Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2013 Sep 19;31(1):38–43. doi: 10.1002/da.22174

Mothers' Own Recollections Of Being Parented And Risk Of Offspring Depression 18 Years Later: A Prospective Cohort Study

Liam Mahedy 1,*, Jon Heron 1, Lexine A Stapinski 1, Rebecca M Pearson 1, Jonathan Evans 1, Carol Joinson 1, Lucy Bowes 1, Glyn Lewis 1
PMCID: PMC3992906  PMID: 24105778

Abstract

Background

Although the relationship between maternal bonding and risk of offspring depression has been demonstrated, it is unclear whether this risk exists for subsequent generations. This study examines the association between maternal reports of her own mother's parenting and later risk of depression in offspring at age 18.

Method

This study is based on data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Mothers enrolled in the study, completed the Parental Bonding Instrument to provide an assessment of how they were parented by their own mothers up to the age of 16. Offspring depression was assessed at age of 18 using the Clinical Interview Schedule-Revised. The sample comprised 10,405 respondents who had completed the Parental Bonding Instrument during the antenatal period. Results were adjusted for grandmother's history of depression, maternal depression, and a range of socioeconomic variables.

Results

A one standard deviation increase in mothers’ perceived lack of care in their own childhood was associated with a 16% increase in the odds of offspring depression at age 18 (odds ratios = 1.16, 95% confidence intervals = [1.04, 1.30]). This effect remained following adjustment for potential confounders (odds ratios = 1.14, 95% confidence intervals = [1.02, 1.27]). There was no evidence for an association between overprotection and offspring depression.

Conclusions

This study is consistent with the hypothesis that sensitive caregiving is important to future risk of depression across generations. Preventative interventions could be aimed at promoting positive parenting practices, which may help to reduce the risk of depression in subsequent generations.

Keywords: adolescence, ALSPAC, depression, maternal bonding

INTRODUCTION

Depression is among the most prevalent of psychiatric disorders in adolescents. Since mental health disorders commonly begin in childhood and adolescence,1 this is an important period for understanding the developmental origins of these disorders. By 2030, depression will rank first in high-income countries among disorders contributing to global disease burden.2 Given the social3 and economic4 costs of depression, it is becoming increasingly important to identify and establish strategies that might prevent the onset of depression.

One area of research that has gained considerable attention is the role of the parent–child relationship as it is thought to provide the platform for socioemotional development and regulation in the child.5,6 Focusing on parenting practices, the Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI)7 is the measure of choice for many researchers as it is a well-validated measure8 and is quick and easy to administer.9 There is consistent evidence that parenting behaviors, as measured by the PBI, are related to offspring depression.10 Similarly, research using animal models has demonstrated that lack of early maternal care influences vulnerability for animal models of stress.11

Although the relationship between parenting and risk of offspring depression has been established, it is still not known whether the risk for depression may persist into subsequent generations. Intergenerational transmission of parenting practices has been well established throughout the literature with previous research focusing on the transmission of negative/harsh parenting12 and warm/sensitive parenting across generations.13,14 Consequently, mothers who experience a lack of care in their own childhood may show a lack of care to their own children, reinforcing a cycle of increased risk of depression from generation to generation.

Research investigating the influence of parenting has been hampered by several methodological problems including method variance, that is, reports of parenting and offspring's depressive symptoms are both derived from a single reporter,15 reliance on small sample sizes,16 cross-sectional design,15 and relatively short follow-up periods of prospective studies.17,18 A strength of the current study is the availability of data on a range of confounding variables assessed during the antenatal period (e.g., maternal age, maternal concurrent depression, and grandmother's history of depression) that have been shown to be associated with maternal recollections of being parented19 and offspring depression.20

The current study, based on a large UK cohort, examines the effect of mother's own recollections of being parented (assessed in the antenatal period) and later risk of offspring self-reported depression (assessed in the offspring at age 18). Focusing specifically on the relationship between mothers’ recollections of being parented and risk of offspring depression at age 18, addresses a number of these methodological limitations. There is no research in large prospective cohorts examining whether a mother's own experiences of being parented by her own mother impacts upon risk of depression in her offspring.

METHOD

SAMPLE

Data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) consist of 15,247 pregnant mothers residing in the former Avon Health Authority in the southwest of England, having an estimated date of delivery between April 1, 1991 and December 31, 1992. The core sample for these analyses was limited to singletons who survived to 1 year (n = 13,617). Please note that the study website contains details of all data that is available through a fully searchable data dictionary.21 Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees. For further details on the cohort profile, see.22

From the sample of 13,617 respondents, 1,169 (8.5%) respondents did not return the questionnaire, while an additional number of 622 (5%) did not complete any of the PBI items. A further 1,421 (10%) respondents were omitted from the sample as they completed the PBI during the postnatal stage. Our core working sample is defined by the 10,405 respondents who had completed at least one item of the PBI during the antenatal period. Of these, 9,223 (88.7%) respondents had complete information on the PBI.

MEASURES

Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI)

The PBI7 is a 25-item self-rating instrument asking individuals, over the age of 16, about their own experiences of being parented up to the age of 16. In our study, the PBI was completed by mothers throughout the antenatal period. Previously in the literature, care and protection/control were the two original parenting dimensions.7 More specifically, 12 care items are comprised warmth, empathy, and affection while 13 protection/control items comprised overprotection, control, and prevention of independence versus autonomy. Although there has been general consensus about the care factor, there is less certainty about overprotection. Previous studies suggest that overprotection could be divided into two factors to incorporate protectiveness and authoritarianism.23

The version used in the ALSPAC study was adapted by24 and contained a number of further adaptations. During the piloting stage, three items were omitted (Did not want me to grow up, made me feel I wasn't wanted, and tried to make me feel dependent on her) from the original version as they duplicated existing items. Thirteen items were given responses as never, sometimes, or usually, while the remaining nine items were given dichotomous yes/no response options. Due to many small cell counts in the uncollapsed data, all items were dichotomized in order to help with estimation.

Adolescent Depression

Offspring adolescent depression data from 4,566 adolescents who completed the Clinical Interview Schedule-Revised (CIS-R)25 at age 18 were examined. The CIS-R is a self-administered computerized interview, which derives a diagnosis of depression for algorithms based on ICD-10 criteria for depression. A binary yes/no variable indicating a primary diagnosis of major depression was the outcome measure for this study.

Confounding Variables

Adjustment was made for potential confounding variables recorded by self-report postal questionnaires at 32 weeks gestation. These included maternal age at birth; maternal concurrent depression using the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS),26 maternal education (<O level, O level, ≥A level); maternal social class (ranked from high to low at five intervals); and grandmother's history of depression (yes/no) measured at 12 weeks gestation. Figure 1 (online supplement) shows a timeline of variables used in the analysis, focusing on both maternal and offspring self-reports.

STATISTICAL ANALYSIS

Statistical analysis was conducted using Mplus v7.27 Initially, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was used to establish the factor structure of the PBI. CFA was utilized using the WLSMV (mean and variance adjusted weighted least squares) estimator,28 as this estimator performs best in the CFA modeling of categorical data.29 In assessing the fit of the CFA model, four fit statistics were used (1) chi-square statistic (χ2); (2) root-mean-square error of approximations (RMSEA);30 (3) comparative fit index (CFI)31; and (4) Tucker–Lewis fit index (TLI).32 Goodness of fit was determined in accordance with33 and was indicated by a χ2 < 0.05, CFI and TLI values of over 0.95, and RMSEA of less than 0.06. Multiple indices were used as they provide a more comprehensive evaluation of model fit. Logistic regression was used to examine the relationship between the PBI and offspring adolescent depression. Odds ratios (OR) and their 95% confidence intervals (CI) were calculated using logistic regression.

Missing Data

The outcome variable at 18 years (n = 4,566) comprised just under half of the core working sample (n = 10,405). Since using complete case analysis and not taking missing data into account can result in biased estimates,34 an approach was taken to impute missing data using multivariate imputation by chained equations via the ice method35 in Stata 12. This method is based on the assumption that data are missing at random conditional on the observed covariates and outcomes. A number of auxiliary variables predictive of incomplete variables and/or missingness were included in the model; these included sociodemographic variables and reports of depression at additional time points. To ensure an accurate representation of the data, 100 imputed datasets were derived, each entailing 20 cycles of regression switching.

Sensitivity analyses examined the impact of various sources of missing data (online Supporting Information Table 1). The first model comprised the complete cases sample (n = 3,171). Imputation was then carried out on three further models, Model 1: n = 4,566 (information available for outcome measure, but an incomplete set of exposure and confounding variables); Model 2: n = 9,223 (complete information on exposure, and incomplete data on confounding and outcome variables); and Model 3: n = 10,405 (information on exposure measure, but incomplete data on outcome and/or confounding variables).

RESULTS

CFA (Table 1) was conducted using the core working sample of 10,405 respondents who completed the PBI during the antenatal period, mean age = 28.3, standard deviation = 4.8. For our purposes, two items were omitted from the analysis. Item 15: babied by mother was omitted due to a low standardized factor loading of 0.28 on the overprotection factor while Item 20: allowed by mother to go out anytime was omitted as it was highly correlated with Item 21: was overprotective of me.

Table 1.

Confirmatory factor analysis of the PBI (standardized coefficients)

Item Yes (%) Lack of care
1. Spoken to warmly by mother 79.1 0.886
2. Helped as needed by mother 80.6 0.908
4. Apparent coldness from mothera 4.9 0.764
5. Problems seem understood by mother 51.0 0.835
6. Affection by mother 70.4 0.870
10. Felt unwanted by mothera 3.8 0.634
11. Things talked over by mother 55.3 0.829
13. Praised by mother 52.4 0.812
14. Mother enjoyed talking things over 80.7 0.894
15. Frequently smiled at by mother 89.6 0.893
17. Needs understood by mother 78.3 0.921
18. Upsets comforted by mother 86.4 0.886
Yes (%) Overprotection
3. Allowed by mother to do as likea 42.0 0.809
7. Control attempted by mother 25.4 0.594
8. Privacy invaded by mother 7.9 1.036
9. Allowed own decisions by mothera 45.3 0.700
12. Allowed freedom by mothera 36.3 0.752
16. Babied by mother —— ——
19. Felt helpless without mother 15.6 0.542
20. Allowed by mother to go out anytime —— ——
21. Overprotective mother 20.6 0.473
22. Allowed by mother to dress as likeda 42.4 0.445
a

Items were reversed scored.

Table 1 reports the standardized factor loadings of the PBI. The correlation between the two factors was .62. The results from a two factor model indicate factor loadings ranging from .63 to .92 for the care factor and from 0.45 to 1.04 for the overprotection factor. A two-factor model provided an adequate description of the data (χ2 = 7308.819, df 207, P = .001; RMSEA = 0.061 (90% CI = [0.060, 0.062]), CFI = 0.957; and TLI = 0.951).

Overall, 4,566 adolescents completed the CIS-R at 18 years of age. Fifty-six percent of the sample who completed the CIS-R at 18 years were female. A primary diagnosis of ICD-10 depression was given for 360 (8%) of offspring respondents. Of these 270 (75%) of the sample who were classified as having a diagnosis of depression at 18 years were female.

From the sample who completed the CIS-R (n = 4,566), 3,521 (77%) respondents also had available data on all items of the PBI. Demographics for the complete cases sample compared to the rest of the core sample are provided in Table 2. Respondents from the complete cases sample were older, better educated, had lower incidence of maternal depression, and were of higher social class.

Table 2.

Demographic variables for the sample with and without outcome data at 18 years

CIS-R 18 years n = 3,171 CIS-R missing n = 6,884 χ2 (df) P-value
Maternal education
 <O level 463 (14.6%) 1,737 (25.2%) 482.30 (3) .000
 O level 1,087 (34.3%) 2,298 (33.4%)
 ≥A level 1,490 (47.0%) 1,966 (28.6%)
 Missing 131 (4.1%) 883 (12.8%)
Maternal depression
 Yes 375 (11.9%) 1,015 (14.7%)
 No 2,750 (86.7%) 5,583 (81.1%) 125.52 (2) .000
 Missing 46 (1.4%) 286 (4.2%)
Grandmother's depression
 Yes 604 (19.0%) 1,410 (20.5%)
 No 2,567 (81.0%) 5,474 (79.5%) 2.28 (1) .131
 Missing 0 0
Maternal social class
 High 1 230 (7.3%) 240 (3.5%) 387.49 (6) .000
 2 1,039 (32.8%) 1,507 (21.9%)
 3 1,310 (41.3%) 2,783 (40.4%)
 4 214 (6.8%) 542 (7.9%)
 Low 5 32 (1.0%) 125 (1.8%)
 Missing 346 (10.9%) 1,687 (24.5%)
Mean (S.D.) Mean (S.D.) t (df) P-value
Maternal age 29.31 (4.50) 27.77 (4.89) 15.50 .000

There was strong evidence of an association (P = .007) between maternal lack of care and offspring depression at age of 18 (Table 3). For a one standard deviation increase of reporting maternal lack of care in childhood, there was a 1.16 (95% CI = [1.04, 1.30]) increase in the odds of offspring depression at age of 18. This relationship was not substantially affected after adjusting for a range of confounding variables (OR = 1.14; 95% CI = [1.02, 1.26]). There was no evidence to suggest that recollection of overprotection during childhood was associated with risk of offspring depression 0.99 (95% CI = [0.88, 1.11]). This relationship was not substantially affected after adjusting for a range of confounding variables (OR = 1.01, 95% CI = [0.90, 1.11]). Results from pre- and postimputation models were consistent across all analyses of varying sample sizes (online Supporting Information Table 2).

Table 3.

Standardized regression coefficients for offspring depression and maternal bonding

Unadjusted model OR (95% CI) Adjusted model OR (95% CI) P-value
Lack of care 1.16 (1.04, 1.30) 1.14 (1.02, 1.26) .007
Overprotection 0.99 (0.88, 1.11) 1.01 (0.90, 1.11) .624

Model adjusted for maternal age, maternal social class, maternal education, maternal depression, and grandmother's history of depression.

DISCUSSION

Our findings demonstrated an association between maternal experiences of being parented and risk of offspring depression at 18 years of age. Offspring of mothers who reported they received a lack of care from their own mothers in childhood had a 14% increase in odds of experiencing depression at age 18. This relationship persisted after controlling for other variables that have been shown to be related to maternal reports of being parented and risk for adolescent depression, for example, maternal age, depression, education, and social class. In comparison, there was no evidence that the odds of offspring depression were increased in 18-year-old offspring of mothers who endorsed items pertaining to overprotection in the PBI.

STRENGTHS AND LIMITATIONS

The present study has a number of strengths, including data from a long follow-up period between the mother's antenatal assessment of being parented and depression in offspring at age of 18. Having self-reports of maternal recollections of being parented and depression in the offspring addresses the issue of method variance. This study is further strengthened by the large sample size of over 10,000 respondents who completed the PBI during the antenatal period. Furthermore, since maternal-reported parenting was assessed prior to the infant being born, it eliminates the potential for infant temperament biasing the PBI rating.

Given the longitudinal nature of this study, we can exclude the possibility of reverse causality, as adolescent depression cannot influence the manner in which mothers bond with their own mothers in childhood. Although the strength of the relationship is modest, it does demonstrate an association over an 18-year period. Furthermore, the inclusion of a range of possible confounding variables was shown not to alter the strength of the relationship between exposure and outcome variables.

A number of limitations are also noted. First, a limitation common to most longitudinal studies is sample attrition. Adolescents who attended the clinic at age of 18 were more likely to come from families that had lower incidence of maternal depression, were better educated, and of higher social class. To address biases that may be present due to response attrition, a number of sensitivity analyses using imputed data were conducted. The findings were consistent across analyses suggesting a robust pattern of results.

Although retrospective questionnaires have been widely used throughout the literature, criticism surrounds the use of self-report methods. For example, one concern is that depression can bias memory toward negative events.36 Despite these criticisms, two recent studies have examined the stability of the PBI over a 20-year period. For example, one study found that recollections of one's parental environment is not substantially influenced by gender, depression history, and life experience and that subject's perceptions do not shift with fluctuations in depressed mood or neuroticism level.37 More recently,19 it has been demonstrated that although the PBI is sensitive to sample characteristics, time and mood fluctuations, this sensitivity does not appear to significantly bias the long term stability of the instrument.

A further limitation relates to the sole focus on maternal bonding, while the role of fathers was not examined. That being said, the majority of previous research has demonstrated a greater association with psychopathology for maternal bonding as compared to paternal bonding.15 Traditionally, mothers are typically the primary emotional caregiver and their parenting behavior may be more influential than fathers.38

IMPLICATIONS

Expanding on previous research that found the early caregiving environment to be a risk factor for mental health problems,15,18 our findings highlight the intergenerational consequences of suboptimal caregiving. In doing so, they extend the findings from previous studies15,18 who found that maternal lack of care was a risk factor for adult psychopathology, by demonstrating that experiencing a lack of maternal care in childhood places subsequent generations at risk of depression.

Although this study does not suggest that parenting practices in one generation are repeated in the next, our findings reveal that a grandmother's parenting behavior influences their grandchild's risk of depression in adolescence. Although maternal parenting practices in relation to risk of offspring depression were not examined in this study, it is possible that mothers who experience a lack of care in their own childhood show a lack of care to their own children. The specific mechanisms underlying this intergenerational transmission may be clarified in future research.

Although we cannot completely rule out the role of shared genetic vulnerability, the findings are shown to be independent of grandmother and maternal concurrent depression that were included as possible confounding variables. Research using animal models further demonstrates that lack of early maternal care acts as a risk factor in the regulation of emotional and cognitive responses to stress,39 which may influence vulnerability for mood disorders.11 Francis et al.40 further demonstrated using a cross-fostering study (young rat pups adopted by a caring mother from a neglecting mother and vice versa) that this effect was independent of genetic influences, as infants’ tolerance to stress was consistent with the caring behavior of their adopted rather than biological mother. These findings indicate that parenting styles may influence the development of emotion regulation capacities and the risk of poor regulatory capacities may persist across subsequent generations.

CONCLUSION

The findings add to the understanding of the relationship between the long-term effects of the early caregiving environment and adolescent depression and may have implications for policy making and parenting practices.41 For example, women who have been identified as experiencing a lack of care in their own childhood are at greater risk of having offspring who develop depression during adolescence. In this regard, potential mothers can be identified at an early age and targeted for early intervention. Given the intergenerational nature of these findings, it is suggested that the promotion of positive parenting programs could reduce the risk of depression across generations in the long term.

Acknowledgments

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists, and nurses. The Wellcome Trust (Grant ref: 084268/Z/07/Z) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. The publication is the work of the authors Liam Mahedy, Jon Heron, Lexine A. Stapinski, Rebecca M. Pearson, Carol Joinson, Jonathan Evans, Lucy Bowes, & Glyn Lewis and will serve as guarantors for the contents of this paper. This work was carried out in the Centre for Mental Health, Addiction and Suicide Research, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Oakfield House, Oakfield Road, UK.

Conflict of interest

The authors have declared no conflict of interest.

da0031-0038-sd1.doc (99.5KB, doc)

References

  • 1.Kessler RC, Amminger GP, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, et al. Age of onset of mental disorders: a review of recent literature. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2007;20:359–364. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e32816ebc8c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Mathers CD, Loncar D. Projections of global mortality and burden of disease from 2002 to 2030. PLoS Med. 2006;11:e442. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0030442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Fredman L, Weissman MM, Leaf PJ, Bruce ML. Social functioning in community residents with depression and other psychiatric disorder: results of the Newhaven Epidemiologic Catchment Area Study. J Affect Disord. 1998;15:103–112. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(88)90077-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Chisholm D. The Economic Consequences of Depression. In: Dawson A, Tylee A, editors. Depression: Social and Economic Timebomb. London: BMJ Books; 2001. p. 122. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Calkins SD, Hill A. Caregiver Influences on Emerging Emotion Regulation: Biological and Environmental Transactions in Early Development. In: Gross JJ, editor. The Handbook of Emotion Regulation. New York: Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 229–248. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Thompson RA, Meyer S. Socialization of Emotion Regulation in the Family. In: Gross JJ, editor. The Handbook of Emotion Regulation. New York: Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 249–268. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Parker G, Tupling H, Brown LB. A parental bonding instrument. Br J Med Psychol. 1979;52:1–10. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Wilhelm K, Parker G. Reliability of the parental bonding instrument and intimate bond measure scales. Aust NZ J Psychiatry. 1990;24:199–202. doi: 10.3109/00048679009077683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Murphy E, Brewin CR, Silka L. The assessment of parenting using the Parental Bonding Instrument: two or three factors? Psychol Med. 1997;27:333–342. doi: 10.1017/s0033291796004606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Patton GC, Coffery C, Posterino M, et al. Parental ‘affectionless control’ in adolescent depressive disorders. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2001;36:475–480. doi: 10.1007/s001270170011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Champagne DL, Bagot RC, van Hasselt F, et al. Maternal care and hippocampal plasticity: evidence for experience-dependent structural plasticity, altered synaptic functioning, and differential responsiveness to glucocorticoids and stress. J Neurosci. 2008;28:6037–6045. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0526-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Capaldi DM, Pears KC, Patterson GR, Owen LD. Continuity of parenting practices across generations in an at-risk sample: a prospective comparison of direct and mediated associations. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2003;31:127–142. doi: 10.1023/a:1022518123387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Belsky J, Jaffee S, Sligo J, et al. Intergenerational transmission of warm-sensitive-stimulating parenting: a prospective study of mothers and fathers of 3-year olds. Child Dev. 2005;76:384–396. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00852.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Chen Z, Kaplan HB. Intergenerational transmission of constructive parenting. J Marriage Fam. 2001;63:17–31. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Enns MW, Cox BJ, Clara I. Parental bonding and adult psychopathology: results from the US National Comorbidity Survey. Psychol Med. 2002;32:997–1008. doi: 10.1017/s0033291702005937. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Modestin J, Marrer K, Agarwalla P. Parenting and psychopathology in sibling pairs. Eur J Psychiatry. 2008;22:99–105. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Branje SJT, Hall WW, III, Frijns T, Meeus WHJ. Longitudinal associations between perceived parent-child relationship quality and depressive symptoms in adolescence. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2010;38:751–763. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9401-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Overbeek GJ, Have Mten, Vollebergh WAM, Graaf R., de Parental lack of care and overprotection: longitudinal associations with DSMIIIR disorders. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2007;42:87–93. doi: 10.1007/s00127-006-0115-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Murphy EJ, Wickramaratne P, Weissman MM. The stability of parental bonding reports: a 20-year follow-up. J Affect Disord. 2010;125:307–315. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.01.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Davalos DB, Yandon CA, Tregellas HC. Untreated prenatal maternal depression and the potential risks to offspring: a review. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2012;15:1–14. doi: 10.1007/s00737-011-0251-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, Data Dictionary. Available at: http://www.bris.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/data-access/data-dictionary/ (accessed January 08, 2013)
  • 22.Fraser A, Macdonald-Wallis C, Tilling K, et al. Cohort Profile: The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42:97–110. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Cox BJ, Enns MW, Clara IP. The parental bonding instrument: confirmatory evidence for a three-factor model in a psychiatric clinical sample and in the national comorbidity survey. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2000;35:353–357. doi: 10.1007/s001270050250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Gamsa A. A note on a modification of the Parental Bonding Instrument. Br J Med Psychol. 1987;60:291–294. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1987.tb02745.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Lewis G, Pelosi AJ, Araya R, Dunn G. Measuring psychiatric disorder in the community: the development of a standardized assessment for use by lay interviewers. Psychol Med. 1992;22:465–486. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700030415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R. Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;150:782–786. doi: 10.1192/bjp.150.6.782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User's Guide. 7th ed. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; pp. 1998–2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Muthén BO, du Toit SHC, Spisic D. Robust inference using weighted least squares and quadratic estimating equations in latent variable modeling with categorical and continuous outcomes. Los Angeles, CA: University of California; 1997. Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Brown TA. Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Steiger JH. Causal Modeling: A Supplementary Module for SYSTAT and SYGRAPH. Evanston, IL: SYSTAT; 1989. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol Bull. 1990;107:238–246. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Tucker LR, Lewis C. A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika. 1973;38:1–10. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling. 1999;6:1–55. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Sterne JAC, White IR, Carlin JB, et al. Multiple imputation for missing data in epidemiological and clinical research: potential and pitfalls. Br Med J. 2009;338:157–160. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b2393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Royston P. Multiple imputation of missing values: further update of ice, with an emphasis on categorical variables. Stata J. 2009;9:466–477. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Beck CT. Revision of the postpartum depression predictors inventory. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs. 2002;31:394–402. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6909.2002.tb00061.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Wilhelm K, Niven H, Parker G, Hadzi-Pavlovic D. The stability of the Parental Bonding Instrument over a 20-year period. Psychol Med. 2005;35:387–393. doi: 10.1017/s0033291704003538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Cinnamon RG, Rich Y. Gender differences in the importance of work and family roles. Sex Roles. 2002;47:531–41. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Meaney MJ. Maternal care, gene expression, and the transmission of individual differences in stress reactivity across generations. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:116–1192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.1161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Francis DD, Champagne FA, Liu D, Meaney MJ. Maternal care, gene expression, and the development of individual differences in stress reactivity. Ann NY Acad Sci. 1999;896:66–84. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08106.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Morgan Z, Brugha TS, Fryers T, Stewart-Brown SL. The effects of parent-child relationships on later life mental health status in two national birth cohorts. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2012;47:1707–1715. doi: 10.1007/s00127-012-0481-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

da0031-0038-sd1.doc (99.5KB, doc)

Articles from Depression and Anxiety are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES