Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Apr 29.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Sci. 2012 Oct 15;23(11):1410–1416. doi: 10.1177/0956797612444903

Feeling Blue or Turquoise? Emotional Differentiation in Major Depressive Disorder

Emre Demiralp 1, Renee J Thompson 2, Jutta Mata 2, Susanne M Jaeggi 3, Martin Buschkuehl 1,3, Lisa Feldman Barrett 4,5, Phoebe C Ellsworth 1, Metin Demiralp 6, Luis Hernandez-Garcia 1, Patricia J Deldin 1, Ian H Gotlib 2, John Jonides 1
PMCID: PMC4004625  NIHMSID: NIHMS565739  PMID: 23070307

Abstract

Some individuals have very specific and differentiated emotional experiences, such as anger, shame, excitement, and happiness, whereas others have more general affective experiences of pleasure or discomfort that are not as highly differentiated. Considering that individuals with major depressive disorder (MDD) have cognitive deficits for negative information, we predicted that people with MDD would have less differentiated negative emotional experiences than would healthy people. To test this hypothesis, we assessed participants' emotional experiences using a 7-day experience-sampling protocol. Depression was assessed using structured clinical interviews and the Beck Depression Inventory-II. As predicted, individuals with MDD had less differentiated emotional experiences than did healthy participants, but only for negative emotions. These differences were above and beyond the effects of emotional intensity and variability.

Keywords: emotions, depression, happiness, emotional control, individual differences


Imagine yourself at the funeral of a friend from high school. You might be filled with an intense feeling of sorrow. You might also feel happy that he lived a fulfilling life. And you might feel awed by the large crowd that has gathered to honor him. Perhaps you also feel surprised to see acquaintances and friends that you have not seen in some time, along with frustration about losing someone close to you, but also determination to make the most of every moment of every day that follows.

At every waking moment, people have access to exteroceptive information from their senses, interoceptive information from their muscles and internal organs, and conceptual information about the past, the present, and the future. Emotional processes combine these pieces of information, allowing a person to experience discrete states of happiness, sadness, anger, elation, and a variety of other emotional states (Barrett, Mesquita, Ochsner, & Gross, 2007; Ellsworth & Scherer, 2003; Frijda, 1986). Thus, during daily events, people often experience a changing stream of emotions. Moreover, the situations encountered during daily life are often multidimensional, with elements that can lead a person to feel both happiness and sadness at the same moment.

Sometimes the emotions people experience are discrete and highly specific, as in this funeral example. But another person in the same situation might feel a general gnawing unpleasantness or deep despair with no distinguishing features. The more differentiated people's emotional reactions, the better able they are to calibrate their behavioral responses to the demands of specific situations (Barrett, Gross, Christensen, & Benvenuto, 2001). For instance, whether one feels undifferentiated anger or anger laced with shame and guilt determines the course of action one will take in response to a specific situation (Ellsworth & Tong, 2006). Previous research has conceptualized and quantified the differentiation among emotions (Barrett, 2004; Barrett & Bliss-Moreau, 2009). Furthermore, investigators have shown that people differ in their degree of emotional differentiation (e.g., Barrett, 2004; Kashdan, Ferssizidis, Collins, & Muraven, 2010). In this article, we report an investigation of whether people diagnosed with major depressive disorder (MDD) experience less differentiated emotions in daily life than do healthy people. We use our results to ground a discussion of how the ability to differentiate a variety of emotional experiences plays an adaptive role in dealing with life stressors.

MDD is a common and debilitating psychiatric condition. Approximately one in six people in the United States experience MDD during their lifetime. The cost of medical treatment and lost productivity is approximately $85 billion each year (Greenberg et al., 2003). Depressed people are 30 times more likely to commit suicide than are healthy individuals (Joiner, 2010) and 5 times more likely to abuse drugs. They are twice as likely to take sick days and 7 times more likely to be unemployed (Lerner et al., 2004). Depression also aggravates the course of cardiovascular conditions; it is linked to obesity, osteoporosis, arthritis, type 2 diabetes, certain cancers, periodontal disease, and frailty. Moreover, depression down-regulates immune responses, and makes it more difficult to quit smoking.

A primary reason for the widespread and cascading adverse effects of depression is that this mood disorder fundamentally alters the way in which the self is situated in the physical and social world. People with depression have overly general autobiographical memory (Williams & Scott, 1988). Compared with healthy individuals, they have greater difficulty removing irrelevant information from short-term memory (Joormann, Nee, Berman, Jonides, & Gotlib, 2010); are less able to perceive contrasts in the visual world (Bubl, Kern, Ebert, Bach, & Ludger, 2010); are less sensitive to context in emotional processing (Rottenberg, Gross, & Gotlib, 2005); and are impaired in executive functioning (Joormann, 2005). The common theme among these findings is that depression is associated with a diminished ability to differentiate elements of information from one another, whether at the level of perception, memorial processing, or executive functioning. Each of these elements is thought to be associated with momentary emotional experience (Barrett, 2006b). Accordingly, we hypothesized that people with depression have less differentiated emotional experiences in their daily lives than do healthy people.

When people experience a discrete emotional state, such as anger or sadness or fear, they attend to certain features of the stimulus field and ignore others. People's states of pleasure and arousal become meaningfully conceptualized, so that it is possible to make reasonable inferences about these states, to predict what to do to resolve or enhance them, and to communicate these experiences to other people (e.g., Barrett, 2006b). For instance, consider two situations that typically provoke anger. When healthy people miss an appointment because of not waking up on time, they usually feel angry with themselves (Ellsworth & Tong, 2006); that is, they experience anger directed at the self rather than the world. In contrast, when a driver disregards a stop sign and crashes into another person's car, a healthy victim's experience of anger does not have elements of shame and guilt; rather, the referent of the experience of anger is the driver who disregarded the stop sign. To ameliorate these situations, healthy people would take actions that are appropriate to their specific emotional experience of anger directed at the self or the other person, respectively. People who lack the ability to differentiate these particular emotional states from each other or from a general feeling of unpleasantness might choose actions that are not appropriate to the current context and might exacerbate the problem.

Studying the experience of emotion presents a challenge to researchers because it is difficult to study subjective experience objectively. Reviews of the literature indicate that there is no essential signature within the brain or body that is specific to a particular emotion as humans experience it (Barrett, 2006a; Mauss & Robinson, 2009)—much as a particular wavelength of light is assigned to different color categories by different people (Barrett, 2006b; Berlin & Kay, 1969). Furthermore, extensive research suggests that people make biased responses when asked to evaluate their emotional lives (Dunning, Heath, & Suls, 2004); therefore, it is not possible to obtain an accurate measure of emotional differentiation simply by asking individuals how differentiated they feel their emotions are (although this might be useful information for understanding meta-emotional processes). One of the best ways to study subjective experiences in a more objective way is to use experience sampling to assess the richness of momentary emotional experience as it is lived (Barrett & Barrett, 2001; Larsen & Csikszentmihalyi, 1983). With this procedure, individuals are probed at various times throughout the day and are asked to characterize their momentary experience using a set of emotion adjectives.

We used this method to measure participants' emotional experiences over the course of a week. The ratings were later analyzed to reveal the extent to which people reported differentiated versus global emotional feelings (Barrett & Bliss-Moreau, 2009). Specifically, the patterns of correlation in the reports provide an objective estimate of emotional differentiation. For example, if temporal fluctuations in anger are highly correlated with temporal fluctuations in sadness across situations, then from a statistical standpoint, “anger” and “sadness” describe the same state (i.e., negative emotion), and these emotions are not well differentiated (e.g., Barrett, 2004). The less related changes in the two emotions are over time, the higher an individual's emotional differentiation.

We predicted that people with MDD would experience less differentiated emotions than would demographically matched healthy individuals. Given that the cognitive biases exhibited by individuals diagnosed with MDD appear to be stronger for the processing of negative than of positive information, we hypothesized that the reduction in emotional differentiation would be limited to negative emotional experiences. We predicted further that emotional differentiation is unique from other emotional constructs implicated in MDD, such as emotional intensity or variability (an association between depression and alterations of the latter constructs within the same sample used in the present study has been described in Thompson et al., 2012). We did not have specific predictions about whether emotional differentiation would be related to average emotional intensity; however, given that previous work has shown that lower clarity of emotion is related to higher emotional variability (Thompson, Dizén, & Berenbaum, 2009), we did expect to find an inverse relation between differentiation and emotional variance throughout the experience-sampling period.

Method

Participants

One hundred six participants between the ages of 18 and 40 (M = 27.8 years, SD = 6.5 years) were recruited for the current study, which was part of a larger project (see Mata et al., 2012; Thompson et al., 2011; Thompson et al., 2012). All of the participants were native English speakers. Individuals were eligible to participate in the control group (n = 53; 71.7% women, 28.3% men) if they had no current or past history of any mental health disorders and scored below 9 on the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI; Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996). To be eligible for the depressed group (n = 53; 67.9% women, 32.1% men), individuals had to have a current diagnosis of MDD, as assessed by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1997), and a BDI-II score above 13; in addition, they could not have (a) a history of alcohol or drug dependence in the past 6 months, (b) a Bipolar I or II diagnosis, or (c) any psychotic disorders. The MDD and control groups did not differ in years of education, gender, or race and ethnicity composition; however, depressed participants (M = 28.2, SD = 6.4) were on average 3 years older than healthy participants (M = 25.4, SD = 6.4), t(104) = −2.19, p = .03. Including age as a covariate in the analyses did not influence any of the reported results.

Participants were recruited from the communities surrounding the University of Michigan in Ann Arbor, Michigan, and Stanford University in Stanford, California. Advertisements were posted online (e.g., Craigslist) and at local agencies and businesses (e.g., bulletin boards). Approximately equivalent numbers of participants were recruited in the two locations. Participants recruited at the two sites differed in gender composition, χ2(1, N = 106) = 11.77, p < .01, with the Michigan sample having more men (44.6% men) than the Stanford sample (14.0 % men). There was also a significant difference in years of education between the two sites, χ2(3, N = 106) = 9.67, p < .05: Whereas the majority of the Michigan sample (55.4%) reported having completed “some college,” the majority of the Stanford sample (66%) reported having earned a bachelor's degree or a professional degree. This difference in education status is also reflected in an age difference: The Michigan sample (M = 24.2 years, SD = 5.5 years) was younger than the Stanford sample (M = 29.7 years, SD = 6.5 years), t(104) = 4.69, p < .01. The two sites did not differ in ethnic and racial distribution, χ2(5, N = 106) = 4.78, or in depression status, χ2(1, N = 106) = 1.00, both ps > .1. Because the samples did not differ on central variables of interest (i.e., emotion ratings), we combined the two samples for the remaining analyses (see Table S1 in the Supplemental Material available online for detailed demographic information).

Materials and procedure

Participants were administered the SCID-I and BDI prior to the experience-sampling period. If more than 2 weeks had passed since the administration of the SCID-I, participants' diagnostic status was reassessed with another SCID-I to ensure eligibility. Participants were provided with handheld electronic devices (Palm Pilot Z22), were individually instructed on the experience-sampling protocol, and completed a full practice trial. The handheld devices were programmed using the Experience Sampling Program, Version 4.0 (Barrett & Barrett, 2001). Participants were prompted (via a tone signal) eight times per day between 10 a.m. and 10 p.m. The majority of the participants carried the device for 7 to 8 days. Prompts (trials) occurred at random times within eight 90-min windows per day; thus, prompts could occur from as little as 2 min apart to almost 180 min apart. After participants were prompted for a given trial, they had 3 min to respond to the initial question on the Palm Pilot; otherwise, the device switched to hibernation until the next prompt, and the data for that trial were recorded as missing. Up to 56 trials of data were recorded for each participant. The depressed and control participants did not differ in the number of completed trials. Participants provided informed consent and were compensated for their participation in the study; they received an extra incentive for responding to more than 90% of the prompts.

On each trial, participants were asked to use a 4-point scale (not at all = 1, little = 2, much = 3, a great deal = 4) to indicate the degree to which each of 11 emotion adjectives described their current emotional state. There were 7 negative-emotion adjectives (sad, anxious, angry, frustrated, ashamed, disgusted, and guilty) and 4 positive-emotion adjectives (happy, excited, alert, and active). The adjectives were drawn from various sources, such as the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988) and previous studies (e.g., Ekman, Friesen, & Ellsworth, 1972).

Calculation of emotion differentiation, intensity, and variability

To quantify emotional differentiation, we calculated for each participant the Pearson's correlations between all possible pairs of negative emotions (rsad,anxious, rsad,angry, rsad,frustrated, …) and all possible pairs of positive emotions (rhappy,excited, rhappy,alert, rhappy,active, …; Tugade, Fredrickson, & Barrett, 2004). The average of the Fisher's z-transformed correlations was used to quantify positive and negative emotional differentiation for each participant. The larger the average correlation, the less the person distinguished between various categories of emotional experience when describing his or her feelings. We then transformed the scores by subtracting them from 1, such that larger values indicate higher differentiation.

Emotional intensity was measured by averaging the emotion ratings at each prompt, separately for the seven negative and four positive emotions. Then, we obtained one negative and one positive intensity score for each participant by calculating the mean for each set of adjectives across the entire sampling period. Higher scores indicated that the individual experienced emotions with higher intensity.

Temporal variability was measured by calculating the variance of the intensity of each emotion over the sampling period, again separately for the negative and positive emotions. Higher scores indicated that the individual experienced emotions with greater variability. Emotions with zero temporal variance were pruned from the correlation-based differentiation analyses. Considering that four positive and seven negative emotions were used in this study, we excluded from the analyses participants with three or more positive emotions with zero temporal variance (1 MDD participant) and participants with six or more negative emotions with zero temporal variance (5 control participants) because correlations could not be calculated with such data (see Bootstrap Analyses in the Supplemental Material for a description of the robustness of our analyses to excluding these participants). In order to investigate the effect of differentiation above and beyond the effects of emotional intensity and variability, we included intensity and variability as covariates in our analyses.

Results

Emotional differentiation

As predicted, people with MDD had less differentiated negative emotions (Table 1) than did healthy participants, F(1, 98) = 7.18, p < .01, d = −0.54. Also as predicted, there was no difference between the MDD and control participants in differentiation of positive emotions. A two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) yielded no significant main effects of participant group or valence, but did yield a significant interaction of participant group and valence, F(1, 196) = 6.60, p = .01, suggesting that the differences in differentiation between people with MDD and control participants was limited to negative emotions. Negative and positive emotional differentiation were not correlated for people in either group, which suggests that differentiation of positive and negative emotions depends on different psychological mechanisms (see Table S2 in the Supplemental Material for differentiation scores for individual emotion pairs).

Table 1. Mean Emotion Differentiation, Intensity, and Variability Scores.

Positive emotions Negative emotions


Measure Participants with MDD Control participants Participants with MDD Control participants
Differentiation 0.57 (0.21) 0.54 (0.18) 0.51 (0.22) 0.64 (0.27)
Intensity 1.68 (0.39) 2.17 (0.45) 1.88 (0.53) 1.15 (0.17)
Variability 0.51 (0.24) 0.50 (0.27) 0.59 (0.29) 0.14 (0.13)

Note: Standard deviations are in parentheses. MDD = major depressive disorder.

An examination of the mean levels of differentiation of emotion for men and women led us to investigate the role of gender. Although we had no a priori hypotheses concerning gender differences in emotion differentiation in MDD, we conducted an exploratory three-way ANOVA on emotion differentiation including gender as the third factor (with valence and group). The three-way interaction was not significant, F(1, 192) = 2.32, p ≤ .13; thus, gender was not a significant moderator of the relation between depression and emotional differentiation (see Table S3 in the Supplemental Material for results broken down by gender and for a description of additional bootstrapping analyses that explain and confirm this finding).

Emotional differentiation versus emotional intensity and variability

It was possible that the observed differences in differentiation were due to group differences in emotional intensity or variability. In fact, in previous research examining the same data, we found that the MDD and control groups differed in emotional intensity (Mata et al., 2012) and variability (Thompson et al., 2012). To test whether group differences in intensity (Table 1) and variability (Table 1) were linked to group differences in emotional differentiation, we used two multiple regression models, one predicting changes in positive emotional differentiation and one predicting changes in negative emotional differentiation. Predictors were depression as a nominal variable and intensity and variability as continuous predictors. After we controlled for intensity and variability, depression remained a significant predictor of low differentiation only for negative emotions, F(1, 96) = 7.51, p < .01; thus, between-group differences in negative differentiation were not due to differences in intensity and variability. Emotional variability remained a significant predictor of both negative emotional differentiation, F(1, 96) = 6.53, p < .02, and positive emotional differentiation, F(1, 96) = 5.21, p < .03, whereas emotional intensity was a significant predictor of only positive emotional differentiation, F(1, 96) = 4.25, p < .05.

Positive emotional intensity was modestly negatively correlated with emotional differentiation for both people with MDD, r(50) = −0.19, p < .09, and control participants, r(46) = −0.21, p < .08; that is, individuals who experienced positive emotions with higher intensity tended to have less differentiated positive emotional experiences. There was no significant relation between negative emotional intensity and differentiation for either group (see Table 2).

Table 2. Correlations of Emotional Differentiation With Emotional Intensity and Variability.

Positive emotions Negative emotions


Measure Participants with MDD Control participants Participants with MDD Control participants
Intensity -0.19, p < .09 -0.21, p < .08 0.12, p > .19 -0.13, p > .17
Variability -0.35, p < .005 -0.22, p < .07 -0.27, p < .03 -0.21, p < .08

Note: MDD = major depressive disorder.

For people with MDD, emotional variability was negatively correlated with both positive emotional differentiation, r(50) = -.35, p < .005, and negative emotional differentiation, r(50) = −.27 p < .03. Similarly, control participants exhibited a tendency for emotional variability to be negatively correlated with both positive emotional differentiation, r(46) = −.22, p < .07, and negative emotional differentiation, r(46) = −.21, p < .08. These results suggest that individuals whose emotional experiences were more variable across time had less differentiated experiences of positive and negative emotions (see Table 2; also see Tables S3, S4, and S5 in the Supplemental Material for adjusted mean differentiation scores; for differentiation, intensity, and variability scores by gender; and for correlations of intensity and variability with differentiation by gender).

Discussion

The present study is the first to show that people diagnosed with MDD experience negative emotions with less differentiation in their daily lives than do healthy individuals. We found that the relation between emotional differentiation and depression could not be accounted for by emotional intensity or variability. This finding suggests a fundamental way in which the emotional lives of individuals diagnosed with MDD are altered independently of increased negative emotional intensity and emotional variability.

Earlier research supporting the affect-as-information perspective has shown that specific emotional states (e.g., anger, sadness, and fear) have more adaptive value than do global affective states (e.g., pleasant and unpleasant states). This is because specific and differentiated negative emotional experiences are less subject to misattribution errors (Schwarz & Clore, 1996). One of the important distinguishing features of discrete emotional states is that specific emotions are generally associated with a causal object, whereas undifferentiated, global affective states are not (Russell & Barrett, 1999). It is important to identify the source and cause of an emotional state in order to generate an adaptive response. In fact, earlier research has shown that negative emotional differentiation is correlated with emotion regulation (Barrett et al., 2001). Accordingly, decreased differentiation might lead people with MDD to regulate their emotions less frequently than healthy people do. Future research should specifically test this hypothesis.

It is important to note that people with MDD did not differ from control participants in positive emotional differentiation. First, this finding suggests that the psychological mechanisms underlying emotional differentiation can be selectively altered for negative emotions, as one might expect to be the case in individuals with MDD. Second, given that participants with MDD experienced positive emotions with decreased intensity (see Table 1) but exhibited unaltered differentiation, this finding further supports the claim that emotional differentiation, intensity, and variability are independent constructs. It could be that people with MDD use differentiated positive emotional experiences as a buffer against life stressors (Tugade et al., 2004), and this may be one of the reasons that they retain differentiation among positive emotions. It could also be that the high-arousal positive emotions that we sampled are not representative of the domains in which people with MDD might experience alterations in emotional differentiation. In future studies, we plan to use a larger number of positive emotions, including such emotions as compassion and calmness.

Previous research has shown that depression is associated with alexithymia (Honkalampi, Hintikka, Tanskannen, Lehtonn, & Vilnamaki, 2000), defined as the inability to recognize and verbalize emotions. Alexithymia is often associated with emptiness of feelings, poverty of imagination, difficulty in communicating with other people, lack of positive emotions, and high prevalence of negative emotion. The major difficulty in interpreting this body of research is its reliance on people's self-reports of their ability to differentiate emotions (Dunning et al., 2004). Earlier research suggests that people make flawed, biased responses when asked to evaluate their abilities globally, and that global abilities are best captured using skill-based measurements. Therefore, it is important to measure individuals' subjective emotional experiences and to quantify their degree of emotional differentiation. This is especially important if one wants to understand the mechanisms of emotional experience in healthy and depressed populations.

Despite some success in developing empirically validated diagnoses and interventions for depression and other mental health disorders, the mechanisms of even the most successful treatments are still unclear. For example, in a recent summary of the literature on mechanisms of change in psychotherapy research, Kazdin (2007) concluded, “After decades of psychotherapy research, we cannot provide an evidence based explanation for how or why even our most well studied interventions produce change, that is, the mechanism(s) through which they operate” (p. 1). This noted lack of progress may be due, in part, to how researchers measure emotional states in various psychopathologies. The present results indicate that the use of momentary experience sampling, coupled with techniques that extract relations among emotions, may be helpful in increasing understanding of the structure and dynamics of emotional experience in depression (Palmier-Claus et al., 2011; Peeters, Nicolson, Berkhof, Delespaul, & deVries, 2003; Wichers et al., 2012) because experience sampling goes beyond retrospective self-report, which is influenced by beliefs and attitudes. Experience sampling should allow a broader perspective in examinations of other mental illnesses as well, by making it possible to study not only what individuals report feeling in the past, but also what they feel on a moment-to-moment basis in the present (which may not be the same; Robinson & Clore, 2002). Greater breadth of perspective and assessment of momentary experience may inform the development of improved diagnostic criteria, which should play a crucial role in the development and validation of more effective treatments.

Supplementary Material

Bootstrap Analyses
Table S1
Table S2
Table S3
Table S4
Table S5

Acknowledgments

Jutta Mata is now at the University of Basel. The authors thank Courtney Behnke and Sarah Victor for their assistance in project management.

Funding: This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grants MH60655 (to John Jonides), MH59259 (to Ian H. Gotlib), and F32 MH091831 (to Renee J. Thompson); by Swiss National Foundation Fellowship PA001/117473 to Susanne M. Jaeggi; and by Fellowship SFRH/BPD/35953/2007 from the Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia and Fellowship Wi3496/41 from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, awarded to Jutta Mata.

Footnotes

Declaration of Conflicting Interests: The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.

References

  1. Barrett LF. Feelings or words? Understanding the content in self-report ratings of experienced emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2004;87:266–281. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.2.266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Barrett LF. Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2006a;1:28–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00003.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barrett LF. Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the experience of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2006b;10:20–46. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barrett LF, Barrett DJ. An introduction to computerized experience sampling in psychology. Social Science Computer Review. 2001;19:175–185. [Google Scholar]
  5. Barrett LF, Bliss-Moreau E. Affect as a psychological primitive. In: Zanna MP, editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. Vol. 41. London, England: Academic Press; 2009. pp. 167–218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barrett LF, Gross J, Christensen TC, Benvenuto M. Knowing what you're feeling and knowing what to do about it: Mapping the relation between emotion differentiation and emotion regulation. Cognition & Emotion. 2001;15:713–724. [Google Scholar]
  7. Barrett LF, Mesquita B, Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. The experience of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology. 2007;58:373–403. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  9. Berlin B, Kay P. Basic color terms: Their universality and evolution. Berkeley: University of California Press; 1969. [Google Scholar]
  10. Bubl E, Kern E, Ebert D, Bach M, Ludger TE. Seeing gray when feeling blue? Depression can be measured in the eye of the diseased. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;68:205–208. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dunning D, Heath C, Suls J. Flawed self-assessment: Implications for health, education, and the workplace. Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 2004;5:69–106. doi: 10.1111/j.1529-1006.2004.00018.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Ekman P, Friesen WV, Ellsworth P. Emotion in the human face: Guidelines for research and integration of human findings. New York, NY: Pergamon Press; 1972. [Google Scholar]
  13. Ellsworth PC, Scherer KR. Appraisal processes in emotion. In: Davidson RJ, Scherer KR, Goldsmith HH, editors. Handbook of affective sciences. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2003. pp. 572–595. [Google Scholar]
  14. Ellsworth PC, Tong EMW. What does it mean to be angry at yourself? Categories, appraisals, and the problem of language. Emotion. 2006;6:572–586. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.4.572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders: Clinician's version. New York, NY: New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research Department; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  16. Frijda NH. The emotions. London, England: Cambridge University Press; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  17. Greenberg PE, Kessler RC, Birnbaum HG, Leong SA, Lowe SW, Berglund PA, Corey-Lisle PK. The economic burden of depression in the United States: How did it change between 1990 and 2000? Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2003;64:1465–1475. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v64n1211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Honkalampi K, Hintikka J, Tanskannen A, Lehtonn J, Vilna-maki H. Depression is strongly associated with alexi-thymia in the general population. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2000;48:99–104. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(99)00083-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Joiner T. Myths about suicide. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  20. Joormann J. Inhibition, rumination and mood regulation in depression. In: Engle RW, Sedek G, von Hecker U, McIntosh DN, editors. Cognitive limitations in aging and psychopa-thology: Attention, working memory, and executive functions. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2005. pp. 275–312. [Google Scholar]
  21. Joormann J, Nee DE, Berman MG, Jonides J, Gotlib IH. Interference resolution in major depression. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2010;10:21–33. doi: 10.3758/CABN.10.1.21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kashdan TB, Ferssizidis P, Collins RL, Muraven M. Emotion differentiation as resilience against excessive alcohol use: An ecological momentary assessment in underage social drinkers. Psychological Science. 2010;21:1341–1347. doi: 10.1177/0956797610379863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kazdin AE. Mediators and mechanisms of change in psychotherapy research. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2007;3:1–27. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Larsen R, Csikszentmihalyi M. The experience sampling method. In: Reis HT, editor. Naturalistic approaches to studying social interaction. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 1983. pp. 41–56. [Google Scholar]
  25. Lerner D, Adler DA, Chang H, Lapitsky L, Hood MY, Peris-sinotto C, et al. Rogers WH. Unemployment, job retention, and productivity loss among employees with depression. Psychiatric Services. 2004;55:1371–1378. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.55.12.1371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Mata J, Thompson RJ, Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Gotlib IH. Walk on the bright side: Physical activity and affect in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2012;121:297–308. doi: 10.1037/a0023533. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Mauss IB, Robinson MD. Measures of emotion: A review. Cognition & Emotion. 2009;23:209–237. doi: 10.1080/02699930802204677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Palmier-Claus JE, Myin-Germeys I, Barkus E, Bentley L, Udachina A, Delespaul PA, et al. Dunn G. Experience sampling research in individuals with mental illness: Reflections and guidance. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2011;123:12–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01596.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Peeters F, Nicolson NA, Berkhof J, Delespaul P, deVries M. Effects of daily events on mood states in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2003;112:203–211. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.112.2.203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Robinson MD, Clore GL. Belief and feeling: Evidence for an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128:934–960. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Rottenberg J, Gross JJ, Gotlib IH. Emotion context insensitivity in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2005;114:627–639. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Russell JA, Barrett LF. Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the elephant. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1999;76:805–819. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.76.5.805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Schwarz N, Clore GL. Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In: Higgins ET, Kruglanski A, editors. Social psychology: A handbook of basic principles. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1996. pp. 433–465. [Google Scholar]
  34. Thompson RJ, Dizén M, Berenbaum H. The unique relations between emotional awareness and facets of affective instability. Journal of Research in Personality. 2009;43:875–879. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Thompson RJ, Mata J, Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Gotlib IH. Concurrent and prospective relations between attention to emotion and affect intensity: An experience sampling study. Emotion. 2011;11:1489–1494. doi: 10.1037/a0022822. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Thompson RJ, Mata J, Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Gotlib IH. The everyday emotional experience of adults with major depressive disorder: Examining emotional instability, inertia, and reactivity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2012 doi: 10.1037/a0027978. Advance online publication. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Tugade MM, Fredrickson BL, Barrett LF. Psychological resilience and positive emotional granularity: Examining the benefits of positive emotions on coping and health. Journal of Personality. 2004;72:1161–1190. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00294.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;54:1063–1070. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Wichers M, Peeters F, Rutten BP, Jacobs N, Derom C, Thiery E, et al. van Os J. A time-lagged momentary assessment study on daily life physical activity and affect. Health Psychology. 2012;31:135–144. doi: 10.1037/a0025688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Williams JMG, Scott J. Autobiographical memory in depression. Psychological Medicine. 1988;18:689–695. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700008370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Bootstrap Analyses
Table S1
Table S2
Table S3
Table S4
Table S5

RESOURCES