Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2014 Feb 1;57(1):158–171. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0215)

Interaction of Language Processing and Motor Skill in Children with Specific Language Impairment

Andrea C DiDonato Brumbach 1, Lisa Goffman 1
PMCID: PMC4004610  NIHMSID: NIHMS571959  PMID: 24023372

Abstract

Purpose

To examine how language production interacts with speech motor and gross/fine motor skill in children with specific language impairment (SLI).

Method

Eleven children with SLI and 12 age-matched peers (4–6 years) produced structurally primed sentences containing particles and prepositions. Utterances were analyzed for errors and for articulatory duration and variability. Standard measures of motor, language, and articulation skill were also obtained.

Results

Sentences containing particles, as compared with prepositions, were less likely to be produced in a priming task and were longer in duration, suggesting increased difficulty with this syntactic structure. Children with SLI demonstrated higher articulatory variability and poorer gross and fine motor skills compared with aged matched controls. Articulatory variability was correlated with generalized gross and fine motor performance.

Conclusions

Children with SLI show co-occurring speech motor and generalized motor deficits. Current theories do not fully account for the present findings, though the Procedural Deficit Hypothesis provides a framework for interpreting overlap among language and motor domains.

Keywords: Children, Language, Language disorders, Specific language impairment, Speech motor control, Speech production, Syntax

Introduction

Over the past several years, it has been suggested that complex and hierarchical language production interacts in specific ways with motor skill (e.g., Greenfield, 1991; Iverson, 2010; Thelen & Smith, 1994). Because children with specific language impairment (SLI), by definition, show dissociations among aspects of cognitive and language development, they provide a particularly strong test of whether and how domain general mechanisms may interact with language. Some theoreticians propose a common mechanism underlying language and motor processing in SLI (e.g., Tomblin, Maniela-Arnold, & Zhang, 2006; Ullman & Pierpont, 2005), while others suggest a co-morbidity, with independent deficits associated with language and motor components (e.g., Locke, 1997). It is our objective to begin to assess how grammatical aspects of language production cohere with speech motor and gross and fine motor performance in children diagnosed with SLI.

Evidence for a Motor Deficit in SLI

SLI is explicitly defined based on language deficits; these children show impairments in expressive and possibly receptive language that cannot be explained by hearing, neurological, or gross and fine motor deficits (Leonard, 1998; Stark & Tallal, 1981). Yet, as a group, children with SLI perform poorly on a range of motor tasks and have been identified as having soft neurological signs, such as minor abnormalities in behavior and coordination (Bishop & Edmundson, 1987; Hill, 2001; Powell & Bishop, 1992). Further, children diagnosed with SLI tend to be motorically clumsy (Powell & Bishop, 1992; Zelaznik & Goffman, 2010) and to demonstrate poor motor skill and haptic object recognition (Müürsepp, Aibast, Gapeyeva & Pääsuke, 2012; Müürsepp, Aibast, & Pääsuke, 2011). In a review of 29 studies, Hill (2001, p. 166) concluded that there is a “substantial co-morbidity between SLI and poor motor skills.” These children show deficits in fine motor hand, limb, and finger movements (Hill, 1998; Marton, 2009; Noterdaeme, Amorosa, Ploog, & Scheimann, 1998), peg moving, bead threading, and buttoning (Owen & McKinlay, 1997), representational gestures (Hill, Bishop, & Nimmo-Smith, 1998), as well as a weak hand preference (Hill & Bishop, 1998). It is now well known that deficits observed in children with SLI are not confined purely to the linguistic domain.

Based on neuroanatomical evidence, it is not surprising that children with SLI show co-occurring deficits in language and motor domains. Broca’s area, which is implicated in syntactic language functions (Caplan, Alpert, Waters, & Olivieri, 2000; Nishitani & Hari, 2000), also coordinates the mirror neuron system (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004), which supports the notion of a specific relationship between syntactic and motor abilities. Consistent with this view, neuroanatomical studies suggest an interaction between language and motor systems in SLI. Jancke, Siegenthaler, Preis, and Steinmetz (2007) showed that children with developmental language disorder had decreased white matter volume in motor areas of the left hemisphere and also corresponding behavioral deficits in a complex manual coordination task.

Perhaps the most detailed hypothesis to emerge to date is Ullman and Pierpont’s (2005) Procedural Deficit Hypothesis. In this view, the grammatical deficits associated with SLI are part of a larger unified deficit in the brain systems underlying procedural learning. Procedural learning is implicit and is required for the acquisition of new sequential skills, such as riding a bicycle, tying shoes, or producing a sentence. Among other capacities, motor sequencing and grammar are both explicitly implicated in the Procedural Deficit Hypothesis.

As motivated by the neuroanatomical evidence and by the Procedural Deficit Hypothesis, our objective is to contribute to understanding the documented relationship between grammatical aspects of language production, articulatory sequencing, and motor skill. In the following sections, we first describe the syntactic constructions we examine and the standard analyses used to evaluate children’s performance. We then turn to articulatory sequencing, and finally relate both language and speech motor performance to measures of generalized gross and fine motor skill.

Syntactic Constructions

In the first component of the present study, we investigated error patterns in children’s productions of syntactic structures that varied in difficulty. Children with SLI show particular deficits in their production of grammatical inflections (Rice, Wexler, & Hershberger, 1998). Specifically, children with SLI have difficulty with morphemes that connect to a verb. Unstressed function words pose problems for English speaking children with SLI, particularly verb inflections such as third person singular (-s), auxilary and copula (-s), and regular past tense (-d) (Bedore & Leonard, 1998; Rice et al., 1998). The particle, which shares many of these syntactic and prosodic characteristics, is also prone to difficulty in children with SLI. Watkins and Rice (1991) found that four and five year old children with SLI tended to omit particles more frequently compared to their age matched peers. Particles and prepositions are similar phonologically and lexically but differ in their syntactic organization, which meets the methodological constraints of the current study.

Critically, verb particles are syntactically distinct from prepositions (Cappelle, 2004), since verb particles form a unit with a verb whereas prepositions operate independently of the verb (Watkins & Rice, 1991). English verb particles can either be split (1) or joined (2) in phrases with a full noun phrase.

  • (1)

    Mary [kicked over [the chair]]

  • (2)

    Mary [kicked [the chair] over]

In sentences with full noun phrases the placement of the particle is influenced by characteristics of the sentence structure. These factors include the length of the noun phrase and the focus of the sentence. In sentences with a pronominal noun phrase the particle must follow the pronoun.

  • (3)

    She kicked it over

  • (4)

    *She kicked over it

The prepositional phrase, on the other hand, is fixed in its syntactic position. The preposition which heads a noun phrase must always come before the noun.

  • (5)

    She jumped [over the chair]

  • (6)

    She jumped [over it]

  • (7)

    *She jumped the chair over

  • (8)

    *She jumped it over

Since the verb particle and the preposition can be phonetically similar but differ syntactically, the present study utilized these constructions to assess influences of syntax on language production abilities in children with SLI.

Relationship of Speech Motor Skill and Language Complexity

The production of sentences containing verb particles and prepositions also incorporates complex articulatory sequencing goals. These particular language targets were selected because they are difficult for children with SLI and are also amenable to speech motor analysis. An approach to assess language and motor relations is to directly record the movement of the articulators while children and adults produce different linguistic structures. In this way it can be shown how movements of the articulators interact with higher levels of representation.

Articulatory movements during speech may be affected by complex language. Maner, Smith, and Grayson (2000) found that normally developing (ND) 5 year old children and adults showed increased articulatory variability for a phrase spoken in longer and more complex sentences compared to the same sentence spoken in isolation. Length and linguistic complexity influence speech-motor performance in ND children. Kleinow and Smith (2006) controlled for utterance length when manipulating syntactic complexity. They found that ND 9-year-old children showed more articulatory variability when imitating a sentence containing a more complex relative clause than a syntactically simpler conjunction. These results support a more tightly connected language and articulatory system than has previously been considered.

The specific interaction between syntactic complexity and articulatory abilities found by Kleinow and Smith (2006) suggests that general motor capacities could relate to language abilities. Children with SLI demonstrate poorer articulatory movement skills compared with age matched peers. Specifically, they show increased variability in the patterning of oral movements during repetitions of a specific word or sentence (Goffman, 1999; 2004). Further, in their production of varying prosodic sequences, they have difficulty producing the small and short movements associated with weak syllables (Goffman, 1999). It is not known if these language and speech motor deficits relate to those frequently cited in the gross and fine motor domains (Bishop & Edmundson, 1987; Hill, 2001).

Syntactic Priming Compared With Sentence Imitation

Previous studies that have investigated speech motor control have used imitations to elicit target utterances. This line of research has utilized word and sentence repetitions since it requires highly specified targets and has focused on investigating speech motor skill, such as variability, amplitude and duration of articulatory movements (Goffman, 1999; Goffman, Gerken, & Lucchesi, 2007; Smith & Goffman, 2004). In the present study we aimed to tax children’s language and motor systems during language formulation and to evaluate the effects on both grammatical and articulatory sequencing.

A major obstacle to increasing formulation demands in speech motor control research has been the requirement to elicit target utterances through imitation. While imitations presumably incorporate many components of language processing, it could be beneficial to elicit sentences that demand additional syntactic processing. Structural priming techniques may provide an ideal approach for assessing motor aspects of sentence production. Huttenlocher (2004) found that ND children were more likely to produce a target syntactic form if it had been modeled with different lexical items (e.g., prime- “the girl is throwing the ball to the boy;” target- “the man is handing the book to the girl”). In addition, the use of syntactic priming has been useful for eliciting particular grammatical structures in children with SLI (Leonard et al., 2000). A priming paradigm could be applied to studies of speech motor control to increase the speaker’s processing load.

Current Study

There were three major and inter-related hypotheses:

  1. Children with SLI would have more overt errors in the relatively difficult particle compared with the less difficult preposition structure, replicating Watkins and Rice (1991).

  2. Children with SLI would show increased articulatory variability and longer duration than their ND peers (Goffman, 1999; 2004). Further, the sentences containing the particle will show increased motor variability and longer duration compared with the prepositions, consistent with earlier findings that syntactic difficulty influences articulatory aspects of production (Kleinow & Smith, 2006).

  3. Children with SLI will, as a group, demonstrate impairments in gross and fine motor skill, similar to that reported by Hill (2001), Bishop and Edmundson (1987), Zelaznik and Goffman (2010) and others.

Together, the findings from this study will provide a window in how generalized motor, speech motor, and language deficits hang together in children with SLI, and thus into the nature of the well documented co-occurrence of language and motor deficits.

Method

Participants

A total of 23 individuals participated including 11 four- to six-year-old children diagnosed with SLI (6 females) and 12 age matched ND children (6 females). Parents showed similar levels of education across groups (SLI, M = 16.18 years, SD = 2.42; ND, M = 17.55 years, SD = 2.13).

All children’s non-verbal cognitive skills were within the normal range (SLI, M = 106, SD = 14.89; ND, M = 121.2, SD = 8.93) as measured by the Columbia Mental Maturity Scale (Burgemeister, Blum, & Lorge, 1972). In addition, all children had normal hearing as indicated by responses to pure tones presented at 25 dB for the frequencies .5 kHz, 1 kHz, 2 kHz, 4 kHz, and 6 kHz. All participants also had normal structural and functional oral motor skills as measured by the Clinical Assessment of Oropharyngeal Motor Development (St. Louis & Ruscello, 1987). Children were given a standardized test of articulation, the Bankson-Bernthal Test of Phonology (Bankson & Bernthal, 1990). As shown on Table 1, all of the children with SLI performed at least 1 SD below expected levels on this single word articulation test.

Table 1.

Individual Results of Tests.

Subject Comprehension
SPELT CMMS BBTOP NWR FVM
Particle Preposition
SLI
SLI-1a no data no data < 1 100 71 0.46 0.47
SLI-2 a 83% 100% < 1 99 71 0.71 0.78
SLI-3 a 83% 100% < 1 111 75 0.57 0.76
SLI-4 a 67% 71% < 1 107 72 0.63 0.95
SLI-5 a 83% 86% 4 85 68 0.78 0.91
SLI-6 a 33% 86% < 1 125 82 0.64 0.77
SLI-7 no data no data < 1 85 76 0.64 0.64
SLI-8 no data no data < 1 109 78 0.66 0.57
SLI-9 83% 71% < 1 135 72 0.63 0.82
SLI-10 83% 86% < 1 103 72 0.62 0.93
SLI-11 83% 86% < 1 108 72 0.61 0.82
M 0.75 0.86 1.27 106.09 73.545 0.63 0.76
SD 0.18 0.11 0.90 14.89 3.908 0.08 0.15
Subject Comprehension
SPELT CMMS BBTOP NWR FVM
Particle Preposition
ND
ND-1 67% 100% 71 111 98 no data no data
ND-2 100% 86% 84 140 101 no data no data
ND-3 100% 100% 46 116 100 no data no data
ND-4 83% 100% 62 131 105 no data no data
ND-5 100% 100% 90 122 104 no data no data
ND-6 100% 86% 93 126 95 no data no data
ND-7 83% 100% 80 119 105 no data no data
ND-8 100% 100% 94 115 98 no data no data
ND-9 100% 100% 99 125 105 no data no data
ND-10 100% 100% 79 110 80 no data no data
ND-11 100% 100% 70 118 89 no data no data
M .94 .97 78.91 121.18 98.18
SD 0.11 0.06 15.78 8.93 7.81

Note. CMMS = Columbia Mental Maturity Scale (standard scores reported); BBTOP = Bankson-Bernthal Test of Phonology Consonant Inventory (standard scores reported); SPELT = Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test (percentiles reported); NWR= Non word Repetition, FVM=Finite Verb Morphology.

The children were diagnosed with SLI based on their performance on the Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test (Dawson & Stout, 2003); all children with SLI performed below the 4th percentile on this test. Their age-matched ND peers scored within the normal range (between the 46th and 99th percentile). Because nonword repetition (NWR) and the finite verb morphology composite (FVMC) have been found to be especially sensitive markers of SLI (Dollaghan & Campbell, 1998; Leonard, Miller, & Gerber, 1999), these two measures were used to further verify the group status of the children with SLI. The finite-verb morphology composite (Leonard, Miller, & Gerber, 1999) was calculated as a percent correct of grammatical morphemes marking tense and agreement (i.e., past tense -ed, third person singular -s, the copula and auxillary forms of is, are, and am). For a summary of findings from the FVMC and NWR tasks also see Table 1.

As a standardized measure of gross and fine motor skill, children below the age of 6;0 (SLI, n=10; ND, n=11) were assessed using the Peabody Developmental Motor Scales (PDMS) (Folio & Fewell, 2000). One child with SLI and her age matched control were 6;0 and did not meet the standardization age requirement of the PDMS and thus were administered the Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Integration (Bruininks, 1978). These two older children were not included in the analyses comparing groups based on gross and fine motor performance on the PDMS; however they were included in all other analyses. Typical gross and fine motor skill was considered inclusionary for the children who were ND. Because of the complex and poorly understood relationship between SLI and developmental coordination disorder (e.g., Hill, 2001), we felt that typical performance on tests of both language and motor skill would result in more interpretable results. Based on these inclusionary criteria, one child who was ND was excluded as the result of scoring greater than 1 SD below the mean on the PDMS. Gross and fine motor descriptive data are shown in Table 2.

Table 2.

Individual scores on the Peabody Developmental Motor Scales.

Subject Stationary Locomotion Object manip. Grasping Visual motor GMQ FMQ TMQ
SLI
SLI-1 7 9 6 5 8 83 79 79
SLI-3 7 9 7 3 8 85 73 78
SLI-4 9 9 10 2 7 96 67 82
SLI-5 6 8 11 6 9 89 85 86
SLI-6 8 10 7 5 7 89 76 82
SLI-7 9 9 7 8 10 89 94 90
SLI-8 10 10 9 10 12 98 106 101
SLI-9 7 12 8 11 12 94 109 100
SLI-10 7 8 10 10 8 89 94 90
SLI-11 12 10 10 9 9 104 94 100
Subject Stationary Locomotion Object manip. Grasping Visual motor GMQ FMQ TMQ
ND
ND-2 12 4 7 11 13 85 112 96
ND-3 11 13 10 11 9 109 100 105
ND-4 10 10 9 9 12 119 103 100
ND-5 8 10 8 12 8 91 100 94
ND-6 11 9 7 6 12 94 94 93
ND-7 11 10 7 11 10 96 103 98
ND-8 9 10 7 11 11 91 106 97
ND-9 8 13 8 12 9 98 103 100
ND-10 9 11 8 9 12 96 103 98
ND-11 10 8 9 11 11 94 121 98

To assess comprehension of the particle and preposition structures utilized in the experimental task, children were asked to demonstrate their knowledge by pointing to pictures and manipulating objects. For example, using a doll and small objects, participants were asked to show the clinician actions associated with the commands: “Jump over the bucket,” “tip over the bucket,” “lift up the bucket,” “climb up the bucket,” “turn on the flashlight,” “turn the flashlight,” “take off the shoe,” and “take the shoe.” For results of the comprehension task also see Table 1. The comprehension probe was incorporated into the project after three participants completed the study; therefore data from these children were unavailable.

Stimuli and Procedures

Each child participated in two thirty-minute experimental sessions in addition to standardized testing. During the experimental sessions, movements of the lips and the jaw were tracked using the Northern Digital Optotrak 3020 three-camera system, a system designed for recording human movement in three dimensions, at a sampling rate of 250 samples/second. The kinematic data were low-pass filtered with a cut-off of 10 Hz. Three infrared light emitting diodes (IREDs) recorded articulatory movement, and were placed on the upper lip, the lower lip, and on a small splint on the jaw. Four other IREDs were used as a reference frame for the subtraction of head movement and were placed on modified sports goggles and on the forehead. An acoustic signal that was time locked to the movement signal was also recorded. Finally, a video recording was used to identify production errors.

Children produced target phrases (particle or preposition) that were matched for word length and only differed in whether the phrase contained a particle or a preposition. The target sentence frames were as follows:

Session A: Preposition: Jump over the block
Particle: Tip over the block
Session B: Preposition: Climb up the boxes
Particle: Lift up the boxes

In each experimental session, in addition to the target utterances, there were also 12 particle foils (e.g., “knock over the box”) and 12 preposition foils (e.g., “run up the hill”). See Appendix A for a sample block within an experimental session.

A highly structured priming paradigm in the form of a game used foils to elicit target phrases. The participants were first exposed to the paradigm during an instructional phase of the experiment. During the experimental priming task, participants watched a video and listened to a priming sentence composed of a specific syntactic form (i.e., preposition or particle). The syntactic prime was produced by a female mid-western accented talker (e.g., Cue: “What did the teacher tell the girl to do?”… Prime: “Step over the book”). After the participant was exposed to the priming sentence, he or she was immediately presented with a video that was semantically unrelated to the priming sentence (e.g., a video of a girl jumping over a block). A cue was then given to prompt the participant to produce the target phase (“Now it’s your turn, what did you tell the girl to do?”). Following the structural priming, it was expected that participants would describe the video with the same syntactic form as the preceding priming sentence and thus produce the correct target utterances. If a child did not produce the target utterance, there were a series of prompts designed to elicit a fluent target utterance that was captured by the Optotrak system. These prompts began with asking the child to “say that again” and proceeded to include increased support until the child could produce the target form (e.g., imitation: “Say; ‘Tip over the block’”).

Each experimental session consisted of three blocks of stimuli (Appendix A shows an example of one block), presented in a quasi-random order. The stimulus blocks included five tokens of each target with no more than three particles or prepositions in a row, and the same prime was not used twice in a row. The target utterance occurred as a prime only once in each block and did not prime itself. Across the three blocks, there were opportunities to elicit each target utterance 15 times. The particle/preposition foils in Session A and B respectively utilized the lexical entries ‘over’ and ‘up’. The order of administration of the two experimental sessions was counterbalanced.

Analyses

Perceptual analysis

Videos of all experimental sessions were observed by two trained transcribers. The first author orthographically transcribed the utterances and a research assistant, a graduate student training in Speech-Language Pathology, independently verified these transcriptions. The observers documented the amount of cueing required for a correct target utterance, orthographically transcribed the productions following syntactic primes and also documented participants’ errors. The utterances that the children with SLI and their ND peers produced directly after the syntactic prime were scored as correct or as inconsistent with the target. ‘Correct’ utterances included the critical grammatical elements (i.e., [verb] [preposition/particle] [determiner] [noun]). The proportion of correctly primed utterances for each child was calculated to assess the influence of group and syntactic condition on the production of correct primes. In addition to omission errors, disfluencies were documented (e.g., lift up ththththe the box), and were compared across particle and preposition conditions.

Kinematic analysis

The utterances perceived as fluent and consistent were pooled together for kinematic analysis. Only utterances that contained errors that were stable across productions were subjected to kinematic analysis (e.g., Target-“Tip over the block”, Child’s repeated utterance-“Tip over a block” throughout the session). For this analysis each child’s first 10 fluent and phonetically consistent spontaneous target productions were analyzed. If there were not 10 fluent spontaneous productions of the target utterance, a second pass through the data included self corrections (e.g., ‘push over the boxes, no I mean, tip over the boxes’). Iterative passes through the data were completed, beginning with primes and ending with direct imitation, until 10 fluent productions were selected. In many cases 10 productions were obtained, however a minimum of 5 phonetically comparable utterances were included (30% of cells for children with SLI had between 5 and 9 productions, and 13% of cells for ND children). The number of utterances assessed kinematically was matched across particle/preposition conditions for individual participants. Data from two children with SLI in one syntactic frame (lift/climb) could not be analyzed kinematically.

Kinematic records for the two phonetically similar (e.g., Jump over the block/Tip over the block) syntactic targets were extracted from long data files (see Figure 1). The actual portions selected for analysis (“p over the b”) were identical. In this example, the onset of the selected movement record corresponded to the closure of the lips for the /p/ (the final consonant in the verbs ‘jump’ and ‘tip’). This segment of the movement record was initially identified visually in Matlab at the point of peak displacement of the lower lip. Then an algorithm selected the maximum displacement (within a 25-point, or 100 ms, analysis window) which corresponded to the velocity zero-crossing. As shown in Figure 1, the offset of the target lower lip movement was selected in a similar fashion and corresponded to the peak displacement of the word initial /b/ in the word ‘block’. The kinematic selections were then confirmed by playing the time-locked acoustic signal.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Extracted portion of long data file. The top panel depicts the movement of the lower lip and jaw while a participant produces the particle phrase ‘Tip over the block’. The bottom panel depicts the lower lip and jaw movement during the production of the prepositional phrase ‘Jump over the block’. Downward movement corresponds to lower lip opening, and upward movement corresponds to lip closing. The area between the two vertical lines indicates the portion of the data that was extracted and analyzed.

Stability

To determine the stability of the underlying movement patterning of the articulators, the 10 movement trajectories extracted from the long data files were amplitude and time normalized (Smith, Goffman, Zelaznik, Ying, & McGillem, 1995; Smith, Johnson, McGillem, & Goffman, 2000). Amplitude normalization was accomplished by setting the mean of each movement trajectory to zero and the standard deviation to 1 (top panel of Figure 2). Each movement record was forced to the same time scale of 1,000 points, using a spline function to interpolate between points. The purpose of the normalization procedure was to remove the effects of changes in rate and loudness and to reveal the variability of the spatiotemporal organization of the repeated movement sequences. An example of this normalization is shown in the middle panel of Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Extracted portion of target utterances. The top panel displays 10 amplitude-normalized sequences of movement extracted from the long data file from one child. The center panel illustrates these movement trajectories after both time- and amplitude- normalization. The bottom panel shows standard deviation values obtained at 2% intervals across the movement trajectory. The lip aperture variability (LAVAR), which is shown above the bottom panel, is the sum of these 50 standard deviations.

The lip aperture variability (LAVAR) index is a measure that quantifies the stability of the underlying movement patterning from the normalized records. It is calculated as the difference between the upper and lower lips and directly measures lip aperture. All time and amplitude normalized movement trajectories are shown on the middle panel of Figure 2. Standard deviations of the 10 normalized trials were computed at 2% intervals (bottom panel of Figure 2). The LAVAR index was then computed, which is a numerical value of the sum of the 50 standard deviations. The LAVAR index was used to compare the articulatory stability of the productions of the preposition vs. the particle sentences in ND children compared to those with SLI. A high LAVAR indicates increased articulatory variability.

Duration

The duration of the kinematic records was also measured to assess influences of linguistic difficulty on the production of particles compared with prepositions. It is important to note that, as in the variability analysis, the extracted duration segments were phonetically identical across particle/preposition pairs (e.g., from the /p/ to the /b/ in “Tip over the block”/ “Jump over the block”).

Results

Perceptual Analyses

Comprehension

Children with SLI understood both syntactic structures more poorly than their ND peers, F(1,17) = 15.5, p < .005. In addition there was a trend towards a particle/preposition effect, F(1,17) = 3.51, p = .078, with particles marginally weaker than prepositions. There were no interactions, F(1,17) =.919, p = .351 (see Table 1).

Accuracy of Particles vs. Prepositions

Utterances were scored as correct if they included the critical grammatical elements. There was a group effect, F(1,20) = 8.63, p = .008, with children with ND more frequently producing the correct structure after a syntactic prime (see Figure 3). There was no condition effect of syntactic frame, F(1,20) = .37, p = .551, suggesting that the two different sentences used to test prepositions and the two different sentences used to test particles did not differ from one another. There was also no effect of particle vs. preposition, F(1,20) = .02, p = .899, revealing that overall the particle and preposition were comparable in accuracy. There was an interaction of syntactic frame and particle/preposition, F(1,20) = 10.96, p = .003, and there was also a 3 way interaction between group, syntactic frame, and preposition/particle F(1,20) = 8.28 p = .009. As depicted in Figure 3, this interaction was due to more accurately produced prepositions than particles by ND children. Children with SLI did not show this effect; they showed opposite patterns of performance for the particle and the preposition in the two sentence frames. Overall, children with SLI were not sensitive to the syntactic differences in particles and prepositions based on this accuracy measure.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Frequency of accurately primed utterances as a function of group and preposition/particle. Symbols represent means. Error bars indicate standard error. There was a main effect of Group. There also was an interaction; children who were ND were more likely to be primed for prepositions than for particles.

Disfluencies

There were no significant main effects for group, F(1,20) = 2.47, p = .13, or sentence frame, F(1,20) = .99, p = .333. There was a trend towards a syntactic condition effect, F(1,20) = 3.69, p = .069, with more disfluencies tending to be produced in the particle condition than the preposition. There were no interactions, F(1,20) = 1.87, p = .181. Both groups of children trended towards producing more disfluencies in sentences containing particles than prepositions.

Speech Motor Effects

Primed vs. Imitated Productions

For the kinematic analysis, it was required that productions be fluent and phonetically consistent. To achieve this goal, in addition to primed utterances, those that required verbal cuing were also included. For this reason, only a subset of the data reported in the behavioral results above were included in the kinematic analysis. For this subset of accurate and/or stable productions, it is important to note that children with SLI had a lower percentage of primed productions than their ND peers, F(1,21) = 7.79, p = .01, revealing that the children with SLI required more cues. There was also a particle/preposition condition effect, F(1,21) = 5.47, p = .03, with fewer primed particles than prepositions. There was no significant Group*Condition interaction, F(1,21) = .41, p = .53. Overall, children with SLI were primed for 49% (SE = 7.72) of particle constructions and 56% (SE = 7.32) of preposition constructions. Their ND peers were primed for 73% (SE = 7.39) of particles and 85% (SE = 7.01) of prepositions. The remainder of the productions for both groups required increased cuing, ranging from “use tip” or “say the whole thing” to direct imitations.

Duration of Movement

There was no group effect for duration, F(1,18) = .30, p = .589, as illustrated in Figure 4. There was a significant effect of sentence frame, F(1,18) = 27.49, p < .005, which was expected as the result of the inherent differences in duration in the production of ‘over’ and ‘up’ due to syllable length. Importantly, there was a particle vs. preposition effect, F(1,18) = 17.46, p = .001. Pairwise comparisons using the Tukey HSD procedure revealed that particles were produced with longer durations than prepositions for both groups (p =.007). There was also a sentence by particle/preposition interaction, F(1,18) = 19.45, p < .001, once again illustrating the inherent differences between the two sentence frames.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Duration of trimmed utterance that were segmentally and lexically identical across the particle and preposition conditions (e.g., ‘tip over the block/ jump over the block). Symbols represent means. Error bars indicate standard error. There was a main effect for particle vs. preposition. There was also an effect for sentence frame, due to the inherent duration differences in “over the block” compared with “up the boxes.”

Stability of movement

There was a group effect of LAVAR, F(1,18) = 5.17, p = .035, demonstrating that children with SLI produced more variable articulatory movement patterns than their ND peers (see Figure 5). There was no effect of sentence frame, F(1,18) = .03, p = .861, or of particle vs. preposition, F(1,18) = .96, p = .340. The lack of a three-way interaction of sentence, particle vs. preposition and group, F(1,18) = .25 p = .626, suggests that the particle and preposition were comparable in both sentence frames.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Lip Aperture Variability (LAVAR) as a function of group (SLI, ND), particle/preposition, and sentence frame. Symbols represent means. Error bars indicate standard error. There was a main effect of Group.

Gross and Fine Motor Effects

A repeated measures ANOVA included the children under the age of 6 who completed the Peabody Developmental Motor Scales with group (SLI and ND) as the between subjects factor and motor scores (gross motor quotient and fine motor quotient) as the within subjects factors. As shown on Figure 6, our results replicated previous findings showing that children with SLI had significantly lower scores on motor scales than their ND peers, F(1, 18) = 11.98, p = .003. There was no condition effect, F(1,18) = .31, p =.586, with gross motor and fine motor abilities similar overall.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Gross and fine scores on the Peabody Developmental Motor Scales. Symbols represent means. Error bars indicate standard error. There was a main effect of Group.

Exploratory Analyses of Cross-Domain Relationships

As reported above, children with SLI showed poorer motor performance than their ND peers. However, only approximately half (5 of 11) of the children with SLI showed an overt motor impairment, defined by performance at or greater than 1SD below the mean on the total motor quotient (TMQ) of the Peabody Developmental Motor Scales or the Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency. While there were too few participants to conduct analyses of subgroups, correlational analysis of the entire group (ND and SLI) revealed that children’s generalized motor and speech motor skills were related. Children with poorer fine motor skill were more likely to demonstrate increased articulatory variability (r = −.71, p< .05; −.61, p < .05; −.36, p > .05 and −.16, p > .05) across all four target sentences. Gross motor skills did not relate to articulatory variability (r = −.15, −.15, −.14, and .09 across the four target sentences, all nonsignificant).

Another potential relationship to explore is whether performance on behavioral language and speech motor measures relate. In one sentence frame, “jump/tip over the block,” lip aperture variability correlated with accuracy (“tip,” r = −.62, p < .05; “jump,” r = −.71, p < .05). For this frame, increased errors corresponded with increases in speech motor variability. However, for the frame “climb/lift up the boxes,” no significant correlations were observed (“lift,” r = .02, ns; “climb,” r = .17, nonsignificant). It is unclear what is contributing to these effects and more detailed exploration of language and speech motor interactions is warranted.

We also considered whether two related issues, that is cognitive status or severity of language deficit, corresponded to speech motor skill. Cognitive measures were obtained from the entire group of participants. These were uncorrelated with speech motor skill across all four sentences (r = −.28, −.25, −.04, and −.11, all nonsignificant). Additionally, performance on an articulation test, the Bankson Bernthal Test of Phonology, was unrelated to articulatory variability (r = −.43, −.38, −.22, −.09, all nonsignificant). From the SLI group only, we evaluated two measures of language performance that show good sensitivity and specificity, the non-word repetition task (r = −.11, .16, .27, and .42, all nonsignificant) and the finite verb morphology composite (r = .06, .25, .14, and .56, all nonsignificant). These hallmark capacities were not related to speech motor skill.

In a final set of exploratory analyses, we wondered whether cognitive, language, and speech variables correlated with gross and fine motor skill. No significant relationships were observed with the exception of the score on the Bankson Bernthal Test of Phonology, which correlated significantly with fine motor (r = .52, p < .05), but not gross motor (r = .38, ns) performance.

While these data are preliminary, due to the small sample size and their correlational nature, they suggest as a whole that (1) not all children with SLI show an overt motor deficit; (2) the frequently observed motor deficit is correlated with speech motor skill. However, speech motor skill is not correlated with cognitive or language performance. It is intriguing that fine motor skill relates to performance on a test of speech sound accuracy, the Bankson Bernthal Test of Phonology. Both speech accuracy and fine motor performance likely relate to higher order components of cognitive processing, while articulatory variability more closely indexes motor implementation.

Discussion

One objective of this research was to develop a speech motor control paradigm that requires talkers to generate sentences without relying on imitation. We used a heavily structured priming task to ask whether children with SLI and their ND peers showed predicted deficits in the production of relatively difficult compared with simple syntactic structures and how these capacities related to motor skill. Using this paradigm, as is consistent with prior research relying on imitation of target structures, children with SLI were shown to have deficits in producing complex sequences of articulatory movement (e.g., Goffman, 2004). This was even the case though an increased number of imitations were incorporated into the analysis from the children with SLI compared with their ND peers (52% primed for SLI and 79% for ND) due to their difficulties with the sentence generation task. In future work, it will be important to compare the influence of increased processing load in priming directly to imitation. It is evident that children with SLI show weaknesses in the production of stable sequential articulatory movements, since differences emerge even when a higher proportion of imitated sentences are incorporated into their variability measures. Also corresponding with previous work, children with SLI as a group demonstrated motor weaknesses (Hill, 2001). In the present study, based on performance on the Peabody Developmental Motor Scales, fine motor skills were especially weak.

In the error analysis portion of the study, we did not replicate the findings of Watkins and Rice (1991). We used a similar hierarchy of prompts to those incorporated in the earlier study. However, because our study required similar productions to make them amenable to kinematic analysis, prompts were used, sometimes even including imitation. In addition, while the prepositions and particles were inter-mixed, we embedded repetitions of the same utterances. These factors may have reduced the processing demands of the task and resulted in fewer overt errors. However, there was some confirmation that particles are more difficult to produce than prepositions. There was a trend toward increased disfluencies in the particle compared with the preposition condition. Even more critically, particles were produced with relatively longer duration than prepositions. Finally, prepositions were more likely to be primed than particles. We interpreted these findings as indicative of an increase in syntactic complexity for the particle form. Unlike previous work (Watkins and Rice, 1991; Kleinow & Smith, 2006), this increased syntactic complexity did not influence either transcription accuracy or kinematic variability. Further research is needed to determine the specific locus of interactions between processing difficulty and articulatory variability in children with and without language impairment.

There are some additional factors that may have influenced our findings that also need to be considered, the first being the small number of participants. Further, because we required phonetic similarity across the particles and prepositions, we may not have included a sufficiently taxing complex syntactic condition. Finally, the frequency and phonological content of the target words could have influenced our results. For instance, the two verbs in the particle phrases ‘tip’ and ‘lift’ are less frequent than the two verbs in the prepositional phrases ‘jump’ and ‘climb’ (Carroll, Davies, & Richman, 1971). However, if the frequency of the verbs in the study contributed to the results, we would have expected increased errors and higher articulatory variability during the production of the less frequent particles; this was not the case. The incorporation of a priming task was both a strength and weakness. Processing load was clearly affected; however, as a consequence children with SLI had more difficulty than their age matched peers and required more cues. It is possible that this could have allowed children with SLI more learning opportunities and motor practice (Walsh, Smith, & Weber-Fox, 2006) which may have increased the similarities between the groups.

Children with SLI showed increased articulatory variability and relative weaknesses in gross and fine motor skill in comparison with their ND peers. We report some preliminary findings that merit further investigation. Interestingly, while gross and fine motor and speech motor skill were correlated, other core aspects of SLI, particularly nonword repetition and finite verb morphology, were not related to speech motor skill. Grammatical deficits are thought to be the hallmark of SLI (Leonard, 1998) and are hypothesized to comprise a common factor underlying movement sequencing and language deficits (Ullman & Pierpont, 2005). The present data provide some preliminary support that other mechanisms may need to be considered when relating language and motor domains in the developmental profile observed in children with SLI.

Theoretical implications

Consistent with previous findings, this study revealed that children with SLI, as a group, demonstrate both language and motor deficits. However, not surprisingly, children with SLI are heterogeneous, and only some show an overt motor deficit. It seems from these preliminary results that the motor impairments in children with SLI may not be due to a single deficit or a global maturational impairment as has been posited in some accounts of language impairment (Locke, 1997; Bishop and Edmundson, 1987; Kail, 1994). The interaction of language and motor domains appears more complex.

Ullman and Pierpont (2005) propose a framework for considering these more complex interactions. They hypothesize that SLI may be explained by a deficit in procedural memory, an organized network of neural structures that control learning and execution of motor and cognitive skills. This system is theorized to be important in learning concrete and abstract rules and sequences (e.g., riding a bike, producing a sentence). It is often described as implicit, since rule learning is not a conscious process. In this system, there are tendencies for particular neural structures to be involved and for a set of deficits, including, for example, co-occurring difficulties in sequential ordering in speech production and movement (Tomblin, Maniela-Arnold, & Zhang, 2007). Indeed, this profile may describe children with SLI who participated in the present study in reference to their language and motor deficits

However, these relationships are complex, as acknowledged by Ullman and Pierpont (2005) who do discuss heterogeneity and variability. Ullman and Pierpont point out that, depending on the location and extent of the affected brain region, the domains involved (i.e., language, motor, memory) may differ, as may the severity of the disorder. The procedural memory system contains, but is not restricted to, Broca’s area. In this theory, grammatical and motor deficits are linked to Broca’s area (Arbib, 2006; Greenfield, 1991; Kent, 2004). The co-occurrence of language and motor deficits in some children could be due to lesions in motor tracts that lead to Broca’s area, while other children with SLI may have deficits in tracts that affect only language ability. The Procedural Deficit Hypothesis provides a framework to begin to describe and understand the motor and language deficits in children with SLI.

As suggested by Ullman and Pierpont (2005), it may be that the procedural deficit is not a necessary condition underlying SLI. While children with SLI are more likely than their ND peers to show a speech motor or generalized motor deficit, 5 of the 11 children studied here demonstrated no overt motor impairment. It is intriguing that, contrary to our expectation grammatical factors do not appear to be the connector linking motor and language variables. Performance on language measures did not relate in any consistent manner to speech motor or generalized motor skill. Grammatical sequencing has been proposed as particularly likely to be implicated.

These findings, while preliminary, suggest that more work needs to be done to evaluate whether the motor and language deficits in SLI may share common mechanisms or are relatively independent co-morbidities. Uncovering this relationship, or of factors underlying individual differences in these children, is significant for understanding the nature of SLI and for developing appropriate intervention approaches for these children.

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by the National Institutes of Health, National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders Grant R01 DC04826. The first author was also supported by the Frederick N. Andrews Fellowship from Purdue University. Portions of the research were presented at the Symposium for Research in Child Language Disorders, Madison, WI. We thank Laurence Leonard and Elaine Francis for their guidance and expertise and Janna Berlin, Pat Deevy, Brooke Adams, Kelsey Pithoud, Rachel Brunner, Michelle Wiersma, and Ilana Feld for their invaluable contributions. We are also grateful to the children and families who participated in this study.

Appendix A. Example of one block of stimuli

Category Prime Sentence Produced sentence
Particle Knock over the box Tip over the block
Preposition Lean over the table Walk over the book
Preposition Walk over the book Jump over the block
Target Preposition Jump over the block Climb over the wall
Particle Push over the glass Knock over the box
Particle Turn over the book Tip over the block
Preposition Walk over the book Jump over the block
Preposition Lean over the table Jump over the block
Target Particle Tip over the block Kick over the chair
Particle Kick over the chair Tip over the block
Preposition Step over the book Jump over the block
Preposition Climb over the wall Lean over the table
Particle Turn over the book Tip over the block
Particle Kick over the chair Turn over the book
Particle Push over the glass Tip over the block
Preposition Climb over the wall Step over the book
Particle Knock over the box Push over the glass
Preposition Step over the book Jump over the block

References

  1. Arbib MA. Action to language via the mirror neuron system. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bankson NW, Bernthal JE. Bankson Bernthal Test of Phonology. Chicago: Riverside Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bedore LM, Leonard LB. Specific language impairment and grammatical morphology: A discriminant function analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1998;41:1185–1192. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4105.1185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bishop DVM, Edmundson A. Specific language impairment as a maturational lag: Evidence from longitudinal data on language and motor development. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 1987;29:442–459. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1987.tb02504.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bruininks RH. Bruininks -Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  6. Burgemeister B, Blum L, Lorge I. Columbia Mental Maturity Scale, Third edition. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich; 1972. [Google Scholar]
  7. Caplan D, Alpert N, Waters G, Olivieri A. Activation of broca’s area by syntactic processing under conditions of concurrent articulation. Human Brain Mapping. 2000;9:65–71. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(200002)9:2&#x0003c;65::AID-HBM1&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cappelle B. The particularity of particles, or why they are not just ‘intransitive prepositions’. Belgian Journal of Linguistics. 2004;18:29–57. [Google Scholar]
  9. Carroll J, Davies P, Richman B. The American Heritage word frequency book. New York: American Heritage; 1971. [Google Scholar]
  10. Dawson JI, Stout CE, Eyer JA. Structured photographic expressive language test. 3. DeKalb, IL: Janelle Publications; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  11. Dollaghan C, Campbell TF. Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1998;41:1136–1146. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4105.1136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Folio M, Fewell R. Peabody developmental motor scales. 2. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  13. Goffman L. Prosodic influences on speech production in children with specific language impairment and speech deficits: Kinematic, acoustic, and transcription evidence. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1999;42:1499–1517. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4206.1499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Goffman L. Kinematic differentiation of prosodic categories in normal and disordered language development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2004;47:1088–1102. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/081). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Goffman L, Gerken LA, Lucchesi J. Relations between segmental and motor variability in prosodically complex non-word sequences. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2007;50:444–458. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/031). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Greenfield PM. Language, tools and brain: The ontogeny and phylogeny of hierarchically organized sequential behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1991;14:531–595. [Google Scholar]
  17. Hill EL. A dyspraxic deficit in specific language impairment and developmental coordination disorder? Evidence from hand and arm movements. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 1998;40:388–395. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1998.tb08214.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hill EL. Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: A review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders. 2001;36:149–171. doi: 10.1080/13682820010019874. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hill EL, Bishop DV. A reaching test reveals weak hand preference in specific language impairment and developmental co-ordination disorder. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition. 1998;3:295–310. doi: 10.1080/713754314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hill E, Bishop D, Nimmo-Smith I. Representational gestures in developmental coordination disorder and specific language impairment: Error-types and the reliability of ratings. Human Movement Science. 1998;17:655–678. [Google Scholar]
  21. Huttenlocher J. Syntactic priming in young children. Journal of Memory and Language. 2004;50:182–195. [Google Scholar]
  22. Jancke L, Siegenthaler T, Preis S, Steinmetz H. Decreased white matter density in left-sided fronto-temporal network in children with developmental language disorder: Evidence from anatomical anomalies in motor-language network. Brain and Language. 2007;102:91–98. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Iverson JM. Developing language in a developing body: the relationship between motor development and language development. Journal of Child Language. 2010;37:229–61. doi: 10.1017/S0305000909990432. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kail R. A method for studying the generalized slowing hypothesis in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research. 1994;37:418–421. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3702.418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kent RD. Models of speech motor control: Implications from recent developments in neurophysiological and neurobehavioral science. In: Maasen B, Kent R, Peters H, vanLieshout P, Hulstijn W, editors. Speech motor control in normal and disordered speech. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2004. pp. 3–27. [Google Scholar]
  26. Kleinow J, Smith A. Potential interactions among linguistic, autonomic, and motor factors in speech. Developmental Psychobiology. 2006;48:275–287. doi: 10.1002/dev.20141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Leonard LB. Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  28. Leonard L, Miller C, Gerber E. Grammatical morphology and the lexicon in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1999;42:678–689. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4203.678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Leonard LB, Miller CA, Grela B, Holland AL, Gerber E, Petucci M. Production operations contribute to the grammatical morpheme limitations of children with specific language impairment. Journal of Memory and Language. 2000;43:362–378. [Google Scholar]
  30. Locke JL. A theory of neurolinguistic development. Brain and Language. 1997;58:265–326. doi: 10.1006/brln.1997.1791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Marton K. Imitation of body postures and hand movements in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2009;102:1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.07.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Maner KJ, Smith A, Grayson L. Influences of utterance length and complexity on speech motor performance in children and adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2000;43:560–573. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4302.560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Miller C, Leonard L. Deficits in finite verb morphology: Some assumptions in recent accounts of specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1998;41:701–707. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4103.701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Müürsepp I, Aibast H, Pääsuke M. Motor performance and haptic perception in preschool boys with specific impairment of expressive language. Acta Paediatr. 2011;7:1038–42. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2011.02201.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Müürsepp I, Aibast H, Gapeyeva H, Pääsuke M. Motor skills, haptic perception and social abilities in children with mild speech disorders. Brain & Development. 2012;34(2):128–32. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2011.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Nishitani N, Hari R. Temporal dynamics of cortical representation for action. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America. 2000;97:913–918. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.2.913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Noterdaeme M, Amorosa H, Ploog M, Scheimann G. Quantitative and qualitative aspects of associated movements in children with specific developmental speech and language disorders and in normal pre-school children. Journal of Human Movement Studies. 1998;15:151–169. [Google Scholar]
  38. Owen S, McKinlay I. Motor difficulties in children with developmental disorders of speech and language. Child Care, Health & Development. 1997;23:315–325. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2214.1997.864864.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Powell R, Bishop D. Clumsiness and perceptual problems in children with specific language impairment. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 1992;34:755–765. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1992.tb11514.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Rice ML, Wexler K, Hershberger S. Tense over time: The longitudinal course of tense acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1998;41:1412–1431. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4106.1412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Rizzolatti G, Craighero L. The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2004;27:169–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Smith A, Goffman L. Interaction of motor and language in the development of speech production. In: Maasen B, Kent R, Peters H, vanLieshout P, Hulstijn W, editors. Speech motor control in normal and disordered speech. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2004. pp. 227–252. [Google Scholar]
  43. Smith A, Goffman L, Zelaznik H, Ying S, McGillem C. Spatiotemporal stability and patterning of speech movement sequences. Experimental Brain Research. 1995;104:493–501. doi: 10.1007/BF00231983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Smith A, Johnson M, McGillem C, Goffman L. On the assessment of stability and patterning of speech movements. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2000;43:277–286. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4301.277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. St Louis KO, Ruscello DM. Oral Speech Mechanism Screening Examination Revised. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  46. Stark R, Tallal P. Selection of children with specific language deficits. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1981;46:114–122. doi: 10.1044/jshd.4602.114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Thelen E, Smith LB. A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  48. Tomblin JB, Maniela-Arnold E, Zhang X. Procedural learning in adolescents with and without specific language impairment. Language Learning and Development. 2007;3:269–293. [Google Scholar]
  49. Ullman MT, Pierpont EI. Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The Procedural Deficit Hypothesis. Cortex. 2005;41:399–433. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70276-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Walsh B, Smith A, Weber-Fox C. Short-term plasticity in children’s speech motor systems. Developmental Psychobiology. 2006;48:660–674. doi: 10.1002/dev.20185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Watkins RV, Rice ML. Verb particle and preposition acquisition in language impaired preschoolers. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research. 1991;34:1130–1141. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3405.1130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Zelaznik H, Goffman L. Generalized motor abilities and timing behavior in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2010;53:383–39. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0204). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES