Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2014 Apr 24;3:e01936. doi: 10.7554/eLife.01936

Midbrain dopamine neurons sustain inhibitory transmission using plasma membrane uptake of GABA, not synthesis

Nicolas X Tritsch 1, Won-Jong Oh 2, Chenghua Gu 2, Bernardo L Sabatini 1,*
Editor: Gary L Westbrook3
PMCID: PMC4001323  PMID: 24843012

Abstract

Synaptic transmission between midbrain dopamine neurons and target neurons in the striatum is essential for the selection and reinforcement of movements. Recent evidence indicates that nigrostriatal dopamine neurons inhibit striatal projection neurons by releasing a neurotransmitter that activates GABAA receptors. Here, we demonstrate that this phenomenon extends to mesolimbic afferents, and confirm that the released neurotransmitter is GABA. However, the GABA synthetic enzymes GAD65 and GAD67 are not detected in midbrain dopamine neurons. Instead, these cells express the membrane GABA transporters mGAT1 (Slc6a1) and mGAT4 (Slc6a11) and inhibition of these transporters prevents GABA co-release. These findings therefore indicate that GABA co-release is a general feature of midbrain dopaminergic neurons that relies on GABA uptake from the extracellular milieu as opposed to de novo synthesis. This atypical mechanism may confer dopaminergic neurons the flexibility to differentially control GABAergic transmission in a target-dependent manner across their extensive axonal arbors.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.01936.001

Research organism: mouse

eLife digest

The electrical signals that are fired along neurons cannot be transmitted across the small gaps, called synapses that are found between neurons. Instead, the neuron sending the signal releases chemicals called neurotransmitters into the synapse. These neurotransmitters bind to receptor proteins on the surface of the second neuron and control how it fires.

A neurotransmitter called dopamine plays a key role in the circuits of the brain that control how we learn certain tasks involving movement. In particular, two populations of neurons from the midbrain that release dopamine target the striatum, an area of the brain that is responsible for motor control. These neurons also release other neurotransmitters, but the identity of these other chemicals is not known, and the details of the interaction between the neurons and the striatum are poorly understood.

Previous research showed that some of the midbrain neurons activate receptors that normally respond to a neurotransmitter called gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA). However, several different chemicals can trigger this receptor. Using a range of techniques, Tritsch et al. now confirm that dopamine neurons release GABA alongside dopamine, and that this applies to both sets of the dopamine-producing neurons that feed into the striatum.

Some neurons can manufacture GABA from amino acids found in their internal fluid. However, Tritsch et al. could not detect the enzymes needed for this reaction in dopamine-producing neurons. Instead, these neurons contain proteins that can transport GABA across the cell membrane, which suggests that the neurons collect GABA from the extracellular fluid that surrounds them.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.01936.002

Introduction

Dopamine (DA)-releasing neurons in the mammalian midbrain play an important role in fundamental behaviors, including motivation, reinforcement learning and motor control, and their dysfunction is associated with a wide range of neuropsychiatric disorders (Wise, 2004; Costa, 2007; Schultz, 2007; Morris et al., 2009; Redgrave et al., 2010). The major target of midbrain DA neurons is the striatum, a large subcortical structure implicated in the selection and reinforcement of motor actions. DA neurons located in the substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc) project mainly to the dorsal striatum (also known as the caudate and putamen, CPu), whereas those in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) innervate the ventral striatum (or nucleus accumbens, NAc), forming the nigrostriatal and mesolimbic pathways, respectively. The striatum controls motor behavior through two parallel output streams with opposing effects; the so-called ‘direct’ and ‘indirect’ pathways. Each pathway arises from distinct groups of GABAergic striatal projection neurons (SPNs) that differ, amongst other things, in their response to DA (Gerfen and Surmeier, 2011; Tritsch and Sabatini, 2012). By providing DA to the striatum, SNc and VTA neurons are believed to play a pivotal role in balancing the activity of direct- and indirect-pathway SPNs. However, our understanding of the cellular and molecular mechanisms employed by DA neurons to modulate striatal function remains incomplete.

Although the activation of metabotropic receptors following the release of DA from SNc/VTA neurons is undoubtedly central to their function, DA neurons also co-release several other transmitters that shape striatal output (Hnasko et al., 2010; Stuber et al., 2010; Tecuapetla et al., 2010; Tritsch et al., 2012). In particular, we recently showed that DA neurons in SNc potently inhibit action potential firing in SPNs by releasing a transmitter that activates GABAA receptors (Tritsch et al., 2012). Release of this neurotransmitter requires activity of the vesicular monoamine transporter Slc18a2 (VMAT2), which can be replaced by exogenous expression of the vesicular GABA transporter Slc32a1 (also known as VGAT or VIAAT). In addition, VMAT2 expression in SPNs lacking VGAT is sufficient to sustain GABAergic transmission. Collectively, these findings suggest that SNc neurons co-release GABA using VMAT2 for vesicular loading. However, this study raises several important questions. First, does GABAergic transmission generalize to DA neurons in the VTA, or is it limited to nigrostriatal afferents? Second, what is the identity of the transmitter released by DA neurons? GABA exists as a zwitterion at neutral pH and lacks the characteristic molecular structure of VMAT2 substrates, which typically feature an aromatic ring and a positive charge (Yelin and Schuldiner, 1995). GABAA receptors can be activated by several naturally-occurring agonists and allosteric modulators, including β-alanine, taurine, imidazole-4-acetic acid and neurosteroids (Johnston, 1996; Belelli and Lambert, 2005). Although none of these molecules constitute ideal candidates for VMAT2-dependent vesicular transport based on their structure, charge, or both, they nonetheless raise the possibility that DA neurons liberate a transmitter other than GABA to inhibit SPNs. Third, do all DA neurons contribute to GABAergic signaling, or is it reserved to a subpopulation of cells, similar to glutamate co-release from DA neurons (Hnasko and Edwards, 2011). Finally, how is inhibitory synaptic transmission sustained in DA neurons? Addressing this point will help identify molecules required for GABAergic transmission by DA neurons and will permit the development of genetic tools to determine the relative contribution of SPN inhibition by DA neurons in vivo under normal and pathological conditions.

In this study, we address these questions by examining the cellular and molecular mechanisms that underlie the rapid GABAA receptor-mediated inhibition of SPNs upon stimulation of DA axons. Our results provide strong evidence that the inhibitory transmitter released by midbrain DA neurons is GABA, and suggest that GABA co-release is a general feature of all midbrain DA neurons. Moreover, we reveal that DA neurons rely on GABA uptake through the plasma membrane—but not on de novo GABA synthesis—to sustain GABAergic transmission.

Results

Stimulation of mesolimbic axons evokes inhibitory currents in striatal projection neurons

We recently reported that activation of SNc axons in dorsal striatum evokes monosynaptic, GABAA receptor-mediated inhibitory postsynaptic currents (IPSCs) in SPNs (Tritsch et al., 2012; Figure 1—figure supplement 1). To determine whether this GABAergic signaling extends to DA neurons projecting to ventral striatum, we expressed channelrhodopsin 2 (ChR2) in the VTA using one of two approaches (‘Materials and methods’). We either injected Slc6a3-ires-Cre mice (Backman et al., 2006) stereotaxically with an adeno-associated virus expressing Cre recombinase-dependent ChR2 (Figure 1A) or drove ChR2 expression genetically by crossing Slc6a3-ires-Cre mice to transgenic mice (Madisen et al., 2012) containing a conditional allele of ChR2 in the Rosa26 locus (Figure 1D). Mice also harbored Drd2-Egfp or Drd1a-tdTomato bacterial artificial chromosome (BAC) transgenes to permit distinction between direct- and indirect-pathway SPNs, respectively (Gong et al., 2003; Ade et al., 2011). We performed whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings from SPNs in sagittal brain slices of NAc in the presence of inhibitors of ionotropic glutamate receptors (NBQX and R-CPP) and metabotropic GABAB receptors (CGP55845) to prevent excitatory synaptic transmission by dopaminergic axons, as well as modulatory effects of GABAB receptors, respectively. Under our recording conditions, optogenetic stimulation of VTA axons with brief (1 ms) flashes of blue light reliably evoked inward IPSCs in direct- and indirect-pathway SPNs that were blocked by the GABAA receptor antagonists picrotoxin (n = 12; Figure 1B,E) and SR95531 (n = 4, not shown). With the exception of synaptic latency, optogenetically-evoked IPSCs (oIPSCs) exhibited similar properties in both experimental systems and were consequently pooled for analysis. They averaged 353 ± 60 pA in peak amplitude (range = 68–905 pA; n = 16) and their kinetics were similar to those observed in dorsal striatum (Tritsch et al., 2012; Figure 1—figure supplement 1), with a 10–90% rise time of 2.4 ± 0.2 ms and a decay time constant of 46.8 ± 7.2 ms. The synaptic latency of oIPSCs in NAc did not differ from that observed in dorsal striatum under similar experimental conditions (Figure 1C,F), indicating that the connection is monosynaptic. Synaptic stimulation of midbrain DA neurons therefore directly engages GABAA receptors on SPNs in both dorsal and ventral striatum.

Figure 1. Stimulation of VTA axons evokes GABAergic currents in nucleus accumbens SPNs.

ChR2 was expressed in VTA DA neurons virally (AC) or genetically (DF). (A) Coronal cross section of a Slc6a3-ires-Cre mouse ventral midbrain immunolabeled for TH (red) showing viral transduction of ChR2-EYFP in the VTA (green). (B) Representative oIPSC recorded from an indirect-pathway SPN in NAc before (black trace) and after (gray trace) bath application of the GABAA receptor antagonist picrotoxin (100 μM). Blue line depicts a 1-ms full field flash of 473 nm laser light (5 mW·mm−2). All recordings in this and subsequent figures were performed at −70 mV using a high Clinternal solution in the presence of NBQX (10 μM), R-CPP (10 μM), and CGP55845 (2–5 μM) in the perfusate. (C) Latency from flash onset to oIPSC onset in NAc and dorsal striatum (Caudate/Putamen, CPu) SPNs. For analysis of oIPSCs in CPu, ChR2 was expressed in SNc in a separate cohort of mice. White circles depict individual recordings, red circles are mean ± SEM. (DF) As in (AC) for recordings in Slc6a3-ires-Cre;Ai32 mice. Note that the synaptic latency in these mice is significantly longer than in virally transduced Slc6a3-ires-Cre mice in both CPu and NAc (p<0.001, Mann–Whitney test), presumably because of lower ChR2 expression in Ai32 mice.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.01936.003

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Properties of DA neuron oIPSCs in dorsal striatum.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

ChR2 was expressed in SNc DA neurons virally. (A) or genetically (B). (A) Overlay of fifteen consecutive light-evoked (1ms, 473 nm, 5 mW·mm−2; blue line) IPSCs recorded under control conditions at −70 mV from a SPN in the dorsal striatum of a Slc6a3-ires-Cre mouse previously injected in the SNc with an AAV encoding Cre-dependent ChR2-mCherry. The first trace obtained after break-in is in black and subsequent oIPSCs are shown in progressively lighter shades of gray. The magenta trace depicts the average waveform of the first five oIPSCs. Recordings were performed with NBQX (10 μM), R-CPP (10 μM), and CGP55845 (2 μM) in the perfusate. (B) As in (A) for a SPN recorded under control conditions in the dorsal striatum of a Slc6a3-ires-Cre;Ai32 mouse. (C) Plot of the amplitude of consecutive oIPSCs shown in (A; magenta) and (B; green) over time. (D) As in (C) for oIPSCs recorded in dorsal striatum SPNs from AAV-infected mice (n = 17; magenta), Ai32 mice (n = 8; green), or both (n = 25, black). Amplitude is normalized to the first oIPSC after break-in. Note how the amplitude of oIPSCs progressively decreases with each stimulus under control conditions, regardless of the method used for expressing ChR2. This decrement in oIPSC amplitude is specific to dopaminergic synapses, as oIPSCs recorded from the collaterals of iSPNs in Adora2a-Cre expressing ChR2-mCherry in the dorsal striatum remained maintained their amplitude for the duration of the recording (n = 8; gray). Data represent mean ± SEM. *p<0.001 vs black trace; Sidak's multiple comparison test. (E) Mean latency from flash onset to oIPSC onset in individual dorsal striatum SPNs recordings from AAV-infected (magenta) and Ai32 (green) mice. The difference in synaptic delay are likely due to differences in expression levels of ChR2. Mean (±SEM) shown in red. *p<0.001, Mann–Whitney test. (F) Mean standard deviation (SD) of the synaptic latency of oIPSCs in individual cells indicates little temporal jitter in both experimental models, in agreement with monosynaptic transmission. Mean (±SEM) shown in red. n.s., no significant difference between means (p=0.8; Mann–Whitney test). (G) Time course of oIPSC amplitude rundown starting at the time of drug application (or the 5th stimulus for control recordings in ACSF). The number of SPN recordings in each condition are: ACSF, 25; picrotoxin, 19; SR95531, 11; SKF 89976A, 9; SKF 89976A+SNAP-5114, 8. Changes in the decay kinetics of oIPSCs shown in Figure 2 were quantified 3–4 min after initiating the perfusion of SKF89976A, just as the effects of the GABAA receptor antagonists begin to significantly inhibit oIPSCs. Note that acute application of GAT antagonists does not immediately affect the amplitude of oIPSCs in SPNs. Data represent mean ± SEM. *p<0.01, picrotoxin or SR95531 vs black trace; Tukey's multiple comparison test.

Dopaminergic IPSCs are shaped by membrane GABA transporters

The chemical identity of a synaptic neurotransmitter is traditionally established by determining whether a synapse fulfills several necessary criteria. They include the presence of (1) presynaptic synthetic enzymes, (2) presynaptic vesicular transporters, (3) postsynaptic receptors, and (4) a biochemical mechanism for inactivation. In the case of GABAergic signaling at dopaminergic synapses, conditions 2 and 3 are respectively satisfied by the presence of VMAT2 in DA neurons and GABAA receptors in SPNs. For many central nervous system transmitters, condition 4 is mediated by diffusion and reuptake through plasma membrane transporters. Of the four genes identified in mice that encode high affinity membrane GABA transporters, Slc6a1 is expressed predominantly throughout the brain (Borden, 1996; Lein et al., 2007). Its gene product, mGAT1, distributes mainly to presynaptic terminals of GABAergic neurons and serves to shape the amplitude and time course of IPSCs as well as regulate extrasynaptic levels of GABA (Isaacson et al., 1993; Jensen et al., 2003; Overstreet and Westbrook, 2003; Conti et al., 2004; Chiu et al., 2005; Bragina et al., 2008; Kirmse et al., 2008; Cepeda et al., 2013). Importantly, mGAT1 is highly selective for GABA and does not transport structurally similar GABAA receptor agonists such as β-alanine, taurine, and muscimol (Johnston et al., 1978; Tamura et al., 1995). Thus, we reasoned that if dopaminergic IPSCs are modulated by inhibition of mGAT1, it would provide compelling evidence that GABA is the neurotransmitter released.

To test this, we recorded dopaminergic oIPSCs from SPNs in dorsal striatum (Figure 2). Stimulation intensity was calibrated to evoke sub-maximal IPSCs (approximately 20% of IPSC peak amplitude at maximum intensity; mean: 232 ± 40 pA, n = 19) in order to minimize spillover of GABA at dopaminergic synapses (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). We compared the amplitude and kinetics of IPSCs recorded before (baseline) and during the first 3–4 min following bath application of control saline (ACSF; n = 9; Figure 2A) or saline containing SKF 89976A, a selective mGAT1 antagonist (n = 10; Figure 2B). In both conditions, the IPSC amplitude progressively declined to ∼70% of baseline (p<0.01 each, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; Figure 2C), whereas the synaptic latency and 10–90% rise time remained unchanged relative to baseline (p>0.1, Wilcoxon signed-rank test). The decrease in amplitude is consistent with previous reports of activity-dependent rundown of GABA and DA release from midbrain DA neurons in vitro (Schmitz et al., 2003; Tritsch et al., 2012; Ishikawa et al., 2013) and did not differ between conditions (p=0.8, Mann–Whitney test; Figure 2C, Figure 1—figure supplement 1G), indicating that acute inhibition of mGAT1 does not interfere with neurotransmitter release or postsynaptic GABAA receptors. By contrast, mGAT1 antagonism significantly affected decay kinetics: whereas oIPSC decay time constants remained stable in control recordings (baseline: 33.6 ± 5.8 ms; 3–4 min after ACSF wash-in: 32.1 ± 5.8 ms; p=0.6, Wilcoxon signed-rank test), they were prolonged by a factor of 3 in the presence of SKF 89976A (baseline: 38.6 ± 6.0 ms; 3–4 min after SKF 89976A wash-in: 115.9 ± 26.3 ms; p=0.002, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; Figure 2D). These results indicate that mGAT1 activity normally shortens the duration of dopaminergic IPSCs, likely by clearing released neurotransmitter from the extracellular space.

Figure 2. The decay kinetics of dopaminergic IPSCs are shaped by membrane GABA transporters.

(A) Two representative oIPSCs recorded in SPNs using sub-maximal ChR2 stimulation (1ms; 0.3–2 mW·mm−2; blue line) before (baseline, left black trace) and 3–4 min after (middle gray trace) bath application of control saline (ACSF). Right, overlay of peak-normalized oIPSCs showing identical decay kinetics. (B) As in (A) for oIPSCs recorded in SKF 89976A (10 μM, in green). (C) Histogram of mean (±SEM) peak oIPSC amplitude normalized to baseline for SPNs perfused in ACSF (gray) and SKF 89976A (green). Number of recordings indicated in parentheses. (D) Plot of individual oIPSC decay time constants before and after bath application of ACSF and SKF 89976A. Mean (±SEM) shown in red. *p=0.002 vs baseline, Wilcoxon signed-rank test.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.01936.005

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. oIPSC decay time constant increases with stimulus strength.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) The decay time constant of oIPSCs evoked with maximal light intensity (5 mW·mm−2) does not correlate with oIPSC peak amplitude (n = 25 SPNs; R2 = 0.01, linear regression; red line). (BC) In a subset of SPN recordings (n = 6), the amplitude (B) and decay time constant (C) of oIPSCs were measured at different light power densities (blue: 5 mW·mm−2; gray: 0.3 mW·mm−2). Maximal stimulation strength increased the mean amplitude and mean decay time constant of oIPSCs (red circles; p<0.04 for both, Wilcoxon signed-rank test), suggesting that the ability of GATs to limit pooling and/or spillover of GABA at dopaminergic synapses is compromised at high light stimulation intensities.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. mGAT1 controls ambient levels of GABA in the striatum.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(A) Continuous whole-cell voltage-clamp recording (Vhold = −70 mV) from a direct pathway SPN (dSPN) upon bath application of the GABAA receptor antagonist picrotoxin (100 μM, black bar) in the presence of NBQX (10 μM), R-CPP (10 μM), and CGP55845 (5 μM). Downward deflections represent spontaneous IPSCs. The presence of a standing inward current mediated by extracellular GABA is revealed upon application of picrotoxin, which evokes a shift in baseline holding current. (B) As in (A) for an indirect pathway SPN (iSPN) upon bath application of SKF 89976A (10 μM) and picrotoxin (indicated by green and black bars, respectively). Green box indicates the 3–4 min window following the onset of SKF 89976A perfusion during which oIPSCs, eIPSCs, sIPSCs and SKF 89976A-evoked currents are quantified. (C) Histogram of picrotoxin-evoked shift in holding current recorded in dSPNs under control conditions (ACSF, gray) or upon acute inhibition of mGAT1 (SKF 89976A, green). Number of recordings is indicated in parentheses. *p<0.01, Mann–Whitney test. (D) As in (C) for iSPNs. Note that no differences in tonic GABAA-receptor-mediated currents were observed between iSPNs and dSPNs, either in ACSF or SKF 89976A.

In the striatum, mGAT1 controls ambient levels of GABA that evoke a tonic GABAA receptor-mediated conductance in SPNs (Ade et al., 2008; Kirmse et al., 2008; Santhakumar et al., 2010; Cepeda et al., 2013). Consistent with this, acute pharmacological inhibition of mGAT1 was accompanied by a significant increase in holding current caused by a threefold increase in tonic GABA current in both direct- and indirect-pathway SPNs (Figure 2—figure supplement 2). To exclude the possibility that the increase in oIPSC decay time constant stems from changes in recording conditions during bath application of SKF 89976A, we also monitored spontaneous (s) IPSCs as well as IPSCs evoked electrically within the striatum (eIPSCs; Figure 3). Unlike dopaminergic oIPSCs, neither the amplitude nor the kinetics of eIPSCs were significantly affected after acute mGAT1 inhibition (baseline amplitude: 304 ± 70 pA; 3–4 min after SKF 89976A wash-in: 339 ± 80 pA. Baseline 10–90% rise time: 1.3 ± 0.4 ms; 3–4 min after SKF 89976A wash-in: 1.3 ± 0.4 ms. Baseline decay time constant: 14.8 ± 2.6 ms; 3–4 min after SKF 89976A wash-in: 19.6 ± 4.9 ms; n = 9; p>0.1 for all, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; Figure 3A,B), despite a notable increase in holding current during the first few minutes following bath application of SKF 89976A (baseline: −82 ± 16 pA; SKF 89976A: −143 ± 21 pA; n = 9; p<0.001, Wilcoxon signed-rank test). Moreover, although the amplitude of sIPSCs decreased slightly in SKF 89976A relative to baseline (97 ± 9 vs 83 ± 7 pA, n = 14; p=0.01, Wilcoxon signed-rank test), the frequency and kinetics of sIPSCs remained unchanged (baseline frequency: 2.7 ± 0.5 Hz; 3–4 min after SKF 89976A wash-in: 2.5 ± 0.5 Hz. Baseline 10–90% rise time: 0.9 ± 0.1 ms; 3–4 min after SKF 89976A wash-in: 0.8 ± 0.1 ms. Baseline decay time constant: 6.8 ± 0.4 ms; 3–4 min after SKF 89976A wash-in: 6.4 ± 0.5 ms; p>0.05 for all, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; Figure 3C,D). These results are consistent with previous reports in the striatum and hippocampus (Isaacson et al., 1993; Kirmse et al., 2008). Thus, our results indicate that prolongation of light-evoked IPSCs by SKF 89976A is specific to dopaminergic synapses and not secondary to increased GABAergic tone. Together, these data reveal that the duration of dopaminergic IPSCs is critically dependent on mGAT1 function and, therefore, strongly support that GABA is the transmitter co-released by DA neurons.

Figure 3. The decay kinetics of electrically-evoked and spontaneous IPSCs are insensitive to mGAT1 inhibition.

Figure 3.

(A) Representative electrically-evoked IPSC before (baseline, black trace) and 3–4 min after (green trace) bath application of SKF 89976A (10 μM). Inset, overlay of peak-normalized eIPSCs. (B) Plot of individual eIPSC decay time constants before (black) and after (green) SKF 89976A application. Mean (±SEM) shown in red. (C and D) As in (A and B) for spontaneous IPSCs. Note that sIPSCs have much faster kinetics compared to eIPSCs.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.01936.008

Mouse midbrain DA neurons do not express GABA synthetic enzymes

Previous reports indicate that up to 10% of midbrain DA neurons in rat contain detectable levels of mRNA for the 65 kDa isoform of glutamic acid decarboxylase (GAD65) (Gonzalez-Hernandez et al., 2001; 2004). Together with GAD67, these enzymes constitute the major biosynthetic pathway for GABA in the central nervous system (Soghomonian and Martin, 1998). To determine the fraction of midbrain DA neurons capable of synthesizing (and by extension releasing) GABA in mice, we performed double fluorescence in situ hybridization for Slc18a2 (Vmat2) and Gad1 or Gad2 (which encode GAD67 and GAD65, respectively). We focused our analyses to regions highlighted in Figure 4A, which consist of the SNc and lateral VTA. In agreement with previous findings (Gonzalez-Hernandez et al., 2001, 2004), we did not detect significant overlap between Vmat2 and Gad1, as only 4 out of 958 Vmat2+ neurons (0.4%) showed weak signal for Gad1 in SNc and lateral VTA (Figure 4A,B). To our surprise, we were also unable to detect Gad2 mRNA in these regions (Figure 4D,E): only 7 out of 1200 Vmat2+ cells (0.6%) were weakly Gad2+, despite the presence of numerous brightly-labeled GABAergic neurons in neighboring substantia nigra pars reticulata (SNr) and within the SNc and lateral VTA. We confirmed our ability to detect co-labeling by examining the histaminergic tuberomamillary nucleus (TMN) and dopaminergic A13 cell group (Figure 4C,F), both of which express GADs (Lin et al., 1990; Esclapez et al., 1993).

Figure 4. Midbrain DA neurons do not express Gad1 or Gad2.

Figure 4.

(A) Two-color in situ hybridization of Slc18a2 (Vmat2; top, red) and Gad1 (middle, green) demonstrates the absence of co-labeled DA neurons (bottom) in a coronal section through lateral VTA and SNc (dashed outline). Nuclei are stained blue. SNr, substantia nigra pars reticulata; D, dorsal; V, ventral; M, medial; L, lateral. (B) Representative high magnification confocal image of Slc18a2 (red) and Gad1 (green) expression in SNc. (C) Double fluorescence in situ hybridization for Slc18a2 and Gad1 exhibits considerable overlap in the tuberomamillary nucleus (TMN). (D and E) As in (A and B) for Slc18a2 (Vmat2) and Gad2 expression. (F) Slc18a2 and Gad2 expression co-localize in the A13 dopaminergic cell group.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.01936.009

Levels of mRNA for GAD65 and GAD67 in DA neurons may be below the detection threshold, yet high enough to sustain GABA synthesis. To address this concern, we attempted to directly visualize GAD protein in DA neurons by immunofluorescence using antibodies directed against GAD65/67 and tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) to label catecholaminergic neurons. However, the high density of GABAergic axons converging into the substantia nigra, combined with the low concentration of GADs in cell bodies compared to axon terminals prevented us from clearly identifying DA neurons that expressed GADs (not shown). We instead imaged coronal brain sections from knock-in mice (Tamamaki et al., 2003; Taniguchi et al., 2011) expressing either EGFP or Cre recombinase under transcriptional control of the endogenous promoter for Gad1 or Gad2 (Figure 5). Cre expression was visualized using a sensitive fluorescent reporter allele (Madisen et al., 2010) and TH expression was revealed via immunofluorescence. These mice offer the advantage of having bright somatic labeling including, in the case of Gad2-ires-Cre mice, of cells with very little transcriptional activity. However, we were unable to detect any DA neuron expressing fluorescent reporter protein driven by either Gad1 (0 out of 519 TH+ neurons) or Gad2 (0 out of 526 TH+ neurons) promoters (Figure 5A–D). Collectively, these results indicate that midbrain DA neurons in the SNc and lateral VTA of mice do not express the GABA synthetic enzymes GAD65 and GAD67.

Figure 5. DA neurons in SNc/VTA do not express GAD65 or GAD67.

Figure 5.

(A) Low magnification epifluorescence image of a coronal section through the ventral midbrain showing the absence of overlap between tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) immunofluorescence (red) and endogenous EGFP (green) in the SNc and lateral VTA of Gad1-Egfp knock-in mice. Blue, nuclear stain. D, dorsal; V, ventral; M, medial; L, lateral. (B) Representative high magnification confocal image of SNc in Gad1-Egfp mice showing mutually exclusive expression of EFGP and TH, confirming that GAD67 is not expressed in SNc DA neurons. (C and D) As in (A and B) for coronal brain sections from Gad2-ires-Cre knock-in mice expressing a fluorescence Cre reporter allele (Ai14). TH immunolabeling and Cre reporter fluorescence respectively depicted in red and green for consistency. The absence of overlap indicates that GAD65 is not expressed in DA neurons of the SNc and lateral VTA.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.01936.010

Midbrain DA neurons express GABA transaminase

Mammalian cells possess alternative means of synthesizing GABA (Seiler, 1980; Caron et al., 1987; Petroff, 2002). GABA transaminase is an enzyme expressed at high levels in GABAergic neurons, including in the ventral midbrain that can reversibly convert succinate semialdehyde into GABA, and vice versa (Bessman et al., 1953; Roberts and Bregoff, 1953; Medina-Kauwe et al., 1994). Because succinate semialdehyde is readily oxidized to succinate to sustain energy production as part of the Krebs cycle, GABA transaminase is thought to participate in the degradation of GABA in most cells, not its synthesis (Balazs et al., 1970). Nevertheless, we considered the possibility that GABA transaminase might contribute GABA in DA neurons. Double fluorescence in situ hybridization for Vmat2 and Abat (the gene encoding GABA transaminase) revealed strong co-labeling in 701 out of 738 DA neurons (95%) within the SNc and lateral VTA (Figure 6A,B), indicating that DA neurons express genes involved in GABA metabolism. To test whether DA neurons employ GABA transaminase to synthesize GABA, we incubated slices of striatum in control ACSF or vigabatrin (VGT)—an irreversible inhibitor of GABA transaminase—for at least thirty minutes prior to recording oIPSCs in SPNs. In agreement with prior work (Overstreet and Westbrook, 2001), this manipulation significantly elevated extracellular GABA levels (picrotoxin-sensitive tonic current in SPNs in ACSF: 24 ± 3 pA, n = 11; in VGT: 115 ± 23 pA, n = 12; p<0.001, Dunn's Multiple Comparison Test), confirming the effectiveness of the drug. To minimize differences in light-evoked responses within experiments, we obtained maximal oIPSCs using strong ChR2 stimulation and limited comparisons to SPNs located in similar areas of dorsal striatum in adjacent slices. Under these conditions, we did not detect significant differences in oIPSC amplitude between both groups (ACSF: 1.1 ± 0.3 nA; VGT: 1.4 ± 0.2 nA; n = 14; p=0.3, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; Figure 6C,D), indicating that GABA transaminase function is not required for GABAergic signaling by DA neurons.

Figure 6. GABA transaminase is not required for GABA release by DA neurons.

Figure 6.

(A) Double fluorescence in situ hybridization for Slc18a2 (Vmat2; red) and Abat (green) reveals that midbrain DA neurons overwhelmingly express GABA transaminase. Blue, nuclear stain. D, dorsal; V, ventral; M, medial; L, lateral. (B) High magnification confocal image of Slc18a2 (red) and Abat (green) mRNA distribution in SNc. (C) Light-evoked IPSCs recorded from SPNs upon strong ChR2 stimulation (1 ms, 5 mW·mm−2; blue line) after prolonged incubation in ACSF (black) or vigabatrin (VGT, 100 μM; magenta). (D) Plot of mean peak oIPSC amplitude in SPNs recorded in similar regions of dorsal striatum in adjacent slices (depicted by gray lines) incubated in either ACSF (black) or VGT (magenta). Mean (±SEM) values for each group shown in red.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.01936.011

Midbrain DA neurons express plasma membrane GABA transporters

Our data indicate that inhibitory synaptic transmission from DA neurons does not depend on synthesis of GABA by either GADs or GABA transaminase. In the central nervous system, several synapses rely on neurotransmitter reuptake across the plasma membrane as opposed to de novo synthesis to sustain synaptic transmission (Torres et al., 2003; Edwards, 2007). It is therefore conceivable that DA neurons inhibit SPNs by releasing GABA they acquire from the extracellular environment. mGAT1 and mGAT4 (encoded by Slc6a11) are the two major plasma membrane GABA transporters expressed in the mouse midbrain (Borden, 1996; Lein et al., 2007). We therefore performed double fluorescence in situ hybridization for Vmat2 and mGat1 or mGat4 to establish whether either isoform is present in DA neurons (Figure 7). Surprisingly, we found considerable expression of mGat1 in 667 out of 748 Vmat2+ neurons (89%), particularly within the SNc. mGat4 localized predominantly to glial cells, consistent with previous reports (Borden, 1996), although faint labeling above background was also detected in a substantial fraction (80%) of DA neurons in SNc and lateral VTA (568 out of 713 Vmat2+ neurons).

Figure 7. Midbrain DA neurons express plasma membrane GABA transporters.

Figure 7.

(A) Two color in situ hybridization for Slc18a2 (Vmat2; top, red) and mGat1 (middle, green) shows considerable overlap in SNc and lateral VTA (bottom). Nuclei are stained blue. D, dorsal; V, ventral; M, medial; L, lateral. (B) Representative high magnification confocal image of Slc18a2 (red) and mGat1 (green) in SNc confirms that DA neurons express mRNA for mGAT1. (C) Same as (A) for Slc18a2 (top, red) and mGat4 (middle, green). Note that mGat4 is most strongly expressed in star-shaped glial cells. (D) Confocal image through SNc reveals strong expression of mGat4 mRNA in glial cells and weak labeling in DA neurons.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.01936.012

GAT function is required for dopaminergic IPSCs

The expression of mRNA for mGAT1 and mGAT4 in DA neurons raises the possibility that DA neurons acquire GABA not through de novo synthesis, but rather through plasma membrane uptake. Being almost exclusively composed of GABAergic neurons, the striatum represents a rich source of extracellular GABA for dopaminergic terminals (Ade et al., 2008; Kirmse et al., 2008; Janssen et al., 2009; Santhakumar et al., 2010; Cepeda et al., 2013). Inhibition of mGAT1 for a few minutes does not affect the release of GABA from SNc axons (Figure 2C), indicating either that this pharmacological manipulation incompletely blocks GABA reuptake, or that it is too short to deplete presynaptic GABA levels. To determine whether uptake of ambient GABA is necessary for sustaining GABAergic transmission by DA neurons, we therefore inhibited both mGAT1 and mGAT4 in striatal slices using a cocktail of SKF 89976A and SNAP-5114 (a mGAT4 antagonist) for at least 30 min prior to examining oIPSCs in SPNs in the continued presence of GAT antagonists. Whereas oIPSCs obtained under control conditions averaged 1.3 ± 0.2 nA in amplitude (n = 15), oIPSCs recorded in adjacent slices with GABA transport chronically blocked were significantly smaller (0.20 ± 0.07 nA, n = 16; p<0.001 vs ACSF, Dunn's Multiple Comparison Test; Figure 8A,B). Importantly, prolonged exposure to SKF 89976A and SNAP-5114 did not prevent transmitter release non-specifically, as light-evoked dopamine release from SNc axons as well as GABA release from striatopallidal terminals were maintained under these conditions (Figure 8—figure supplement 1). Moreover, the small picrotoxin-sensitive oIPSCs that remained exhibited extremely long decay time constants (>1.5 s), consistent with persistent GABAergic signaling in the absence of plasma membrane reuptake.

Figure 8. Sustained GABAergic signaling from DA neurons requires GAT function.

(A) Dopaminergic IPSCs evoked by strong ChR2 stimulation (1 ms, 5 mW·mm−2; blue line) in slices incubated for at least 30 min in control ACSF (left, black), muscimol (0.1 μM; right, gray) or a cocktail of mGAT1 and mGAT4 antagonists (10 μM SKF 89976A + 50 μM SNAP-5114, respectively; middle, green). Recordings were performed in the continued presence of each drug, in addition to the GABAB receptor antagonist CGP55845 (2–5 μM) and the glutamate receptor blockers NBQX (10 μM) and R-CPP (10 μM). Inset, oIPSC in GAT antagonists shown on longer time scale to illustrate slow kinetics. (B) Plot of peak oIPSC amplitudes recorded from individual SPNs in adjacent slices after prolonged incubation in ACSF (black), mGAT1 and mGAT4 inhibitors (green) and muscimol (gray). Mean (±SEM) indicated in red. *p<0.01 for indicated comparisons, Dunn's Multiple Comparison Test. (C) Histogram of tonic GABA current (IGABA) measured in SPNs as the reduction in holding current evoked by bath application of the GABAA receptor antagonist picrotoxin (100 μM) under control conditions (black), or after prolonged incubation in mGAT1 and mGAT4 blockers (10 μM SKF 89976A + 50 μM SNAP-5114; green), muscimol (0.1 μM; gray) or vigabatrin (100 μM; magenta). *p<0.01 vs ACSF, Dunn's Multiple Comparison Test. Number of recordings indicated in parentheses. (D) Plot of consecutive oIPSC amplitudes (normalized to the first light-evoked response) over time under control conditions (ACSF; black; n = 25) and after prolonged incubation in a cocktail of GAT antagonists (10 μM SKF 89976A + 50 μM SNAP-5114; green; n = 11). *p<0.001 vs ACSF, Sidak's multiple comparison test. (E) As in (D) for slices supplied with exogenous GABA (100 μM) for 5–10 min before obtaining oIPSCs from SPNs in dorsal striatum (blue; n = 10), and slices supplied with exogenous GABA after prolonged inhibition of GATs with 10 μM SKF 89976A + 50 μM SNAP-5114 (magenta; n = 12). *p<0.05 vs ACSF, #p<0.005 vs GABA, Tukey's multiple comparison test. Control traces in (D) and (E) are the same as in Figure 1—figure supplement 1D.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.01936.013

Figure 8.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1. Chronic GAT block does not non-specifically affect synaptic transmission at GABAergic and dopaminergic synapses.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1.

(A) Example traces of light-evoked (1 ms, 5 mW·mm−2; blue line) IPSCs recorded from neurons in the external segment of the globus pallidus (GPe; Vhold = −70 mV) in Adora2a-Cre;Ai32 mice, which express ChR2-EYFP in indirect-pathway SPNs. GPe boundaries were identified using the strong EYFP fluorescence of iSPN axonal terminals. Slices were incubated for at least 30 min in ACSF (left, black) or in a cocktail of 10 μM SKF 89976A and 50 μM SNAP-5114 (right, green). Pre-incubations and recordings were performed in the continued presence of each drug, in addition to CGP55845 (5 μM), NBQX (10 μM) and R-CPP (10 μM). (B) Amplitude normalized traces from (A) shown on an expanded time scale to reveal the slow decay kinetics of IPSCs in the presence of GAT antagonists. (C) The peak amplitude of striatopallidal oIPSCs did not significantly differ (p=0.7; Mann–Whitney test) between slices bathed in ACSF (black; n = 11 GPe neurons) and slices chronically incubated in GAT blockers (green; n = 16 GPe neurons), indicating that this pharmacological manipulation does not inhibit synaptic transmission at ‘classical’ GABAergic synapses. Mean (±SEM) shown in red. (D) Representative traces of light-evoked (1 ms, 5 mW·mm−2; blue lines) DA release in the dorsal striatum of Slc6a3-ires-Cre;Ai32 measured by carbon-fiber amperometry. Pharmacological conditions are identical to those in (AC). Stimulation artifacts are blanked for clarity. (E) Peak extracellular DA concentration measured in dorsal striatum slices incubated in ACSF (black; n = 9 slices) or in GAT antagonists (green; n = 14 slices). Population means (±SEM in red) did not differ significantly (p=0.09; Mann–Whitney test).
Figure 8—figure supplement 2. oIPSC rundown is activity dependent.

Figure 8—figure supplement 2.

(A) The peak amplitude of the first oIPSC recorded in dorsal striatum SPNs within the first 1.5 hr after slicing (black) was comparable to that recorded during the next 1.5 hr (gray), indicating that oIPSCs do not rundown with time in the absence of stimulation. Data in this and subsequent panels represent mean ± SEM. The number of SPNs recorded is indicated in parentheses. (B) The time course of synaptic transmission rundown by consecutive light stimuli was similar in the two groups of slices, confirming that recording conditions in the first and second halves of recording sessions are comparable, and that synaptic rundown of DA neuron oIPSCs is dependent on activity. (CD) As in (AB) for slices chronically incubated for 0.5–1.0 hr (dark green) or 1.1–2.0 hr (light green) in SKF 89976A (10 μM) + SNAP-5114 (50 μM). The effect of GAT inhibition on oIPSC amplitude does not vary with pre-incubation duration (C), indicating that our pharmacological manipulation depletes synaptic GABA levels within 30 min. Importantly, stimulation-evoked rundown in GAT blockers is accelerated (contrast panels B and D), consistent with activity dependent depletion of vesicular GABA when cytoplasmic GABA levels are not replenished by GATs.

Chronic inhibition of GABA reuptake was also accompanied by considerable elevation of ambient GABA in the striatum, as evidenced by a 7.5-fold increase in tonic GABAA receptor-mediated current in SPNs (picrotoxin-evoked tonic current in ACSF: 24 ± 3 pA, n = 11; in GAT antagonists: 182 ± 32 pA, n = 11; p<0.001, Dunn's Multiple Comparison Test; Figure 8C). This increase in extracellular GABA might account for the reduced amplitude of oIPSCs by promoting the desensitization of GABAA receptors, by shunting synaptic currents, or both. To examine these possibilities, we included an additional experimental condition in which slices were pre-incubated in muscimol before evoking dopaminergic oIPSCs (Figure 8A,B). Muscimol is a potent synaptic and extrasynaptic GABAA receptor agonist that promotes GABAA receptor desensitization to the same extent as GABA (Mortensen et al., 2010), but displays minimal affinity for plasma membrane GABA transporters (Johnston et al., 1978). The concentration of muscimol was titrated to evoke tonic GABAA receptor-mediated currents similar those that developed upon persistent GAT inhibition (picrotoxin-sensitive tonic current: 212 ± 46 pA, n = 10; p<0.001 vs ACSF; p>0.05 vs GAT antagonists, Dunn's Multiple Comparison Test; Figure 8C). Strikingly, this manipulation did not reduce the amplitude of dopaminergic oIPSCs (1.3 ± 0.3 nA, n = 12; p>0.05 vs ACSF, p<0.01 vs GAT antagonists, Dunn's Multiple Comparison Test; Figure 8A,B), indicating that the reduction in oIPSC amplitude observed following GAT inhibition is not secondary to elevation of ambient GABAergic tone. In agreement with this conclusion, the amplitude of oIPSCs was not diminished by prolonged treatment with VGT either (Figure 6C,D), despite significant increases in extracellular GABA (picrotoxin-sensitive tonic current in VGT: 115 ± 23 pA, n = 12; p<0.01 vs ACSF, p>0.05 vs GAT antagonists or muscimol, Dunn's Multiple Comparison Test; Figure 8C).

These results collectively indicate that GAT function is necessary for GABAergic transmission from SNc axons. If membrane GABA reuptake is required for supplying GABA for vesicular exocytosis, inhibiting GATs should prevent DA neurons from maintaining GABA release with repeated stimulation. Indeed, chronic inhibition of GATs precipitated the rundown of GABAergic transmission from DA neurons in an activity-dependent manner (Figure 8D, Figure 8—figure supplement 2). A second prediction is that elevating extracellular GABA should attenuate synaptic rundown. To test this, we locally applied GABA (100 μM) to the dorsal striatum for 5–10 min prior to recording oIPSCs from SPNs. Following this manipulation, the amplitude of oIPSCs was maintained for several minutes after break-in (Figure 8E). Importantly, this increase in oIPSC amplitude by exogenous GABA was prevented in slices incubated in GAT antagonists (Figure 8E). These data therefore provide strong evidence that membrane GABA transporters play an important role in sustaining GABAergic transmission from DA neurons by supplying the neurotransmitter GABA to presynaptic terminals for vesicular loading and exocytosis.

Discussion

Despite the importance of midbrain DA neurons, the mechanisms by which they shape the activity of target neurons in the striatum remain poorly understood. In this study, we combined histochemical and electrophysiological approaches to define the cellular and molecular mechanisms that govern GABAergic signaling by DA neurons. Our findings indicate that the vast majority of midbrain DA neurons have the capacity to co-release GABA and identify presynaptic membrane GABA transporters as the likely mechanism employed by DA neurons to acquire GABA for vesicular exocytosis.

Midbrain DA neurons co-release GABA

The chemical identity of a synaptic transmitter is typically established only after several independent observations converge on a plausible candidate. In this case, the transmitter released by midbrain DA neurons (1) functions as a GABAA receptor agonist, (2) serves as a substrate for VGAT (Tritsch et al., 2012), and (3) is a substrate for mGAT1. Although VGAT can transport GABA, glycine, and β-alanine into synaptic vesicles (Gasnier, 2000; Wojcik et al., 2006; Juge et al., 2013), glycine does not function as a GABAA receptor agonist, and the low affinity of β-alanine for GABAA receptors would give rise to IPSCs with faster kinetics than the ones we observed (Jones et al., 1998a). In addition, mGAT1 is highly selective for GABA (Borden, 1996), and applying GABA exogenously helped sustain IPSCs from DA neurons in a GAT-dependent fashion. Although we did not detect GABA synthetic enzymes in DA neurons in our experiments, SNc and VTA neurons display a GABAergic phenotype, as they contain mRNA for GABA transaminase and the membrane GABA transporters mGAT1 and mGAT4. Together, these results strongly suggest that the neurotransmitter released by midbrain DA neurons is GABA. In agreement with this conclusion, a recent electron microscopic study detected GABA in close association with VMAT2-containing synaptic vesicles in dopaminergic terminals within striatum (Stensrud et al., 2013). Moreover, other dopaminergic neurons in the central nervous system have been reported to contain and/or release GABA, including amacrine cells in the retina (Hirasawa et al., 2012), periglomerular cells in the olfactory bulb (Maher and Westbrook, 2008; Borisovska et al., 2013; Liu et al., 2013), as well as dopaminergic cell groups throughout the brain (Bjorklund and Dunnett, 2007; Stamatakis et al., 2013), suggesting that GABA co-release may be a general property of dopaminergic cells.

Midbrain DA neurons sustain inhibitory transmission using plasma membrane uptake of GABA

Synaptic transmission requires constant refilling of new and recycled vesicles, which is contingent upon the availability of transmitter in the cytosol. Most GABAergic neurons sustain inhibitory transmission by maintaining high cytosolic concentrations of GABA using de novo synthesis from glutamate by GAD67 and to a lesser extent GAD65 (Asada et al., 1997; Tian et al., 1999; Petroff, 2002). Given that all cells contain glutamate, an amino acid necessary for the production of proteins, the expression of either GAD67 or GAD65 in conjunction with a vesicular GABA transporter is sufficient to mediate vesicular release of GABA. Previous reports have estimated that up to 10% of DA neurons in the SNc and VTA of rats express mRNA for GAD65 (Gonzalez-Hernandez et al., 2001, 2004), suggesting that GABA release may be limited to a subpopulation of DA neurons. We were unable to replicate this observation using two separate approaches, indicating that DA neurons in the SNc and lateral VTA of mice do not rely on de novo synthesis of GABA and have instead adopted other means of obtaining GABA.

Most classical transmitters are transported back into the presynaptic terminal after vesicular release using plasma membrane transporters. This process is important not only for limiting the duration of synaptic currents and preventing extrasynaptic spillover, but also for replenishing presynaptic transmitter levels (Edwards, 2007; Conti et al., 2011). In fact, several synapses, including monoaminergic (Giros et al., 1996; Bengel et al., 1998; Jones et al., 1998b; Xu et al., 2000), glycinergic (Gomeza et al., 2003a, 2003b; Rousseau et al., 2008; Apostolides and Trussell, 2013) as well as some GABAergic terminals (Mathews and Diamond, 2003; Bak et al., 2006; Fricke et al., 2007; Brown and Mathews, 2010; Wang et al., 2013) rely heavily on membrane transporter function to maintain cytosolic transmitter pools available for vesicular loading. Our results show that DA neurons contain mRNA for mGAT1 and mGAT4, indicating that DA neurons might acquire GABA through membrane transport. Indeed, we find that prolonged inhibition of mGAT1 and mGAT4 inhibited GABAergic transmission by DA neurons and precipitated the rundown of oIPSCs, suggesting that DA neurons critically depend on extracellular GABA uptake to maintain cytoplasmic GABA levels and fill synaptic vesicles. By contrast, application of exogenous GABA helped sustain inhibitory transmission from DA neurons. By virtue of the fact that the vast majority of DA neurons in the SNc and lateral VTA contain mRNA for mGATs, our results suggest that GABA co-release is, unlike glutamate co-transmission (Hnasko and Edwards, 2011), a common feature of these cells in the adult nervous system. The reliance on plasma membrane uptake may partially explain why release of both DA and GABA from these neurons is prone to rundown upon repeated stimulation in brain slices (Schmitz et al., 2003; Tritsch et al., 2012; Ishikawa et al., 2013), whereas release of glutamate from dopaminergic neurons or GABA from SPNs (which presumably depend on neurotransmitter synthesis to sustain release) do not suffer from the same shortcoming (Tritsch et al., 2012; Figure 1—figure supplement 1D). Nevertheless, these findings provide evidence that the expression of GABA synthetic enzymes is not required to sustain GABAergic transmission, and identify a GABAergic synapse that instead relies entirely on recycling extracellular GABA. Interestingly, a similar mechanism underlies exocytic release of serotonin from dopaminergic neurons (Zhou et al., 2005) as well as from glutamatergic thalamocortical neurons during development (Lebrand et al., 1996).

Implications of GABAergic signaling for striatal function

Although DA neurons in SNc and VTA differ in the inputs they receive and the signals they convey (Matsumoto and Hikosaka, 2009; Lammel et al., 2011, 2012; Watabe-Uchida et al., 2012; Roeper, 2013), we find that both cell populations are capable of co-releasing GABA with DA, pointing to a fundamental property of dopaminergic signaling. Morphological studies of SPNs at the electron microscopic level have revealed that the majority of dopaminergic synapses terminate on the neck of spines that receive excitatory inputs from cortex and thalamus (Wickens and Arbuthnott, 2005). Although the actions of DA are not believed to be spatially localized (Arbuthnott and Wickens, 2007), this unique synaptic arrangement is indicative of an additional, point-to-point mode of action. An intriguing possibility is that GABA co-release may function to hyperpolarize spines or shunt excitatory cortical and thalamic inputs by activating GABAA receptors. Phasic activation of DA neurons may thereby dampen ongoing cortical and thalamic drive onto SPNs to limit DA receptor-mediated plastic changes to synaptic inputs most strongly activated by salient or rewarding stimuli. In addition, the mechanism adopted by DA neurons to obtain GABA may confer these cells the flexibility to dynamically and locally control GABAergic transmission across their extensive axonal arbors. For instance, VTA neurons projecting to NAc and medial prefrontal cortex may only co-release GABA in the former, where extracellular GABA levels are high compared to cortex (Drasbek and Jensen, 2006; Weitlauf and Woodward, 2008). Alternatively, phasic and chronic changes in mGAT function or extracellular GABA levels resulting from synaptic activity, drugs or disease may alter the amplitude and kinetics of GABAergic currents arising from midbrain dopamine neurons. A greater understanding of the relative effects of DA and GABA on the activity of striatal circuits will help reveal how DA neurons contribute to behavior in health and disease.

Materials and methods

Experimental subjects and stereotaxic surgery

All experimental manipulations were performed in accordance with protocols approved by the Harvard Standing Committee on Animal Care following guidelines described in the US National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. All mice were group-housed and maintained on a 12-hr light cycle with ad libitum access to food and water. Slc6a3-ires-Cre knock-in mice expressing Cre recombinase in DA neurons (Backman et al., 2006) were obtained from Jackson Labs (Bar Harbor, ME; stock # 006660). These mice were crossed with Drd2-Egfp (GENSAT, founder line S118) or Drd1a-tdTomato (stock # 016204; Jackson Labs) BAC transgenic mice to permit distinction between direct- and indirect-pathway SPNs (Gong et al., 2003; Ade et al., 2011). To genetically target indirect pathway SPNs, Adora2a-Cre BAC transgenic mice (GENSAT, founder line KG139) were used. For stereotaxic viral injections, postnatal day 18–25 mice were anesthetized with isoflurane, placed in a small animal stereotaxic frame (David Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA) and injected with 1 μl of an adeno-associated virus (∼1012 genome copies per ml; UNC Vector Core Facility, Chapel Hill, NC) encoding Cre-dependent ChR2 (AAV2/8.EF1α.DIO.hChR2(H134R)-mCherry or AAV2/8.EF1α.DIO.hChR2(H134R)-EYFP), as described previously (Tritsch et al., 2012). Injection coordinates were 0.8 mm anterior from Lambda, 1.3 mm lateral and 4.4 mm below pia for SNc, and 0.8 mm anterior from Lambda, 0.6 mm lateral and 4.4 mm below pia for VTA. Alternatively, ChR2(H134R)-EYFP was expressed genetically in Cre-containing cells by crossing Slc6a3-ires-Cre;Drd2-Egfp or Slc6a3-ires-Cre;Drd1a-tdTomato mice with Ai32 mice (stock # 012569; Jackson Labs) (Madisen et al., 2012). The knock-in lines used in Figure 5 include Gad1-Egfp (Tamamaki et al., 2003) and Gad2-ires-Cre mice (Taniguchi et al., 2011). The latter were crossed with mice bearing a Cre-dependent tdTomato reporter transgene (Ai14; stock # 007914; Jackson Labs) to reveal the distribution of Cre+ cells (Madisen et al., 2010). Mice were maintained on a C57BL/6 background. Only mice heterozygous for all transgenes were used for experiments.

Immunocytochemistry and in situ hybridization

Mice were anesthetized with isoflurane and transcardially perfused with phosphate buffered saline followed by 4% (wt/vol) paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M sodium phosphate buffer. 50-micrometer coronal brain sections were subject to immunohistochemical staining for tyrosine hydroxylase (AB152; 1:2000; Millipore, Billerica, MA), as described previously (Tritsch et al., 2012). Endogenous tdTomato and EGFP fluorescence were not immuno-enhanced. Double fluorescence in situ hybridization was performed using a tyramide signal amplification method according to the manufacturer's instructions (NEL753001KT; PerkinElmer, Waltham, MA), as previously described (Kwon et al., 2012). Briefly, brains from 4-week old mice were dissected and immediately frozen in liquid nitrogen. They were then cut in 25-μm-thick sections with a cryostat (Leica, Buffalo Grove, IL), postfixed in 4% PFA, acetylated in 1% triethanolamine and 0.25% acetic anhydride, dehydrated serially in ethanol, bleached in 3% hydrogen peroxide diluted in methanol, prehybridized, and hybridized at 65°C using the following anti-sense probes provided by the Allen Institute for Brain Science (riboprobe ID specified in parentheses): Gad1 (RP_040324_01_F01), Gad2 (RP_071018_02_B07), Vmat2 (RP_071218_02_B08), mouse Gat1 (RP_071204_04_H01), mouse Gat4 (RP_050428_04_D04), and Abat (RP_050301_02_C02). For in vitro transcription, each cDNA was cloned out from a mouse brain cDNA library using the primer sets suggested in the in situ hybridization data portal from the Allen Institute for Brain Science. Two fluorescein- or digoxigenin-labeled riboprobes generated by an in vitro transcription method (Promega, Madison, WI) were hybridized simultaneously and stained by fluorescein or Cy3 chromogens, respectively. After staining, sections were mounted with Prolong Gold antifade reagent with DAPI nuclear stain (Life Technologies, Grand Island, NY).

Slice electrophysiology

Acute brain slices and whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings from identified SPNs were obtained using standard methods, as described previously (Tritsch et al., 2012). Briefly, mice (49–178 days old; median = 107 days) were anesthetized and perfused with ice-cold artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) containing (in mM) 125 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 25 NaHCO3, 2 CaCl2, 1 MgCl2, 1.25 NaH2PO4 and 11 glucose (295 mOsm·kg−1). Parasagittal slices of striatum (275-μm thick) were subsequently obtained in cold choline-based cutting solution (in mM: 110 choline chloride, 25 NaHCO3, 2.5 KCl, 7 MgCl2, 0.5 CaCl2, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 25 glucose, 11.6 ascorbic acid, and 3.1 pyruvic acid). Following 15 min recovery in ACSF at 34°C, slices were kept at room temperature (20–22°C) until use. All solutions were constantly bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2. Whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings were established from direct- and indirect-pathway SPNs in dorsal or ventral striatum resting 18–75 μm below the slice surface (median: 40 μm) in ACSF warmed to 32–34°C. Direct-pathway SPNs were identified as tdTomato+ cells in Drd1a-tdTomato mice or EGFPcells in Drd2-EGFP mice, and indirect-pathway SPNs as tdTomatoor EGFP+ cells in Drd1a-tdTomato and Drd2-EGFP mice, respectively. Patch pipettes (2–4 MΩ) were filled with (in mM) 125 CsCl, 10 TEA-Cl, 10 HEPES, 0.1 Cs-EGTA, 3.3 QX-314 (Clsalt), 4 Mg-ATP, 0.3 Na-GTP, 8 Na2-Phosphocreatine (pH 7.3 adjusted with CsOH; 295 mOsm·kg−1). The recording perfusate always contained NBQX (10 μM) and R-CPP (10 μM) to block AMPA and NMDA receptor-mediated inward currents, respectively, as well as CGP55845 (2–5 μM) to prevent GABAB receptor-evoked pre- and post-synaptic modulation. For some experiments (Figures 6 and 8, Figure 8—figure supplements 1 and 2), slices were incubated for at least 30 min (and up to two hours) in ACSF containing CGP55845 (2–5 μM) in addition to vigabatrin (100 μM), muscimol (0.1 μM), or SKF 89976A (10 μM) + SNAP-5114 (50 μM) to allow for the depletion of cytosolic GABA levels and synaptic vesicles containing GABA, and to control for the effects of chronically elevated GABAergic signaling. In these cases, drugs continued to be present in the recording chamber for the duration of the experiment. For Figure 8E, GABA (100 μM, in ACSF containing either 5 μM CGP55845 or 5 μM CGP55845 + 10 μM SKF 89976A + 50 μM SNAP-5114) was locally applied to the slice via a gravity-fed 250-μm-wide flow pipe for 5–10 min prior to recording. For all voltage-clamp experiments, errors due to the voltage drop across the series resistance (<20 MΩ) were left uncompensated. Membrane potentials were corrected for a ∼5-mV liquid junction potential. Under these conditions, GABAA receptor-mediated currents appeared inward when SPNs were held at negative membrane potentials (Vhold = −70 mV). To activate ChR2-expressing fibers, light from a 473-nm laser (Optoengine, Midvale, UT) was focused on the back aperture of the microscope objective to produce wide-field illumination of the recorded cell. Brief pulses of light (1-ms duration; 5 mW·mm−2 under the objective for maximal stimulation, 0.3–2 mW·mm−2, for sub-maximal stimulation) were delivered at the recording site at 30 s intervals under control of the acquisition software. Epifluorescence illumination was used sparingly to minimize ChR2 activation prior to recording. eIPSCs were evoked using constant-current pulses (0.1 ms, 7–90 μA) delivered every 15 s through a bipolar tungsten stimulating electrodes positioned within striatum, 100–200 μm away from the recorded cell. Constant-potential amperometry was performed as before (Tritsch et al., 2012) using commercial glass-encased carbon-fiber microelectrodes (Carbostar-1, Kation Scientific, Minneapolis, MN) placed within dorsal striatum slices and held at 600 mV. All recordings were obtained within 4 hr of slicing. All pharmacological agents were obtained from Tocris (Minneapolis, MN).

Data acquisition and analysis

Brain sections processed for in situ hybridization or immunofluorescence were imaged with an Olympus VS110 slide-scanning microscope. High-resolution images of regions of interest were subsequently acquired with a Zeiss LSM 510 META confocal microscope (Harvard NeuroDiscovery Center). Individual imaging planes were overlaid and quantified for colocalization in ImageJ (NIH) and thresholded for display in Photoshop (Adobe). Confocal images in figures represent maximum intensity projections of 3-µm confocal stacks. Membrane currents were amplified and low-pass filtered at 3 kHz using a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices, Sunnyvale, CA), digitized at 10 kHz and acquired using National Instruments acquisition boards and a custom version of ScanImage (Pologruto et al., 2003; available upon request or from https://openwiki.janelia.org/wiki/display/ephus/ScanImage) written in MATLAB (Mathworks). Electrophysiology data were analyzed offline using Igor Pro (Wavemetrics, Natick, MA). In figures, voltage-clamp traces represent the averaged waveform of 3–5 consecutive acquisitions. Averaged waveforms were used to obtain current latency, peak amplitude, 10–90% rise time and decay time constant. The latter was estimated by measuring the time elapsed from the peak of the IPSC to 1/e (36.8%) of the peak amplitude. Detection threshold for sIPSCs was set at 40 pA to facilitate event detection in the presence of large and noisy tonic GABA currents. For pharmacological analyses in Figures 2 and 3, the peak amplitude of IPSCs measured 3–4 min following the onset of drug perfusion were averaged, normalized to baseline averages obtained immediately prior to drug application and compared statistically to values obtained at corresponding times in control preparations bathed in ACSF. Data (reported in text and figures as mean ± SEM) were compared using Prism 6 (GraphPad, La Jolla, CA) with the following non-parametric statistical tests (as indicated in the text): Mann–Whitney for group comparisons, Wilcoxon signed-rank for paired comparisons, and Kruskal–Wallis analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by Dunn's Multiple Comparison Test for multiple group comparisons. In experiments characterizing the time course of synaptic transmission rundown, two-way ANOVAs were used followed by the Sidak's and Tukey's multiple comparison tests for comparisons between two and more conditions, respectively. p values smaller than 0.05 were considered statistically significant. N values represent the number of recorded cells. For most experiments, a single cell was recorded from in each slice, with each animal contributing fewer than five recordings to individual data sets.

Acknowledgements

We thank R Shah, N Mulder, and R Pemberton for technical support and members of the Sabatini laboratory for discussions throughout the course of this study.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institutes of Health FundRef identification ID: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100000002 NS046579 to Bernardo L Sabatini.

  • Nancy Lurie Marks Family Foundation to Nicolas X Tritsch.

  • Lefler Postdoctoral Fellowship to Won-Jong Oh.

  • Alice and Joseph Brooks Fund to Won-Jong Oh.

  • Howard Hughes Medical Institute FundRef identification ID: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100000011 to Bernardo L Sabatini.

  • National Institutes of Health FundRef identification ID: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100000002 NS064583 to Chenghua Gu.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

NXT, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

BLS, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

W-JO, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data.

CG, Conception and design, Drafting or revising the article, Contributed unpublished essential data or reagents.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: This study was performed in strict accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health. All experimental manipulations were performed in accordance with protocols approved by the Harvard Medical Area Standing Committee on Animal Care (#03551).

References

  1. Ade KK, Janssen MJ, Ortinski PI, Vicini S. 2008. Differential tonic GABA conductances in striatal medium spiny neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 28:1185–1197. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3908-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ade KK, Wan Y, Chen M, Gloss B, Calakos N. 2011. An improved BAC transgenic fluorescent reporter line for sensitive and specific identification of Striatonigral medium spiny neurons. Frontiers in System Neuroscience 5:32. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Apostolides PF, Trussell LO. 2013. Rapid, activity-independent turnover of vesicular transmitter content at a mixed glycine/GABA synapse. The Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 33:4768–4781. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5555-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Arbuthnott GW, Wickens J. 2007. Space, time and dopamine. Trends in Neurosciences 30:62–69. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Asada H, Kawamura Y, Maruyama K, Kume H, Ding RG, Kanbara N, Kuzume H, Sanbo M, Yagi T, Obata K. 1997. Cleft palate and decreased brain gamma-aminobutyric acid in mice lacking the 67-kDa isoform of glutamic acid decarboxylase. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 94:6496–6499. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.12.6496 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Backman CM, Malik N, Zhang Y, Shan L, Grinberg A, Hoffer BJ, Westphal H, Tomac AC. 2006. Characterization of a mouse strain expressing Cre recombinase from the 3' untranslated region of the dopamine transporter locus. Genesis: the Journal of Genetics and Development 44:383–390 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bak LK, Schousboe A, Waagepetersen HS. 2006. The glutamate/GABA-glutamine cycle: aspects of transport, neurotransmitter homeostasis and ammonia transfer. Journal of Neurochemistry 98:641–653. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2006.03913.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Balazs R, Machiyama Y, Hammond BJ, Julian T, Richter D. 1970. The operation of the gamma-aminobutyrate bypath of the tricarboxylic acid cycle in brain tissue in vitro. The Biochemical Journal 116:445–461 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Belelli D, Lambert JJ. 2005. Neurosteroids: endogenous regulators of the GABA(A) receptor. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6:565–575. doi: 10.1038/nrn1703 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bengel D, Murphy DL, Andrews AM, Wichems CH, Feltner D, Heils A, Mossner R, Westphal H, Lesch KP. 1998. Altered brain serotonin homeostasis and locomotor insensitivity to 3, 4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (“Ecstasy”) in serotonin transporter-deficient mice. Molecular Pharmacology 53:649–655 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bessman SP, Rossen J, Layne EC. 1953. Gamma-Aminobutyric acid-glutamic acid transamination in brain. The Journal of Biological Chemistry 201:385–391 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bjorklund A, Dunnett SB. 2007. Dopamine neuron systems in the brain: an update. Trends in Neurosciences 30:194–202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Borden LA. 1996. GABA transporter heterogeneity: pharmacology and cellular localization. Neurochemistry International 29:335–356 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Borisovska M, Bensen AL, Chong G, Westbrook GL. 2013. Distinct modes of dopamine and GABA release in a dual transmitter neuron. The Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 33:1790–1796. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4342-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bragina L, Marchionni I, Omrani A, Cozzi A, Pellegrini-Giampietro DE, Cherubini E, Conti F. 2008. GAT-1 regulates both tonic and phasic GABA(A) receptor-mediated inhibition in the cerebral cortex. Journal of Neurochemistry 105:1781–1793. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2008.05273.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Brown MN, Mathews GC. 2010. Activity- and age-dependent modulation of GABAergic neurotransmission by system A-mediated glutamine uptake. Journal of Neurochemistry 114:909–920. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.06823.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Caron PC, Kremzner LT, Cote LJ. 1987. GABA and its relationship to putrescine metabolism in the rat brain and pancreas. Neurochemistry International 10:219–229 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cepeda C, Galvan L, Holley SM, Rao SP, André VM, Botelho EP, Chen JY, Watson JB, Deisseroth K, Levine MS. 2013. Multiple sources of striatal inhibition are differentially affected in Huntington's disease mouse models. The Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 33:7393–7406. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2137-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Chiu CS, Brickley S, Jensen K, Southwell A, McKinney S, Cull-Candy S, Mody I, Lester HA. 2005. GABA transporter deficiency causes tremor, ataxia, nervousness, and increased GABA-induced tonic conductance in cerebellum. The Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 25:3234–3245. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3364-04.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Conti F, Melone M, Fattorini G, Bragina L, Ciappelloni S. 2011. A role for GAT-1 in presynaptic GABA homeostasis? Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience 5:2. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2011.00002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Conti F, Minelli A, Melone M. 2004. GABA transporters in the mammalian cerebral cortex: localization, development and pathological implications. Brain Research Brain Research Reviews 45:196–212. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2004.03.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Costa RM. 2007. Plastic corticostriatal circuits for action learning: what's dopamine got to do with it? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1104:172–191. doi: 10.1196/annals.1390.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Drasbek KR, Jensen K. 2006. THIP, a hypnotic and antinociceptive drug, enhances an extrasynaptic GABAA receptor-mediated conductance in mouse neocortex. Cerebral Cortex 16:1134–1141. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Edwards RH. 2007. The neurotransmitter cycle and quantal size. Neuron 55:835–858. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Esclapez M, Tillakaratne NJ, Tobin AJ, Houser CR. 1993. Comparative localization of mRNAs encoding two forms of glutamic acid decarboxylase with nonradioactive in situ hybridization methods. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 331:339–362. doi: 10.1002/cne.903310305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fricke MN, Jones-Davis DM, Mathews GC. 2007. Glutamine uptake by System A transporters maintains neurotransmitter GABA synthesis and inhibitory synaptic transmission. Journal of Neurochemistry 102:1895–1904. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2007.04649.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gasnier B. 2000. The loading of neurotransmitters into synaptic vesicles. Biochimie 82:327–337. doi: 10.1016/S0300-9084(00)00221-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Gerfen CR, Surmeier DJ. 2011. Modulation of striatal projection systems by dopamine. Annual Review of Neuroscience 34:441–466. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113641 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Giros B, Jaber M, Jones SR, Wightman RM, Caron MG. 1996. Hyperlocomotion and indifference to cocaine and amphetamine in mice lacking the dopamine transporter. Nature 379:606–612. doi: 10.1038/379606a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gomeza J, Hulsmann S, Ohno K, Eulenburg V, Szoke K, Richter D, Betz H. 2003a. Inactivation of the glycine transporter 1 gene discloses vital role of glial glycine uptake in glycinergic inhibition. Neuron 40:785–796. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00672-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gomeza J, Ohno K, Hulsmann S, Armsen W, Eulenburg V, Richter DW, Laube B, Betz H. 2003b. Deletion of the mouse glycine transporter 2 results in a hyperekplexia phenotype and postnatal lethality. Neuron 40:797–806. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00673-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gong S, Zheng C, Doughty ML, Losos K, Didkovsky N, Schambra UB, Nowak NJ, Joyner A, Leblanc G, Hatten ME, Heintz N. 2003. A gene expression atlas of the central nervous system based on bacterial artificial chromosomes. Nature 425:917–925. doi: 10.1038/nature02033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gonzalez-Hernandez T, Barroso-Chinea P, Acevedo A, Salido E, Rodriguez M. 2001. Colocalization of tyrosine hydroxylase and GAD65 mRNA in mesostriatal neurons. The European Journal of Neuroscience 13:57–67. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2001.01371.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gonzalez-Hernandez T, Barroso-Chinea P, Rodriguez M. 2004. Response of the GABAergic and dopaminergic mesostriatal projections to the lesion of the contralateral dopaminergic mesostriatal pathway in the rat. Movement Disorders: official Journal of the Movement Disorder Society 19:1029–1042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hirasawa H, Betensky RA, Raviola E. 2012. Corelease of dopamine and GABA by a retinal dopaminergic neuron. The Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 32:13281–13291. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2213-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hnasko TS, Chuhma N, Zhang H, Goh GY, Sulzer D, Palmiter RD, Rayport S, Edwards RH. 2010. Vesicular glutamate transport promotes dopamine storage and glutamate corelease in vivo. Neuron 65:643–656. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hnasko TS, Edwards RH. 2011. Neurotransmitter corelease: mechanism and physiological role. Annual Review of Physiology 74:225–243. doi: 10.1146/annurev-physiol-020911-153315 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Isaacson JS, Solis JM, Nicoll RA. 1993. Local and diffuse synaptic actions of GABA in the hippocampus. Neuron 10:165–175. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(93)90308-E [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Ishikawa M, Otaka M, Huang YH, Neumann PA, Winters BD, Grace AA, Schluter OM, Dong Y. 2013. Dopamine triggers heterosynaptic plasticity. The Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 33:6759–6765. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4694-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Janssen MJ, Ade KK, Fu Z, Vicini S. 2009. Dopamine modulation of GABA tonic conductance in striatal output neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 29:5116–5126. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4737-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Jensen K, Chiu CS, Sokolova I, Lester HA, Mody I. 2003. GABA transporter-1 (GAT1)-deficient mice: differential tonic activation of GABAA versus GABAB receptors in the hippocampus. Journal of Neurophysiology 90:2690–2701. doi: 10.1152/jn.00240.2003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Johnston GA. 1996. GABAA receptor pharmacology. Pharmacology & Therapeutics 69:173–198. doi: 10.1016/0163-7258(95)02043-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Johnston GA, Kennedy SM, Lodge D. 1978. Muscimol uptake, release and binding in rat brain slices. Journal of Neurochemistry 31:1519–1523. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1978.tb06579.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Jones MV, Sahara Y, Dzubay JA, Westbrook GL. 1998a. Defining affinity with the GABAA receptor. The Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 18:8590–8604 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Jones SR, Gainetdinov RR, Jaber M, Giros B, Wightman RM, Caron MG. 1998b. Profound neuronal plasticity in response to inactivation of the dopamine transporter. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 95:4029–4034. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.7.4029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Juge N, Omote H, Moriyama Y. 2013. Vesicular GABA transporter (VGAT) transports beta-alanine. Journal of Neurochemistry 127:482–486. doi: 10.1111/jnc.12393 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kirmse K, Dvorzhak A, Kirischuk S, Grantyn R. 2008. GABA transporter 1 tunes GABAergic synaptic transmission at output neurons of the mouse neostriatum. The Journal of Physiology 586:5665–5678. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2008.161943 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kwon HB, Kozorovitskiy Y, Oh WJ, Peixoto RT, Akhtar N, Saulnier JL, Gu C, Sabatini BL. 2012. Neuroligin-1-dependent competition regulates cortical synaptogenesis and synapse number. Nature Neuroscience 15:1667–1674. doi: 10.1038/nn.3256 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Lammel S, Ion DI, Roeper J, Malenka RC. 2011. Projection-specific modulation of dopamine neuron synapses by aversive and rewarding stimuli. Neuron 70:855–862. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lammel S, Lim BK, Ran C, Huang KW, Betley MJ, Tye KM, Deisseroth K, Malenka RC. 2012. Input-specific control of reward and aversion in the ventral tegmental area. Nature 491:212–217. doi: 10.1038/nature11527 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Lebrand C, Cases O, Adelbrecht C, Doye A, Alvarez C, El Mestikawy S, Seif I, Gaspar P. 1996. Transient uptake and storage of serotonin in developing thalamic neurons. Neuron 17:823–835. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80215-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Lein ES, Hawrylycz MJ, Ao N, Ayres M, Bensinger A, Bernard A, Boe AF, Boguski MS, Brockway KS, Byrnes EJ, Chen L, Chen L, Chen TM, Chin MC, Chong J, Crook BE, Czaplinska A, Dang CN, Datta S, Dee NR, Desaki AL, Desta T, Diep E, Dolbeare TA, Donelan MJ, Dong HW, Dougherty JG, Duncan BJ, Ebbert AJ, Eichele G, Estin LK, Faber C, Facer BA, Fields R, Fischer SR, Fliss TP, Frensley C, Gates SN, Glattfelder KJ, Halverson KR, Hart MR, Hohmann JG, Howell MP, Jeung DP, Johnson RA, Karr PT, Kawal R, Kidney JM, Knapik RH, Kuan CL, Lake JH, Laramee AR, Larsen KD, Lau C, Lemon TA, Liang AJ, Liu Y, Luong LT, Michaels J, Morgan JJ, Morgan RJ, Mortrud MT, Mosqueda NF, Ng LL, Ng R, Orta GJ, Overly CC, Pak TH, Parry SE, Pathak SD, Pearson OC, Puchalski RB, Riley ZL, Rockett HR, Rowland SA, Royall JJ, Ruiz MJ, Sarno NR, Schaffnit K, Shapovalova NV, Sivisay T, Slaughterbeck CR, Smith SC, Smith KA, Smith BI, Sodt AJ, Stewart NN, Stumpf KR, Sunkin SM, Sutram M, Tam A, Teemer CD, Thaller C, Thompson CL, Varnam LR, Visel A, Whitlock RM, Wohnoutka PE, Wolkey CK, Wong VY, Wood M, Yaylaoglu MB, Young RC, Youngstrom BL, Yuan XF, Zhang B, Zwingman TA, Jones AR. 2007. Genome-wide atlas of gene expression in the adult mouse brain. Nature 445:168–176. doi: 10.1038/nature05453 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Lin CS, Nicolelis MA, Schneider JS, Chapin JK. 1990. A major direct GABAergic pathway from zona incerta to neocortex. Science 248:1553–1556. doi: 10.1126/science.2360049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Liu S, Plachez C, Shao Z, Puche A, Shipley MT. 2013. Olfactory bulb short axon cell release of GABA and dopamine produces a temporally biphasic inhibition-excitation response in external tufted cells. The Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 33:2916–2926. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3607-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Madisen L, Mao T, Koch H, Zhuo JM, Berenyi A, Fujisawa S, Hsu YW, Garcia AJ, III, Gu X, Zanella S, Kidney J, Gu H, Mao Y, Hooks BM, Boyden ES, Buzsáki G, Ramirez JM, Jones AR, Svoboda K, Han X, Turner EE, Zeng H. 2012. A toolbox of Cre-dependent optogenetic transgenic mice for light-induced activation and silencing. Nature Neuroscience 15:793–802. doi: 10.1038/nn.3078 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Madisen L, Zwingman TA, Sunkin SM, Oh SW, Zariwala HA, Gu H, Ng LL, Palmiter RD, Hawrylycz MJ, Jones AR, Lein ES, Zeng H. 2010. A robust and high-throughput Cre reporting and characterization system for the whole mouse brain. Nature Neuroscience 13:133–140. doi: 10.1038/nn.2467 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Maher BJ, Westbrook GL. 2008. Co-transmission of dopamine and GABA in periglomerular cells. Journal of Neurophysiology 99:1559–1564. doi: 10.1152/jn.00636.2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Mathews GC, Diamond JS. 2003. Neuronal glutamate uptake Contributes to GABA synthesis and inhibitory synaptic strength. The Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 23:2040–2048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Matsumoto M, Hikosaka O. 2009. Two types of dopamine neuron distinctly convey positive and negative motivational signals. Nature 459:837–841. doi: 10.1038/nature08028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Medina-Kauwe LK, Tillakaratne NJ, Wu JY, Tobin AJ. 1994. A rat brain cDNA encodes enzymatically active GABA transaminase and provides a molecular probe for GABA-catabolizing cells. Journal of Neurochemistry 62:1267–1275. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1994.62041267.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Morris G, Schmidt R, Bergman H. 2009. Striatal action-learning based on dopamine concentration. Experimental Brain Research 200:307–317. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2060-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Mortensen M, Ebert B, Wafford K, Smart TG. 2010. Distinct activities of GABA agonists at synaptic- and extrasynaptic-type GABAA receptors. The Journal of Physiology 588:1251–1268. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.182444 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Overstreet LS, Westbrook GL. 2001. Paradoxical reduction of synaptic inhibition by vigabatrin. Journal of Neurophysiology 86:596–603 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Overstreet LS, Westbrook GL. 2003. Synapse density regulates independence at unitary inhibitory synapses. The Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 23:2618–2626 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Petroff OA. 2002. GABA and glutamate in the human brain. The Neuroscientist: a Review Journal Bringing Neurobiology, Neurology and Psychiatry 8:562–573. doi: 10.1177/1073858402238515 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Pologruto TA, Sabatini BL, Svoboda K. 2003. ScanImage: flexible software for operating laser scanning microscopes. Biomedical Engineering Online 2:13. doi: 10.1186/1475-925X-2-13 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Redgrave P, Rodriguez M, Smith Y, Rodriguez-Oroz MC, Lehericy S, Bergman H, Agid Y, DeLong MR, Obeso JA. 2010. Goal-directed and habitual control in the basal ganglia: implications for Parkinson's disease. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11:760–772. doi: 10.1038/nrn2915 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Roberts E, Bregoff HM. 1953. Transamination of gamma-aminobutyric acid and beta-alanine in brain and liver. The Journal of Biological Chemistry 201:393–398 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Roeper J. 2013. Dissecting the diversity of midbrain dopamine neurons. Trends in Neurosciences 36:336–342. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.03.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Rousseau F, Aubrey KR, Supplisson S. 2008. The glycine transporter GlyT2 controls the dynamics of synaptic vesicle refilling in inhibitory spinal cord neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 28:9755–9768. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0509-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Santhakumar V, Jones RT, Mody I. 2010. Developmental regulation and neuroprotective effects of striatal tonic GABAA currents. Neuroscience 167:644–655. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.02.048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Schmitz Y, Benoit-Marand M, Gonon F, Sulzer D. 2003. Presynaptic regulation of dopaminergic neurotransmission. Journal of Neurochemistry 87:273–289. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2003.02050.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Schultz W. 2007. Multiple dopamine functions at different time courses. Annual Review of Neuroscience 30:259–288. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135722 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Seiler N. 1980. On the role of GABA in vertebrate polyamine metabolism. Physiological Chemistry and Physics 12:411–429 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Soghomonian JJ, Martin DL. 1998. Two isoforms of glutamate decarboxylase: why? Trends in Pharmacological Sciences 19:500–505. doi: 10.1016/S0165-6147(98)01270-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Stamatakis AM, Jennings JH, Ung RL, Blair GA, Weinberg RJ, Neve RL, Boyce F, Mattis J, Ramakrishnan C, Deisseroth K, Stuber GD. 2013. A unique population of ventral tegmental area neurons inhibits the lateral Habenula to promote reward. Neuron 80:1039–1053. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Stensrud MJ, Puchades M, Gundersen V. 2013. GABA is localized in dopaminergic synaptic vesicles in the rodent striatum. Brain Structure & Function doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0609-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Stuber GD, Hnasko TS, Britt JP, Edwards RH, Bonci A. 2010. Dopaminergic terminals in the nucleus accumbens but not the dorsal striatum corelease glutamate. The Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 30:8229–8233. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1754-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Tamamaki N, Yanagawa Y, Tomioka R, Miyazaki J, Obata K, Kaneko T. 2003. Green fluorescent protein expression and colocalization with calretinin, parvalbumin, and somatostatin in the GAD67-GFP knock-in mouse. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 467:60–79. doi: 10.1002/cne.10905 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Tamura S, Nelson H, Tamura A, Nelson N. 1995. Short external loops as potential substrate binding site of gamma-aminobutyric acid transporters. The Journal of Biological Chemistry 270:28712–28715 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Taniguchi H, He M, Wu P, Kim S, Paik R, Sugino K, Kvitsiani D, Fu Y, Lu J, Lin Y, Miyoshi G, Shima Y, Fishell G, Nelson SB, Huang ZJ. 2011. A resource of Cre driver lines for genetic targeting of GABAergic neurons in cerebral cortex. Neuron 71:995–1013. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.07.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Tecuapetla F, Patel JC, Xenias H, English D, Tadros I, Shah F, Berlin J, Deisseroth K, Rice ME, Tepper JM, Koos T. 2010. Glutamatergic signaling by mesolimbic dopamine neurons in the nucleus accumbens. The Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 30:7105–7110. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0265-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Tian N, Petersen C, Kash S, Baekkeskov S, Copenhagen D, Nicoll R. 1999. The role of the synthetic enzyme GAD65 in the control of neuronal gamma-aminobutyric acid release. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 96:12911–12916. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.22.12911 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Torres GE, Gainetdinov RR, Caron MG. 2003. Plasma membrane monoamine transporters: structure, regulation and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4:13–25. doi: 10.1038/nrn1008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Tritsch NX, Ding JB, Sabatini BL. 2012. Dopaminergic neurons inhibit striatal output through non-canonical release of GABA. Nature 490:262–266. doi: 10.1038/nature11466 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Tritsch NX, Sabatini BL. 2012. Dopaminergic modulation of synaptic transmission in cortex and striatum. Neuron 76:33–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Wang L, Tu P, Bonet L, Aubrey KR, Supplisson S. 2013. Cytosolic transmitter concentration regulates vesicle cycling at hippocampal GABAergic terminals. Neuron 80:143–158. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Watabe-Uchida M, Zhu L, Ogawa SK, Vamanrao A, Uchida N. 2012. Whole-brain mapping of direct inputs to midbrain dopamine neurons. Neuron 74:858–873. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Weitlauf C, Woodward JJ. 2008. Ethanol selectively attenuates NMDAR-mediated synaptic transmission in the prefrontal cortex. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research 32:690–698. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00625.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Wickens J, Arbuthnott G. 2005. Structural and functional interactions in the striatum at the receptor level. In: Dunnett SB, et al., editors. Dopamine. San Diego: Elsevier; vol 21 p. 199–236 [Google Scholar]
  91. Wise RA. 2004. Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5:483–494. doi: 10.1038/nrn1406 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Wojcik SM, Katsurabayashi S, Guillemin I, Friauf E, Rosenmund C, Brose N, Rhee JS. 2006. A shared vesicular carrier allows synaptic corelease of GABA and glycine. Neuron 50:575–587. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Xu F, Gainetdinov RR, Wetsel WC, Jones SR, Bohn LM, Miller GW, Wang YM, Caron MG. 2000. Mice lacking the norepinephrine transporter are supersensitive to psychostimulants. Nature Neuroscience 3:465–471. doi: 10.1038/74839 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Yelin R, Schuldiner S. 1995. The pharmacological profile of the vesicular monoamine transporter resembles that of multidrug transporters. FEBS Letters 377:201–207. doi: 10.1016/0014-5793(95)01346-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Zhou FM, Liang Y, Salas R, Zhang L, De Biasi M, Dani JA. 2005. Corelease of dopamine and serotonin from striatal dopamine terminals. Neuron 46:65–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2014 Apr 24;3:e01936. doi: 10.7554/eLife.01936.016

Decision letter

Editor: Gary L Westbrook1

eLife posts the editorial decision letter and author response on a selection of the published articles (subject to the approval of the authors). An edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the substantive concerns or comments; minor concerns are not usually shown. Reviewers have the opportunity to discuss the decision before the letter is sent (see review process). Similarly, the author response typically shows only responses to the major concerns raised by the reviewers.

Thank you for sending your work entitled “Midbrain dopamine neurons sustain inhibitory transmission using plasma membrane uptake of GABA, not synthesis” for consideration at eLife. Your article has been favorably evaluated by a Senior editor and 3 reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors. The Reviewing editor and the other reviewers discussed their comments before reaching this decision, and the Reviewing editor has assembled the following comments to help you prepare a revised submission.

This relatively straightforward report follows up on prior work of these authors examining the corelease of GABA from “dopamine” neurons. In this report they extend their studies from the nigrostriatal pathway to the mesolimbic pathway. Interestingly GABA is supplied not by synthetic enzymes in the neurons, but rather by expression of membrane GABA transporters. As previously shown, vesicular uptake of GABA can be performed by the vesicular monamine transporter VMAT2. The questions addressed in this manuscript are whether such corelease occurs in the mesolimbic pathway and whether the agonist is GABA or a related analog.

The authors nicely demonstrate in Figure 1 that VTA inputs to NAc have similar properties with respect to GABA corelease as the previously demonstrated SNc inputs to dorsal striatum. The majority of the figures then focus on the identity and source of GABA in the VTA-NAc pathway. These experiments are carefully conducted and well presented and convincingly demonstrate that GABA is the principal agonist and that the main source is membrane uptake rather than intracellular synthesis. Although these results are somewhat predictable from prior studies and could be further extended, the results are important in understanding the rather novel, albeit still somewhat mysterious, role of a fast-acting transmitter in midbrain dopamine neurons, and thus should be of interest to a broad community of neuroscientists. The substantial comments of the 3 reviewers are numbered below.

1) These data make several specific, testable predictions that would enhance the strength of the main interpretation. For example, increasing extracellular GABA should increase the amplitude of DA-IPSCs through an mGAT-dependent mechanism. Does bath application of GABA, or perhaps extracellular GABA uncaging, increase DA-IPSCs? And is this effect blocked by the acute presence of mGAT blockers during said GABA application? Wang et al. 2013 (cited) showed that GAT activity can rescue transmission in conventional GABAergic synapses when GABA is bath applied in the presence of gabazine. This experiment would reveal the timecourse over which DA neurons take up GABA from the extracellular space and package it into synaptic vesicles, and it would be important to contrast the timecourse of the authors' novel mechanism with that of enzymatic synthesis at more traditional GABA synapses. This experiment would enhance also the claim that GAT supplies the GABA used for synaptic vesicles.

2) A second prediction is that rundown of transmission induced by SKF should be activity dependent. I found it confusing at first that Figure 2 shows little effect of SKF on IPSC amplitude and Figure 7 shows a large effect, and I am sure readers will also be caught off guard. The difference is that with time the synapses in SKF are depleted of GABA. Therefore if the synapses were stimulated one might expect to accelerate the process, confirming that it reflects depletion of vesicular GABA rather than a side effect of the drug.

3) The authors deal with a number of potential confounding factors such as the increased tonic GABA currents produced by GABA transporter inhibition, showing that this does not affect the kinetics of quantal GABA events from sources other than dopamine neurons. On the other hand, it is remarkable that the inhibitor does not affect the kinetics of these other events – is it so selective for GABA release from dopamine neurons because the transporter is only expressed on those terminals? It would be helpful if the authors could address this surprising result – previous work has indicated expression of GAT-1 by SPNs and an effect of a different inhibitor (NO-711) on amplitude and decay time of events evoked by local depolarization (Kirmse et al. 2008).

4) Treatment with GAT-1 inhibitor for longer times effectively eliminates the GABA release by dopamine neurons, as anticipated if it depends on the GABA taken up from other cells. However, it is also possible that dopamine neurons make GABA themselves, and the main argument used here against this is the apparent lack of expression by a variety of criteria. Although these seem reasonable, other work cited by the authors in a different context (Stensrud et al. 2013) along with other work they do cite suggests expression of GAD by a subset of dopamine neurons. Rather than rely on the anatomy, it would help greatly to demonstrate that inactivation of GAD specifically in dopamine neurons does not eliminate the dopaminergic IPSCs. This might require a substantial amount of work that would lie outside the scope of this manuscript (for example a double knockout of both isoforms that is conditional and may not even be available), but it would certainly be helpful. However, showing that expression of a GAD isoform in dopamine neurons makes the dopaminergic IPSCs resistant to inhibition of GAT-1 would be considerably simpler.

5) The authors also suggest that GABA corelease involves both mesostriatal and mesolimbic projections, in contrast to the apparent corelease of glutamate by a subset of VTA neurons. However, Stensrud et al. (2013) also showed that only ∼10 % of striatal terminals apparently contain GABA, raising the question whether the dopaminergic IPSCs derive from a subset of both populations, or all of them. It would be great if the authors could address this point, and one way might be to determine whether GAT-1 inhibition influences dopamine release measured by voltammetry-GABA corelease by many terminals should have some effect on total dopamine release, but involvement of only a subset would produce minimal if any effect. This would also help to address one important functional outcome of corelease that has not yet been examined.

eLife. 2014 Apr 24;3:e01936. doi: 10.7554/eLife.01936.017

Author response


We are thankful for the reviewers’ prompt evaluation of our manuscript and for their valuable comments and suggestions. We are delighted that our work was reviewed favorably and described as “straightforward”, “carefully conducted” and “well-presented”. We agree with the reviewers that the results described in our study are an important and necessary first step to elucidate the mysterious role of fast-acting neurotransmission from midbrain dopamine neurons. However, we take exception with the notion that the principal findings of this study were in any way predictable from prior work. Although our initial study (Tritsch et al. 2012) pointed to GABA as being the transmitter released by dopamine neurons, we acknowledged that other molecules might perform similar functions, and remained agnostic as to the identity of the co-released transmitter as we initiated and carried out the present study. This was particularly true when we failed to detect GABA synthetic enzymes in midbrain dopamine neurons. Moreover, we are not aware of any prior evidence that might have predicted that midbrain dopamine neurons acquire GABA through membrane uptake as opposed to intracellular synthesis.

Please find below our response to each of the reviewer’s comments. We hope the reviewers will find our revised manuscript improved and suitable for publication.

1) These data make several specific, testable predictions that would enhance the strength of the main interpretation. For example, increasing extracellular GABA should increase the amplitude of DA-IPSCs through an mGAT-dependent mechanism. Does bath application of GABA, or perhaps extracellular GABA uncaging, increase DA-IPSCs? And is this effect blocked by the acute presence of mGAT blockers during said GABA application? Wang et al. 2013 (cited) showed that GAT activity can rescue transmission in conventional GABAergic synapses when GABA is bath applied in the presence of gabazine. This experiment would reveal the timecourse over which DA neurons take up GABA from the extracellular space and package it into synaptic vesicles, and it would be important to contrast the timecourse of the authors' novel mechanism with that of enzymatic synthesis at more traditional GABA synapses. This experiment would enhance also the claim that GAT supplies the GABA used for synaptic vesicles.

We performed the experiment suggested by the reviewers, in which we show that bath application of GABA increases the amplitude of IPSCs evoked by dopaminergic neuron stimulation in a GAT-dependent manner. These data are consistent with the claim that GABA is the transmitter released, and that it is supplied by GATs for vesicular release. These results are now shown in Figure 8, and discussed in the text.

Technical limitations unfortunately prevented us from performing the second series of experiments, which would have allowed us to contrast the time course of GABA uptake and vesicular filling in dopamine neurons with that of ‘classical’ GABAergic synapses that depend on GABA synthesis. The studies by Wang et al. (2013) were performed in culture and the authors benefited from stable IPSC amplitudes, as well as the ability to rapidly apply and remove extracellular solutions containing GABA, gabazine and SKF89976A. In our experiments in which, the axons are, by necessity, severed from the cell bodies, the amplitude of dopaminergic IPSCs runs down considerably with time (see discussion below and Figure 1–figure supplement 1) and complete washout of gabazine is slow (tens of minutes) and variable in duration, effectively preventing us from carrying out similar analyses. It is also unclear that we would be able to assign differences in time course to enzymatic synthesis vs. membrane uptake, as classical GABAergic synapses and this one also rely on distinct vesicular transporters (VGAT and VMAT2, respectively), which most certainly differ in transport kinetics.

2) A second prediction is that rundown of transmission induced by SKF should be activity dependent. I found it confusing at first that Figure 2 shows little effect of SKF on IPSC amplitude and Figure 7 shows a large effect, and I am sure readers will also be caught off guard. The difference is that with time the synapses in SKF are depleted of GABA. Therefore if the synapses were stimulated one might expect to accelerate the process, confirming that it reflects depletion of vesicular GABA rather than a side effect of the drug.

We apologize for the confusion and thank the reviewers for pointing out the need to clarify the description of our experimental manipulations. As the reviewers indicate, the major difference between Figures 2 and 7 is the duration of drug application. Figure 2 describes the acute effects of SKF89976A within the first few minutes of bath application. Under these conditions, we did not observe any effect of SKF89976A (or SKF89976A + SNAP-5114; See Figure 1–figure supplement 1) on the amplitude of oIPSCs. This indicates that blocking membrane GABA reuptake does not immediately interfere with the ability of dopaminergic terminals to release GABA, presumably because synaptic vesicles are already filled and cytoplasmic GABA levels have not yet been depleted. In Figure 8, slices were incubated for thirty or more minutes in SKF89976A + SNAP-5114 to allow for depletion of cytoplasmic (and by extension vesicular) GABA in dopaminergic synapses. We have modified the text to stress this distinction.

Following the reviewers’ suggestion, we now provide evidence in Figure 8D that the rundown process is accelerated following chronic GAT blockade, and in Figure 8–figure supplements 1 and 2 that the effect of this manipulation on oIPSC amplitude does not reflect a side effect of the drug. Specifically, we show that, in addition to reducing the amplitude of optogenetically-evoked IPSCs, prolonged GAT blockade significantly accelerates rundown of GABAergic transmission. We did not detect differences in either the amplitude of IPSCs or the time course of rundown between slices that were incubated in SKF89976A + SNAP-5114 for less than one hour or between one and two hours, indicating that rundown is activity dependent. Importantly, preventing membrane GABA uptake for thirty minutes or longer does not prevent synaptic release of dopamine from nigrostriatal axons or GABA from striatopallidal terminals indicating that SKF89976A and SNAP-5114 do not impair synaptic transmission non-specifically. These results are therefore consistent with the hypothesis that chronic block of membrane GABA reuptake selectively inhibit GABAergic transmission at dopaminergic synapses by depleting cytosolic GABA levels.

3) The authors deal with a number of potential confounding factors such as the increased tonic GABA currents produced by GABA transporter inhibition, showing that this does not affect the kinetics of quantal GABA events from sources other than dopamine neurons. On the other hand, it is remarkable that the inhibitor does not affect the kinetics of these other events – is it so selective for GABA release from dopamine neurons because the transporter is only expressed on those terminals? It would be helpful if the authors could address this surprising result – previous work has indicated expression of GAT-1 by SPNs and an effect of a different inhibitor (NO-711) on amplitude and decay time of events evoked by local depolarization (Kirmse et al. 2008).

The different sensitivity of IPSCs originating from dopamine neurons vs. ‘classical’ GABAergic neurons to SKF89976A is indeed remarkable, pointing to fundamental structural and/or biochemical differences between both synapses. It is unlikely that selective expression of mGAT1 in dopamine neurons fully accounts for this difference, as mGAT1 is expressed widely throughout the brain, including in the striatum. However, it is possible that dopaminergic synapses rely exclusively on mGAT1 for membrane GABA transport, whereas classical GABAergic synapses may utilize other transporters in the presence of SKF89976A, such as mGAT4. Indeed, the tonic GABA current evoked by acute bath application of SKF89976A and SNAP-5114 (a mGAT4 antagonist) is more than twice that of SKF89976A alone (picrotoxin-evoked current in SKF89976A: 90 ± 9 pA, n = 15 SPNs; in SKF89976A+SNAP-5114: 198 ± 29 pA, n = 10 SPNs; P = 0.005, Mann Whitney test). Additional factors may contribute to differences in the sensitivity of IPSC kinetics to GAT blockers, including GABAA receptor subunit composition (Ade et al. 2008), GABAA receptor desensitization (Overstreet et al. 2000; Overstreet and Westbrook 2001), stimulation strength (Isaacson et al. 1993) and synapse density (Overstreet and Westbrook, 2003), amongst others.

The reviewers correctly note that another group (Kirmse et al. 2008) previously reported changes in the amplitude and kinetics of electrically-evoked IPSCs in striatum following mGAT1 inhibition. In that study, the observed decrease in IPSC amplitude was attributed to presynaptic GABAB receptors, which were pharmacologically blocked in our experiments. The effect on IPSC kinetics consisted of a slight increase (13 %) in the time taken for the current to decay to 10 % of the peak amplitude in P12-14 slices recorded at ambient temperature. It is unclear whether a significant effect would have been detected using more classical measures of decay time, or if this effect would be maintained in older animals and at elevated temperatures. Importantly, GABAergic IPSCs are particularly heterogeneous in their sensitivity to GAT blockade, which is strongly influenced by experimental conditions (Isaacson et al. 1993; Overstreet and Westbrook, 2003).

4) Treatment with GAT-1 inhibitor for longer times effectively eliminates the GABA release by dopamine neurons, as anticipated if it depends on the GABA taken up from other cells. However, it is also possible that dopamine neurons make GABA themselves, and the main argument used here against this is the apparent lack of expression by a variety of criteria. Although these seem reasonable, other work cited by the authors in a different context (Stensrud et al. 2013) along with other work they do cite suggests expression of GAD by a subset of dopamine neurons. Rather than rely on the anatomy, it would help greatly to demonstrate that inactivation of GAD specifically in dopamine neurons does not eliminate the dopaminergic IPSCs. This might require a substantial amount of work that would lie outside the scope of this manuscript (for example a double knockout of both isoforms that is conditional and may not even be available), but it would certainly be helpful. However, showing that expression of a GAD isoform in dopamine neurons makes the dopaminergic IPSCs resistant to inhibition of GAT-1 would be considerably simpler.

Stensrud et al. (2013) used immunogold EM to localize GABA within TH+ and VMAT2+ terminals in the striatum of mice and rats, but did not examine the distribution of GADs. We only know of two studies having directly investigated whether SNc and VTA dopamine neurons express GADs (Gonzalez-Hernandez et al. 2001, 2004). These studies reported that while 6-10 % of midbrain dopamine neurons in rat contain detectable levels of mRNA for GAD65, their soma is immuno- negative for GABA and GAD65, indicating that SNc/VTA neurons may not be able to synthetize GABA. Our in situ hybridization data in wildtype mice, in combination with our immuno-histochemical analyses in transgenic reporter animals are consistent with this conclusion, as both approaches convincingly showed that dopamine neurons in the SNc and lateral VTA do not express the GABA synthetic enzymes GAD65 or GAD67.

In the revised manuscript, we provide additional functional evidence for GATs being necessary for GABAergic transmission in dopaminergic neurons, and include control experiments showing that chronic inhibition of GATs does not non-specifically impair vesicular release from midbrain dopamine neurons, or GABA release from a classical GABAergic synapse. Thus, we are currently unaware of any compelling anatomical or functional evidence in favor of midbrain dopamine neurons directly synthetizing GABA for release using GADs.

Global and conditional knockout mice for GAD65 and GAD67 exist, respectively (Asada et al. 1996; Obata et al. 2008), but we currently do not have access to them. Carrying out the suggested GAD knockout experiment would require a substantial amount of work (breeding mice harboring 5 transgenic alleles: Gad1f/f; Gad2-/-; DatIRES-Cre/wt) that is not motivated by prior or current evidence for GAD expression in midbrain dopamine neurons. We therefore agree with the reviewers that this experiment is beyond the scope of this manuscript.

Although the suggested gain-of-function experiment is considerably simpler, it too requires experimental tools (viral vectors encoding GADs) that are currently not at our disposal and would take several months to generate and characterize. We agree that it would nicely demonstrate that the inhibition of oIPSCs by GAT antagonists does not stem from unintended effects of these drugs on synaptic vesicle exocytosis or GABA synthesis by GADs. However, this experiment would, by itself, offer little additional insight as to the molecular mechanism normally responsible for providing GABA to midbrain dopamine neurons. Control experiments are now provided in Figure 8–figure supplement 1, which demonstrate that blockade of GATs has no effect on dopamine release from SNc axons and no effect on GABA release from striatopallidal terminals. In light of these findings, we hope that the reviewers agree that the results of the proposed gain-of-function experiment would not affect our conclusions, and that this experiment is therefore not essential to substantiate the claim that GATs are necessary for sustaining GABAergic transmission in midbrain dopamine neurons.

5) The authors also suggest that GABA corelease involves both mesostriatal and mesolimbic projections, in contrast to the apparent corelease of glutamate by a subset of VTA neurons. However, Stensrud et al. (2013) also showed that only ∼10 % of striatal terminals apparently contain GABA, raising the question whether the dopaminergic IPSCs derive from a subset of both populations, or all of them. It would be great if the authors could address this point, and one way might be to determine whether GAT-1 inhibition influences dopamine release measured by voltammetry-GABA corelease by many terminals should have some effect on total dopamine release, but involvement of only a subset would produce minimal if any effect. This would also help to address one important functional outcome of corelease that has not yet been examined.

The study by Stensrud et al. (2013) is remarkable in that it convincingly reveals the existence dopaminergic terminals in the striatum of mice and rats that contain a high density of immunogold particles for GABA. However, immunogold EM is a challenging technique that severely underlabels molecules of interest, and therefore does not easily lend itself to absolute measures of expression: the fact that 10 % of dopaminergic terminals contained enough immunogold particles to qualify as being GABAergic cannot be interpreted to mean that 90 % do not contain GABA. Instead, their data indicate 1) that dopaminergic terminals contain the neurotransmitter GABA, 2) that GABA is present in or near VMAT+ synaptic vesicles, and 3) that the concentration of GABA is large enough to be detected in a sizeable fraction of dopaminergic terminals.

Nevertheless, the question raised by the reviewers is important, as establishing whether GABA co-release is common to all midbrain dopamine neurons or a unique feature of a subset of cells is essential to determine its functional relevance. It should be noted that regardless of the molecular mechanisms engaged or the fraction of midbrain dopamine neurons implicated, GABA co-transmission may be a general feature of monoaminergic neurons, as opposed to an exception, suggesting an important role for dopamine-GABA co-transmission (see references in this manuscript and Tritsch et al. 2012).

Determining whether optogenetically-evoked GABAergic currents arise from release in all terminals, or from a fraction of terminals is challenging, and it is unclear how the suggested electrochemical experiment would address this point. We currently do not have evidence pointing to a GABAergic modulation of dopamine exocytosis (measured by amperometry): light-evoked dopamine release is unaffected by inhibition of GABAA receptors (Tritsch et al. 2012) or by activation of GABAB receptors with baclofen (5 μM; unpublished observation). Moreover, we now provide amperometry data showing that dopamine release is not changed after chronic inhibition of GAT (Figure 8–figure supplement 1), indicating that neither buildup of extracellular GABA in the slice, nor depletion of cytosolic GABA in dopaminergic terminals significantly affect dopamine release.

The suggestion that GABA co-release in the striatum may apply to all midbrain dopamine neurons (as opposed to a subset of SNc and VTA neurons) is based on several observations. First, oIPSCs are large and present in every SPN studied to date, as opposed to glutamatergic events, which are significantly smaller in amplitude and only observed in a fraction SPNs (Tritsch et al. 2012). Second, minimum stimulation experiments suggest that oIPSCs in SPNs represent compound responses to numerous small-amplitude GABAergic inputs as opposed to a few large ones (Tritsch et al. 2012). Third, mGat1 mRNA was detected in the vast majority of midbrain dopamine neurons, suggesting that most cells have the ability to take up GABA from the extracellular space in GABA-rich brain regions such as the striatum. Although not definitive, these experiments strongly suggest that GABA co-release is a general feature of most dopaminergic terminals in the striatum.


Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

RESOURCES