Abstract
BACKGROUND:
Television and insufficient sleep are associated with poor mental and physical health. This study assessed associations of TV viewing and bedroom TV with sleep duration from infancy to midchildhood.
METHOD:
We studied 1864 children in Project Viva. Parents reported children’s average daily TV viewing and sleep (at 6 months and annually from 1–7 years) and the presence of a bedroom TV (annually 4–7 years). We used mixed effects models to assess associations of TV exposures with contemporaneous sleep, adjusting for child age, gender, race/ethnicity, maternal education, and income.
RESULTS:
Six hundred forty-three children (35%) were racial/ethnic minorities; 37% of households had incomes ≤$70 000. From 6 months to 7 years, mean (SD) sleep duration decreased from 12.2 (2.0) hours to 9.8 (0.9) hours per day; TV viewing increased from 0.9 (1.2) hours to 1.6 (1.0) hours per day. At 4 years, 17% had a bedroom TV, rising to 23% at 7 years. Each 1 hour per day increase in lifetime TV viewing was associated with 7 minutes per day (95% confidence interval [CI]: 4 to 10) shorter sleep. The association of bedroom TV varied by race/ethnicity; bedroom TV was associated with 31 minutes per day shorter sleep (95% CI: 16 to 45) among racial/ethnic minority children, but not among white, non-Hispanic children (8 fewer minutes per day [95% CI: −19 to 2]).
CONCLUSIONS:
More TV viewing, and, among racial/ethnic minority children, the presence of a bedroom TV, were associated with shorter sleep from infancy to midchildhood.
Keywords: television, sleep duration, sleep hygiene, childhood
What’s Known on This Subject:
Inadequate sleep in childhood is associated with poor mental and physical health. Numerous cross-sectional studies reveal associations between television viewing and the presence of a bedroom TV and inadequate sleep in older children and adolescents, but longitudinal research is limited.
What This Study Adds:
More TV viewing, and, among racial/ethnic minority children, bedroom TV, were associated with shorter sleep from infancy to midchildhood. These results raise the possibility that interventions to reduce TV could improve children’s sleep.
Inadequate sleep in childhood is associated with poor mental and physical health, including impaired academic performance, depression, injury, and increased obesity risk.1–8 Among many factors that influence children’s sleep, television 1RCMB_20130063OC_wc_f4.tif (TV) viewing and the presence of a TV in the bedroom (bedroom TV) are increasingly prevalent in young children’s lives.9,10 TV behaviors may directly displace sleep time or increase mental/emotional arousal and light exposure, which can be detrimental to sleep onset, duration, and quality.11–15 Numerous cross-sectional studies identify associations of TV behavior with inadequate sleep or sleep disturbance in older children and adolescents,11,16–19 but longitudinal research is limited.20 For example, evening media use has been associated with increased sleep problems in preschool-aged children,14 and National Sleep Foundation data reveal associations of bedroom TV with shorter sleep duration in 6- to 10-year-olds.21 A recent study in Finnish schoolchildren, the first to examine TV and sleep duration longitudinally, revealed that TV viewing predicted shorter weekday sleep 18 months later.20
Our study examines longitudinal associations of TV viewing and bedroom TV with sleep duration from infancy to midchildhood and explores differences by gender and race/ethnicity. Sleep in early childhood is particularly important to child health.8,22 We hypothesized that greater TV viewing and bedroom TV would predict shorter sleep duration.
Methods
Population and Study Design
We studied participants in Project Viva, a longitudinal cohort of 2128 children (born 1999–2003) and their mothers founded to examine effects of diet and other factors during pregnancy and after birth. This analysis included 1864 mothers and children (86%) who reported average sleep and TV viewing at ≥1 assessment from infancy (6 months) to midchildhood (7 years); Table 1. We recruited women at prenatal visits to 8 Harvard Vanguard Medical Associates practices in Massachusetts. Details of recruitment/retention procedures are available elsewhere.23 After obtaining written informed consent, we performed in-person study visits with mothers and children in infancy (mean age, 6 months) and early (mean age, 3.2 years) and midchildhood (mean age, 7.9 years). Mothers completed questionnaires at 1, 2, 4, 5, and 6 years postpartum. Institutional review boards of participating institutions approved the study.
TABLE 1.
Overall | Lifetime Average TV Viewing <2 h per Daya (N = 1507; 81%) | Lifetime Average TV Viewing ≥2 h per Day (N = 357; 19%) | |
---|---|---|---|
Maternal characteristics | |||
Age at enrollment, mean (SD), y | 32.1 (5.1) | 32.4 (4.9) | 30.7 (5.8) |
Prepregnancy BMI, mean (SD) | 24.9 (5.5) | 24.6 (5.2) | 26.0 (6.2) |
Educational attainment, N (%) | |||
College graduate or higher | 1254 (68) | 1083 (71) | 171 (51) |
Nulliparous, N (%) | |||
Yes | 896 (48) | 734 (48) | 162 (49) |
Household income, N (%) | |||
>$70 000 | 1064 (63) | 931 (67) | 133 (47) |
Married or cohabitating, N (%) | |||
Yes | 1710 (92) | 1434 (94) | 276 (83) |
Child characteristics, N (%) | |||
Gender | |||
Boy | 958 (51) | 786 (51) | 172 (52) |
Race/ethnicity | |||
White | 1215 (65) | 1041 (68) | 174 (52) |
Black | 288 (16) | 199 (13) | 89 (27) |
Latino | 102 (5) | 76 (5) | 26 (8 |
Asian | 76 (4) | 66 (4) | 10 (3) |
Other | 177 (10) | 142 (9) | 35 (10) |
Bedroom TV any time between 4- and 7-y assessments | 397 (28) | 245 (21) | 152 (54) |
Lifetime average TV viewing is calculated as the subject-specific mean TV viewing averaged over all measurement occasions from infancy to midchildhood.
Main Exposures: TV Viewing and Bedroom TV
At 6 months and 1 year, mothers reported average hours and minutes in the past week children were in a place where a TV that was on could be seen. At each assessment from 2 to 7 years, mothers selected children’s average hours per day of TV and video viewing in the past month from response categories (None, <1, 1–3, 4–6, 7–9, and ≥10 hours per day), separating weekdays and weekends. We assigned numeric values to categories (0, 0.5, 2, 5, 8, and 10, respectively) and computed weighted averages to construct an approximately continuous measure of TV in hours per day. This TV viewing measure, adapted from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth,24 is linked in a dose-response manner with childhood overweight.25 At each assessment from 4 to 7 years, parents reported the presence of a TV in the room where children slept (bedroom TV [yes or no]).
Main Outcome: Sleep Duration
At 6 months, mothers reported children’s average 24-hour sleep duration over the past month in hours and minutes in response to 3 separate questions about morning naps, afternoon naps, and nighttime sleep. At 1 year, respondents reported average total 24-hour sleep duration over the past month in hours and minutes including morning naps, afternoon naps, and nighttime sleep in response to a single question. From 2 to 4 and at 7 years, mothers selected children’s average 24-hour sleep duration over the past month (separating weekdays and weekends) from categories (<9, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, and ≥14 hours per day), which we combined in a weighted average sleep by assigning each a value in hours per day (8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, and 14, respectively). At 5 and 6 years, mothers reported average nightly sleep in hours per day without separating weekdays from weekends. We combined all sleep questions to form an approximately continuous measure of sleep duration in hours per day.
Study Covariates
Using self-administered questionnaires and interviews, we collected information about prespecified potential confounders including maternal age, education, prepregnancy BMI, parity, household income, the child’s race/ethnicity, and active play.
Statistical Analysis
We examined longitudinal and cross-sectional associations of TV exposures with sleep duration. Cross-sectional analyses of sleep as predicted by TV viewing used separate linear regression models for each visit or questionnaire adjusted, first, for age in days; a second model additionally adjusted for race/ethnicity, gender, maternal education, and household income. For 4 years or later, we additionally adjusted for bedroom TV. We fit separate models for sleep predicted by bedroom TV, adjusting for the covariates above and for TV viewing. Other potential confounders did not meet inclusion criteria.
Longitudinal analyses used linear mixed effects models with random intercepts and random linear and quadratic slopes for age (including a quadratic term improved model fit) to account for correlations between repeated measures within subjects and allow participants with partial missing data to contribute information. We adjusted for age and age2 (the square of age), then additionally for race/ethnicity, gender, education, and income.
For all longitudinal analyses, we separated time-varying exposure effects into 2 distinct sources of variation. One source is the within-subject variation in sleep, TV viewing, and bedroom TV, which reflects that these factors may change over time within subjects. The within-subject effect addresses the question: if a given child increased/decreased TV viewing, was his/her sleep duration over that period affected? The other source is the between-subject variation, which reflects that, at any given measurement occasion, different individuals have different values for sleep duration and for TV viewing and/or bedroom TV. The between-subject effect addresses the question: do children who watch more/less TV on average from infancy to midchildhood sleep more/less than other children?
To separate within- and between-subject sources of variation, we included mean-centered (subtracting the subject-specific average across time) and subject-specific mean (averaged within-subject over time) variables for both TV viewing and bedroom TV as covariates in models.26 For bedroom TV, we subtracted the average presence of a TV (a proportion) from the presence or not (1 or 0) at each assessment point. The standard linear mixed effects model estimates effects of time-varying covariates on the basis of optimal combinations of within-subject and between-subject variation; if within-subject and between-subject effects are conflicting, they should not be combined in a standard model. For this reason, we separated within-subject effects (centered on the subject-specific mean) from between-subject (averaged from infancy to midchildhood) effects.
To assess potential effect modification, we included interaction terms of gender, race/ethnicity, and age with mean-centered and subject-specific mean TV exposures. We used likelihood ratio tests (LRTs) to determine if full models including interaction terms fit better than simpler models with fewer interactions. Where appropriate (LRT P < .05), we report stratified results and interaction P values.
All analyses used SAS version 9.3 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC).
Results
The sample consisted of equal proportions of girls and boys and was multiethnic (35% racial/ethnic minority). At enrollment, most mothers (68%) had college education and household incomes > $70 000 (63%). Mean (SD) sleep duration increased from 6 months to 1 year from 12.2 (2.0) to 12.8 (1.6) hours per day, remaining relatively constant from 2 to 5 years, then decreasing to 9.8 (0.9) hours per day by 7 years. TV viewing increased 6 months to 4 years from 0.9 (1.2) to 1.7 (1.0) hours per day, then declined slightly to 1.6 (1.0) hours per day at 7 years. At 4, 5, and 6 years, ∼15% of children had a bedroom TV, rising to 23% at 7 years (Table 2). Ever having a bedroom TV was more common among racial/ethnic minority children (53%) than non-Hispanic white children (14%).
TABLE 2.
Assessment | Average Daily Sleep, Hours per Day | Average Daily TV Viewing, Hours per Day | Bedroom TV | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | Mean (SD) | N | Mean (SD) | N (%) | |
6 mo | 1673 | 12.2 (2.0) | 1673 | 0.9 (1.2) | — |
1 y | 1227 | 12.8 (1.6) | 1224 | 1.2 (1.5) | — |
2 y | 1360 | 12.0 (1.3) | 1266 | 1.4 (1.2) | — |
3 y | 1242 | 11.2 (1.1) | 1263 | 1.7 (1.1) | — |
4 y | 1202 | 10.8 (1.1) | 1235 | 1.7 (1.0) | 207 (17) |
5 y | 863 | 10.6 (1.0) | 859 | 1.6 (1.0) | 128 (15) |
6 y | 955 | 10.3 (0.9) | 941 | 1.5 (0.9) | 143 (15) |
7 y | 1170 | 9.8 (0.9) | 1162 | 1.6 (1.0) | 269 (23) |
Data from 1864 participants in the Project Viva cohort.
Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Associations
In cross-sectional models, greater TV viewing was associated with shorter sleep duration. After multivariate adjustment, each additional hour of TV viewing was associated with 3 fewer minutes per day (95% confidence interval [CI]: −2 to 8) of sleep at 6 months; 2 fewer minutes per day (95% CI: −2 to 6) at 3 years; and 6 fewer minutes/day (95% CI: 3 to 9) at 7 years (Table 3). Bedroom TV was also associated with less sleep after multivariate adjustment, independent of TV viewing, at 5, 6, and 7 years.
TABLE 3.
Mean Difference in Sleep Duration, Minutes per Day (95% CI) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | Age-Adjusteda | n | Multivariate-Adjustedb | n | Bedroom TV-Adjustedc | |
TV Viewing | ||||||
6 mo | 1666 | −3 (−8 to 1) | 1512 | −3 (−8 to 2) | — | — |
1 y | 1200 | −7 (−11 to −4) | 1119 | −6 (−9 to −2) | — | — |
2 y | 1224 | −8 (−11 to −5) | 1145 | −6 (−10 to −2) | — | — |
3 y | 1233 | −5 (−9 to −2) | 1153 | −2 (−6 to −2) | — | — |
4 y | 1197 | −5 (−8 to −1) | 1109 | −4 (−8 to 0) | 1108 | −4 (−8 to 0) |
5 y | 850 | −7 (−12 to −3) | 807 | −7 (−11 to −2) | 802 | −6 (−10 to −1) |
6 y | 935 | −12 (−16 to −8) | 879 | −12 (−16 to −7) | 876 | −11 (−15 to −6) |
7 y | 1158 | −13 (−16 to −10) | 1058 | −6 (−9 to −3) | 1054 | −5 (−9 to −2) |
Bedroom TV | ||||||
4 y | 1201 | −16 (−25 to −6) | 1113 | −7 (−19 to 5) | 1108 | −16 (−18 to 6) |
5 y | 858 | −23 (−35 to −11) | 815 | −19 (−32 to −6) | 802 | −18 (−31 to −5) |
6 y | 952 | −31 (−41 to −21) | 896 | −21 (−32 to −10) | 876 | −14 (−26 to −3) |
7 y | 1162 | −36 (−44 to −29) | 1062 | −15 (−24 to −7) | 1054 | −14 (−23 to −5) |
Linear regression models fit in PROC GLM adjusted for child age in years at time of assessment.
Additional adjustments for race/ethnicity, gender, maternal education, and household income.
Additional adjustment for TV viewing or bedroom TV.
In longitudinal models, between-subject analyses revealed that each additional hour per day of lifetime average TV viewing was associated with 7 fewer minutes per day of sleep (95% CI: 4 to 10; Table 4). The effect of lifetime TV viewing did not vary by age, gender, or race/ethnicity, but between-subject associations of bedroom TV with sleep did vary by race/ethnicity (P interaction .002). For racial/ethnic minority children, bedroom TV from 4 to 7 years was associated with 32 fewer minutes per day of sleep (95% CI: 18 to 46; Table 5) over the same period. Additional adjustment for TV viewing did not substantially change this between-subject effect. For non-Hispanic white children, bedroom TV was associated with 12 fewer minutes per day of sleep (95% CI: 1 to 22), which attenuated after adjustment for TV viewing (Table 5). Between-subject effects of bedroom TV did not vary by age or gender.
TABLE 4.
TV Viewing | Mean Difference in Sleep Duration, Minutes per Day (95% CI)a | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
All Children | Male Children | Female Children | P for Interaction | ||||
Age-Adjusted 1864 Subjects; 9463 Observations | Multivariate-Adjusted 1672 Subjects; 8782 Observations | Age-Adjusted 958 Subjects; 4821 Observations | Multivariate-Adjusted 847 Subjects; 4406 Observations | Age-Adjusted 906 Subjects; 4642 Observations | Multivariate- Adjusted 825 Subjects; 4376 Observations | Multivariate-Adjusted 1672 Subjects; 8782 Observations | |
Between-subject (averaged over lifetime) | −11 (−14 to −8) | −7 (−10 to −4) | −12 (−16 to −8) | −8 (−12 to −4) | −11 (−15 to −7) | −6 (−10 to −2) | .72 |
Within-subject (centered on subject-specific mean) | −2 (−4 to 0) | −2 (−3 to 0) | −5 (−7 to −2) | −4 (−7 to −2) | 1 (−2 to 3) | 1 (−2 to 3) | .005 |
Models fit combined with no interactions, as well as separately for boys and girls. Age-adjusted estimates are from linear mixed effects models with random intercepts and slopes for age and age2 fit in PROC MIXED adjusted for age and age2. To decompose within-subject and between-subject effects, we include between-subject (averaged over time) and within-subject (centered on the subject-specific mean) covariates for TV viewing. Multivariate models include additional adjustments for race/ethnicity, maternal education, and household income.
TABLE 5.
Bedroom TV | Mean Difference in Sleep Duration, Minutes per Day (95% CI)a | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
All Children | Non-Hispanic White Children | Racial/Ethnic Minority Children | P for Interaction | |||||||
Age-Adjusted 1438 Subjects; 4173 Observations | Multivariate-Adjusted 1312 Subjects; 3886 Observations | TV Viewing Adjusted 1310 Subjects; 3840 Observations | Age-Adjusted 947 Subjects; 2925 Observations | Multivariate-Adjusted 911 Subjects; 2827 Observations | TV Viewing Adjusted 909 Subjects; 2790 Observations | Age-Adjusted 489 Subjects; 1244 Observations | Multivariate- Adjusted 401 Subjects; 1059 Observations | TV Viewing Adjusted 401 Subjects; 1050 Observations | Multivariate-Adjusted 1312 Subjects; 3886 Observations | |
Between-subject (averaged 4–7 y) | −38 (−44 to −31) | −22 (−31 to −14) | −20 (−28 to −11) | −22 (−32 to −12) | −12 (−22 to −1) | −8 (−19 to 2) | −37 (−48 to −25) | −32 (−46 to −18) | −31 (−45 to −16) | .002 |
Within-subject (centered on subject-specific average) | 0 (−8 to 9) | 1 (−7 to 10) | 1 (−7 to 10) | −3 (−13 to 8) | 0 (−11 to 11) | 0 (−11 to 11) | 1 (−13 to 16) | 0 (−15 to 15) | −1 (−16 to 14) | .87 |
Models fit combined with no interactions, as well as separately for non-Hispanic white and racial/ethnic minority children. Age-adjusted estimates are from linear mixed effects models with random intercepts and slopes for age and age2 fit in PROC MIXED adjusted for age and age2. To decompose within-subject and between-subject effects, we include between-subject (averaged over time) and within-subject (centered on the subject-specific mean) covariates for bedroom TV. Bedroom TV was first asked about at the 4-year assessment; models including bedroom TV were re-centered for this time period. Multivariate models include additional adjustments for gender, maternal education, and household income, and TV-viewing adjusted models include additional adjustment for within and between-subject effects of TV viewing.
Within-subject analyses revealed that associations of individual-level changes in TV viewing with sleep duration varied by gender (P interaction .005). If an individual boy increased TV viewing by 1 hour per day over any period, this change was accompanied by a 4-minute per day decrease in his average sleep duration (95% CI: 2 to 7) over the same period (Table 4). By contrast, individual-level changes in TV viewing among girls did not influence sleep (1 minute per day, 95% CI: −2 to 3; Table 4). In within-subject analyses of bedroom TV, sleep was not associated with individual-level changes in the presence of a bedroom TV (1 minute per day, 95% CI: −7 to 10; Table 5).
Racial/ethnic minority (versus non-Hispanic white) children had lower predicted sleep duration regardless of lifetime TV viewing or bedroom TV (Figs 1 and 2). Overall, between-subject differences (Figs 1 and 2) had stronger associations with sleep duration than within-subject changes in TV viewing (Fig 3). For example, comparing hypothetical white boys with college-educated mothers from households earning > $70 000 year with different durations of TV viewing, our model predicted that a boy who never watched TV and never had a bedroom TV would sleep 19 minutes per day longer in midchildhood than a boy who increased average daily TV viewing from 0 hours per day in infancy to 3 hours per day by midchildhood; 26 minutes per day longer than a boy who watched 3 hours per day of TV every year; and 34 minutes per day longer than a boy who watched 3 hours per day of TV every year and ever had a bedroom TV.
Discussion
In this prospective study of 1864 children, greater TV viewing and bedroom TV were associated with decreased sleep duration. Overall, each additional hour per day of average lifetime TV viewing from infancy through midchildhood was associated with 7 fewer minutes per day of sleep over the same period. By contrast, individual-level changes in TV viewing decreased sleep only among boys. For racial/ethnic minority children, bedroom TV was associated with 31 fewer minutes per day of sleep from 4 to 7 years independent of TV viewing, whereas for non-Hispanic white children associations disappeared after adjustment for TV viewing.
Between-subject (average lifetime TV) and within-subject (individual-level fluctuations) analyses yielded different effect estimates for TV viewing. For all groups, between-subject associations were of greater magnitude than within-subject associations. Within-subject effects are not susceptible to confounding by factors that do not vary within a subject (eg, race/ethnicity and, arguably, home environment or systematic reporting biases). However, between-subject effects could arise from differences in unmeasured factors (eg, parenting skills). Our data suggest that research on the basis of cross-sectional analyses alone may overestimate TV’s influence on children’s sleep. It is also possible that within-subject effects could be underestimated due to random error in reporting.
Of interest, we observed stronger within-subject effects of TV viewing in boys versus girls, and stronger between-subject effects of bedroom TV for racial/ethnic minority versus white children. Differences in effects may be due to differences in underlying susceptibility to external stimuli across population groups: other studies also indicate boys are particularly vulnerable to electronic media’s influence on sleep,20 as well as to the obesogenic effects of inadequate or disturbed sleep.27 Subgroup differences may also relate to differences in patterns of TV viewing or content. For example, TV close to bedtime may be alerting, resulting in a phase delay in sleep onset. Exposure to violent content may lead to later sleep onset or interrupted sleep.14 It is also possible that parents differentially report behaviors such as sleep and TV for boys versus girls, or that parents’ race/ethnicity influences reporting biases.
Previous studies have been mostly cross-sectional or measured sleep problems rather than duration.18,19,28–33 The magnitude of association in our study is similar to that observed in the only other longitudinal study of TV viewing and child sleep duration: among 353 Finnish schoolchildren 10 to 11 years old, TV viewing predicted shorter weekday sleep duration 18 months later (5 fewer minutes per day, 95% CI: 1 to 10; adjusted for gender, grade level, and family structure). However, in the Finnish study, the presence of a bedroom TV was not associated with shorter sleep though it predicted later bedtimes in boys.20 Our study extends a sparse literature by describing longitudinal associations of TV viewing and bedroom TV with sleep duration over a longer follow-up period in a larger and more diverse sample. Further, ours is the first study to examine this important period from infancy to midchildhood when habits are being formed that are likely to persist into later childhood.34
Consistent with our finding that bedroom TV is associated with shorter sleep duration, cross-sectional National Sleep Foundation data reveal that children with a bedroom TV (versus without) slept 9 fewer minutes per day (95% CI: 1 to 19).21 In our study, between-subject associations with bedroom TV were particularly strong among racial/ethnic minorities. This between-subject difference may partially reflect unmeasured confounders, differential reporting by race/ethnicity or unmeasured TV viewing (the presence of a bedroom TV may make it more difficult for parents to accurately report children’s viewing and/or sleep, particularly if children fall asleep watching). However, the strength of the observed association (31 minutes less sleep for bedroom TV) may indicate an adverse influence of bedroom TV on children’s sleep independent of increased TV viewing; bedroom TV may increase screen illumination and mental/emotional arousal near bedtime, negatively influencing sleep duration.13,35 Racial/ethnic minority children in our study watched more TV and slept less than white non-Hispanic children; others have noted similar disparities by race/ethnicity in TV viewing, bedroom TV,10,36 and sleep duration.37,38
Our study makes a unique contribution to the literature: to our knowledge, no previous study of TV and sleep duration includes a cohort of children followed from infancy to midchildhood. Further, no previous study has separated time-varying TV exposures into within- and between-subject sources of variation to address the different impacts on sleep duration of lifetime average TV and individual-level changes in TV. This study also had several limitations. Measures of TV and sleep were from parent-report, which may result in misclassification. Because children sleep less as they age,39 reverse causation (less sleep leading to greater TV) is a possibility. However, we controlled for age-related declines in sleep by adjusting for age and including random slopes for age and age2 in longitudinal models. Finally, although there are many forms of screen media (eg, mobile phones), TV is still the largest single contributor to children’s total screen time.10 In this study, we first measured total screen exposure at 7 years and found that cross-sectional associations were similar to those observed with TV viewing alone, suggesting that longitudinal associations with total screen time would also be similar.
Finally, bias due to nonrandom dropout is possible: mothers whose children’s sleep and TV relationship was different might be less likely to return questionnaires. This bias would attenuate our estimates over time: racial/ethnic minority (vs white) participants had a higher probability of missing data at later assessment points, and racial/ethnic minority children watched more TV and slept less.
Conclusions
Our study supports a negative influence of TV viewing and bedroom TV on children’s sleep. Although a comparison of within- and between-subject analyses suggests that associations previously reported by using cross-sectional data alone may have been inflated, we demonstrated that subgroups of young children, in particular, boys and racial/ethnic minorities, may be more vulnerable to TV’s effects on sleep. TV viewing and the presence of a bedroom TV track over time40,41; thus, modest decreases in sleep duration could form lasting habits leading to substantial sleep deficits as children age. Given the associations between greater TV viewing and shorter sleep suggested by this study and the strong evidence that greater TV viewing and shorter sleep are associated with poor outcomes, screen time interventions have the potential to improve sleep.
Acknowledgment
We thank the Project Viva participants and staff.
Glossary
- CI
confidence interval
- LRT
likelihood ratio test
- TV
television
Footnotes
Ms Cespedes conceptualized and designed the present analysis, conducted all data analysis, drafted the initial manuscript, and completed all subsequent revisions until submission; Dr Gillman, Dr Kleinman, and Ms Rifas-Shiman were involved in the design and implementation of the parent study, Project Viva, advised in the design and presentation of the present analysis, and revised drafts critically for important intellectual content; Dr Redline advised in the design of the present analysis, and revised drafts critically for important intellectual content; Dr Taveras conceptualized the present analysis, was involved in the design and implementation of the Project Viva study, advised in presentation of analysis results, and revised drafts critically for important intellectual content; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted.
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.
FUNDING: This study was funded by National Institutes of Health (NIH) grants NCI 1U54CA15562601 and R37 HD 034568. Funded by the National Institutes of Health (NIH).
POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.
References
- 1.Curcio G, Ferrara M, De Gennaro L. Sleep loss, learning capacity and academic performance. Sleep Med Rev. 2006;10(5):323–337 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Drescher AA, Goodwin JL, Silva GE, Quan SF. Caffeine and screen time in adolescence: associations with short sleep and obesity. J Clin Sleep Med. 2011;7(4):337–342 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Koulouglioti C, Cole R, Kitzman H. Inadequate sleep and unintentional injuries in young children. Public Health Nurs. 2008;25(2):106–114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Landhuis CE, Poulton R, Welch D, Hancox RJ. Childhood sleep time and long-term risk for obesity: a 32-year prospective birth cohort study. Pediatrics. 2008;122(5):955–960 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Lin JD, Tung HJ, Hsieh YH, Lin FG. Interactive effects of delayed bedtime and family-associated factors on depression in elementary school children. Res Dev Disabil. 2011;32(6):2036–2044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Patel SR, Hu FB. Short sleep duration and weight gain: a systematic review. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2008;16(3):643–653 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Rutters F, Gerver WJ, Nieuwenhuizen AG, Verhoef SP, Westerterp-Plantenga MS. Sleep duration and body-weight development during puberty in a Dutch children cohort. Int J Obes (Lond). 2010;34(10):1508–1514 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Taveras EM, Rifas-Shiman SL, Oken E, Gunderson EP, Gillman MW. Short sleep duration in infancy and risk of childhood overweight. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2008;162(4):305–311 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.National Sleep Foundation. Sleep in America poll. Available at: www.sleepfoundation.org/sites/default/files/FINAL%20SOF%202004.pdf Published 2004. Accessed February 28, 2014
- 10.Roberts D, Foehr U, Rideout V. Generation M: Media in the Lives of 8–18 Year-olds. Report #7251. Menlo Park, CA: Kaiser Family Foundation; 2005 [Google Scholar]
- 11.Adam EK, Snell EK, Pendry P. Sleep timing and quantity in ecological and family context: a nationally representative time-diary study. J Fam Psychol. 2007;21(1):4–19 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Alexandru G, Michikazu S, Shimako H, et al. Epidemiological aspects of self-reported sleep onset latency in Japanese junior high school children. J Sleep Res. 2006;15(3):266–275 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Cain N, Gradisar M. Electronic media use and sleep in school-aged children and adolescents: A review. Sleep Med. 2010;11(8):735–742 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Garrison MM, Liekweg K, Christakis DA. Media use and child sleep: the impact of content, timing, and environment. Pediatrics. 2011;128(1):29–35 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Van den Bulck J. The effects of media on sleep. Adolesc Med State Art Rev. 2010;21(3):418–429, vii [vii.] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Garmy P, Nyberg P, Jakobsson U. Sleep and television and computer habits of Swedish school-Age children. J Sch Nurs. 2012;28(6):469–476 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hense S, Barba G, Pohlabeln H, et al. Factors that influence weekday sleep duration in European children. Sleep. 2011;34(5):633–639 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Li S, Jin X, Wu S, Jiang F, Yan C, Shen X. The impact of media use on sleep patterns and sleep disorders among school-aged children in China. Sleep. 2007;30(3):361–367 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Owens J, Maxim R, McGuinn M, Nobile C, Msall M, Alario A. Television-viewing habits and sleep disturbance in school children. Pediatrics. 1999;104(3). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/104/3/e27 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Nuutinen T, Ray C, Roos E. Do computer use, TV viewing, and the presence of the media in the bedroom predict school-aged children’s sleep habits in a longitudinal study? BMC Public Health. 2013;13:684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Calamaro CJ, Yang K, Ratcliffe S, Chasens ER. Wired at a young age: the effect of caffeine and technology on sleep duration and body mass index in school-aged children. J Pediatr Health Care. 2012;26(4):276–282 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Bernier A, Beauchamp MH, Bouvette-Turcot AA, Carlson SM, Carrier J. Sleep and cognition in preschool years: specific links to executive functioning. Child Dev. 2013;84(5):1542–1553 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Gillman MW, Rich-Edwards JW, Rifas-Shiman SL, Lieberman ES, Kleinman KP, Lipshultz SE. Maternal age and other predictors of newborn blood pressure. J Pediatr. 2004;144(2):240–245 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Baker P, Keck C, Mott F, Quinlan S. NLYS Child Handbook, Revised Edition: A Guide to the 1986–1990 National Longitudinal Survey of Youth Child Data. Columbus, OH: Center for Human Resource Research Ohio State University; 1993 [Google Scholar]
- 25.Gortmaker SL, Must A, Sobol AM, Peterson K, Colditz GA, Dietz WH. Television viewing as a cause of increasing obesity among children in the United States, 1986-1990. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 1996;150(4):356–362 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Fitzmaurice GM, Laird NM, Ware JH. Applied Longitudinal Analysis. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2011 [Google Scholar]
- 27.Tatone-Tokuda F, Dubois L, Ramsay T, et al. Sex differences in the association between sleep duration, diet and body mass index: a birth cohort study. J Sleep Res. 2012;21(4):448–460 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Paavonen EJ, Pennonen M, Roine M, Valkonen S, Lahikainen AR. TV exposure associated with sleep disturbances in 5- to 6-year-old children. J Sleep Res. 2006;15(2):154–161 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Thompson DA, Christakis DA. The association between television viewing and irregular sleep schedules among children less than 3 years of age. Pediatrics. 2005;116(4):851–856 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Van den Bulck J. Television viewing, computer game playing, and Internet use and self-reported time to bed and time out of bed in secondary-school children. Sleep. 2004;27(1):101–104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Wells JC, Hallal PC, Reichert FF, Menezes AM, Araújo CL, Victora CG. Sleep patterns and television viewing in relation to obesity and blood pressure: evidence from an adolescent Brazilian birth cohort. Int J Obes (Lond). 2008;32(7):1042–1049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Garaulet M, Ortega FB, Ruiz JR, et al. Short sleep duration is associated with increased obesity markers in European adolescents: effect of physical activity and dietary habits. The HELENA study. Int J Obes (Lond). 2011;35(10):1308–1317 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Johnson JG, Cohen P, Kasen S, First MB, Brook JS. Association between television viewing and sleep problems during adolescence and early adulthood. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2004;158(6):562–568 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Certain LK, Kahn RS. Prevalence, correlates, and trajectory of television viewing among infants and toddlers. Pediatrics. 2002;109(4):634–642 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Rüger M, St Hilaire MA, Brainard GC, et al. Human phase response curve to a single 6.5 h pulse of short-wavelength light. J Physiol. 2013;591(pt 1):353–363 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Dennison BA, Erb TA, Jenkins PL. Television viewing and television in bedroom associated with overweight risk among low-income preschool children. Pediatrics. 2002;109(6):1028–1035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Taveras EM, Gillman MW, Kleinman K, Rich-Edwards JW, Rifas-Shiman SL. Racial/ethnic differences in early-life risk factors for childhood obesity. Pediatrics. 2010;125(4):686–695 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Spilsbury JC, Storfer-Isser A, Drotar D, et al. Sleep behavior in an urban US sample of school-aged children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2004;158(10):988–994 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Olds T, Blunden S, Petkov J, Forchino F. The relationships between sex, age, geography and time in bed in adolescents: a meta-analysis of data from 23 countries. Sleep Med Rev. 2010;14(6):371–378 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Hesketh K, Wake M, Graham M, Waters E. Stability of television viewing and electronic game/computer use in a prospective cohort study of Australian children: relationship with body mass index. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2007;4:60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Saelens BE, Sallis JF, Nader PR, Broyles SL, Berry CC, Taras HL. Home environmental influences on children’s television watching from early to middle childhood. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2002;23(3):127–132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]