental program. The dopamine-signaling mechanism underlying the developmental plasticity is only induced when food is present, and increases in significance when food is abundant. Thus, natural selection has favored the existence of two programs regulating larval arm growth: a developmental program that optimizes food acquisition potential and another that confers developmental plasticity by adjusting the operation of this program to, in part, optimize use of energy stores. An interpretation of this developmental response mechanism might be that there is a finite or diminishing return for increased ciliary feeding at high food concentrations, possibly due to physiological constraints26. However, benefits of the short-armed phenotype, which is induced by food through dopamine signaling, could be a driving factor whether or not ciliary feeding capacity (arm length) is limited.

The ability to allocate maternal energy stores to other purposes could have substantial consequences for survival and performance success across multiple life-history stages through latent effects28-30. For example, through increased allocation towards development of the juvenile, larvae might be able to decrease the length of time they are exposed to predators in the plankton, or there might be an increase in the quality of the settled juveniles29, 30. Since S. purpuratus has a high post-settlement mortality rate with infrequent successful recruitment events31, 32, condition at settlement could be an important factor controlling the dynamics of benthic sea urchin populations, which have community level consequences33,34. If mismatches between larval abundances and food availability increase in frequency or severity, the reduced algal abundances during the pre-feeding stage would result in an increased occurrence of long-armed larvae and a loss of any benefits of short-armed larvae, such as reallocation of maternal energy stores.

Our data demonstrate the adaptive use of dopamine signaling to rapidly alter development of feeding structures in anticipation of food. Only by gaining a mechanistic understanding could this prey-induced change in form be placed in the appropriate ecological and also evolutionary context. Diverse organisms, from worms to humans, use dopamine signaling35, 36, through dopamine type-D2 receptors in particular 37, 38, to anticipate food consumption. However, in C. elegans35, 38, insects39, 40, adult sea urchins41, and mammals, including humans36, dopamine signaling alters behavior in anticipation of food, whereas pre-feeding sea urchin larvae alter their development and form, as presented here. Thus, the dopamine signaling involved in regulating larval development may represent a co-option or redeployment of the highly conserved use of dopamine signaling in mediating behavior in anticipation of food.

Materials and Methods

Embryo and larval culture

Adult sea urchins, Strongylocentrotus purpuratus, from Cultured Abalone, Goleta, CA and the Point Loma Marine Invertebrate Lab, CalTech, CA were maintained in seawater at 10°C. Gametes were harvested via intracoelomic injections of 0.55 M KCl. Embryos were cultured using standard methods at densities of 1-5 embryos ml−1 in artificial seawater (ASW) at 15°C, with or without algae (~5,000 cells ml−1 of Dunaliella sp.).

Quantification of skeletal lengths

Post-oral arm and body rod lengths (n ≥ 20, each) were assayed from a random sample taken at 5 days pf, just before feeding begins, except for morpholino-injected embryos which were sampled at 4 days pf to ensure that morpholino knockdown of TH protein synthesis was still effective. Larvae were squash mounted on microscope slides to position the skeletal elements in the same plane, then imaged on a Zeiss Axiovert 200M inverted microscope at 20X under differential inference contrast which readily identifies the birefringent skeletal elements. The skeletal lengths were quantified from the digital images using AxioVision software (Carl Zeiss MicroImaging). In some cases (< 1%), a skeletal element could not be measured due to breakage or exit from the plane of focus.

Pharmaceutical perturbations

Dopamine signaling was perturbed using quinpirole, a widely used and specific type-D2 receptor agonist42, amisulpride, a highly selective D2/D3 dopamine receptor antagonist43, and α-Methyl-DL-tyrosine methyl ester hydrochloride, a selective inhibitor of TH44, administered directly via dilution into the culture medium (ASW) to full-sibling larvae at late gastrula stage (48-52 hpf), unless otherwise noted in the text. Pharmaceutical experiments were performed in duplicate with different female-male pairings.

Microinjections

Fertilized eggs were microinjected with filtered solutions containing 20% glycerol and rhodamine-dextran without morpholinos (glycerol control), with 0.4 mM TH MASO1, or with 0.15 mM TH MASO2 using an Eppendorf Femtojet-Injectman NI2 micromanipulator attached to a Leica inverted fluorescent microscope, as in 45. TH MASO1 injections were replicated with two different female-male pairings. Glycerol and uninjected embryos were compared and no significant changes in skeletal lengths, TH immunostaining, or response to algae were detected; thus, some later experiments used uninjected embryos as controls. The translation-blocking TH MASO1 was designed based on the predicted tyrosine hydroxylase (GLEAN 00836) 5’UTR, which was confirmed by RACE. TH MASO1: 5’-GCGTCCTGCTGTAGAAGATACTTGA-3’. TH MASO2 targeted the intron 8/exon 9 boundary to disrupt the catalytic domain of the enzyme46. TH MASO2: 5’- GCCTACGATGAACAAGAACAAATGT-3’.

Statistical Analyses for Dopamine Perturbations

The dose-response of post-oral arm length to treatment with quinpirole was assessed using a one-way ANOVA and subsequent Bonferroni-corrected pair-wise comparisons versus the control to determine if the change relative to control was significant. The response to algae was assessed in pharmaceutical and TH MASO1 perturbations by a three-way ANOVA where perturbation treatment, algal treatment (with vs. without algae), and experimental replicate were fixed effects and the mean square error (MSE) was used as the denominator of the f-ratio47. Since all experiments showed a significant (P > 0.05) interaction between perturbation and algal treatments for post-oral arm length, subsequent Bonferroni-corrected pair-wise comparisons were performed to determine if the algae-induced change was significant. TH MASO2 was not replicated and was thus assessed using Student's two-tailed t-tests. All statistical analyses were done in SYSTAT v10 with output to three decimal places, thus exact P values are given if P > 0.001.

Immunostains and FISH

Immunostains for dopamine (1:1500, ab6428, Abcam), Tyrosine Hydroxylase (1:200, ImmunoStar), Msp130 (1:5, 6a9)48, SM30 (1:400, 3814, courtesy of F.H. Wilt)49, and Synaptotagmin B (1:500, rat, courtesy of R. Burke) were performed as in 50. To determine the time of initial expression of TH, embryos were incubated in the TH antibody at 4°C over two nights to improve sensitivity. Fluorescent whole-mount in situ hybridization for dopa decarboxylase was carried out as in 45, 51, except for inclusion of two more stringent post-hybridization washes in 70% hybridization buffer for 45 min each. Two fragments (470 bp and 758 bp) of the dopa decarboxylase gene isolated with primers: forward1, 5’-CTCCGATGATTTCCGTGTCT-3’; reverse1, 5’-CTGCCAGCAAGGCTACTAGA-3’; forward2, 5’-CCACCGATGACAAAGGTTCT-3’; and reverse2, 5’-CCAGGCATACGTCATGTGTC-3’ were used to generate probes used simultaneously to detect gene expression. Embryos were imaged using a Zeiss Axiovert 200M inverted microscope at 20X or 40X. Optical sections were obtained with an ApoTome unit (Zeiss, Thornwood, NY) and stacked images were prepared using Imaris (Bitplane Inc., St. Paul, MN).

Feeding Experiment

We treated half-sib larvae from three females with artificial seawater (control), DRD2 agonist (quinpirole) and DRD2 antagonist (amisulpride) to maximize the size range of the feeding structures. Larvae from each of these treatments were combined into three replicate cultures, transferred to ASW for four hours, and then allowed to feed on a gently stirred suspension of algal mimics, 10 μm Polybead ® Microspheres (Polysciences, Inc) coated with BSA at 1,500 or 4,000 beads ml−1, for 45 minutes. The number of beads ingested and skeletal lengths were measured for at least 30 larvae from each replicate as above. There was no significant difference between replicate samples fed at 1,500 beads ml−1 (two-factor ANOVA, two-tailed P = 0.950, SYSTAT v.10), so the replicates were pooled and analyzed using a model I regression.

Lipid Analyses

Duplicate cultures, each consisting of half-sibs from three females, were cultured in gently stirred 500 ml beakers within a 15°C incubator. At the onset of arm elongation (~65 hpf), the cultures were divided into three treatments: control, amisulpride 25 μM and quinpirole 50 μM. Replicate samples of 250 individuals each were collected from each treatment on days 0, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 8, and 10 pf and stored frozen at −80 °C until lipid analysis using an Iatroscan Mark 5new TLC/FID (flame ionization detection). Lipid was extracted from frozen embryo and larval samples with ketone as an internal standard24. Immediately before spotting, the lipid extract was dried in a stream of N2 gas and 10 μl of HPLC-grade chloroform was added using a Gilson positive displacement pipette. The rack containing 10 Chromarods was positioned on a warm hot-plate during spotting of 5 μl of the lipid extract onto 8 individual Chromarods to ensure that the sample remained in a narrow band at the origin. The remaining two Chromarods were used to run an unspotted negative control24 and a composite lipid standard to confirm peak identification and prepare standard curves for quantification of the 8 lipid classes24 plus diglyceride. The Chromarods were run in a double development: Hexane:Diethyl Ether:Formic acid (98.5:1:0.05) for two sequential developments of 25 and 20 min followed by 5 min drying at 60°C and a partial scan; 36 minutes in Hexane:Diethyl Ether:Formic acid (79:20:1), drying and a full scan. We present triglyceride concentrations for each treatment and time-point based on lipid extracts obtained from three replicate samples, each containing 250 individuals, as a measurement of maternal energetic stores.

Normalized, mean post-oral arm length and triglyceride levels were assessed using a Model II geometric mean regression52 to take into account the error associated with both variables. Post-oral arm length was normalized to the maximum. Triglyceride concentration was normalized to the initial concentration in fertilized eggs (day 0, pre-cleavage). Treatments within a replicate culture had the same initial triglyceride concentrations, allowing for direct comparisons between treatments. Since there was no interaction with cultures (two-factor ANOVA, two-tailed P = 0.478, SYSTAT v.10), the two cultures were pooled for the regression.

Supplementary Material

1
2

Acknowledgements

We are grateful for discussions with R. Range, A. Sethi, and Z. Wei. Support was provided by the Division of Intramural Research in the National Institute for Dental and Craniofacial Research, National Institutes of Health and the University of Auckland.

Footnotes

Author contributions D.K.A. led the design and execution of the experiments, data analyses, and manuscript preparation. M.A.S. contributed the lipid analysis design and execution. L.M.A. and R.C.A. contributed to the experimental design and data analyses. All authors contributed to manuscript preparation.

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Supporting Information

Supporting Information includes Supplementary Figures S1-S4 and Supplementary Tables S1-S3.

References

  • 1.Agrawal AA. Phenotypic plasticity in the interactions and evolution of species. Science. 2001;294:321–326. doi: 10.1126/science.1060701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Miner BG, Sultan SE, Morgan SG, Padilla DK, Relyea RA. Ecological consequences of phenotypic plasticity. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2005;20:685–692. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2005.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Fordyce JA. The evolutionary consequences of ecological interactions mediated through phenotypic plasticity. J. Exp. Biol. 2006;209:2377–2383. doi: 10.1242/jeb.02271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Beldade P, Mateus ARA, Keller RA. Evolution and molecular mechanisms of adaptive developmental plasticity. Mol. Ecol. 2011 doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2011.05016.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Stearns SC. The evolutionary significance of phenotypic plasticity. Bioscience. 1989;39:436–444. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Mougi A, Kishida O, Iwasa Y. Coevolution of phenotypic plasticity in predator and prey: Why are inducible offenses rarer than inducible defenses? Evolution. 2011;65:1079–1087. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01187.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Smith LD, Palmer AR. Effects of manipulated diet on size and performance of brachyuran crab claws. Science. 1994;264:710–712. doi: 10.1126/science.264.5159.710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Padilla DK. Food and environmental cues trigger an inducible offence. Evol. Ecol. Res. 2002;3:15–25. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Gilbert SF, Epel D. Ecological Developmental Biology: Integrating Epigenetics, Medicine, and Evolution. Sinauer Associates Inc; Sunderland, MA USA: 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Aubin-Horth N, Renn SCP. Genomic reaction norms: using integrative biology to understand molecular mechanisms of phenotypic plasticity. Mol. Ecol. 2009;18:3763–3780. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2009.04313.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Mori T, et al. Genetic basis of phenotypic plasticity for predator-induced morphological defenses in anuran tadpole, Rana pirica, using cDNA subtraction and microarray analysis. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 2005;330:1138–1145. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2005.03.091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Tollrian R, Leese F. Ecological genomics: steps towards unraveling the genetic basis of inducible defenses in Daphnia. BMC Biology. 2010;7:51. doi: 10.1186/1741-7007-8-51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Hart MW, Strathmann RR. Functional consequences of phenotypic plasticity in echinoid larvae. Biol. Bull. (Woods Hole) 1994;186:291–299. doi: 10.2307/1542275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Boidron-Metairon IF. Morphological plasticity in laboratory-reared echinoplutei of Dendraster excentricus (Eschscholtz) and Lytechinus variegatus (Lamarck) in response to food conditions. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 1988;119:31–41. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Miner BG. Larval feeding structure plasticity during pre-feeding stages of echinoids: Not all species respond to the same cues. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 2007;343:158–165. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Edwards M, Richardson AJ. Impact of climate change on marine pelagic phenology and trophic mismatch. Nature. 2004;430:881–884. doi: 10.1038/nature02808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hays GC, Richardson AJ, Robinson C. Climate change and marine plankton. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2005;20:337–344. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2005.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Burke RD, et al. Neuron-specific expression of a synaptotagmin gene in the sea urchin Strongylocentrotus purpuratus. J. Comp. Neurol. 2006;496:244–251. doi: 10.1002/cne.20939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bisgrove BW, Burke RD. Development of the nervous system of the pluteus larva of Strongylocentrotus droebachiensis. Cell Tissue Res. 1987;248:335–343. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Shilling FM. Morphological and physiological responses of echinoderm larvae to nutritive signals. Am. Zool. 1995;35:399–414. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Manahan DT, Davis JP, Stephens GC. Bacteria-free sea urchin larvae: Selective uptake of neutral amino acids from seawater. Science. 1983;220:204–206. doi: 10.1126/science.220.4593.204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Fenaux L, Cellario C, Etienne M. Variations in the ingestion rate of algal cells with morphological development of larvae of Paracentrotus lividus (Echinodermata: Echinoidea). Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 1985;24:161–165. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Strathmann RR. The feeding behavior of planktotrophic echinoderm larvae: Mechanisms, regulation, and rates of suspension-feeding. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 1971;6:109–160. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Sewell MA. Utilization of lipids during early development of the sea urchin Evechinus chloroticus. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 2005;304:133–142. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Meyer E, Green AJ, Moore M, Manahan DT. Food availability and physiological state of sea urchin larvae (Strongylocentrotus purpuratus). Mar. Biol. (Berl.) 2007;152:179–191. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Miner BG. Evolution of feeding structure plasticity in marine invertebrate larvae: a possible trade-off between arm length and stomach size. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 2005;315:117–125. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Clay TW, Grunbaum D. Swimming performance as a constraint on larval morphology in plutei. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 2011;423:185–196. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Padilla DK, Miner BG. Legacies in life histories. Integr. Comp. Biol. 2006;46:217–223. doi: 10.1093/icb/icj029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Pechenik JA. Larval experience and latent effects--metamorphosis is not a new beginning. Integr. Comp. Biol. 2006;46:323–333. doi: 10.1093/icb/icj028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Miller BA, Emlet RB. Development of newly metamorphosed juvenile sea urchins (Strongylocentrotus franciscanus and S. purpuratus): morphology, the effects of temperature and larval food ration, and a method for determining age. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 1999;235:67–90. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Pearse JS, Hines AH. Long-term population dynamics of sea urchins in a central California kelp forest: rare recruitment and rapid decline. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 1987;39:275–283. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Kenner MC. Population dynamics of the sea urchin Strongylocentrotus purpuratus in a Central California kelp forest: recruitment, mortality, growth, and diet. Mar. Biol. (Berl.) 1992;112:107–118. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Watanabe JM, Harrold C. Destructive grazing by sea urchins Strongylocentrotus spp. in a central California kelp forest: potential roles of recruitment, depth, and predation. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 1991;71:125–141. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Pearse JS. Ecological role of purple sea urchins. Science. 2006;314:940–941. doi: 10.1126/science.1131888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Sawin ER, Ranganathan R, Horvitz HR. C. elegans locomotory rate is modulated by the environment through a dopaminergic pathway and by experience through a serotonergic pathway. Neuron. 2000;26:619–631. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81199-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Stice E, Spoor S, Ng J, Zald DH. Relation of obesity to consummatory and anticipatory food reward. Physiol. Behav. 2009;97:551–560. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.03.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Johnson PM, Kenny PJ. Dopamine D2 receptors in addiction-like reward dysfunction and compulsive eating in obese rats. Nat. Neurosci. 2010;13:635–41. doi: 10.1038/nn.2519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Suo S, Culotti JG, Van Tol HHM. Dopamine counteracts octopamine signaling in a neural circuit mediating food response in C. elegans. EMBO (Eur. Mol. Biol. Organ.) J. 2009;28:2437–2448. doi: 10.1038/emboj.2009.194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Wright GA, et al. Parallel reinforcement pathways for conditioned food aversions in the honeybee. Curr. Biol. 2010;20:2234–2240. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.11.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Honjo K, Furukubo-Tokunaga K. Distinctive neuronal networks and biochemical pathways for appetitive and aversive memory in Drosophila larvae. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:852–862. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1315-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Van Alstyne KL, Nelson AV, Vyvyan JR, Cancilla DA. Dopamine functions as an antiherbivore defense in the temperate green alga Ulvaria obscura. Oecologia (Berl.) 2006;148:304–311. doi: 10.1007/s00442-006-0378-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Maggio R, Millan MJ. Dopamine D2-D3 receptor heteromers: pharmacological properties and therapeutic significance. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 2010;10:100–107. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2009.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.McKeage K, Plosker GL. Amisulpride: a review of its use in the management of schizophrenia. CNS Drugs. 2004;18:933–956. doi: 10.2165/00023210-200418130-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Mackay AV. The long-term regulation of tyrosine hydroxylase activity in cultured sympathetic ganglia: role of ganglionic noradrenaline content. Br. J. Pharmacol. 1974;51:509–520. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.1974.tb09669.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Sethi AJ, Angerer RC, Angerer LM. Gene regulatory network interactions in sea urchin endomesoderm induction. PLoS Biol. 2009;7:e1000029. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Goodwill KE, et al. Crystal structure of tyrosine hydroxylase at 2.3 Å and its implications for inherited neurodegenerative diseases. Nat. Struct. Biol. 1997;4:578–585. doi: 10.1038/nsb0797-578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Devore JL. Probability and Statistics for Engineering and the Sciences. Duxbury Pacific Grove. 2000 [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Leaf DS, et al. Antibodies to a fusion protein identify a cDNA clone encoding msp130, a primary mesenchyme-specific cell surface protein of the sea urchin embryo. Dev. Biol. 1987;121:29–40. doi: 10.1016/0012-1606(87)90135-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Killian CE, Wilt FH. Characterization of the proteins comprising the integral matrix of Strongylocentrotus purpuratus embryonic spicules. J. Biol. Chem. 1996;271:9150–9159. doi: 10.1074/jbc.271.15.9150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Yaguchi S, Yaguchi J, Angerer RC, Angerer LM. A Wnt-FoxQ2-nodal pathway links primary and secondary axis specification in sea urchin embryos. Dev. Cell. 2008;14:97–107. doi: 10.1016/j.devcel.2007.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Minowaka T, Rast JP, Arenas-Mena C, Franco CB, Davidson EH. Expression patterns of four different regulatory genes that function during sea urchin development. Gene Expr. Patterns. 2004;4:449–456. doi: 10.1016/j.modgep.2004.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ. Biometry: The Principles and Practices of Statistics in Biological Research. W.H. Freeman; New York: 1994. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1
2

RESOURCES