Abstract
We are endlessly fascinated by memory; we desire to improve it and fear its loss. While it has long been recognized that brain regions such as the hippocampus are vital for supporting memories of our past experiences (autobiographical memories), we still lack fundamental knowledge about the mechanisms involved. This is because the study of specific neural signatures of autobiographical memories in vivo in humans presents a significant challenge. However, recent developments in high-resolution structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging coupled with advanced analytical methods now permit access to the neural substrates of memory representations that has hitherto been precluded in humans. Here, I describe how the application of ‘decoding’ techniques to brain-imaging data is beginning to disclose how individual autobiographical memory representations evolve over time, deepening our understanding of systems-level consolidation. In particular, this prompts new questions about the roles of the hippocampus and ventromedial prefrontal cortex and offers new opportunities to interrogate the elusive memory trace that has for so long confounded neuroscientists.
Introduction
There are many brain regions that co-operate to enable us to form, store and retrieve memories of our personal past experiences, which are known as autobiographical memories (Maguire, 2001; Svoboda et al. 2006). However, there is one undoubted star of the show and that is the hippocampus, buried deep in the brain's temporal lobes (Fig. 1). The consequences of hippocampal damage can be devastating and far-reaching, not only robbing us of our past (Scoville & Milner, 1957), but preventing us from inhabiting our imagination (Hassabis et al. 2007b; Mullally et al. 2012) and contemplating the future (Tulving, 1985; Klein et al. 2002; Hassabis et al. 2007b; Rosenbaum et al. 2009; Andelman et al. 2010; Race et al. 2011), as well as impairing the ability to navigate effectively in the environment (Maguire et al. 2006) and even altering our perception of the world (Graham et al. 2010; Lee et al. 2012; Mullally et al. 2012). The hippocampus therefore seems to make fundamental contributions to our everyday mental experience, but there is still much we do not know about how it accomplishes these feats.
Models of memory posit the existence of some form of memory representation within the hippocampus that is vital for retrieving an entire memory, the constituent elements of which may be distributed in the cortex (Marr, 1971; O'Reilly & McClelland, 1994; Treves & Rolls, 1994; McClelland & Goddard, 1996; O'Reilly & Rudy, 2000). The theoretical nature of this hippocampal memory trace has been modelled over the years, and related computations have been extensively studied in the rodent hippocampus (Vazdarjanova & Guzowski, 2004; Lee et al. 2004; Leutgeb et al. 2004, 2007; Wills et al. 2005). While unquestionably offering insights into memory representations, it is unknown whether non-humans possess the capacity for autobiographical memory (Suddendorf, 2013; but see Clayton et al. 2003; Corballis, 2013), rendering the relevance of this work for complex autobiographical memories in humans unclear.
In contrast, humans are undeniably adept at recalling and describing their past experiences, but studies have lacked the precision of animal work in trying to elucidate how individual autobiographical memories are represented by neuronal populations in the human hippocampus. This is, in part, a result of the dominant methodological approaches in human cognitive neuroscience. Neuropsychological studies of patients with bilateral hippocampal lesions have been invaluable for mapping out the specific patterns of mnemonic sparing and deficits that arise following such damage (Scoville & Milner, 1957; Mayes, 1988; Spiers et al. 2001; Winocur & Moscovitch, 2011). Conventional functional neuroimaging, such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), has also proved effective for localizing a wide range of cognitive functions to specific brain regions while also highlighting the importance of network activity (e.g. Bullmore & Sporns, 2009; Yarkoni et al. 2011). However, neither neuropsychology nor conventional fMRI can inform directly about the neuronal representation of specific autobiographical memory traces.
This is problematic, because some of the key questions about memory are best leveraged by examining individual memory representations. However, in recent years new ways of analysing fMRI data have emerged. Here, using one of these methods (for others see Chadwick et al. 2012), I will describe how it is now possible to detect specific memory representations in vivo in the human brain, and how we can use this ability to gain new insights into an issue at the heart of memory neuroscience, namely, how do autobiographical memories evolve over time, a process known as systems-level consolidation (Dudai, 2004, 2012; Nadel et al. 2007b2007b, 2012).
Multi-voxel pattern analysis
In a conventional fMRI analysis, the responses to memories of the same type (for example, a set of autobiographical memories) are pooled together. We cannot normally examine individual memories because the fMRI signal would be insufficient; hence, we average across a group of memories. The response to that memory type is assessed in each voxel – a voxel being the smallest measureable unit in a three-dimensional brain image volume (Fig. 2A) – to establish which individual voxels pass an a priori statistical threshold of activation (Fig. 2B). In this approach, therefore, every voxel is considered separately. However, important findings could be missed from conventional fMRI analyses if information is represented in distinct patterns across voxels and, by inference, across the underlying neuronal population, rather than in the number of separate individual voxels that reach a threshold of activation (Fig. 2B). Analysing fMRI data in terms of the activity patterns across multiple voxels is known as multi-voxel pattern analysis (MVPA; Haynes & Rees, 2006; Norman et al. 2006).
In an MVPA analysis, an fMRI data set is split into two; one part is set aside, and the other is used to train a computer algorithm, or classifier, to learn over multiple trials the patterns of activity across voxels that are associated with specific stimuli. The classifier is then applied to the data that were not used for training to ascertain whether it can predict significantly better than chance which specific stimulus the participant had been processing during that part of the fMRI experiment. If the classifier is successful at predicting which stimulus the participant was processing, it must mean that there is information about that stimulus represented in the brain region where the pattern of voxels was identified. Crucially, this type of approach permits examination of patterns of fMRI activity associated with specific stimuli.
With that in mind, using MVPA we found that it was possible to predict a participant's precise spatial location within a virtual reality environment solely from the patterns of fMRI activity across voxels in the hippocampus (Hassabis et al. 2009). In another study, participants watched short movie clips prior to entering the scanner, and then during scanning had to recall the movie clips. Once again, it was possible to predict which specific movie a participant was recalling solely from the pattern of fMRI activity across voxels in the hippocampus (Chadwick et al. 2010). In these studies, the items to be remembered were relatively simple and were provided by the experimenters. Autobiographical memories, in contrast, are rich and complex and are unique to each person. As such, they might be expected to pose a substantial challenge for MVPA classifiers. Would it really be possible to predict which specific autobiographical memory a person is recalling solely from patterns of fMRI activity?
Theoretical considerations
This question begs another. Are events that we recently experienced represented in a similar or different way by the hippocampus compared with autobiographical experiences from long ago? Consolidation of memories undoubtedly occurs rapidly at the synaptic level (Dudai, 2004). In contrast, systems-level consolidation (Dudai, 2004, 2012) and how the neural instantiation of autobiographical memories might change over longer time scales remain uncertain. In general, extant theories agree that the cortex comes to play a greater role in supporting autobiographical memories over time (Marr, 1971; Teyler & DiScenna, 1985; Squire, 1992; Nadel & Moscovitch, 1997; Squire & Wixted, 2011; Winocur & Moscovitch, 2011; Nadel et al. 2012). The precise areas of cortex that may be involved are often not specified, although the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), in particular, has been highlighted as potentially influential for memory consolidation (Bontempi et al. 1999; Frankland & Bontempi, 2005; reviewed by Nieuwenhuis & Takashima, 2011).
There is also agreement that autobiographical memories depend on the hippocampus during initial encoding (Scoville & Milner, 1957). However, its role in supporting autobiographical memories when they are more remote is contentious. The standard model of consolidation (Fig. 3A) argues that memories (including autobiographical memories) become less dependent on the hippocampus, eventually eschewing the need for its involvement altogether during retrieval (Marr, 1971; Teyler & DiScenna, 1985; Squire, 1992; Squire & Wixted, 2011). Alternative theories (multiple trace theory, scene construction theory; Fig. 3B) propose instead that the hippocampus is necessary for retrieving vivid autobiographical memories in perpetuity (Nadel & Moscovitch, 1997; Hassabis & Maguire, 2007, 2009; Winocur & Moscovitch, 2011; Maguire & Mullally, 2013). There is neuropsychological evidence from patients with bilateral hippocampal damage, including those with lesions apparently restricted to the hippocampi, in support of both views (for a review see Winocur & Moscovitch, 2011). Some patients have a limited retrograde amnesia (that is, amnesia for experiences that occurred prior to lesion onset), supporting the standard model of consolidation, because it predicts an impairment of recent memories that might still be dependent on the hippocampus, but sparing of remote memories, which are available in the undamaged cortex. In contrast, other patients are reported who are amnesic for events throughout their lifetime, suggesting that the hippocampus is vital for retrieving even very remote autobiographical memories. Conventional fMRI studies exploring this issue have also produced conflicting findings (e.g. Maguire et al. 2001; Ryan et al. 2001; Maguire & Frith, 2003; Gilboa et al. 2004; Piolino et al. 2004; Rekkas & Constable, 2005; Steinvorth et al. 2006; Viard et al. 2007; Watanabe et al. 2012; but see Niki & Luo, 2002; Piefke et al. 2003) and so contribute to the stalemate.
The key question in this debate, when reduced to its simplest form, concerns where information about specific autobiographical memories is located. Multi-voxel pattern analysis seems well suited to addressing this question and adjudicating between the opposing theories. The standard consolidation view predicts that recent autobiographical memories are represented in the hippocampus and remote autobiographical memories in the cortex. Alternative theories predict that for vivid autobiographical memories, both recent and remote memories are represented in the hippocampus, while the cortex contains more information about remote autobiographical memories.
Detecting autobiographical memory representations
In a recent study, we tested these predictions using MVPA (Bonnici et al. 2012). A week prior to scanning, each subject was interviewed to ascertain details of numerous autobiographical memories. From this pool of memories, three recent memories that were 2 weeks old and three remote memories that were 10 years old were selected for inclusion in the fMRI experiment. From the descriptions of these memories provided by participants we could discern no differences between the recent and remote autobiographical memories. Specifically, great care was taken to ensure that the recent and remote memories were re-experienced with equal and high vividness. In addition, based on analysis of the memory descriptions and ratings provided by the participants, the frequency of recall, level of detail, emotional valence and the perspective taken were all matched between recent and remote memories. We also made strenuous efforts to study memories that were unique, excluding events that occurred repeatedly or were similar to other events. We confirmed that subjects did not think about the previous week's stimulus-eliciting interview at all during scanning. From an intensely interrogated phenomenological perspective, therefore, the recent and remote memories did not differ in any measureable way. During scanning, subjects recalled the memories, and after each recall trial rated whether or not the memory was recalled with high vividness, with only vividly recalled memories included in the MVPA analysis.
As well as the hippocampus, a number of cortical brain areas were examined, namely, entorhinal and perirhinal cortices, parahippocampal and retrosplenial cortices, temporal pole, lateral temporal cortex and vmPFC (Fig. 4A). As shown in Fig. 4B, it was possible to predict which recent and also which remote autobiographical memory was being recalled significantly above chance in all brain areas, underlining the distributed nature of complex autobiographical memory representations (Lashley, 1950). Notably, there was no significant difference in the classifiers’ performances for decoding recent and remote memories in the hippocampus. In contrast, vmPFC contained more information about remote autobiographical memories compared with recent memories.
There were two other interesting findings. Although the hippocampus seemed to represent recent and remote memories in a similar manner, when the voxels that best discriminated between the recent memories and those that best discriminated between the remote memories were reprojected back into the hippocampi, it quickly became clear that the clusters of voxels hardly overlapped (Fig. 5A). This was confirmed by a formal analysis, which showed that the overlap for recent and remote memory reprojections was significantly below chance for the hippocampus. This was in contrast to cortical areas, such as vmPFC, where the voxel patterns (and by inference the underlying neuronal populations) that supported the recent memories overlapped with those supporting the remote memories. Moreover, the location of recent and remote voxel patterns had a systematic distribution down the long axis of the hippocampus. In the anterior hippocampus, there was no significant difference in classification accuracies for the two types of memories. In contrast, classification accuracies were significantly higher in the posterior hippocampus for remote memories compared with recent memories (Fig. 5B). Thus, exactly as with vmPFC, the hippocampus too seems to respect the distinction between recent and remote autobiographical memories.
In interpreting these results, it is important to consider whether any other factors besides the recency/remoteness of autobiographical memories could have influenced the results. For instance, during scanning perhaps participants were recalling the prescan interview where the memories were initially elicited. However, when asked how often they had thought about the memories since the session a week earlier, their ratings confirmed that they had hardly given them any thought. Moreover, the interview concerned both the recent and remote memories, and so the differential effects for the two types of memories that we found could not have arisen from this common interview experience.
The scanning protocol required participants to recall the memories a number of times during scanning, and it could be argued that the memories were re-encoded on each trial, which may have polluted the recall effects. As with the interview above, if re-encoding did occur, it would presumably have done so for both recent and remote memories, again making the differential results we found difficult to explain. In addition, the nature of MVPA means that classification is only possible if information is shared across training and test trials. Re-encoded memories would have been different for each trial, leading to chance classification, which was not the case here. On a related theme, it could have been that recalling a remote memory reactivated it, effectively transforming it back into a recent memory. If this were the case, then the prediction would be for no difference between recent and remote memories (if all memories were now essentially recent). However, the differential effects, cortically and within the hippocampus itself, clearly contradict this idea.
Repeatedly recalling the memories may have had other effects. For instance, it may have lessened the true episodic nature of the memories and influenced hippocampal engagement. However, previous studies have shown that hippocampal activation does not diminish as a function of multiple retrievals of autobiographical memories (Nadel et al. 2007a2007a; Svoboda & Levine, 2009). The participants also confirmed that repeated recall did not change the memory, and only those trials where participants indicated that they had recalled the memory with high vividness were included in the analysis. Given that ease of recall and other phenomenological factors were also highly similar across the recent and remote memories, neither these, nor the alternatives above, adequately explain the differential findings.
Recently, Bonnici and Maguire (in preparation) rescanned the subjects from Bonnici et al. (2012) to examine how the memories in question had evolved in the intervening 2 years (H.M. Bonnici & E.A. Maguire, unpublished observations). They found that the now 2- and 12-year-old memories were indistinguishable in terms of involvement of the vmPFC, while within the hippocampus, the 2-year-old memories were represented more posteriorly than they were 2 years previously. This is despite the vividness and other phenomenological aspects of the memories remaining essentially unchanged.
As well as having anterior and posterior portions, the hippocampus is composed of a number of subregions, i.e. CA1, CA2 and CA3 (Lorente de No, 1934), bordered by the dentate gyrus (DG) and subiculum (Amaral & Lavenex, 2007; Fig. 6A). The findings of Bonnici et al. (2012) gave no indication of whether the anterior–posterior differential effects were being driven by all subfields or by one or two in particular. In a recent study, therefore, Bonnici et al. (2013a), using high-resolution fMRI combined with MVPA, found that it was possible to detect representations of specific autobiographical memories in individual hippocampal subfields. Moreover, while subfields in the anterior hippocampus contained information about both recent and remote autobiographical memories, posterior CA3 and DG contained only information about the remote memories (Fig. 6B). Thus, the subfields in posterior hippocampus seem to be involved differentially in the representation of recent and remote autobiographical memories during vivid recall.
Re-examining the role of the hippocampus
This work shows that despite being rich, detailed and complex, using MVPA it is possible to predict which autobiographical memory a person is recalling solely from patterns of fMRI activity across voxels. This implies that information about recent and remote autobiographical memories is represented in the hippocampus, while within the cortex remote memories are better represented, in particular in vmPFC. These findings clearly support the alternative theories of consolidation (Nadel & Moscovitch, 1997; Hassabis & Maguire, 2007, 2009; Winocur & Moscovitch, 2011; Maguire & Mullally, 2013; Fig. 3B), but the findings of Bonnici et al. (2012, 2013a) go further and raise some interesting new issues about the functioning of the hippocampus.
While the overall performance of the hippocampal classifiers for recent and remote memories was similar, when the voxel patterns were examined an anterior–posterior distinction emerged, with the posterior hippocampus representing remote autobiographical memories more than recent. This suggests that as well as consolidation occurring within the cortex (vmPFC), there are changes within the hippocampus also. Moreover, this may account for the disparate findings across patients with hippocampal damage; if a lesion encroaches more on posterior hippocampus, then remote autobiographical memories may be more compromised than if damage was more anterior (Penfield & Mathieson, 1974; Winocur & Moscovitch, 2011). While Gilboa et al. (2004) suggested that remote autobiographical memories might be distributed throughout the hippocampus, Bonnici et al. (2012) have shown that there is a clear anterior–posterior distinction that is not predicted by any extant theory. Indeed, when Bonnici et al. (2012) pooled across all memories they found it was possible to classify any given memory as recent or remote solely from fMRI activity across voxels in the hippocampus. This suggests that something is signalling that a memory is recent or remote; after all, people can readily distinguish between recent and remote autobiographical memories, suggesting that there is something different about them, but what might this be?
As described earlier, the recent and remote memories were scrupulously matched on a wide range of variables; they were all vividly recalled, and it was not that the remote memories were more semanticised, at least at an explicit, phenomenological level. When the memories were retested 2 years later, the representations of the memories that had been a few weeks old but were now some years older differed in terms of the locations of discriminating voxels, with a shift more posteriorly in the hippocampus. This is despite being the same memories, with the same content and similar ratings for vividness and other key variables. One explanation for this finding could be that overlap is still present in some of the initial neuronal connections, but that synaptic pruning of weakly connected neurons resulted in an overall decrease in activity in the original retrieval network, and a reorganization of the memory network involved expansion to include a broader range of connections over time. Alternatively, the shift of autobiographical memory representations from one end of the hippocampus to another, regions that are anatomically distinct and with dissimilar connectivity, could indicate that the recall of autobiographical memories does not necessarily involve the re-instantiation of ensemble activity in the same neurons that were involved when the memories were young. Thus, the traditional view of ‘cells that fire together, wire together’ (Hebb, 1949) may not apply in this case. It is impossible to adjudicate between these explanations currently, but further developments in high-resolution fMRI and multivariate analysis methods may make this issue tractable in the future.
The discovery that the anterior and posterior hippocampus might have different functions has been of increasing interest in the last few years. Poppenk et al. (2013) recently reviewed numerous reports of differential engagement of anterior and posterior hippocampus in humans and other species. They suggested that the posterior hippocampus may represent more fine-grained, detailed memories, whereas the anterior hippocampus might support more gist-like retrieval (see also Evensmoen et al. 2013). However, given that both recent and remote autobiographical memories were rich and detailed and, if anything, recent memories might be expected to be richer and more detailed (although Bonnici et al. 2012 ensured that this was not the case at an explicit level), then it is difficult to explain the propensity of the posterior hippocampus to represent specifically remote autobiographical memories.
The posterior hippocampus has been associated with spatial processing (e.g. Moser & Moser, 1998; Maguire et al. 2000). Perhaps recent memories are experienced as coherent scenes or events that are temporarily represented in the hippocampus (using anterior and posterior aspects), with cortical consolidation happening relatively quickly. The constituent elements of autobiographical memories are then the preserve of the cortical areas. At retrieval, this piecemeal information might be funnelled back automatically into the hippocampus (in a process that may involve vmPFC), but in order to be assembled into a coherent form, this requires a process that takes place in the posterior hippocampus (Bonnici et al. 2012). This may be why the remote memories were discernible to a greater degree in posterior hippocampus, because they rely on this process more than do recent memories. A further speculation might be that the posterior hippocampus may implement the spatial framework into which the elements of a remote memory are reconstructed (Hassabis & Maguire, 2007, 2009; Maguire & Mullally, 2013), in line with findings from patients with hippocampal damage who have lost the ability to construct spatially coherent scenes (e.g. Hassabis et al. 2007b; Race et al. 2011; Mullally et al. 2012).
Studies in rodents and computational models suggest that key computations necessary for memory occur in the subfields, such as pattern separation (in DG and CA3), the process of distinguishing similar memories from each other, as well as pattern completion (in CA3), which facilitates the retrieval of previously stored memories from partial cues (Marr, 1971; Treves & Rolls, 1994; McClelland et al. 1995; Kesner et al. 2004; Leutgeb et al. 2004, 2007; Leutgeb & Leutgeb, 2007; Alvernhe et al. 2008; Hunsaker & Kesner, 2008; Gilbert & Brushfield, 2009; Rolls, 2010; Aimone et al. 2011; O'Reilly et al. 2011). Bonnici et al. (2013a2013a) noted the particular involvement of CA3 and DG subfields in the posterior hippocampus during recall of remote autobiographical memories. If remote memories have to undergo more reconstruction than recent memories, then the accumulation of memory elements and spatial contexts in the posterior hippocampus might trigger CA3-mediated pattern completion to a greater extent, while DG might act to ensure that this reconstructed memory is maintained as distinct from other memories that the hippocampus might be processing at that time or related memories that might be partly coactivated during retrieval.
Gaps in our knowledge about vmPFC function
All of the cortical regions tested by Bonnici et al. (2012) showed above-chance decoding of both recent and remote autobiographical memories. However, the vmPFC was alone in displaying significantly better decoding for remote autobiographical memories. The vmPFC is consistently engaged during autobiographical memory retrieval in conventional fMRI studies. However, there is some variation in the cortical areas labelled as vmPFC in the literature. Using the scheme of Petrides & Pandya (1999; Fig. 7A), I am referring to portions of area 14, ventral parts of 24 and 32, the caudal part of area 10, the medial part of 11, with additional involvement of area 25 (see examples in Fig. 7B and C). Of note, these frontal areas are known to be heavily interconnected with each other (Saleem et al. 2008; Beckmann et al. 2009; Passingham & Wise, 2012); hence, Bonnici et al. (2012) examined them altogether in their vmPFC region of interest (Fig. 7D). This vmPFC region has also been highlighted as potentially influential for memory consolidation in other studies (e.g. Bontempi et al. 1999; Frankland & Bontempi, 2005; Goshen et al. 2011; Nieuwenhuis & Takashima, 2011).
The MVPA data revealed a particular association between remote autobiographical memories and the vmPFC. Moreover, Bonnici et al. (2012) found that recent and remote autobiographical memories were not represented in different parts of the vmPFC region of interest. In contrast to the hippocampus, the discriminating voxels for recent and remote memories overlapped within vmPFC, suggesting that they may share underlying neuronal ensembles. Nieuwenhuis & Takashima (2011) proposed that the vmPFC may link cortical representational areas in remote memory because, over time, direct connectivity with limbic regions, such as the hippocampus, decays (Fig. 3A). However, the idea that the vmPFC assumes the role of the hippocampus over time is not supported by the MVPA findings, where remote memories were still represented in the hippocampus as well as the vmPFC. An alternative view, which accords with the proposal of Winocur & Moscovitch (2011), is that perhaps the quality of the remote autobiographical memory representations in vmPFC differ from those in the hippocampus. It could be that the initial retrieval of a remote memory reactivates both a schematic, gist-like, cortical version and a detailed hippocampal version of the memory. As already noted, at an explicit and phenomenological level the recent and remote autobiographical memories in the Bonnici et al. (2012) study were of equivalent vividness and detail, which speaks against this view. Moreover, whether there is a need for two versions of the same memory, particularly when one is detailed, seems somewhat unlikely, because this redundancy would be an inefficient use of neural resources. Perhaps examination of the wider literature on vmPFC might offer some guidance on interpreting the MVPA autobiographical memory findings.
The fMRI and neuropsychological fields are replete with studies examining the vmPFC, associating it with a range of cognitive functions, including emotional processing (Goel & Dolan, 2003), planning (Burgess & Shallice, 1996), decision making (Shallice & Burgess, 1991; Bechara et al. 1998), moral behaviour (Thomas et al. 2011), schema (Tse et al. 2011; van Kesteren et al. 2013), valuation and reward processing (Jocham et al. 2012), risk taking and gambling (Bechara et al. 1994). While there have recently been some attempts to link the vmPFC and its role, for example, in decision making (Kumaran et al. 2009; Nieuwenhuis & Takashima, 2011) and reward (Barron et al. 2013) to memory and the hippocampus using fMRI, this has not involved autobiographical memory. More surprising still is the dearth of information about autobiographical memory in patients with bilateral vmPFC lesions (Fig. 8).
A complete review of the neuropsychological literature is beyond the scope of this article. To summarize with some examples from the literature, in the plentiful studies of patients with vmPFC lesions in decision making (e.g. Fellows, 2011) and gambling and reward studies (e.g. Bechara et al. 1994), memory scores are almost never reported. Where they are, it is often working memory scores or performance on standardized tests that assess memory within a 30 min time frame. There are reports that patients can describe the instructions or contingencies of tasks, suggesting intact memory (e.g. Rolls et al. 1994), but the status of their autobiographical memory and, in particular, their remote autobiographical memory, is never mentioned. In other studies, including those where autobiographical memory has been tested, vmPFC-damaged patients are typically included as part of a larger group of frontal-damaged patients, and so effects relating to vmPFC per se cannot be evaluated (e.g. Della Sala et al. 1993; Kopelman et al. 1999, 2003; Levine, 2004). In some cases, remote memory for public events has been tested, but not autobiographical memory (Mangels et al. 1996). Where autobiographical memory has been tested in the presence of bilateral vmPFC lesions, impairment has been noted (e.g. Levine et al. 1998; Piolino et al. 2003; Bird et al. 2004), but the lesions were not restricted to vmPFC and included shearing of white matter tracts or diffuse dementing pathologies.
Some clues about the vmPFC function might be gleaned from a related line of research concerning confabulation (Gilboa & Verfaellie, 2010). Patients who confabulate provide information that is verifiably false or that is clearly inappropriate for the context of retrieval, while being unaware of these falsehoods. Damage to vmPFC has been implicated in causing this disorder (Gilboa & Moscovitch, 2002). For instance, Moscovitch & Melo (1997) reported that patients with presumed damage or dysfunction in the region of the vmPFC confabulated in response to cues designed to elicit autobiographical memories. Interestingly, even when not confabulating, they had more difficulty than medial temporal lobe amnesic patients in recovering autobiographical memories related to these cues. There are numerous theories that seek to explain confabulation. For instance, some regard confabulation as temporal context confusion or an inability to filter information according to its relevance to ongoing reality (Schnider & Ptak, 1999; Nahum et al. 2009). Alternatively, strategic retrieval theories focus on investigating and characterizing how memory-monitoring processes break down in confabulation (Moscovitch 1989; Kopelman, 1999; Gilboa & Moscovitch, 2002), which may be related to another acknowledged difficulty that patients with frontal lobe lesions have in organizing information temporally (Moscovitch, 1989; Shimamura et al. 1990). These types of account seem to have relevance for understanding the role of the vmPFC in representing remote autobiographical memories. However, this must be tempered by the fact that confabulation is not associated only with vmPFC damage (Bajo et al. 2010), and many vmPFC-lesioned patients do not confabulate.
In summary, we lack fundamental knowledge about the status of autobiographical memory in patients with vmPFC lesions. Clearly, they are not densely amnesic in the manner of patients with bilateral hippocampal damage. Nevertheless, given vmPFC engagement during autobiographical memory retrieval in conventional fMRI studies, as well as our recent MVPA fMRI findings showing that information about remote autobiographical memories is represented there, there is an urgent need to know more. I suggest we should examine patients with more circumscribed frontal lobe lesions that either comprise only the vmPFC area described in this paper or specific subareas within it. Of course, patients with very specific frontal lesions restricted to locations of interest will be uncommon, but in the same way that rare patients with bilateral focal hippocampal damage are few but make important contributions to our understanding of memory, so it could be with small numbers of patients with focal damage to the ventromedial frontal lobes. They could illuminate the mechanisms of autobiographical memory and provide further insights into system-level memory consolidation. Moreover, they could elucidate the undoubted role of the vmPFC in other functions, such as value representations and reward processing. For instance, it could be that vivid remote memories are more valued and this is why the vmPFC is involved. In contrast, patients with vmPFC damage may have limited access to coherent representations of past experiences (or future scenarios, about which we also know very little in these patients), which could interact with their value-based decision making, rendering them unable to act in a well-informed and normative fashion.
Concluding remarks
Autobiographical memories are dynamic and restless (Dudai, 2012) and, in particular, seem to undergo changes over time that involve the hippocampus and vmPFC. The MVPA of fMRI data is one of a number of multivariate approaches which now enable us to interrogate individual autobiographical memories in humans non-invasively and in vivo (Chadwick et al. 2012). The MVPA results have revealed some surprising findings that invite speculation, provoke numerous questions and suggest obvious targets for future investigations. The focus so far has been on vivid and easily retrievable memories. Studies examining autobiographical memories that vary in vividness, as well as the process of semanticization could provide a more complete picture of the system at work. In addition, exploring how memories develop, how long consolidation takes, the feature dimensions of autobiographical memories, how they are sustained, and how and why they sometimes decay (Hardt et al. 2013), in normal ageing and in the context of pathology (Bonnici et al. 2013b2013b), are important lines of inquiry that are now tractable. It will also be important to consider the computations that operate on memory traces and the nature of the connectivity between regions such as the hippocampus and vmPFC (Takashima et al. 2009; Soderlund et al. 2012). Overall, this decoding approach complements existing methods and may help to bridge the gap between functional neuroimaging and neuropsychology in humans, on the one hand, and electrophysiological and lesion studies in animals, as well as computational models of the hippocampus, on the other.
Call for comments
Readers are invited to give their opinion on this article. To submit a comment, go to: http://ep.physoc.org/letters/submit/expphysiol;99/3/471.
New Findings
What is the topic of this review? This lecture is concerned with autobiographical memory representations, how they evolve and change over time, and the brain regions that support them.
What advances does it highlight? The use of high resolution structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging combined with methods such as multi-voxel pattern analysis are opening up new opportunities to study memory consolidation in vivo in humans.
Acknowledgments
Thanks to all my research fellows, in particular Heidi Bonnici and Martin Chadwick. Thanks also to Dick Passingham and Mike Kopelman for useful discussions, and anonymous reviewers for helpful suggestions.
Additional Information
Competing interests
None declared.
Funding
This work was supported by the Wellcome Trust.
References
- Aimone JB, Deng W, Gage FH. Resolving new memories: a critical look at the dentate gyrus, adult neurogenesis, and pattern separation. Neuron. 2011;70:589–596. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alvernhe A, Van Cauter T, Save E, Poucet B. Different CA1 and CA3 representations of novel routes in a shortcut situation. J Neurosci. 2008;28:7324–7333. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1909-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amaral DG, Lavenex P. Hippocampal neuroanatomy. In: Anderson P, Morris R, Amaral D, Bliss T, O'Keefe J, editors. The Hippocampus Book. New York: Oxford University Press; 2007. pp. 37–114. [Google Scholar]
- Andelman F, Hoofien D, Goldberg I, Aizenstein O, Neufeld MY. Bilateral hippocampal lesion and a selective impairment of the ability for mental time travel. Neurocase. 2010;16:426–435. doi: 10.1080/13554791003623318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bajo A, Fleminger S, Kopelman MD. Confabulations are emotionally charged, but not always for the best. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16:975–983. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710000913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barron HC, Dolan RJ, Behrens TE. Online evaluation of novel choices by simultaneous representation of multiple memories. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16:1492–1498. doi: 10.1038/nn.3515. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition. 1994;50:7–15. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Anderson SW. Dissociation of working memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 1998;18:428–437. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-01-00428.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beckmann M, Johansen-Berg H, Rushworth MFS. Connectivity-based parcellation of human cingulate cortex and its relation to functional specialization. J Neurosci. 2009;29:1175–1190. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3328-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bird CM, Castelli F, Malik O, Frith U, Husain M. The impact of extensive frontal lobe damage on ‘Theory of Mind’ and cognition. Brain. 2004;127:914–928. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonnici HM, Chadwick MJ, Lutti A, Hassabis D, Weiskopf N, Maguire EA. Detecting representations of recent and remote autobiographical memories in vmPFC and hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2012;32:16982–16991. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2475-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonnici HM, Chadwick MJ, Maguire EA. Representations of recent and remote autobiographical memories in hippocampal subfields. Hippocampus. 2013a;23:849–854. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonnici HM, Sidhu M, Chadwick MJ, Duncan JS, Maguire EA. Assessing hippocampal functional reserve in temporal lobe epilepsy: a multi-voxel pattern analysis of fMRI data. Epilepsy Res. 2013b;105:140–149. doi: 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2013.01.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bontempi B, Laurent-Demir C, Destrade C, Jaffard R. Time-dependent reorganization of brain circuitry underlying long-term memory storage. Nature. 1999;400:671–675. doi: 10.1038/23270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bullmore E, Sporns O. Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:186–198. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgess PW, Shallice T. Response suppression, initiation and strategy use following frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia. 1996;34:263–273. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00104-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chadwick MJ, Bonnici HM, Maguire EA. Decoding information in the human hippocampus: a user's guide. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50:3107–3121. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.07.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chadwick MJ, Hassabis D, Weiskopf N, Maguire EA. Decoding individual episodic memory traces in the human hippocampus. Curr Biol. 2010;20:544–547. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.01.053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clayton NC, Bussey TJ, Dickinson A. Can animals recall the past and plan for the future? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4:685–691. doi: 10.1038/nrn1180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corballis MC. The wandering rat: response to Suddendorf. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17:152. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Della Sala S, Laiacona M, Spinnler H, Trivelli C. Autobiographical recollection and frontal damage. Neuropsychologia. 1993;31:823–839. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(93)90131-i. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dudai Y. The neurobiology of consolidations, or, how stable is the engram. Annu Rev Psychol. 2004;55:51–86. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dudai Y. The restless engram: consolidations never end. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2012;35:227–247. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evensmoen HR, Lehn H, Xu J, Witter MP, Nadel L, Håberg AK. The anterior hippocampus supports a coarse, global environmental representation and the posterior hippocampus supports fine-grained, local environmental representations. J Cogn Neurosci. 2013;25:1908–1925. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fellows LK. Orbitofrontal contributions to value-based decision making: evidence from humans with frontal lobe damage. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2011;1239:51–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06229.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frankland PW, Bontempi B. The organization of recent and remote memories. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:119–130. doi: 10.1038/nrn1607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert PE, Brushfield AM. The role of the CA3 hippocampal subregion in spatial memory: a process oriented behavioral assessment. Prog Neuropsychopharm Biol Psychiatry. 2009;33:774–781. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.03.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilboa A, Moscovitch M. The cognitive neuroscience of confabulation: a review and a model. In: Baddeley AD, Kopelman MD, Wilson BA, editors. Handbook of Memory Disorders. 2nd edition. London: John Wiley & Sons; 2002. pp. 315–342. [Google Scholar]
- Gilboa A, Verfaellie M. Telling it like it isn't: the cognitive neuroscience of confabulation. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16:961–966. doi: 10.1017/S135561771000113X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilboa A, Winocur G, Grady CL, Hevenor SJ, Moscovitch M. Remembering our past: functional neuroanatomy of recollection of recent and very remote personal events. Cereb Cortex. 2004;14:1214–1225. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goel V, Dolan RJ. Reciprocal neural response within lateral and ventral medial prefrontal cortex during hot and cold reasoning. Neuroimage. 2003;20:2314–2321. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.07.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goshen I, Brodsky M, Prakash R, Wallace J, Gradinaru V, Ramakrishnan C, Deisseroth K. Dynamics of retrieval strategies for remote memories. Cell. 2011;147:678–689. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.09.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graham KS, Barense MD, Lee AC. Going beyond LTM in the MTL: a synthesis of neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings on the role of the medial temporal lobe in memory and perception. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:831–853. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gupta R, Tranel D, Duff MC. Ventromedial preftonal cortex damage does not impair the development and use of common ground in social interaction: implications for cognitive theory of mind. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50:145–152. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.11.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hardt O, Nader K, Nadel L. Decay happens: the role of active forgetting in memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17:110–120. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hassabis D, Chu C, Rees G, Weiskopf N, Molyneux PD, Maguire EA. Decoding neuronal ensembles in the human hippocampus. Curr Biol. 2009;19:546–554. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.02.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hassabis D, Kumaran D, Maguire EA. Using imagination to understand the neural basis of episodic memory. J Neurosci. 2007a;27:14365–14374. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4549-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hassabis D, Kumaran D, Vann SD, Maguire EA. Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007b;104:1726–1731. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0610561104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hassabis D, Maguire EA. Deconstructing episodic memory with construction. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:299–306. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hassabis D, Maguire EA. The construction system of the brain. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2009;364:1263–1271. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haynes JD, Rees G. Decoding mental states from brain activity in humans. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:523–534. doi: 10.1038/nrn1931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hebb DO. The Organization of Behavior. New York: Wiley & Sons; 1949. [Google Scholar]
- Hunsaker MR, Kesner RP. Evaluating the differential roles of the dorsal dentate gyrus, dorsal CA3, and dorsal CA1 during a temporal ordering for spatial locations task. Hippocampus. 2008;18:955–964. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20455. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jocham G, Hunt LT, Near J, Behrens TE. A mechanism for value-guided choice based on the extinction-inhibition balance in prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15:960–961. doi: 10.1038/nn.3140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kesner RP, Lee I, Gilbert P. A behavioral assessment of hippocampal function based on a subregional analysis. Rev Neurosci. 2004;15:333–352. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.2004.15.5.333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein SB, Loftus J, Kihlstrom JF. Memory and temporal experience: the effects of episodic memory loss on an amnesic patient's ability to remember the past and imagine the future. Soc Cogn. 2002;20:353–379. [Google Scholar]
- Kopelman MD. Varieties of false memories. Cogn Neuropsychol. 1999;16:197–214. [Google Scholar]
- Kopelman MD, Lasserson D, Kingsley DR, Bello F, Rush C, Stanhope N, Stevens TG, Goodman G, Buckman JR, Heilpern G, Kendall BE, Colchester AC. Retrograde amnesia and the volume of critical brain structures. Hippocampus. 2003;13:879–891. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kopelman MD, Stanhope N, Kingsley D. Retrograde amnesia in patients with diencephalic, temporal lobe or frontal lesions. Neuropsychologia. 1999;37:939–958. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(98)00143-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumaran D, Summerfield JJ, Hassabis D, Maguire EA. Tracking the emergence of conceptual knowledge during human decision making. Neuron. 2009;63:889–901. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lashley KS. In search of the engram. Society of Experimental Biology, Symposium. 1950;4:454–482. [Google Scholar]
- Lee AC, Yeung LK, Barense MD. The hippocampus and visual perception. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012;6:91. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee I, Yoganarasimha D, Rao G, Knierim JJ. Comparison of population coherence of place cells in hippocampal subfields CA1 and CA3. Nature. 2004;430:456–459. doi: 10.1038/nature02739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leutgeb JK, Leutgeb S, Moser MB, Moser EI. Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus and CA3 of the hippocampus. Science. 2007;315:961–966. doi: 10.1126/science.1135801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leutgeb S, Leutgeb JK. Pattern separation, pattern completion, and new neuronal codes within a continuous CA3 map. Learn Mem. 2007;14:745–757. doi: 10.1101/lm.703907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leutgeb S, Leutgeb JK, Treves A, Moser MB, Moser EI. Distinct ensemble codes in hippocampal areas CA3 and CA1. Science. 2004;305:1295–1298. doi: 10.1126/science.1100265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levine B. Autobiographical memory and the self in time: brain lesions effects, functional neuroanatomy, and lifespan development. Brain Cogn. 2004;55:54–58. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00280-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levine B, Black SE, Cabeza R, Sinden M, Mcintosh AR, Toth JP, Tulving E, Stuss DT. Episodic memory and the self in a case of isolated retrograde amnesia. Brain. 1998;121:1951–1973. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.10.1951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorente de No R. Studies on the structure of the cerebral cortex II. Continuation of the study of the ammonic system. J Psychol Neurol. 1934;46:113–117. [Google Scholar]
- McClelland JL, Goddard NH. Considerations arising from a complementary learning systems perspective on hippocampus and neocortex. Hippocampus. 1996;6:654–665. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1996)6:6<654::AID-HIPO8>3.0.CO;2-G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McClelland JL, McNaughton BL, Reilly RC. Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychol Rev. 1995;102:419–457. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.3.419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maguire EA. Neuroimaging studies of autobiographical event memory. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2001;356:1441–1451. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2001.0944. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maguire EA, Frith CD. Lateral asymmetry in the hippocampal response to the remoteness of autobiographical memories. J Neurosci. 2003;23:5302–5307. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-12-05302.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maguire EA, Gadian DG, Johnsrude IS, Good CD, Ashburner J, Frackowiak RS, Frith CD. Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000;97:4398–4403. doi: 10.1073/pnas.070039597. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maguire EA, Henson RN, Mummery CJ, Frith CD. Activity in prefrontal cortex, not hippocampus, varies parametrically with the increasing remoteness of memories. Neuroreport. 2001;12:441–444. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200103050-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maguire EA, Mullally SL. The hippocampus: a manifesto for change. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2013;142:1180–1189. doi: 10.1037/a0033650. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maguire EA, Nannery R, Spiers HJ. Navigation around London by a taxi driver with bilateral hippocampal lesions. Brain. 2006;129:2894–2907. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mangels JA, Gershberg FB, Shimamura AP. Impaired retrieval from remote memory in patients with frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychology. 1996;10:31–41. [Google Scholar]
- Marr D. Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1971;262:23–81. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1971.0078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayes AR. Human Organic Memory Disorders. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Moscovitch M. Confabulation and the frontal systems: strategic versus associative retrieval in neuropsychological theories of memory. In: Roediger HL, Craik FI, editors. Varieties of Memory and Consciousness: Essays in Honor of Endel Tulving. Hillsdale, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1989. pp. 133–160. [Google Scholar]
- Moscovitch M, Melo B. Strategic retrieval and the frontal lobes: evidence from confabulation and amnesia. Neurospychologia. 1997;35:1017–1034. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(97)00028-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moser MB, Moser EI. Functional differentiation in the hippocampus. Hippocampus. 1998;8:608–619. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:6<608::AID-HIPO3>3.0.CO;2-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mullally SL, Intraub H, Maguire EA. Attenuated boundary extension produces a paradoxical memory advantage in amnesic patients. Curr Biol. 2012;22:261–268. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nadel L, Campbell J, Ryan L. Autobiographical memory retrieval and hippocampal activation as a function of repetition and the passage of time. Neural Plast. 2007a;2007:90472. doi: 10.1155/2007/90472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nadel L, Hupbach A, Gomez R, Newman-Smith K. Memory formation, consolidation and transformation. Neurosci Biobeh Rev. 2012;36:1640–1645. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nadel L, Moscovitch M. Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1997;7:217–227. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(97)80010-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nadel L, Winocur G, Ryan L, Moscovitch M. Sytems consolidation and the hippocampus: two views. Debates in Neuroscience. 2007b;1:55–66. [Google Scholar]
- Nahum L, Ptak R, Leemann B, Schnider A. Disorientation, confabulation, and extinction capacity: clues on how the brain creates reality. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65:966–972. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nieuwenhuis IL, Takashima A. The role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in memory consolidation. Behav Brain Res. 2011;218:325–334. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niki K, Luo J. An fMRI study on the time-limited role of the medial temporal lobe in long-term topographical autobiographic memory. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14:500–507. doi: 10.1162/089892902317362010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norman KA, Polyn SM, Detre GJ, Haxby JV. Beyond mind-reading: multi-voxel pattern analysis of fMRI data. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10:424–430. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Reilly RC, Bhattacharyya R, Howard MD, Ketz N. Complementary learning systems. Cogn Sci. 2011 doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01214.x. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01214.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Reilly RC, McClelland JL. Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall: avoiding a trade-off. Hippocampus. 1994;4:661–682. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450040605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Reilly RC, Rudy JW. Computational principles of learning in the neocortex and hippocampus. Hippocampus. 2000;10:389–397. doi: 10.1002/1098-1063(2000)10:4<389::AID-HIPO5>3.0.CO;2-P. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Passingham RE, Wise SP. The Neurobiology of the Prefrontal Cortex. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Penfield W, Mathieson G. Memory. Autopsy findings and comments on the role of the hippocampus in experiential recall. Arch Neurol. 1974;31:145–154. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1974.00490390027001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrides M, Pandya DN. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis in the human and macaque brain and corticocortical connection patterns. Eur J Neurosci. 1999;11:1011–1036. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00518.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piefke M, Weiss PH, Zilles K, Markowitsch HJ, Fink GR. Differential remoteness and emotional tone modulate the neural correlates of autobiographical memory. Brain. 2003;126:650–668. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piolino P, Desgranges B, Belliard S, Matuszewski V, Lalevee C, De La Sayette V, Eustache F. Autobiographical memory and autonoetic consciousness: triple dissociation in neurodegenerative diseases. Brain. 2003;126:2203–2219. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piolino P, Giffard-Quillon G, Desgranges B, Chetelat G, Baron JC, Eustache F. Re-experiencing old memories via hippocampus: a PET study of autobiographical memory. Neuroimage. 2004;22:1371–1383. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.02.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poppenk J, Evensmoen HR, Moscovitch M, Nadel L. Long-axis specialization of the human hippocampus. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17:230–240. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Race E, Keane MM, Verfaellie M. Medial temporal lobe damage causes deficits in episodic memory and episodic future thinking not attributable to deficits in narrative construction. J Neurosci. 2011;31:10262–10269. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1145-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rekkas PV, Constable RT. Evidence that autobiographic memory retrieval does not become independent of the hippocampus: an fMRI study contrasting very recent with remote events. J Cogn Neurosci. 2005;17:1950–1961. doi: 10.1162/089892905775008652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. A computational theory of episodic memory formation in the hippocampus. Behav Brain Res. 2010;215:180–196. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.03.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Hornak J, Wade D, McGrath J. Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994;57:1518–1524. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.57.12.1518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenbaum RS, Gilboa A, Levine B, Winocur G, Moscovitch M. Amnesia as an impairment of detail generation and binding: evidence from personal, fictional, and semantic narratives in K.C. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2181–2187. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan L, Nadel L, Keil K, Putnam K, Schnyer D, Trouard T, Moscovitch M. Hippocampal complex and retrieval of recent and very remote autobiographical memories: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging in neurologically intact people. Hippocampus. 2001;11:707–714. doi: 10.1002/hipo.1086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saleem KS, Kondo H, Price JL. Complementary circuits connecting the orbital and medial prefrontal networks with the temporal, insular and opercular cortex in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol. 2008;506:659–693. doi: 10.1002/cne.21577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schnider A, Ptak R. Spontaneous confabulators fail to suppress currently irrelevant memory traces. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:677–681. doi: 10.1038/10236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scoville WB, Milner B. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1957;20:11–21. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shallice T, Burgess PW. Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain. 1991;114:727–741. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.2.727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shimamura AP, Janowsky JS, Squire LR. Memory for the temporal order of events in patients with frontal lobe lesions and amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia. 1990;28:803–813. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(90)90004-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Söderlund H, Moscovitch M, Kumar N, Mandic M, Levine B. As time goes by: hippocampal connectivity changes with remoteness of autobiographical memory retrieval. Hippocampus. 2012;22:670–679. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20927. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spiers HJ, Maguire EA, Burgess N. Hippocampal amnesia. Neurocase. 2001;7:357–382. doi: 10.1076/neur.7.5.357.16245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squire LR. Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychol Rev. 1992;99:195–231. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.99.2.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squire LR, Wixted JT. The cognitive neuroscience of human memory since H.M. Ann Rev Neurosci. 2011;34:259–288. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113720. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinvorth S, Corkin S, Halgren E. Ecphory of autobiographical memories: an fMRI study of recent and remote memory retrieval. Neuroimage. 2006;30:285–298. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suddendorf T. Mental time travel: continuities and discontinuities. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17:151–152. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Summerfield JJ, Hassabis D, Maguire EA. Cortical midline involvement in autobiographical memory. Neuroimage. 2009;44:1188–1200. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Svoboda E, Levine B. The effects of rehearsal on the functional neuroanatomy of episodic autobiographical and semantic remembering: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci. 2009;29:3073–3082. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3452-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Svoboda E, McKinnon MC, Levine B. The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:2189–2208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takashima A, Nieuwenhuis ILC, Jensen O, Talamini LM, Rijpkema M, Fernández G. Shift from hippocampal to neocortical centred retrieval network with consolidation. J Neurosci. 2009;29:10087–10093. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0799-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teyler TJ, DiScenna P. The role of hippocampus in memory: a hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1985;9:377–389. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(85)90016-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas BC, Croft KE, Tranel D. Harming kin to save strangers: further evidence for abnormally utilitarian moral judgments after ventromedial prefrontal damage. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011;23:2186–2196. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treves A, Rolls ET. Computational analysis of the role of the hippocampus in memory. Hippocampus. 1994;4:374–391. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450040319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tse D, Takeuchi T, Kakeyama M, Kajii Y, Okuno H, Tohyama C, Bito H, Morris RG. Schema-dependent gene activation and memory encoding in neocortex. Science. 2011;333:891–895. doi: 10.1126/science.1205274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tulving E. Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology/Pychologie Canadienne. 1985;26:1–12. [Google Scholar]
- van Kesteren MT, Beul SF, Takashima A, Henson RN, Ruiter DJ, Fernández G. Differential roles for the medial prefrontal and medial temporal cortices in schema-dependent encoding: from congruent to incongruent. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51:2352–2359. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.05.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vazdarjanova A, Guzowski JF. Differences in hippocampal neuronal population responses to modifications of an environmental context: evidence for distinct, yet complementary, functions of CA3 and CA1 ensembles. J Neurosci. 2004;24:6489–6496. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0350-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Viard A, Piolino P, Desgranges B, Chetelat G, Lebreton K, Landeau B, Young A, De La Sayette V, Eustache F. Hippocampal activation for autobiographical memories over the entire lifetime in healthy aged subjects: an fMRI study. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:2453–2467. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watanabe T, Kimura HM, Hirose S, Wada H, Imai Y, Machida T, Shirouzu I, Miyashita Y, Konishi S. Functional dissociation between anterior and posterior temporal cortical regions during retrieval of remote memory. J Neurosci. 2012;32:9659–9670. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5553-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wills TJ, Lever C, Cacucci F, Burgess N, O'Keefe J. Attractor dynamics in the hippocampal representation of the local environment. Science. 2005;308:873–876. doi: 10.1126/science.1108905. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winocur G, Moscovitch M. Memory transformation and systems consolidation. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17:766–780. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711000683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yarkoni T, Poldrack RA, Nichols TE, Van Essen DC, Wager TD. Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nat Methods. 2011;8:665–670. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]