Skip to main content
Journal of Diabetes Investigation logoLink to Journal of Diabetes Investigation
. 2011 Oct 11;3(3):325–330. doi: 10.1111/j.2040-1124.2011.00170.x

Characteristics of neuropsychological functions in inpatients with poorly‐controlled type 2 diabetes mellitus

Ai Takeuchi 1,*, Eisuke Matsushima 1, Motoichiro Kato 3, Mika Konishi 3, Hajime Izumiyama 2, Yuji Murata 4, Yukio Hirata 2
PMCID: PMC4014957  PMID: 24843583

Abstract

Aims/Introduction:  It has been suggested that type 2 diabetes is associated with cognitive impairment. We investigated the neuropsychological profile of inpatients with poorly controlled type 2 diabetes and assessed the effects of clinical factors on neuropsychological functions.

Materials and Methods:  Forty‐two patients with type 2 diabetes and 32 non diabetic control subjects were matched for age, sex ratio, and level of education. Attention & working memory, processing speed, verbal memory, visuospatial memory, visuoconstruction, and executive function were tested. Information about physical function, alcohol use, hypertension, dyslipidemia, and myocardial infarction was retrieved from personal interviews and medical records.

Results:  Diabetic patients demonstrated mild cognitive deterioration in attention & working memory, processing speed, verbal memory, and executive function. In particular, neuropsychological decline became prominent when tasks related with speed and verbal stimuli became unstructured and complex. Age was significantly associated with the majority of neuropsychological tests, whereas tasks dealing with working memory and executive function were associated with age only in the diabetic group. Duration of diabetes was associated with Backward Digit Span.

Conclusions:  Accelerated aging had a major influence on cognitive decline in the diabetic group, whereas diminished performance in working memory and executive function might have been more related to diabetes‐related cognitive impairment. (J Diabetes Invest, doi: 10.1111/j.2040‐1124.2011.00170.x, 2011)

Keywords: Neuropsychological function, Poor glycemic control, Type 2 diabetes

Introduction

Epidemiological studies suggest that the prevalence of diabetes mellitus has been increasing at a rapid rate in developed countries. In the Western Pacific region, which includes Japan and 38 other countries, 76.7 million people are reported to have diabetes, and more than one million adults will die of diabetes‐related causes in 20101. According to the National Survey of the Actual Situation of Diabetes Mellitus, the estimated number of patients in Japan suspected of having diabetes mellitus was about 8.9 million, which represented a sharp increase compared to the results of the same survey conducted in 2002 (7.4 million) and 1997 (6.9 million)2. In Japan, approximately 95% of diabetic cases are classified as type 2 diabetes3. Environmental factors such as eating habits and exercise are believed to play a significant role in the pathogenesis of the disease.

In addition to affecting the microvessels of the eyes, kidneys, and peripheral nerves, diabetes has been observed to affect the central nervous system, resulting in cognitive decline among patients with type 2 diabetes4. Furthermore, diabetes is associated with an increased risk of dementia, including Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia5. Case‐control studies indicated cognitive impairment with brief cognitive screening and verbal memory and processing speed tests, although the findings were less consistent for nonverbal memory and executive function tests6,7. However, not all studies have suggested these cognitive impairments. Differences in methods and study designs such as demographic and diabetic characteristics have made some results inconclusive. Inconsistencies may also be due to differences in the control of confounding factors. Although vascular risk factors, such as cardiovascular and cerebrovascular diseases, hypertension, dyslipidemia, and cigarette smoking, as well as psychological distress and lifestyle factors including alcohol consumption, have been suggested to influence cognitive functions8, the relation between these factors and neuropsychological functioning has not been examined in detail. Age is also an important demographic factor for cognitive impairment in type 2 diabetes. The characteristics of diminished neuropsychological functioning in diabetes resemble the pattern of cognitive decline described in normal aging9. Several studies indicated accelerated brain aging caused by diabetes mellitus10,11, and thus assessing broad neuropsychological aspects and providing descriptive evaluations after controlling confounding factors such as age are necessary.

Few studies have evaluated the neuropsychological functions of diabetes in patients with relatively poor glycemic control. We performed neuropsychological tests on hospitalized patients with poorly controlled diabetes and examined the neuropsychological characteristics of patients with poor glycemic control.

Using a case‐control design, the present study aimed to examine the broad neuropsychological aspects and the relationship between confounding factors and declining neuropsychological functions in hospitalized patients with poorly controlled type 2 diabetes.

Methods

Participants

Between November 2005 and October 2007, 42 inpatients with type 2 diabetes and 32 non‐diabetic patients as controls were included. The patients were hospitalized for 2 weeks for diabetic education. Healthy control subjects did not meet criteria for diabetes (HbA1c < 5.8% and fasting plasma glucose < 110 mg/dL). The two groups were matched for age, sex ratio, and education. Inclusion criteria were: (i) diagnosis of type 2 diabetes by a diabetes consultant; (ii) fluency in Japanese; (iii) absence of a documented history of mental retardation or other major psychiatric disorders, head trauma, or macrovascular events; and (iv) absence of a history of alcohol or drug abuse. Prevalent diabetes mellitus was defined as a fasting glucose level ≥ 126 mg/dL and HbA1c ≥ 6.5%. Depression with >15 points on the Japanese version of the Beck Depression Inventory Second Edition (BDI‐II)12 was excluded on consultation with a psychiatrist for depression. Similarly, dementia with <24 points on the Mini Mental State Examination13 and <1 on the Clinical Dementia Rating14 were excluded by fulfilling the criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition. The value for HbA1c (%) is estimated as a National Glycohemoglobin Standardization Program (NGSP) equivalent value (%) calculated by the formula HbA1c (%) = HbA1c (Japan Diabetes Society: JDS (%) + 0.4%, considering the relational expression of HbA1c (JDS) (%) measured by the previous Japanese standard substance and measurement methods and HbA1c (NGSP).

The Tokyo Medical and Dental University Hospital research review board approved the study and granted formal access to the patients. All participants were informed of the nature, risks, and benefits of participation, and written consent was obtained during the recruitment phase. Seventy‐four patients were approached of which 32 were excluded and 42 were eligible. Patients were excluded because of incomplete data due to the duration of hospitalization (26 patients), poor vision related to cataract (four patients), and refusing to participate in the study (two patients).

Clinical information on diabetes and confounding factors

The participants were asked about their educational level (years), the duration of diabetes (years), history of heart disease, the type of treatment for diabetes, alcohol consumption, and smoking status. Alcohol consumption was classified into three categories: none, a few drinks per month and few drinks per week, and daily. Smoking status was classified into two categories: never/formerly and currently. The following confounding variables, which were assessed during hospitalization, were obtained from medical records. Hypertension was defined as systolic blood pressure ≥140 mmHg and/or diastolic blood pressure ≥90 mmHg or use of antihypertensive medication. Dyslipidemia was defined as low‐density lipoprotein cholesterol level ≥140 mg/dL or use of cholesterol‐lowering agents. Body mass index (BMI) was calculated as weight (kg)/height (m2); the participants were categorized as normal or overweight based on established criteria (overweight, BMI > 25; normal, BMI 18.5–24.9).

Neuropsychological assessment

Neuropsychological tests were performed on all participants by a trained neuropsychologist unaware of the diagnoses of the participants. All neuropsychological measures were selected to cover the major cognitive domains, applying the classification of Lezak15; the duration of the test session was approximately 80 min.

‘Attention and working memory’ was assessed by the Forward and Backward Digit Span of a Japanese version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale‐Revised (WAIS‐R)16. The Digit Span Backward is an activity that calls upon the working memory, as distinct from the efficiency of attention measured by the Digit Span Forward15. ‘Processing Speed’ was assessed by the Trail Making Test (TMT)17 and the Digit Symbol of the WAIS‐R. The TMT consists of two parts, TMTA and TMTB, and measures visual scanning involving motor speed and attention function. The Digit Symbol is a test of psychomotor performance. ‘Verbal Memory’ was measured by two tests: (i) word list recall (immediate and delayed) and word list recognition from the Japanese version of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT)18; and (ii) immediate and delayed recall of stories from Logical Memory (LM) of the Wechsler Memory Scale‐Revised (WMS‐R)19. The RAVLT measures immediate memory span, learning ability and the efficiency of retrieval. The LM, a test of memory for a story, measures abilities of immediate and delayed recall when more information are presented than can be fully grasped. ‘Visuospatial memory’, involving visual recall, was assessed by a delayed recall trial of the Rey‐Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (ROCFT)20. ‘Visuoconstruction’, measuring visuospatial and visuomotor integration, was assessed by a copy trial of the ROCFT. ‘Executive Function’ was measured by the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Keio‐version; KWCST)21 and the Verbal Fluency Test (VFT), which includes two category‐naming tasks and three letter‐fluency tasks. The KWCST involves problem‐solving and flexible planning. The VFT measures verbal productivity within a time limit and a subject’s strategy of word searching. The scores used for the analyses were the number of correctly recalled sequences for the Digit Span; the number of seconds to complete the TMT; the number of words and figures generated on VFT and the Digit Symbol; the number of correct words for the LM; the total number of words remembered in five learning trials and the number of words remembered in delayed and recognition trials for the RAVLT; and the number of categories achieved and the number of perseveration for the KWCST. For the ROCFT, we calculated the score according to the original scoring method20.

Statistical analysis

Data were analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS), version 18.0 (SPSS Inc., Cary, NC, USA). For descriptive data, the patient group and the control group were compared for the demographic and health‐related variables, using Student’s t‐tests for parametric data and chi‐square tests for categorical data. Differences between the patient and control groups for the neuropsychological outcomes were assessed with Student’s t‐tests.

The relationships between impaired neuropsychological measures of the diabetic group and diabetes‐related variables including age, sex, years of education, HbA1c, fasting blood glucose, insulin treatment, and duration of diabetes were analyzed using multiple linear regression. Age was included as an independent variable because it was assumed to be a significant factor in cognitive outcomes in previous studies. In the control group, age, sex, years of education, HbA1c, and fasting blood glucose were analyzed using multiple linear regression.

Moreover, to examine if patients with and without hypertension, dyslipidemia, and heart disease differed in neuropsychological functioning, the patients were divided into two groups based on comorbidity of hypertension, dyslipidemia, and heart disease. Between‐group differences were assessed using Student’s t‐tests. Additionally, the influence of insulin treatment, obesity, alcohol consumption, and smoking status on neuropsychological outcomes was examined using Student’s t‐tests or analysis of variance (anova). In the control group, dyslipidemia, obesity, alcohol consumption, and smoking status were analyzed.

Results

The demographic and medical variables used in the study are summarized in Table 1. No significant differences were observed between the diabetic and control groups with regard to age, sex distribution, and level of education. The diabetic patients had had diabetes for 11.5 ± 9.7 years on an average. The average HbA1c level was 9.5%, which indicated poorly controlled diabetes. Overall, 69.0% patients had hypertension, as in the case with patients with dyslipidemia. Individuals with diabetes were more likely to have elevated BMI (P = 0.009), HbA1c (P < 0.001), fasting blood glucose (P < 0.001), and BDI‐II (P = 0.024) levels. Of the patients with diabetes, 29 (69%) were receiving insulin therapy.

Table 1.  Demographic and medical history characteristics of the type 2 diabetes and control samples.

Type 2 diabetic group Non‐diabetic control group P‐value
N 42 32
Male/female ratio 26/16 18/14 0.481
Age (SD) 62.4 (8.5) 63.8 (4.7) 0.921
Years of education (SD) 13.7 (1.8) 14.5 (1.9) 0.090
MMSE 27.9 (1.8) 28.7 (1.7) 0.073
HbA1c: % (SD) 9.5 (1.8) 5.5 (0.3) <0.001***
Fasting blood glucose: mg/dL (SD) 150.2 (38.9) 93.9 (8.2) <0.001***
Diabetes duration (SD) 11.5 (9.7)
BMI (SD) 24.0 (3.8) 22.1 (2.2) 0.035*
Hypertension: n (%) 29 (69.0) 0 <0.001***
Dyslipidemia: n (%) 29 (69.0) 3 (9.4) <0.001***
Diabetic treatment: n
 Diet 6
 OHA 7
 Insulin 29
Heart disease: n 6 0 0.028*
Alcohol consumption: n
 None 20 4
 A few per month 6 12
 A few per week 4 3
 Daily 12 13
Smoking status: n
 Never 25 31
 Former 7 1
 Current 10 0
Scores of BDI‐II 6.5 3.4 0.024*

*P < 0.05; ***P < 0.001.

MMSE, Mini Mental State Examination; BMI, body mass index; OHA, oral hypoglycemic agents; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory.

With regard to the neuropsychological functions, the patient group performed significantly worse than the control group on Backward Digit Span, (P = 0.016), TMTB (P = 0.005), total RAVLT (P = 0.017), immediate LM (P = 0.023), delayed LM (P = 0.005), TMTA (P = 0.001), Digit Symbol (P < 0.001), KWCST category (P = 0.045), KWCST perseveration (P = 0.008), VFT category (P = 0.001), and VFT letter (P = 0.013). Specifically, a significant difference was observed only in trial 1 of RAVLT (P = 0.001). No significant differences were observed in Forward Digit Span, delayed RAVLT, RAVLT recognition, and ROCFT between the diabetic and control groups (see Table 2).

Table 2.  Raw‐scores of neuropsychological functions in the type 2 diabetes and control samples.

Type 2 diabetic group 
Mean (SD) Non‐diabetic control group 
Mean (SD) P‐value
Attention and working memory
 Digit span forward 6.5 (2.2) 7.2 (1.9) 0.149
 Digit span backward 5.6 (1.7) 6.5 (1.4) 0.016*
Processing speed
 TMT part A 125.8 (49.2) 100.1 (29.3) 0.011*
 TMT part B 161.4 (71.0) 124.5 (36.1) 0.005**
 Digit symbol 49.3 (11.4) 59.6 (10.1) <0.001***
Verbal memory
 RAVLT total trials 1–5 41.4 (8.5) 46.6 (9.9) 0.017*
 RAVLT trial 1 4.5 (1.2) 5.8 (1.8) 0.001**
 RAVLT trail 2 7.3 (1.9) 8.2 (2.5) 0.068
 RAVLT trail 3 8.8 (2.3) 10.0 (2.7) 0.057
 RAVLT trail 4 10.1 (2.4) 11.0 (2.4) 0.089
 RAVLT trial 5 10.7 (2.2) 11.7 (2.0) 0.059
 RAVLT delayed trial 8.6 (2.9) 9.7 (3.2) 0.121
 RAVLT recognition trial 14.0 (1.7) 13.8 (1.6) 0.785
 Logical memory A 17.8 (6.7) 21.5 (6.8) 0.023*
 Logical memory B 12.6 (5.6) 16.5 (6.1) 0.005**
Visuospatial memory
 ROCFT delayed recall trial 15.9 (6.1) 17.6 (6.3) 0.236
Visuoconstruction
 ROCFT copy trial 33.7 (2.4) 34.6 (1.9) 0.098
Executive function
 KWCST category 3.2 (2.0) 4.2 (1.8) 0.045*
 KWCST perseveration 6.9 (6.9) 3.2 (3.7) 0.008**
 Verbal fluency category 25.7 (6.5) 31.3 (7.3) 0.001***
 Verbal fluency letter 24.1 (9.2) 29.7 (9.5) 0.013*

*P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001.

TMT, Trail Making Test; RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; ROCFT, Rey‐Osterrieth Complex Figure Test; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.

In multiple linear regression analysis of the diabetic group, age was significantly associated with TMTB (β = 0.576, P < 0.001), TMTA (β = 0.534, P < 0.001), Digit Symbol (β = −0.698, P < 0.001), KWCST category (β = −0.645, P < 0.001), KWCST perseveration (β = 0.483, P = 0.001), VFT category (β = −0.379, P = 0.013), and VFT letter (β = −0.481, P = 0.001). Duration of the disease was associated with Backward Digit Span (β = −0.455, P = 0.002). LM showed no relationship with diabetes‐related factors and age. In controls, Digit Symbol (β = −0.450, P = 0.027) were related with age. Moreover, HbA1c was found to be associated with Backward Digit Span (β = −0.520, P = 0.009).

In subsequent analysis, the relationships between vascular risk factors and neuropsychological results in the diabetic patients were examined. Neuropsychological results did not differ whether the patient had hypertension, dyslipidemia, or heart disease. No significant differences were observed on neuropsychological tasks between the groups for insulin treatment, obesity (overweight and normal), smoking (non/former smoker and current smoker) and consumption of alcohol (none/a few drinks per month and few drinks per week/daily). With regard to insulin treatment, patients with non insulin therapy (diet and oral hypoglycemic agents) had higher scores in the speed‐related tasks, such as TMTA, VFT letter, and Digit Symbol, than the insulin therapy group, although these did not reach a significant level (TMTA, P = 0.070; VFT letter, P = 0.123, Digit Symbol, P = 0.108).

Discussion

In the present study we investigated neuropsychological functioning in poorly controlled type 2 diabetes, controlled for factors that affect cognitive function. The diabetic patients showed a mild decrement in attention, working memory, verbal memory (story recall and word learning), processing speed, and executive function compared with the controls. No significant differences were observed between the two groups in word list recall (delayed and recognition) and visuospatial memory/visuoconstruction. Although ability of retained visuospatial memory and visuoconstruction are consistent with previous findings6,7, poorly controlled diabetic patients displayed mild neuropsychological dysfunction in broader domains. When considering that the diabetic group had an equivalent MMSE score to the control group, and dementia was excluded from participants, the severity of cognitive impairment was very mild.

In a more detailed analysis of neuropsychological measures in middle‐aged adults with type 2 diabetes, patients showed diminished performance in tasks that required speed within a fixed time such as TMTA, TMTB, Digit Symbol, and VFT. This psychomotor slowing was reported in middle‐aged adults with type 2 diabetes22. As previously suggested, mental slowing might be a common manifestation of a ‘central neuropathy’ induced by chronic hyperglycemia. We failed to find a relation between any tasks of processing speed and metabolic control index. To assess whether the impaired processing speed is an intrinsic cognitive dysfunction of diabetes mellitus, and whether this impairment becomes prominent earlier than that for other neuropsychological functions, the longitudinal course of cognitive function with diabetes should be examined in a larger sample.

As another characteristic of neuropsychological data, patients with diabetes showed difficulties in story recall and word learning, whereas word recognition and delayed word recall were not impaired. Moreover, the Backward Digit Span was poorly performed. Both memory retrieval of story and Backward Digit Span require more cognitive resources and mental manipulation than word recognition and Forward Digit Span. In addition, recollecting a story from memory is a more complex task, where subjects are asked to retain considerable information in one hearing compared to remembering smaller speech units. Considering the result of unimpaired scores in trials 2–5 on word list recall among the diabetic population, dysfunction of verbal memory in patients with diabetes mellitus possibly may be more related to the attention component and working memory when verbal stimuli increase in complexity of structure and amount of information, rather than learning mechanisms.

In a previous study, hyperinsulinemia, elevated HbA1c, hypoglycemia, and peripheral neuropathy were shown to be related with cognitive decline in diabetic patients23. No relationship was found between the index of temporary glycemic control and neuropsychological measures in this study. Rather, in the diabetic group the aging effect was significant in several indices such as tasks requiring speed and executive function. As indicated in a previous review24, age is an important risk factor in cognitive impairment of diabetes mellitus. Interestingly, tasks related with attention‐required executive function were not associated with age in the non diabetic group, whereas the tendency of processing speed decrement with advanced age was the same as the diabetic group. In normal aging, accelerated cognitive decline over the age of 50 was evident for speed and reasoning25. Moreover, learning abilities and short‐term memory with task‐required material diminished with aging15. Thus, attention and executive function might be accelerated by diabetic mechanisms as well as by aging. In the literature examining aging‐related declines in relatively mild type 2 diabetes10,11, significant group differences were present in speed‐intensive measures of executive function and semantic speed across the two late life age groups (age 53–70 and age 71–90). This finding supports our hypothesis that the deteriorating attention and executive function with aging is a diabetes‐related impairment. Backward Digit Span showed a relationship with the duration of diabetes. The duration of disease may reflect the cumulative influence of the disease, with the deficit of the Backward Digit Span representing diabetic‐induced cognitive impairment. The hypothesis that decrement in attention, executive function, and working memory are characteristic of diabetes mellitus should be examined in a longitudinal design comparing groups having good and poor glycemic control.

Compared with previous literature examining cognitive dysfunction of type 2 diabetes of a similar age group10,11, the diabetic participants in this study marked a significant lowering in the broader domains of neuropsychological measures. One possible reason for this is that the glycemic control of the diabetes group in the present study was poorer and that the rate of participants who were under treatment of insulin therapy was higher relative to previous studies. Although neither insulin treatment nor any other index of glycemic control associated with cognitive decrement, insulin and insulin receptors have been reported to be involved in learning and memory formation26. In addition, diabetic patients treated with insulin were at higher risk of dementia27. Several studies identified that insulin signaling may play a significant role in the pathophysiology of Alzheimer’s Disease28,29. The long‐term impact of insulin usage on brain mechanisms has to be investigated in a future study.

No relation was found between hypertension, dyslipidemia, obesity, or smoking and diminished neuropsychological functions. According to a systematic review, hypertension is the factor most consistently associated with cognitive decrements, whereas obesity and dyslipidemia are less consistently associated with them30. One study indicated a greater cognitive decline among people with comorbid diabetes and hypertension than among normotensive people with diabetes31. Because the average age of that study population at baseline was approximately 20 years older than that of our study group, the effect of age should be considered when interpreting the results. Another reason for the non significance is that our diabetic participants were treated with antihypertensive medications. Each antihypertensive medication has different effects on the performance of neuropsychological functions32. Thus, the use of antihypertensive medication may affect the cognition of diabetic patients.

Finally, the diabetic group displayed significantly higher scores than the control group on the depression scale. Meta‐analysis suggested that patients with type 2 diabetes had a twofold increased risk of depression compared with non diabetic subjects33. Although the scores ranged within the minimal level, which did not fulfill the diagnosis, diabetic patients had notably more depressive symptoms than non diabetic subjects.

In summary, diabetic patients with poor metabolic control showed mild cognitive dysfunction in a wide range of tasks related with working memory, processing speed, verbal memory, and executive function. The neuropsychological decline tended to be prominent when the tasks required more speed within a fixed time and when verbal stimuli became unstructured and complex. Accelerated aging had a major influence on cognitive decline in the diabetic group, whereas decrement in attention, executive function, and working memory might be more related to diabetes‐related cognitive impairment. Because cross‐sectional studies cannot capture cognitive decline over time, the neuropsychological characteristics should be analyzed in a longitudinal study with a wide range of age. Investigating the relationships between impaired neuropsychological measures and brain imaging is also necessary.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank all patients, consultants, and nurses involved in this study. The authors declare no financial support or relationships that may pose conflict of interest.

References

  • 1.International Diabetes Federation . IDF Diabetes Atlas, 4th edn International Diabetes Federation, Brussels, Belgium, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare . Outline of results from National Health and Nutrition Survey. 2007. Available at http://www.mhlw.go.jp/houdou/2008/12/dl/h1225‐5d.pdf
  • 3.Nakano T, Ito H. Epidemiology of diabetes mellitus in old age in Japan. Diabetes Res Clin Pract 2007; 77: S76–S81 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Gispen W, Biessels G. Cognition and synaptic plasticity in diabetes mellitus. Trends Neurosci 2000; 23: 542–549 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Biessels G, De Leeuw F, Lindeboom J, et al. Increased cortical atrophy in patients with Alzheimer’s disease and type 2 diabetes mellitus. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2006; 77: 304–307 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Stewart R, Liolitsa D. Type 2 diabetes mellitus, cognitive impairment and dementia. Diabet Med 1999; 16: 93–112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Awad N, Gagnon M, Messier C. The relationship between impaired glucose tolerance, type 2 diabetes, and cognitive function. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2004; 26: 1044–1080 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Strachan MWJ, Deary IJ, Ewing FME, et al. Is type II diabetes associated with an increased risk of cognitive dysfunction? A critical review of published studies. Diabetes Care 1997; 20: 438–445 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Tisserand D, Jolles J. On the involvement of prefrontal networks in cognitive ageing. Cortex 2003; 39: 1107–1128 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Brands A, Van den Berg E, Manschot S, et al. A detailed profile of cognitive dysfunction and its relation to psychological distress in patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007; 13: 288–297 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Yeung S, Fischer A, Dixon R. Exploring effects of type 2 diabetes on cognitive functioning in older adults. Neuropsychology 2009; 23: 1–9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Beck A, Steer R, Ball R, et al. Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories ‐IA and ‐II in psychiatric outpatients. J Pers Assess 1996; 67: 588–597 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Folstein M, Folstein S, McHugh P. “Mini‐mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12: 189–198 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Morris J. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology 1993; 43: 2412–2414 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lezak MD. Neuropsychological Assessment 3rd edn Oxford University Press, New York, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale‐Revised manual. The psychological Corporation, New York, 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Corrigan J, Hinkeldey N. Relationships between parts A and B of the Trail Making Test. J Clin Psychol 1987; 43: 402–409 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Rey A. L’examen Clinique en Psychologie. Presses universitaires de, France, Paris, 1964. (French). [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale –Revised manual. The psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX, 1987. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Taylor EM. Psychological Appraisal of Children with Cerebral Deficits. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, 1959. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kato M. A study on the concept formation and shift of the patients with frontal lesions: a neuropsychological investigation by the New Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Keio Igak 1988; 65: 861–885 (Japanese). [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ryan CM, Geckle MO. Circumscribed cognitive dysfunction in middle‐aged adults with type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care 2000; 23: 1486–1493 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Reaven G, Thompson L, Nahum D, et al. Relationship between hyperglycemia and cognitive function in older NIDDM patients. Diabetes Care 1990; 13: 16–21 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Reijmer YD, van den Berg E, Ruis C, et al. Cognitive dysfunction in patients with type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Metab Res Rev 2010; 26: 507–519 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Verhaeghen P, Salthouse T. Meta‐analyses of age‐cognition relations in adulthood: estimates of linear and nonlinear age effects and structural models. Psychol Bull 1997; 122: 231–249 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Zhao WQ, Alkon DL. Role of insulin and insulin receptor in learning and memory. Mol Cell Endocrinol 2001; 177: 125–134 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Ott A, Stolk RP, van Harskamp F, et al. Diabetes mellitus and the risk of dementia: The Rotterdam study. Neurology 1999; 10: 1937–1942 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Burns JM, Donnelly JE, Anderson HS, et al. Peripheral insulin and brain structure in early Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2007; 69: 1094–1104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Takeda S, Sato N, Rakugi H, et al. Molecular mehanisms linking diabetes mellitus and Alzheimer disease: beta‐amyloid peptide, insulin signling, and neuronal function. Mol Biosyst 2011; 7: 1822–1827 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.van den Berg E, Kloppenborg R, Kessels R, et al. Type 2 diabetes mellitus, hypertension, dyslipidemia and obesity: a systematic comparison of their impact on cognition. Biochim Biophys Acta 2009; 1792: 470–481 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Hassing L, Hofer S, Nilsson S, et al. Comorbid type 2 diabetes mellitus and hypertension exacerbates cognitive decline: evidence from a longitudinal study. Age Ageing 2004; 33: 355–361 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Muldoon M, Manuck S, Shapiro A, et al. Neurobehavioral effects of antihypertensive medications. J Hypertens 1991; 9: 549–559 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Anderson R, Freedland K, Clouse R, et al. The prevalence of comorbid depression in adults with diabetes: a meta‐analysis. Diabetes Care 2001; 24: 1069–1078 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Diabetes Investigation are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES