Abstract
Background
In mammals, the slow-oscillations of neuronal membrane potentials (reflected in the electroencephalogram as high-amplitude, slow-waves), which occur during non-rapid eye movement sleep and anesthesia, propagate across the neocortex largely as two-dimensional traveling waves. However, it remains unknown if the traveling nature of slow-waves is unique to the laminar cytoarchitecture and associated computational properties of the neocortex.
Results
We demonstrate that local field potential slow-waves and correlated multiunit activity propagate as complex three-dimensional plumes of neuronal activity through the avian brain, owing to its non-laminar, nuclear neuronal cytoarchitecture.
Conclusions
The traveling nature of slow-waves is not dependent upon the laminar organization of the neocortex, and is unlikely to subserve functions unique to this pattern of neuronal organization. Finally, the three-dimensional geometry of propagating plumes may reflect computational properties not found in mammals that contributed to the evolution of nuclear neuronal organization and complex cognition in birds.
Keywords: Sleep, Slow waves, Propagation, Travelling, Bird, Cortex
Background
Historically, the mammalian neocortex has been viewed as the pinnacle of brain evolution. The highly structured six-layered laminar cytoarchitecture of the neocortex and the associated computational properties contributing to complex cognition added to this view. However, exactly how laminar cytoarchitecture and associated neurophysiological processes mediate complex cognition remains poorly understood. Our understanding of how the neocortex works can be informed by comparisons with other animals. Notably, comparisons with non-mammalian groups lacking laminar cytoarchitecture, as is the case in birds, can be used to isolate traits that depend upon laminar cytoarchitecture from those that do not. In this study, we use this comparative approach to gain insight into the neocortex by examining sleep-related neuronal activity in the avian brain.
A growing body of research suggests that the brain rhythms occurring during sleep are involved in processing information acquired during wakefulness [1]. Notably, the slow (typically <1 Hz) oscillation of neocortical neuronal membrane potentials between a depolarized “up-state” with action potentials, and a hyperpolarized “down-state” with neuronal quiescence occurring during non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep and some types of anesthesia is the focus of several hypotheses for the function of sleep [2-4]. The term ‘slow-oscillation’ is in wide-spread use even though the interval between down-states is variable and individual cycles of the slow-oscillation originate from different neocortical locations [5-8]. Slow-oscillations manifest in electroencephalogram (EEG) or local field potential (LFP) recordings as high-amplitude, slow-waves that propagate across the mammalian neocortex as traveling waves [5,7-15], raising the possibility that they are involved in processing spatially distributed information [1,16] via processes such as spike timing-dependent plasticity [17]. However, it remains unknown whether the traveling nature of slow-oscillations reflects a feature unique to the laminar cytoarchitecture and associated computational properties of the neocortex [18] or a more general aspect of sleep-related neuronal activity.
To distinguish between these alternatives, we studied brain activity in birds, the only non-mammalian group known to exhibit slow-oscillations [19] and associated EEG slow-waves comparable to those observed in mammals during NREM sleep [20,21]. This similarity between mammals and birds is particularly interesting because unlike the laminar mammalian neocortex, neurons in the avian forebrain are arranged in a largely nuclear manner [22]. Specifically, the hyperpallium, a region developmentally homologous and functionally similar to the mammalian primary visual and somatosensory/motor cortices [23,24], lacks the laminar arrangement of neurons, including pyramidal neurons with long trans-layer apical dendrites found in the six-layered mammalian neocortex and in the three-layered dorsal cortex of the closest living reptilian relatives to birds [24]. Instead, the hyperpallium is composed of long flat nuclei stacked one on top of the other running along the dorsal-medial-anterior surface of the brain, each of which is composed of stellate neurons with short spiny dendrites and axonal projections within and between nuclei [24,25]. Interestingly, this cytoarchitecture is even found within high-order association regions in the avian brain (that is, mesopallium and nidopallium) involved in orchestrating complex cognitive tasks, in some cases comparable to those performed by primates [26].
We recorded intracerebral potentials in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata) hyperpallium and nidopallium to evaluate whether traveling slow-waves are unique to mammals, or are shared with birds irrespective of fundamental differences in cytoarchitectonic organization.
Results and discussion
Analogue multiunit activity (AMUA) and LFP were recorded using a 64-channel silicon multi-electrode probe inserted into the hyperpallium (Figure 1A, B) and caudal nidopallium of adult female zebra finches anesthetized with 1 to 1.25% isoflurane [27,28], an anesthetic that activates normal sleep promoting regions in the mammalian [29] and fly [30] brain. Except in a thin layer just below the surface of the brain, hyperpallial LFP oscillations consist of negatively peaked signals (Figure 2A, B). In contrast, the corresponding potentials near the surface of the brain are often positively peaked (Figure 2B-D). Similar intra-hyperpallial phase reversals have also been observed in visually evoked potentials in anesthetized zebra finches [31]. Unlike the mammalian neocortex, wherein long apical dendrites extending toward the surface of the brain are thought to give rise to similar phase reversals [32], there are no known neuronal asymmetries (that is, apical dendrites) that might cause these positively peaked potentials in finches, other than output axons projecting across the surface of the hyperpallium to the septomesencephalic tract [25]. Nonetheless, these oscillations are likely the basis for slow-waves recorded from epidural EEG electrodes during NREM sleep in finches and other birds [33].
At a time scale of seconds, LFP patterns appear visually similar between most electrode sites (Figure 2B), even distant ones, and are strongly correlated (for negatively-peaked sites mean between-site correlation coefficients range from 0.72 to 0.92; median 0.84; n = 11 birds), giving the impression that oscillations are near-synchronous across brain regions and thus globally organized. As is the case in the mammalian neocortex [34,35], at most of these sites negative LFP peaks coincide with bursts of action potential activity (mean correlation coefficients between LFP and AMUA signals range from 0.38 to 0.60; median 0.50, n = 11 birds). Sites with positive LFP peaks near the surface are less likely to be associated with strong action potential firing in the same recording site (Additional file 1: Figure S1).
At shorter time-scales of tens to hundreds of milliseconds, however, single oscillations do not appear as near-synchronous and globally distributed. For example, Figure 2C zooms in on a single LFP peak from Figure 2B, and shows that the peak occurs at slightly different times across the electrode grid, such that it occurs first at sites in the bottom right corner and then later at sites in the top half of the grid. Although it is possible to see such shifting of activity peaks in traditional waveform plots, the temporospatial dynamics are better visualized in sequences of image plots, especially when viewed as videos (see Additional file 2: Video S1, Additional file 3: Video S2, Additional file 4: Video S3, Additional file 5: Video S4, Additional file 6: Video S5, Additional file 7: Video S6, Additional file 8: Video S7, Additional file 9: Video S8, Additional file 10: Video S9). Figure 2D shows in a sequence of image plots that the traveling “peak” event in Figure 2C is a spatially localized plume of field and action potentials that propagates fast across the electrode grid (Additional file 2: Video S1). The fact that the LFP plume co-occurs with action potential firing (AMUA) suggests that it reflects local neurophysiological activity, rather than activity volume-conducted from a remote source.
We compiled videos from all of our recordings from the hyperpallium (n = 11 birds), and all of them show propagating local plumes of local field and action potential activity, such as seen in Figure 2D. Nonetheless, dynamic patterns of activity are generally variable across different plumes, even within the same bird. Figure 3A (Additional file 3: Video S2) shows in a different bird a plume that propagates to the surface of the brain, but in the same recording plumes can be seen propagating in different directions: a complex example is provided in Figure 3B (Additional file 4: Video S3), where a plume of activity propagates in a right (lateral) to left (medial) direction before briefly dissipating. The activity then strengthens again near the lower center of the array and expands outward as a circle of activity. Propagation of plumes is not confined within a two-dimensional layer, as they can travel over considerable distances in variable directions, including diametrically opposite ones, both within orthogonally placed electrode grids (horizontal and sagittal planes), and relative to the plane of the stacked hyperpallial nuclei. As can be seen in the movies (Additional file 2: Video S1, Additional file 3: Video S2, Additional file 4: Video S3, Additional file 5: Video S4, Additional file 6: Video S5, Additional file 7: Video S6, Additional file 8: Video S7, Additional file 9: Video S8, and Additional file 10: Video S9) and Additional file 11: Figure S2, plume propagation velocity across the 2-D electrode grid is variable. However, given that plumes have complex changing shapes and their 3-D trajectories may impinge upon the 2-D electrode grid at variable angles, it is not generally possible to estimate plume propagation velocity from translation velocity across the grid. Thus, whether or not plume propagation velocity is variable remains an open question. Furthermore, it is unclear how the propagation speed of avian plumes compares with that of two-dimensional slow-waves in the mammalian neocortex [5,8,13].
Although we focus on the hyperpallium in the current study, we wondered whether propagating plume activity occurs outside the hyperpallium. To address this question, we added two recordings (n = 2 birds) to our dataset, one in caudolateral nidopallium (NCL) and the other one in caudomedial nidopallium (NCM), which are forebrain regions that are maximally distant from the hyperpallium (Figure 1A). As in the hyperpallium, both recordings consist of prominent propagating plumes of local field and action potential activity (examples Figure 3C, D; Additional file 5: Videos S4 and Additional file 6: Video S5), suggesting that this is a general feature of the avian pallium.
Despite the large variability in propagation patterns that we observed in the videos of all recordings (the Additional file 7: Video S6, Additional file 8: Video S7, Additional file 9: Video S8, Additional file 10: Video S9 provide representative examples), there appear to be some recurrent patterns within recordings and a bias in overall directionality. Although the full complexity of plumes is not yet understood, and probably is too high to be meaningfully expressed in a low-dimensional set of statistics, we reasoned that non-randomness of propagation patterns may be revealed by investigating two directionality characteristics of plumes.
First, to identify systematic time delays in AMUA activity between each electrode site and all other electrode sites, we calculated the maxima of normalized cross-correlation functions for each identified plume in every recording. Illustrating this process for the propagating plume shown in Figure 2C, D, Figure 2E visualizes for three electrode sites the lags of AMUA peak activity at those sites relative to all other sites, which are color-coded in pixels arranged according to the spatial distribution of sites in the electrode grid. AMUA at site 63 is leading with respect to the other sites, such that the maximum cross-correlation is achieved later, overall, as the distance to other electrode sites increases. The lag of site 35 is intermediate relative to that of the other sites, being delayed with respect to sites in the lower right quadrant, but leading with respect to sites in the upper left quadrant of the grid. Site 14 is delayed with respect to most other sites, maximally so relative to sites in the lower right quadrant. The information in each of these image plots is then averaged to a single value that expresses the mean lag of the corresponding site relative to other sites, and these mean values are visualized in a new image plot (Figure 2F). Note that the spatial distribution of site activity lag is consistent with the direction of the AMUA plume propagation visible in Figure 2D. To determine whether or not there is a systematic directionality in plume propagation overall, we calculated electrode site lag matrices for all identified plumes in all recordings, and averaged the result over plumes within recordings (10 minutes duration). An example is given for Bird 5 in Figure 4A, which shows that there is underlying structure in propagation dynamics, with plume action potential activity propagating more often from the bottom-right (posterior-lateral) corner of the grid to the top-left (anterior-medial) corner. The smooth distribution of time lags over space is consistent with the traveling propagation dynamics. Mean lag maps of recordings in the other birds (Figure 4B) show that in the hyperpallium activity of deeper (posterior) recording sites tends to lead with respect to anterior sites toward the surface of the brain, even though there is variability between birds in the exact spatial distribution of time lags. As such, at least some hyperpallial plumes may originate from the underlying mesopallium and/or nidopallium. Finally, as described for propagating slow-waves in the mammalian neocortex [9,15], in general terms, the overall directional bias in hyperpallial plume propagation is parallel to the predominant orientation of myelinated axons in the zebra finch hyperpallium [36]; although, as noted above, individual plumes often propagate orthogonal to this axonal orientation.
Because they are based on normalized cross-correlation functions, lag maps of AMUA plumes capture differences in the timing of oscillation events across the electrode grid, but not their magnitudes. In a second analysis approach we therefore aimed to capture overall propagation directions of predominant concentrations of neural activity based on LFP. LFP fields are less localized than are action potential fields and, therefore, better reflect the magnitude of neural activity in our recordings. We identified, at 1-ms intervals, plumes of LFP in the electrode grid that were stronger (that is, more negative) than a threshold criterion of −250 mV, and tracked the changes of their spatial mean in time (see example, Figure 2G). Plumes propagate in variable trajectories within recordings, both in orthogonal horizontal and sagittal planes (Figure 5A). For each plume, we expressed the net movement across the plane of the electrode grid as a mean vector (Figure 5B). Group mean vectors per recording have significantly non-random directions (Rayleigh-tests; P<0.001) in every recording (n = 11), with an overall tendency for plumes in the hyperpallium to travel anteriorly, toward the surface of the brain (Figure 5C).
Our experimental animals were anesthetized, not sleeping spontaneously. In mammals, some kinds of anesthesia, including isoflurane, activate normal sleep promoting regions in the hypothalamus [29], induce slow oscillatory brain activity resembling that occurring during spontaneous NREM sleep [2,11], and are a well-established method for studying the intracerebral neurophysiological aspects of NREM sleep. Moreover, recent studies suggest that anesthesia may serve a functional role for the brain similar to that of sleep [37]. Importantly, mammalian slow-oscillations have been shown to travel both during anesthesia and NREM sleep [5,7-15]. For these collective reasons, we therefore think that the isoflurane-induced slow-oscillatory patterns of brain activity reported here are likely to be comparable to those of a freely sleeping animal. Indeed, the spectral power distribution recorded under isoflurane (Additional file 12: Figure S3) is similar to that recorded in the EEG during natural NREM sleep in zebra finches [33].
As in the laminar mammalian neocortex, slow-waves propagate through the nuclear avian brain. Thus, an important insight from our findings is that the propagating nature of slow-waves during NREM sleep and anesthesia does not reflect a neural phenomenon that is unique to mammals, or that depends upon uniquely mammalian cortical cytoarchitecture or computational properties. Rather, propagating slow-waves reflect a more general type of sleep-related neural activity. Propagating slow-waves may have evolved independently in birds and mammals, perhaps for functional reasons, despite their differing cortical cytoarchitectures, or it may represent an ancestral trait. Although visually evoked propagating two-dimensional waves have been described in the visual cortex of anesthetized turtles [38], it remains unclear whether the cortex of non-avian reptiles generate slow-oscillations during sleep [39].
Although the presence of traveling slow-waves in avian and mammalian brain structures is highly interesting from a comparative viewpoint, and may be linked in terms of function [20], we do not suggest they are necessarily identical phenomena. Three-hundred million years of separation is a long time even by evolutionary standards, and it would be surprising if this was not in any way reflected in the exact phenomenology of traveling slow-waves, especially given the known differences in cytoarchitecture. Nevertheless, we believe that, as in comparative work in general, differences may be as informative as similarities. Although the overall phenomenology and frequency content of EEG slow-waves during NREM sleep and anesthesia in birds and mammals appear similar, our recordings show that during apparent up-states there are often multiple sharp LFP peaks and AMUA bursts, each of which corresponds to a propagating plume. In mammals, each cycle of a traveling slow-wave (that is, an EEG peak) is thought to reflect one up-state, spreading from a site of origin to neighboring regions. Given the limited data on trans-membrane potentials during NREM sleep or anesthesia in birds, it is not possible to say whether multiple plumes arise and propagate during a single up-state or whether one propagating plume corresponds to one short up-state; although the former seems more likely given the single report of slow-oscillations in birds [19] and the duration of avian EEG slow-waves. Furthermore, it is difficult to conclude whether or not the situation of multiple-propagating-plumes-per-presumed-up-state that we find in birds is different from the one-traveling-wave-per-up-state situation described in mammals. High-frequency activity occurring during up-states in anesthetized cats is correlated across neurons spaced <0.3 mm apart [13], but poorly correlated across neurons spaced 4 mm apart. Although propagation of this fast activity between close neurons has not been examined to our knowledge, it is conceivable that such activity correlates and propagates on fine spatial scales similar to that examined here in birds. Additional studies in mammals and birds employing similar methods are needed to determine whether the propagation of higher frequency activity during apparent up-states is unique to birds.
While the propagating nature of slow-waves is shared by mammals and birds, the geometry of propagation differs between the two. In mammals, although slow-waves may engage different layers of the neocortex at slightly different times [18,40,41], propagation occurs primarily parallel to the layers of the neocortex. In contrast, in birds, slow-waves propagate as complex plumes of action and local field potential activity. While this difference is undoubtedly linked to the nuclear versus laminar arrangement of neurons in birds and mammals, respectively, it may also confer such neural cytoarchitecture with different computational properties.
Depending on the actual function of propagating slow-waves, the three-dimensional nature of plumes in the avian brain may have broader implications for understanding the evolution of the nuclear pallial structures subserving complex cognition in birds. While the propagating nature of slow-waves in general may simply be an epiphenomenon of excitable interconnected neurons [42], it may also play a role in processing information on a systems level. In the former case, while propagation per se may not serve a function, the slow-oscillation may be involved in processes occurring at the local level [3,7,20,43]. In the latter case, the propagating aspect of slow-waves may be involved in the successive reactivation and consolidation of recent memory traces [4], and/or their transfer and integration with older memories [1]. If correct, then the spatial dynamics of wave propagation may shape the nature of information processing [44]. In mammals, propagation occurs primarily parallel to the layers of the neocortex, restricting communication to two dimensions. In contrast, the nuclear arrangement of neurons in the avian brain and associated three-dimensional propagation of waves may confer greater degrees of computational freedom. Albeit speculative, such potential benefits may explain in part why birds 1) evolutionarily “replaced” the laminar dorsal cortex present in their reptilian ancestors with the nuclear hyperpallium, and 2) elaborated extensively upon pre-existing nuclear structures in the reptilian dorsal ventricular ridge to form the mesopallium and nidopallium, large nuclear structures that orchestrate complex cognitive abilities comparable to those orchestrated by high-order association regions in the laminar mammalian neocortex [26].
Finally, our findings may have implications for the interpretation of sleep-related neuronal activity in the song system of sleeping zebra finches, a prominent model for investigating sleep’s role in memory consolidation [45]. In anesthetized and spontaneously sleeping male finches, neurons in nuclei of the forebrain song system exhibit spontaneous bursts of activity [46,47]. Bursts in one nuclei often precede bursts in another distantly located nuclei, raising the possibility that this sequential activity is involved in processing song-related information [48,49]. As such, it would be interesting to determine whether this activity is part of larger plumes of activity propagating in the surrounding forebrain, or whether this activity propagates via dedicated pathways unaffected by surrounding plumes of activity.
Conclusions
We demonstrate that slow-waves propagate as complex three-dimensional plumes of neuronal activity in the nuclear avian brain. These findings indicate that the propagating nature of slow-waves during NREM sleep and anesthesia does not reflect a neural phenomenon that is unique to mammals, or that depends upon uniquely mammalian cortical cytoarchitecture or computational properties. As such, efforts to understand the mechanisms responsible for propagating waves should not focus on lamination, per se, but rather on traits shared by mammalian and avian brains. In this regard, our findings refine our understanding of the neocortex. In addition, the three-dimensional nature of propagating plumes raises the intriguing possibility that such activity reflects computational processes that contributed to the evolution and elaboration of nuclear structures and associated complex cognition in birds. More generally, the presence of mammalian-like activity in the avian brain, despite differences in neuronal cytoarchitecture, suggests that the apparent absence of similar sleep-related activity in prominent invertebrate models of sleep, such as Drosophila melanogaster[50], cannot be simply attributed to their lack of a neocortex. Rather, such animals may lack neuronal properties (for example, slow-oscillations) involved in performing sleep functions shared by mammals and birds.
Methods
Animals
Thirteen adult (>90-day-old) female zebra finches were used in this study. The birds were reared in a colony and housed in an aviary with other adult zebra finches. All procedures were performed under systemic anesthesia under supervision of the institutional Tierschutzbeauftragte, in accordance with German laws and regulations (Tierschutzgesetz, §4, Absatz 3 and §§8b, 9 Absatz 2, Satz 2).
Recordings
After the birds were anesthetized with isoflurane vaporized in oxygen (induction, 3%; maintenance, 1.0 to 1.25%), meloxicam (0.3 mg/kg; intramuscular injection) was administered for systemic analgesia. The head was fixed in a stereotaxic frame while the body rested on a heating pad to maintain body temperature. The angle of the head was 0 to 20º, except for one bird (Bird 13, caudomedial nidopallium, NCM) where it was 45º. After 20 minutes, the skull was exposed and a rectangular craniotomy was performed overlying the multi-electrode probe insertion point. An incision was made in the dura to allow for insertion of the probe. In most birds (n = 11) we recorded from the hyperpallium, 3.8 to 6.6 mm rostral, and centered around 0.9 to 2.3 mm lateral, from the bifurcation of the midsagittal sinus. In seven birds the probe was inserted horizontally, and in four birds sagittally. In six birds we recorded from the right hemisphere, and in five from the left hemisphere. Additionally, we recorded in two birds from within the caudal nidopallium (NC), a forebrain region that is maximally distant from the hyperpallium, one in the caudolateral nidopallium (NCL; 1.1 mm rostral, centered around 4.7 mm lateral) and the other one in the NCM (0.9 mm rostral, centered around 1.3 mm lateral).
To record neural activity, we used 64-channel silicon-based multi-electrode probes (NeuroNexus Technologies, Ann Arbor, MI, USA; a8 × 8 to 5 mm 200-200-413). These had eight parallel shanks (thickness: 15 μm, inter-shank spacing, 200 μm), each with eight iridium electrode sites (site surface, 413 μm2; inter-site spacing 200 μm), so that the 8 × 8 square matrix of electrode sites extended over an area 1,400 × 1,400 μm.
Initially, the tips of the probe shanks were positioned just above the brain surface to verify visually (magnification 60×) that the head was fixed well and did not move due to breathing or the heartbeat. The probe was then lowered in small steps to a position where the deepest shank reached its recording depth. In the hyperpallium, this was a depth of between 1,470 μm and 1,700 μm below the brain surface. This way, most electrode sites were situated in the hyperpallial nuclei (primarily the hyperpallium apicale, the most dorsal and thick nucleus), with the top-most sites just beneath the brain surface, while some of the deepest electrode sites were situated in the underlying mesopallium (Figure 1A). In the caudal nidopallium, recording depth was slightly deeper (2,400 μm in NCL and 1,920 μm in NCM). Recordings started 30 minutes after the probe was in place, and lasted 10 minutes in each bird.
Recorded signals were referenced to a silver wire under the scalp over the cerebellum and buffered by headstage preamplifiers (10× gain; MPA32I; MultiChannel Systems, Reutlingen, Germany) before amplification with a multichannel amplifier (250× gain; bandpass filters 0.1 to 5,000 Hz; PGA64; MultiChannel Systems, Austin, TX, USA). The amplified and filtered signals were digitized with a sampling frequency of 14 kHz and 16-bit resolution (NI9205; National Instruments), and stored on disk in HDF5 files [51].
At the end of the recording session, birds were sacrificed by increasing the level of isoflurane to 5%, after which the brain was removed and frozen for histology to determine electrode site placements [27].
Data analyses
Analyses were performed using the scientific computing package SciPy, version 0.10 [52]. Recorded electrophysiological signals were filtered to high-frequency signals (0.5 to 5 kHz) containing action potentials, and low-frequency signals (0.1 to 350 Hz) containing LFP. The action potential signals were subsequently rectified so that their amplitude reflects the level of multiunit action potential firing in the vicinity of the electrode site (AMUA).
Correlation coefficients reported in the text were calculated using decimated (sampling rate 1.4 kHz) and normalized signals, and were based on full recordings. The correlation coefficient between two signals was defined as the absolute value of their cross-correlation function at τ = 0, that is, without a time lag. Mean between-site correlation coefficients are based on the mean of all possible site pairs (n = 64 × 63 = 4032). n-Values were in practice slightly lower because some sites were excluded (median 2 out of 64 sites per recording) because they were situated just above the brain surface or contained recording artifacts. Mean correlations between LFP and AMUA signals were based on the mean of all possible within-site cross-correlations.
AMUA plume propagation analyses (Figure 2E, F, Figure 4) were based on normalized cross-correlation functions, with time-lag, τ, ranging from −50 to +50 ms, in 1-ms time steps, for each identified plume (based on LFP signals, see below).
Temporospatial propagation of LFP plumes across the 8 × 8 recording electrode matrix was quantified using a different methodology (for example, Figure 2G). We identified a plume as a continuous episode with a duration of at least 20 ms where LFP levels are always lower than −0.25 mV at any three or more electrode sites. The number of plumes identified this way is dependent on the exact threshold that is chosen because weaker plumes may be missed, but using different thresholds (−0.20 mV and −0.30 mV) did not result in a qualitative change of the results reported here. The center of each plume (that is, its spatial mean) was determined at 1-ms intervals by calculating a weighted average of the spatial coordinates of sub-threshold sites (that is, <−0.25 mV), with their absolute potential as a weighting factor. The resulting temporal series of plume center locations is called a traveling plume “trajectory”. The median number of trajectories that were found in the 10-minute recordings was 697 (range: 232 to 969).
Anatomy
Before implantation, the multi-electrode probes were coated with the fluorescent dye DiI for anatomical registration with histological sections. Frozen brains were cut into 25 μm sections using a freezing microtome and mounted on glass slides. Sections were fixed in 4% formaldehyde solution in phosphate-buffered saline, and then DAPI-stained. Fluorescence microscopy was used to verify probe location in 11 of 13 birds.
Videos
Supplementary videos were rendered by low-pass filtering LFP and AMUA signals at 100 Hz and decimating them in the time domain with factors 560 (for real-time speed), 56 (for 0.1× speed), or 22 (for 0.04× speed) to obtain video-compatible sampling rates of 25 Hz.
Abbreviations
AMUA: Analogue multiunit activity; EEG: Eectroencephalogram; LFP: Local field potential; NC: Caudal nidopallium; NCL: Caudolateral nidopallium; NCM: Caudomedial nidopallium; NREM: Non-rapid eye movement.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Author’ contributions
GJLB, JAL, and NCR designed the study. GJLB and JvdM collected the data. GJLB and NCR analyzed the data and wrote the paper. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Supplementary Material
Contributor Information
Gabriël JL Beckers, Email: G.J.L.Beckers@uu.nl.
Jacqueline van der Meij, Email: jvandermeij@orn.mpg.de.
John A Lesku, Email: J.Lesku@latrobe.edu.au.
Niels C Rattenborg, Email: rattenborg@orn.mpg.de.
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by the Max Planck Society and by a Marie Curie Intra European Fellowship within the 7th European Community Framework Programme to GJLB We thank Manfred Gahr for providing laboratory facilities and equipment.
References
- Diekelmann S, Born J. The memory function of sleep. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;12:114–126. doi: 10.1038/nrn2762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steriade M. Grouping of brain rhythms in corticothalamic systems. Neuroscience. 2006;12:1087–1106. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.10.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Massimini M, Tononi G, Huber R. Slow waves, synaptic plasticity and information processing: insights from transcranial magnetic stimulation and high-density EEG experiments. Eur J Neurosci. 2009;12:1761–1770. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06720.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chauvette S, Seigneur J, Timofeev I. Sleep oscillations in the thalamocortical system induce long-term neuronal plasticity. Neuron. 2012;12:1105–1113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Massimini M, Huber R, Ferrarelli F, Hill S, Tononi G. The sleep slow oscillation as a traveling wave. J Neurosci. 2004;12:6862–6870. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1318-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vyazovskiy VV, Olcese U, Lazimy YM, Faraguna U, Esser SK, Williams JC, Cirelli C, Tononi G. Cortical firing and sleep homeostasis. Neuron. 2009;12:865–878. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nir Y, Staba RJ, Andrillon T, Vyazovskiy VV, Cirelli C, Fried I, Tononi G. Regional slow waves and spindles in human sleep. Neuron. 2011;12:153–169. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stroh A, Adelsberger H, Groh A, Rühlmann C, Fischer S, Schierloh A, Deisseroth K, Konnerth A. Making waves: initiation and propagation of corticothalamic Ca2+ waves in vivo. Neuron. 2013;12:1136–1150. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.01.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy M, Riedner BA, Huber R, Massimini M, Ferrarelli F, Tononi G. Source modeling sleep slow waves. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;12:1608–1613. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0807933106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mohajerani MH, McVea DA, Fingas M, Murphy TH. Mirrored bilateral slow-wave cortical activity within local circuits revealed by fast bihemispheric voltage-sensitive dye imaging in anesthetized and awake mice. J Neurosci. 2010;12:3745–3751. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6437-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chauvette S, Crochet S, Volgushev M, Timofeev I. Properties of slow oscillation during slow-wave sleep and anesthesia in cats. J Neurosci. 2011;12:14998–15008. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2339-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hangya B, Tihanyi BT, Entz L, Fabó D, Eróss L, Wittner L, Jakus R, Varga V, Freund TF, Ulbert I. Complex propagation patterns characterize human cortical activity during slow-wave sleep. J Neurosci. 2011;12:8770–8779. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1498-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volgushev M, Chauvette S, Timofeev I. Long-range correlation of the membrane potential in neocortical neurons during slow oscillation. Prog Brain Res. 2011;12:181–199. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53839-0.00012-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luczak A, Barthó P. Consistent sequential activity across diverse forms of UP states under ketamine anesthesia. Eur J Neurosci. 2012;12:2830–2838. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08201.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mohajerani MH, Chan AW, Mohsenvand M, LeDue J, Liu R, McVea DA, Boyd JD, Wang YT, Reimers M, Murphy TH. Spontaneous cortical activity alternates between motifs defined by regional axonal projections. Nat Neurosci. 2013;12:1426–1435. doi: 10.1038/nn.3499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ermentrout GB, Kleinfeld D. Traveling electrical waves in cortex: insights from phase dynamics and speculation on a computational role. Neuron. 2001;12:33–44. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00178-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caporale N, Dan Y. Spike timing–dependent plasticity: a Hebbian learning rule. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2008;12:25–46. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wester JC, Contreras D. Columnar interactions determine horizontal propagation of recurrent network activity in neocortex. J Neurosci. 2012;12:5454–5471. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5006-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reiner A, Stern EA, Wilson CJ. Physiology and morphology of intratelencephalically projecting corticostriatal-type neurons in pigeons as revealed by intracellular recording and cell filling. Brain Behav Evol. 2001;12:101–114. doi: 10.1159/000047264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lesku JA, Vyssotski AL, Martinez-Gonzalez D, Wilzeck C, Rattenborg NC. Local sleep homeostasis in the avian brain: convergence of sleep function in mammals and birds? Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2011;12:2419–2428. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lesku JA, Rattenborg NC. Avian sleep. Curr Biol. 2014;12:R12–R14. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y, Brzozowska-Prechtl A, Karten HJ. Laminar and columnar auditory cortex in avian brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;12:12676–12681. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1006645107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keary N, Voss J, Lehmann K, Bischof H-J, Löwel S. Optical imaging of retinotopic maps in a small songbird, the zebra finch. PLoS One. 2010;12:e11912. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011912. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Medina L, Reiner A. Do birds possess homologues of mammalian primary visual, somatosensory and motor cortices? Trends Neurosci. 2000;12:1–12. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(99)01486-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watanabe M, Ito H, Masai H. Cytoarchitecture and visual receptive neurons in the Wulst of the Japanese quail (Coturnix coturnix japonica) J Comp Neurol. 1983;12:188–198. doi: 10.1002/cne.902130206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirsch JA, Güntürkün O, Rose J. Insight without cortex: lessons from the avian brain. Conscious Cogn. 2008;12:475–483. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beckers GJ, Gahr M. Neural processing of short-term recurrence in songbird vocal communication. PLoS One. 2010;12:e11129. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beckers GJ, Gahr M. Large-scale synchronized activity during vocal deviance detection in the zebra finch auditory forebrain. J Neurosci. 2012;12:10594–10608. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6045-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore JT, Chen J, Han B, Meng QC, Veasey SC, Beck SG, Kelz MB. Direct activation of sleep-promoting VLPO neurons by volatile anesthetics contributes to anesthetic hypnosis. Curr Biol. 2008–2016;12:22. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kottler B, Bao H, Zalucki O, Imlach W, Troup M, van Alphen B, Paulk A, Zhang B, van Swinderen B. A sleep/wake circuit controls isoflurane sensitivity in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2013;12:594–598. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.02.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bredenkötter M, Bischof H-J. Differences between ipsilaterally and contralaterally evoked potentials in the visual wulst of the zebra finch. Vis Neurosci. 1990;12:155–163. doi: 10.1017/S0952523800000201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Da Silva FL. In: EEG - FMRI. Mulert C, Lemieux L, editor. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer; 2010. EEG: origin and measurement; pp. 19–38. [Google Scholar]
- Low PS, Shank SS, Sejnowski TJ, Margoliash D. Mammalian-like features of sleep structure in zebra finches. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;12:9081–9086. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0703452105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Destexhe A, Contreras D, Steriade M. Spatiotemporal analysis of local field potentials and unit discharges in cat cerebral cortex during natural wake and sleep states. J Neurosci. 1999;12:4595–4608. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-11-04595.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehghani N, Hatsopoulos NG, Parker RA, Greger B, Halgren E, Cash SS, Destexhe A. Avalanche analysis from multielectrode ensemble recordings in cat, monkey, and human cerebral cortex during wakefulness and sleep. Front Physiol. 2012;12:302. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2012.00302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karten HJ, Brzozowska-Prechtl A, Lovell PV, Tang DD, Mello CV, Wang H, Mitra PP. Digital atlas of the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata) brain: a high-resolution photo atlas. J Comp Neurol. 2013;12:3702–3715. doi: 10.1002/cne.23443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson AB, Faraguna U, Tononi G, Cirelli C. Effects of anesthesia on the response to sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2010;12:1659–1667. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.12.1659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prechtl JC, Bullock TH, Kleinfeld D. Direct evidence for local oscillatory current sources and intracortical phase gradients in turtle visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000;12:877–882. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.2.877. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rattenborg NC. Response to commentary on evolution of slow-wave sleep and palliopallial connectivity in mammals and birds: a hypothesis. Brain Res Bull. 2007;12:187–193. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakata S, Harris KD. Laminar structure of spontaneous and sensory-evoked population activity in auditory cortex. Neuron. 2009;12:404–418. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chauvette S, Volgushev M, Timofeev I. Origin of active states in local neocortical networks during slow sleep oscillation. Cereb Cortex. 2010;12:2660–2674. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu J-Y, Huang X, Zhang C. Propagating waves of activity in the neocortex: what they are, what they do. Neuroscientist. 2008;12:487–502. doi: 10.1177/1073858408317066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vyazovskiy VV, Harris KD. Sleep and the single neuron: the role of global slow oscillations in individual cell rest. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013;12:443–451. doi: 10.1038/nrn3494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubino D, Robbins KA, Hatsopoulos NG. Propagating waves mediate information transfer in the motor cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2006;12:1549–1557. doi: 10.1038/nn1802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shank SS, Margoliash D. Sleep and sensorimotor integration during early vocal learning in a songbird. Nature. 2009;12:73–77. doi: 10.1038/nature07615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dave AS, Margoliash D. Song replay during sleep and computational rules for sensorimotor vocal learning. Science. 2000;12:812–816. doi: 10.1126/science.290.5492.812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graber MH, Helmchen F, Hahnloser RH. Activity in a premotor cortical nucleus of zebra finches is locally organized and exhibits auditory selectivity in neurons but not in glia. PLoS One. 2013;12:e81177. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0081177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahnloser RH, Fee MS. Sleep-related spike bursts in HVC are driven by the nucleus interface of the nidopallium. J Neurophysiol. 2007;12:423–435. doi: 10.1152/jn.00547.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahnloser RH, Kozhevnikov AA, Fee MS. Sleep-related neural activity in a premotor and a basal-ganglia pathway of the songbird. J Neurophysiol. 2006;12:794–812. doi: 10.1152/jn.01064.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Alphen B, Yap MH, Kirszenblat L, Kottler B, van Swinderen B. A dynamic deep sleep stage in Drosophila. J Neurosci. 2013;12:6917–6927. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0061-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- The HDF Group. Hierarchical data format version 5, 2000–2014. http://www.hdfgroup.org/HDF5.
- Jones E, Oliphant T, Peterson P, Scientific Community. SciPy: open source scientific tools for python. 2001–2014. http://www.scipy.org.
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.