Abstract
This meta-analytic review examines the association between attachment during the early life course and social competence with peers during childhood, and compares the strength of this association with those for externalizing and internalizing symptomatology. Based on eighty independent samples (N = 4,441), the association between security and peer competence was significant (d = 0.39, CI 0.32; 0.47) and not moderated by the age at which peer competence was assessed. Avoidance (d = 0.17, CI 0.05; 0.30), resistance (d = 0.29, CI 0.09; 0.48), and disorganization (d = 0.25, CI 0.10; 0.40) were significantly associated with lower peer competence. Attachment security was significantly more strongly associated with peer competence than internalizing (but not externalizing) symptomatology. Discussion focuses on the significance of early attachment for the development of peer competence versus externalizing and internalizing psychopathology.
Keywords: attachment, social competence, peers, meta-analysis, psychopathology
The introduction and validation of methods for assessing individual differences in the quality of the parent-child attachment relationship (e.g., Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978) sparked a wave of research on the social and emotional sequelae of early (in)security (Berlin, Cassidy, & Appleyard, 2008; DeKlyen & Greenberg, 2008), with no area of development receiving more attention than children’s social competence. Given the vast amount of research that has now accumulated, the present study uses meta-analysis to provide a quantitative review of what is currently known about the role of early attachment security in the development of children’s social competence with peers, updating an earlier effort by Schneider, Atkinson, and Tardif (2001) published over a decade ago. Importantly, the present review is also an extension of a recent series of meta-analyses on the implications of early attachment for children’s mental health, including externalizing behaviors (Fearon, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van IJzendoorn, Lapsley, & Roisman, 2010) and internalizing symptoms (Groh, Roisman, Van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Fearon, 2012); thus, we include here a comparison of the predictive significance of early attachment across these developmental domains.
Central to attachment theory is the claim that early parent-child attachment security plays an important role in promoting positive social and emotional adaptation across the life course (Bowlby, 1969/1982). A number of mechanisms have been proposed to account for such links, with the dominant explanation concerning the internal working model construct (Bowlby, 1973; Bretherton & Munholland, 2008). According to attachment theory, children’s early attachment-relevant experiences with parents become internalized in the form of cognitive-affective representations, or internal working models, which guide thoughts, feelings, and behavior in relation to parents as a potential haven of safety and comfort in times of stress. Over time, such models become generalized and encompass views of the self, others, and the nature of relationships, ultimately contributing to children’s relationships with peers and their mental health. Although such models become resistant to change with development, they are believed to be open to revision given changes in the caregiving environment. In addition to this primary explanatory construct, other potential factors (possibly related to internal working models) have been proposed to mediate links between attachment security and social and emotional competence, including (a) positive social expectations (Dodge & Coie, 1987), (b) continuity in supportive caregiving (Lamb, Thompson, Gardner, Charnov, & Estes, 1984), and (c) social regulation of biological systems mediating effective stress and arousal regulation (Suomi, 2003).
One of the earliest and most influential longitudinal studies of the developmental consequences of mother-child attachment security is the Minnesota Longitudinal Study of Risk and Adaptation (Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson, & Collins, 2005). Findings from this study provided some of the first evidence supporting the predictive significance of early attachment security for children’s social competence with peers (Sroufe, 1983). Drawing on evidence from this study, Sroufe and his colleagues (Sroufe, Egeland, & Kreutzer, 1990) also elaborated on a provocative claim made by attachment theory concerning the implications of early attachment for development across the life course (Bowlby, 1969/1982), arguing that early attachment experiences might be expected to have enduring implications for developmental adaptation.
Importantly, however, the proliferation of research on the developmental consequences of early attachment variation following the development of the Strange Situation Procedure led Sroufe (1988) to caution against the overextension of attachment theory. More specifically, he and others argued that, because it is within the early parent-child attachment relationship that children develop a sense of self-worth and key interaction skills necessary for intimate relationships, variation in the quality of early attachments might be especially relevant for subsequent relationships, including those with peers and particularly with friends, and to a lesser extent emotional and behavioral disorders, including externalizing and internalizing symptomatology (e.g., Belsky & Cassidy, 1995; Sroufe, 1988).
In the current meta-analysis, we evaluate the extant literature in light of these claims regarding the significance of early attachment security for the development of children’s social competence with peers. As we have recently meta-analyzed the literature on attachment and psychopathology, including externalizing (Fearon et al., 2010) and internalizing (Groh et al., 2012) symptomatology, we also address claims regarding the differential predictive significance of attachment variation, by comparing the meta-analytic association between early attachment security and subsequent adaptation across these developmental domains.
In our prior meta-analyses on attachment and externalizing symptoms (Fearon et al., 2010)—including 69 independent samples comprising nearly 6,000 children—and internalizing symptoms (Groh et al., 2012; see also Madigan, Atkinson, Laurin, & Benoit, 2013)—including 42 independent samples comprising over 4,000 children—we provided support for the claim that early attachment insecurity is associated with enhanced risk of externalizing (d = 0.31) and internalizing (d = 0.15) problems. Moreover, the association was significantly larger for externalizing than internalizing symptoms (Groh et al., 2012) and each outcome domain tended to be associated with specific insecure subtypes. Contrary to some theorizing and evidence (Carlson, 1998; Sroufe, 2003), we found that disorganization (d = 0.34) and avoidance (d = 0.12)—but not resistance (d = 0.03, ns)—significantly predicted externalizing symptoms and that avoidance (d = 0.17)—but not resistance (d = 0.03, ns) or disorganization (d = 0.08, ns)—significantly predicted internalizing symptoms. Crucially, we also found meta-analytic support for the claim that early attachment is associated with children’s mental health in enduring ways, in that the age at which the outcome was assessed did not significantly moderate associations between early attachment insecurity and internalizing or externalizing symptoms, indicating that such effects do not wane over the course of childhood. Unlike the effect of insecurity on internalizing symptoms, the effect of insecurity on externalizing symptoms was found to be significantly stronger for boys and in clinical samples.
Taken together, these meta-analyses provide evidence consistent with a modest yet enduring effect of early attachment insecurity on psychopathology (Fearon et al., 2010; Groh et al., 2012). However, in the absence of a comparable meta-analysis, it remains unclear whether there is evidence across the literature that, as hypothesized, early attachment security promotes children’s social competence with peers, whether this association endures over time, whether there are important moderators of this association, and whether early security is more strongly associated with the quality of children’s relationships with friends (vs. non-friends) and peer competence (vs. internalizing and externalizing symptoms).
That said, over a decade ago, Schneider and colleagues (2001) conducted a broad meta-analytic review of the literature on the association between attachment and peer relationship functioning. In 63 studies including over 3,000 individuals, mother-child attachment security (d = 0.41), but not father-child attachment security (k = 5; d = 0.20), was found to be significantly associated with peer relationship functioning. Schneider and colleagues (2001) also found that attachment security was more strongly associated with children’s relationship functioning with friends (d = 0.49) than with non-friends (d = 0.28). Finally, while attachment security was found to be more strongly associated with peer functioning in middle childhood and adolescence (vs. early childhood), type of attachment assessment, environmental risk factors (e.g., socioeconomic status), and child risk factors (e.g., psychological disturbance, child gender) were not found to significantly moderate the association between attachment security and peer functioning.
Schneider et al.’s (2001) meta-analysis made an important contribution to the field in terms of providing meta-analytic support for the predictive significance of attachment for children’s socioemotional development; however, it had a relatively broad focus. Specifically, early observational (e.g., Strange Situation Procedure), middle childhood self-report (e.g., Kerns Security Scale; Kerns, Kelpac, & Cole, 1996), and adolescent representational (e.g., Adult Attachment Interview; George, Kaplan, & Main, 1985) attachment assessments were included in the meta-analysis, which may have contributed to the finding that the effect of attachment security differed by age. Similarly, Schneider and colleagues (2001) included outcomes relevant to internalizing (e.g., social withdrawal) and externalizing (e.g., aggression) symptoms. Although such behavioral problems occur within the milieu of the peer environment, attachment scholars have made specific predictions regarding the significance of attachment security for the development of positive peer functioning, distinct from predictions regarding insecure subtypes for heightening children’s risk for developing specific types of (internalizing and externalizing) psychopathology (see Berlin et al., 2008; DeKlyen & Greenberg, 2008; Sroufe et al., 2005).
Moreover, Schneider et al. (2001) reported the effect of attachment on peer functioning separately by outcome type (aggression, sociability, social withdrawal) and age of attachment assessment. However, because of the broad focus of their meta-analysis, differences for outcome type were confounded by age and vice versa. In short, questions regarding the significance of early attachment security for the development of children’s social competence with peers that lie at the heart of attachment theory and research (Berlin et al., 2008) remain to be tested. Also of note, Schneider et al.’s (2001) meta-analysis focused on the broad comparison of links between secure versus insecure attachment and peer functioning, and it is important to also examine whether insecurity subtypes similarly hinder children’s social competence (Berlin et al., 2008). Given these issues and the fact that a large number of studies (k = 23) have been published since Schneider et al.’s (2001) meta-analysis—including the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (SECCYD), which comprises the largest study of attachment and social competence to date—the field awaits a comprehensive meta-analysis focused specifically on the significance of early attachment for children’s social competence with peers.
For these reasons, we conducted the present meta-analysis of eighty independent samples (N = 4,441) to estimate the association between early attachment (in)security and children’s social competence with peers. Although a number of representational measures of attachment have been developed in recent years, there is a relative lack of validity testing for many of the new assessments because researchers have primarily focused on assessment creation (see Kerns, 2008). In light of this drawback and the centrality of claims regarding the significance of early attachment for children’s social competence (Berlin et al., 2008), we specifically focused on standardized observational assessments of attachment in the current meta-analysis. In line with expectations derived from the literature, we hypothesized that: (1) early attachment security would be significantly associated with peer competence, (2) early attachment insecurity subtypes would be negatively associated with peer competence, and (3) early attachment security would be associated with peer competence in an enduring manner (i.e., the association would not decrease in magnitude as a function of the age at which peer competence was assessed).
Second, we examined a range of factors that might be expected to moderate the association between early attachment quality and peer competence, including: (1) type of measure and rater used to assess attachment, (2) child gender, (3) socioeconomic risk of the cohort (low vs. middle/high), (4) other contextual risk of the cohort (e.g., prenatal exposure to drugs; at-risk vs. not at-risk), (5) type of relationship in which social competence was assessed (friend vs. non-friend), and (6) type of peer competence assessment. We also examined the predictive significance of father-child attachment for peer competence within a smaller set of studies with relevant data, with the hypothesis that—as with mother-child attachment—secure father-child attachment would be significantly associated with peer competence.
Third and finally, we compared the relative magnitude of the meta-analytic association documented here between the quality of attachment and peer competence with estimates we previously established in our prior meta-analyses with respect to attachment variation and externalizing behavior (Fearon et al., 2010) and internalizing symptoms (Groh et al., 2012). We hypothesized that early security would be more strongly associated with peer competence than externalizing and internalizing symptomatology.
Method
Literature Search
A corpus of relevant published articles and dissertations was compiled for this meta-analysis by systematically searching the electronic databases PsycInfo and Web of Science with the keywords social competenc*, peer competenc*, interpersonal function*, social bonding, social function*, peer relation*, sociometric, relation* function*, popular*, social interaction, social reject*, peer reject*, social accept*, peer accept*, sociability, friend*, social aggress*, prosocial, pro-social, antisocial, anti-social, interpersonal interaction, and empathy (asterisks indicate that the search contained the word or word fragment). To further narrow the search, the papers were also required to contain the keyword attachment. This initial search returned 5,363 and 10,161 articles from PsycInfo and Web of Science, respectively. The abstracts of these articles that were written in English or another language understood by the authors (German, Portuguese, French, Turkish, Mandarin Chinese) were reviewed and a large number of clearly irrelevant articles were discarded (e.g., non-empirical papers, studies not involving children), resulting in a total of 284 remaining articles. Each of these articles was carefully examined by the authors according to the criteria described below. Additionally, ten relevant articles were also obtained through searching the reference lists of the obtained empirical articles.
Studies were included in the meta-analysis if they reported on the relation between attachment and social competence with peers. Social competence was defined as social skills (e.g., ability to make friends, interpersonal awareness, empathy, intimacy), peer interaction quality (e.g., initiation into peer group, play behavior, helping behavior), and social status (e.g., popularity, likability). The specific focus of this meta-analysis was on children’s social competence with peers outside of the family context. Accordingly, papers were excluded if they reported on (a) sibling relationships, (b) interactions with adults, (c) constructs pertaining to temperament (e.g., inhibition), (d) constructs pertaining to self development (e.g., self-esteem), (e) broad assessments of affect (e.g., positive emotion), (f) aspects of peer interactions reflecting externalizing (e.g., aggression) or internalizing (e.g., social withdrawal) symptoms, and (g) constructs relevant to social cognition (e.g., social information processing).
Peer competence was assessed using questionnaires completed by parents (e.g., Hubbs-Tait, Osofsky, Hann, & Culp, 1994), teachers/caregivers (e.g., Fagot, 1997) and/or target child (e.g., Bystritsky, 1999); sociometric ratings completed by peers either alone (Veríssimo et al., 2011) or in combination with the target child (e.g., Cassidy, 1988;); and/or observations that occurred in groups (e.g., Waters, Wippman, & Sroufe, 1979) or dyads (Park & Waters, 1989) and that were coded by trained observers. When articles included multiple informants of social competence, data from each informant were combined. Articles were only included if they used observational assessments of attachment security, such as the Strange Situation Procedure (SSP; Ainsworth et al., 1978), Cassidy and Marvin Preschool Attachment system (Cassidy, Marvin, & the MacArthur Working Group on Attachment, 1989), Attachment Q-Sort (AQS; Waters & Deane, 1985), or Main and Cassidy system (Main & Cassidy, 1988). In cases where more than one attachment assessment was used (e.g. SSP followed by second SSP or AQS at a later age) the earliest assessment was selected. For one study, the SSP and AQS were administered at the same time point (Smeekens, Riksen-Walraven, & Van Bakel, 2009). Because the SSP includes information on insecurity subtypes and the AQS does not, the SSP data were extracted. Some studies reported results separately for boys and girls. In these cases, we calculated separate effect sizes for each gender, and the subsamples were treated as independent outcomes in analyses. Ten papers reported on outcome data for father-child attachment security, and were included in a separate meta-analysis on the association between father-child attachment and peer competence.
Several studies presented data on (partly) overlapping samples (e.g., Bureau & Moss, 2010; Moss, Parent, Gosselin, Rousseau, & St-Laurent, 1996). Because participants cannot be included in a meta-analysis more than once, the papers that reported on the earliest outcome assessment were included in our meta-analysis (e.g. Bureau & Moss, 2010), except when the earliest outcome assessment focused on a sub-sample of participants from the larger longitudinal sample (e.g., data from Sroufe, Egeland, & Carlson [1999] were used because they report on the earliest outcome assessment from the full Minnesota longitudinal sample). Several studies reported on data from multiple types of social competence assessments. Data from the earliest outcome were included in the main set of meta-analyses. If more than one peer competence assessment was administered at the earliest time point, these data were combined. We also conducted separate meta-analyses comparing effects by assessment type (see below). For these analyses, data from the earliest outcome for each type of assessment were included. The same procedure was followed for studies that reported on multiple types of relationships (friend, non-friend) and multiple types of observations (group, dyad).
In total, after excluding reports involving overlapping samples, 66 studies were identified that yielded 80 independent samples comprising 4,441 children that could be included in the meta-analyses, with sample sizes ranging from 7 to 516 (see Table 1). Because the AQS does not yield data on the different subtypes of insecurity, studies employing the AQS only appear in the meta-analyses involving the overall contrast between security and insecurity.
Table 1.
Source | Sample Description |
Attachment Measure1 |
Assessment Type2 |
Relationship Type |
Age of Attachment |
Age of Outcome |
N |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Adams, 1994 | Low SES | SSP | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 17 mo. | 93 mo. | 25 |
Adamson, 1990 | AQS (P) | Group Obs | Non-Friend | 54 mo. | 54 mo. | 28 | |
Agnor, 2009 | Low SES; Males | AQS (P) | Soc Skills; Socio | Friend; Non-Friend | 60 mo. | 60 mo. | 16 |
Low SES; Females | AQS (P) | Soc Skills; Socio | Friend; Non-Friend | 60 mo. | 60 mo. | 13 | |
Balentine, 2007 | Mixed At-Risk/Not At-Risk | AQS (P) | Soc Skills; Socio | Non-Friend | 24 mo. | 48 mo. | 165 |
Barglow et al., 1998 | Males | SSP | Dyadic Obs | Non-Friend | 12 mo. | 82 mo. | 47 |
Females | SSP | Dyadic Obs | Non-Friend | 12 mo. | 82 mo. | 45 | |
Bohlin et al., 2000 | SSP | Group Obs | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 96 mo. | 87 | |
SSP | Soc Skills; Socio | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 102 mo. | 87 | ||
Booth et al., 1998 | Males | MSSP | Soc Skills; Blend | Friend; Non-Friend | 52 mo. | 96 mo. | 34 |
Females | MSSP | Soc Skills; Blend | Friend; Non-Friend | 52 mo. | 96 mo. | 24 | |
Bost et al., 1998 | Low SES | AQS (Obs) | Blend | Non-Friend | 42 mo. | 42 mo. | 69 |
Bureau & Moss, 2010 | MSSP | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 76 mo. | 76 mo. | 104 | |
Burmenskaya, 2009 | MSSP | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 72 mo. | 72 mo. | 42 | |
Burns, 2002 | AQS (P) | Soc Skills; Group Obs | Non-Friend | 48 mo. | 48 mo. | 44 | |
Carter et al., 1999 | SSP | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 12 mo. | 12 mo. | 90 | |
Cassibba et al., 2000 | AQS (Obs) | Group Obs | Non-Friend | 26 mo. | 26 mo. | 50 | |
Cassidy, 1988 | MSSP | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 62 mo. | 62 mo. | 52 | |
Cassidy et al., 1996 | MSSP | Socio | Friend | 66 mo. | 66 mo. | 33 | |
Clarke-Stewart, 1981 | MSSP | Dyadic Obs | Friend; Non-Friend | 30 mo. | 30 mo. | 62 | |
Denham, DeMulder, & Mitchell-Copeland Sample | |||||||
DeMulder et al., 2000 | Males | AQS (Obs) | Soc Skills; Socio | Non-Friend | 47 mo. | 47 mo. | 51 |
Females | AQS (Obs) | Soc Skills; Socio | Non-Friend | 47 mo. | 47 mo. | 43 | |
Mitchell-Copeland et al., 1997 | AQS (Obs) | Group Obs | Non-Friend | 45 mo. | 45 mo. | 62 | |
Fagot, 1997 | SSP | Soc Skills; Group Obs | Non-Friend | 18 mo. | 21 mo. | 156 | |
Freitag et al., 1996 | Mother-Child Attachment | SSP | Soc Skills | Friend | 12 mo. | 120 mo. | 40 |
Father-Child Attachment | SSP | Soc Skills | Friend | 18 mo. | 120 mo. | 33 | |
Howes Sample | |||||||
Bystritsky, 1999 | SSP | Social Skills | Friend | 12 mo. | 168 mo | 62 | |
Howes et al., 1990 | SSP | Group Obs | Non-Friend | 12 mo. | 22 mo. | 42 | |
Howes et al., 1990 | AQS (Obs) | Group Obs | Non-Friend | 19 mo. | 19 mo. | 60 | |
Hubbs-Tait et al., 1994 | Low SES | SSP | Soc Skills | Friend; Non-Friend | 13 mo. | 54 mo. | 42; 41 |
Jacobson et al., 1986 | SSP | Dyadic Obs | Non-Friend | 18 mo. | 24 mo. | 24 | |
Kaloustian, 2008 | AQS (Obs) | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 66 mo. | 92 mo. | 33 | |
Kerns & Barth, 1995 | Mother-Child Attachment | ||||||
Males | AQS (P) | Soc Skills; Socio | Non-Friend | 44 mo. | 44 mo. | 18 | |
Females | AQS (P) | Soc Skills; Socio | Non-Friend | 44 mo. | 44 mo. | 18 | |
Father-Child Attachment | |||||||
Males | AQS (P) | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 44 mo. | 44 mo. | 18 | |
Females | AQS (P) | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 44 mo. | 44 mo. | 18 | |
Korntheuer et al., 2010 | MSSP | Blend | Non-Friend | 24 mo. | 24 mo. | 85 | |
Kremmel, 2008 | Low SES | AQS (P) | Socio | Non-Friend | 54 mo. | 54 mo. | 91 |
LaFrenière et al., 1992 | Mother-Child Attachment | ||||||
Males | AQS (P) | Soc Skills; Group Obs | Non-Friend | 44 mo. | 44 mo. | 41 | |
Females | AQS (P) | Soc Skills; Group Obs | Non-Friend | 45 mo. | 45 mo. | 42 | |
Father-Child Attachment | |||||||
Males | AQS (P) | Soc Skills; Group Obs | Non-Friend | 44 mo. | 44 mo. | 41 | |
Females | AQS (P) | Soc Skills; Group Obs | Non-Friend | 45 mo. | 45 mo. | 42 | |
Laible, 2006 | AQS (P) | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 53 mo. | 53 mo. | 51 | |
Lamarca, 1995 | AQS (P) | Socio; Group Obs | Non-Friend | 55 mo. | 55 mo. | 81 | |
Lewis & Feiring, 1989 | MSSP | Soc Skills | Friend | 12 mo. | 108 mo. | 125 | |
Lieberman, 1977 | MSSP | Dyadic Obs | Non-Friend | 36 mo. | 36 mo. | 40 | |
Malatesta-Magai et al., 1994 | SSP | Dyadic Obs | Non-Friend | 22 mo. | 34 mo. | 42 | |
Mallik, 2000 | Low SES; At-Risk | SSP | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 36 mo. | 50 |
McElwain & Volling, 2004 | Mother-Child Attachment | SSP | Dyadic Obs | Friend | 12 mo. | 51 mo. | 30 |
Father-Child Attachment | SSP | Dyadic Obs | Friend | 12 mo. | 51 mo. | 30 | |
Milligan, 2004 | AQS (P) | Dyadic Obs | Friend | 66 mo. | 66 mo. | 30 | |
Minnesota Sample | |||||||
LaFreniére, 1982 | Low SES; Effect for C | SSP | Group Obs | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 50 mo. | 36 |
Pancake, 1988 | Low SES; Effect for A | SSP | Dyadic Obs | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 53 mo. | 12 |
Pastor, 1980 | Low SES; Effect for B/A/C | SSP | Dyadic Obs | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 22 mo. | 37 |
Shulman et al., 1994 | Low SES; Effect for A/C | SSP | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 128 mo. | 32 |
Low SES; Effect for B | SSP | Socio; Dyadic Obs | Friend | 15 mo. | 128 mo. | 32 | |
Sroufe, 1983 | Low SES; Effect for B | SSP | Socio; Group Obs | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 50 mo. | 39 |
Sroufe et al., 1999 | Low SES; Effect for B | SSP | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 48 mo. | 84 |
Nakano, 1984–1985 | Females | SSP | Dyadic Obs | Non-Friend | 18 mo. | 23 mo. | 7 |
Neyer et al., 1998 | MSSP | Blend | Non-Friend | 46 mo. | 46 mo. | 52 | |
NICHD SECCYD Sample | |||||||
Low SES | |||||||
Males | SSP | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 24 mo. | 60 | |
SSP | Group Obs | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 24 mo. | 18 | ||
SSP | Dyadic Obs | Friend | 15 mo. | 36 mo. | 19 | ||
SSP | Socio | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 54 mo. | 32 | ||
Females | SSP | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 24 mo. | 58 | |
SSP | Group Obs | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 24 mo. | 21 | ||
SSP | Dyadic Obs | Friend | 15 mo. | 36 mo. | 18 | ||
SSP | Socio | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 54 mo. | 28 | ||
High/Middle SES | |||||||
Males | SSP | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 24 mo. | 5163 | |
SSP | Group Obs | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 24 mo. | 3033 | ||
SSP | Dyadic Obs | Friend | 15 mo. | 36 mo. | 2783 | ||
SSP | Socio | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 54 mo. | 3253 | ||
Females | SSP | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 24 mo. | 5053 | |
SSP | Group Obs | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 24 mo. | 3073 | ||
SSP | Dyadic Obs | Friend | 15 mo. | 36 mo. | 2843 | ||
SSP | Socio | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 54 mo. | 3613 | ||
Panfile & Laible, 2012 | AQS (P) | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 36 mo. | 36 mo. | 63 | |
Park, 1992 | AQS (P) | Soc Skills | Friend | 54 mo. | 54 mo. | 41 | |
Park & Waters, 1989 | AQS (P) | Dyadic Obs | Friend | 45 mo. | 45 mo. | 33 | |
Pitterle, 2003 | AQS (P) | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 48 mo. | 48 mo. | 60 | |
Purdue Sample | |||||||
Porter, 2009 | AQS (P) | Group Obs | Non-Friend | 48 mo. | 48 mo. | 54 | |
Walls, 1997 | AQS (P) | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 16 mo. | 51 mo. | 33 | |
Regan, 1996 | SSP | Soc Skills | Friend | 16 mo. | 102 mo. | 51 | |
Ritchie, 1995 | Low SES; At-Risk | AQS (Obs) | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 58 mo. | 58 mo. | 23 |
Silverman, 1990 | Males | AQS (Obs) | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 41 mo. | 41 mo. | 18 |
Females | AQS (Obs) | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 41 mo. | 41 mo. | 18 | |
Smeekens et al., 2009 | MSSP | Blend | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 64 mo. | 108 | |
Stams et al., 2002 | At-Risk; Effect for B | SSP | Blend | Non-Friend | 12 mo. | 84 mo. | 145 |
At-Risk; Effect for D | SSP | Blend | Non-Friend | 12 mo. | 84 mo. | 143 | |
Suess et al., 1992 | Mother-Child Attachment | SSP | Group Obs | Non-Friend | 12 mo. | 60 mo. | 35 |
Father-Child Attachment | SSP | Group Obs | Non-Friend | 18 mo. | 60 mo. | 33 | |
Sull, 1995 | AQS (P) | Group Obs | Non-Friend | 56 mo. | 56 mo. | 89 | |
Szewczyk-Sokolowski et al., 2005 | AQS (Obs) | Socio | Non-Friend | 54 mo. | 54 mo. | 98 | |
Thompson, 2000 | Not At-Risk | MSSP | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 54 mo. | 54 mo. | 44 |
At-Risk | MSSP | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 54 mo. | 54 mo. | 37 | |
Turner, 1991 | Males | MSSP | Group Obs | Non-Friend | 54 mo. | 54 mo. | 18 |
Females | MSSP | Group Obs | Non-Friend | 54 mo. | 54 mo. | 22 | |
U. of Texas at Austin Sample | |||||||
Cutler, 1995 | Father-Child Attachment | AQS (P) | Dyadic Obs | Friend | 45 mo. | 45 mo. | 35 |
Goldetsky, 1999 | Mother-Child Attachment | SSP | Dyadic Obs | Friend | 18 mo. | 26 mo. | 28 |
U. of Virginia Sample | |||||||
Cohn, 1990 | Males; Effect for B | MSSP | Soc Skills; Socio | Non-Friend | 74 mo. | 74 mo. | 34 |
Females; Effect for B | MSSP | Soc Skills; Socio | Non-Friend | 74 mo. | 74 mo. | 46 | |
Cohn, 1988 | Males; Effect for D | MSSP | Socio | Non-Friend | 74 mo. | 74 mo. | 42 |
Females; Effect for D | MSSP | Socio | Non-Friend | 74 mo. | 74 mo. | 47 | |
Van IJzendoorn et al., 1992 | SSP | Soc Skills; Group Obs | Non-Friend | 12 mo. | 55 mo. | 56 | |
Vandell et al., 1988 | SSP | Dyadic Obs | Non-Friend | 12 mo. | 15 mo. | 28 | |
Veríssimo et al., 2011 | Mother-Child Attachment | AQS (Obs) | Socio | Friend | 32 mo. | 54 mo. | 35 |
Father-Child Attachment | AQS (Obs) | Socio | Friend | 32 mo. | 54 mo. | 35 | |
Vondra et al., 1999 | Low SES | MSSP | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 18 mo. | 85 mo. | 88 |
Waters et al., 1979 | MSSP | Group Obs | Non-Friend | 15 mo. | 42 mo. | 32 | |
Weatherill, 2007 | Low SES; Not At-Risk | SSP | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 12 mo. | 48 mo. | 29 |
Low SES; At-Risk | SSP | Soc Skills | Non-Friend | 12 mo. | 48 mo. | 56 | |
Wood et al., 2004 | AQS (P) | Socio | Non-Friend | 42 mo. | 57 mo. | 37 | |
Wu, 1992 | AQS (P) | Soc Skills; Socio | Non-Friend | 58 mo. | 58 mo. | 78 | |
Youngblade & Belsky, 1992 | Mother-Child Attachment | SSP | Dyadic Obs | Friend | 12 mo. | 60 mo. | 73 |
Father-Child Attachment | SSP | Dyadic Obs | Friend | 13 mo. | 60 mo. | 66 |
AQS = Waters and Deane (1985) Attachment Q-Set (Obs indicates completed by observer, P indicates completed by parent); MSSP = Modified SSP by reducing number of separations, lengthening duration of separation, combining SSP with Cassidy and Marvin/MacArthur Preschool Attachment Coding System; SSP = Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, and Wall (1978) Strange Situation procedure
Dyadic Obs = Directly observed social competence in dyads; Group Obs = Directly observed social competence in groups; Soc Skills = Reported Social Skills; Socio = Sociometrics; Blend = Blend of reported social skills, sociometrics, and/or direct observation of social competence
Values reflect sample size before winsorizing; winsorized N for High/Middle SES males = 185; winsorized N for High/Middle SES females = 175.
Coding System
A coding system for describing the characteristics of the sample and study design was developed based on the system presented in the meta-analyses on attachment and externalizing and internalizing symptomatology (Fearon et al., 2010; Groh et al., 2012). Attachment was coded based on the observational measure used and all of the studies included one of several well-known attachment assessments (SSP, AQS, Preschool Attachment Assessment, Cassidy & Marvin, Main & Cassidy). For each type of attachment measure, the coder extracted data at the level of the individual attachment classification when possible (i.e., A, B, C and D, note: for studies in which disorganization was assessed, children who received the primary classification of disorganized were always considered in the disorganized category). For the AQS, the informant who completed the sort (observer, mother, father) was coded. In some cases, either the mean, standard deviation, or number of children in attachment categories was not reported. To obtain such crucial statistical information, authors were contacted for five studies. In three cases, the authors were able to provide the relevant information. In the remaining two cases, the authors were unable to provide the relevant information, but other data relevant to the meta-analysis were available in the published literature (e.g., authors no longer had access to sociometric data [Howes, 1991], but complete observational data were available in the literature [Howes, Rodning, Galluzzo, & Myers, 1990]). As in Fearon et al. (2010) and Groh et al., (2012), we analyzed (publicly available) raw data pertinent to the aims of this meta-analysis from the NICHD SECCYD to examine associations between attachment and peer competence within sub-samples (e.g., low vs. higher SES groups).
Several important potential moderators related to the sample were coded: socioeconomic status (high or middle vs. low), risk status (at-risk vs. not at-risk), child gender, type of relationship in which peer competence was assessed (friend vs. non-friend), and type of peer competence measure (e.g., reported social skills, sociometrics vs. observation [group vs. dyad]). When socio-economic status was not noted, a default of high/middle class was recorded. Risk status was recorded if the authors indicated the sample experienced psychosocial (target child met clinical cut-off for externalizing symptoms) or contextual (e.g., prenatal exposure to drugs) risk. As in Schneider et al. (2001), studies were coded as pertaining to friendship if the authors indicated the outcome was children’s social competence with peers nominated by either the mother or the target child as friends. Age at attachment and peer competence assessment were also coded. To assess inter-rater reliability, twenty (30%) randomly selected studies were coded by two coders. The agreement between the coders across the moderator variables was 98%.
Meta-Analytic Procedures
Analogous to our previous meta-analyses (Fearon et al., 2010; Groh et al., 2012) we conducted four separate meta-analyses on the social competence construct, one for the relation between attachment security and social competence, one for the relation between avoidance and social competence, one for the relation between resistance and social competence, and one for the relation between disorganization and social competence. In these main analyses we compared the social competence of children in each attachment classification with all other classifications combined (e.g., insecure-avoidant vs. not-avoidant [B, C, D]). In a separate set of analyses on a smaller set of studies with pertinent data, we also compared each insecure classification with the secure classification (e.g., insecure-avoidant vs. secure) and with each of the other insecure classifications (e.g., insecure-avoidant vs. insecure-resistant).
A set of moderation analyses were conducted to determine whether socioeconomic status, risk status, type of attachment assessment, or child gender moderated the effect of attachment on social competence. In addition, we examined whether type of relationship or type of peer competence assessment moderated the effect of attachment on peer competence. These meta-analyses focused on (partially) overlapping groups of participants, and the 85% confidence intervals were reported to allow for exploratory comparisons (see below).
The meta-analyses were performed using the Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (CMA) program (Borenstein, Rothstein, & Cohen, 2005, Version 2). For each study, an effect size (d) was calculated as the standardized difference between the two pertinent groups (e.g., secure vs. insecure). In cases where continuous attachment scores were correlated with social competence (e.g., studies reporting on the AQS) we re-computed the statistic into Cohen’s d (see Mullen, 1989, and Mullen and Rosenthal, 1985, chapter 6, for the formulae for transformation of various statistics into Cohen’s d). Effect sizes indicating a positive relation between social competence and security (higher levels of competence in the secure group compared to the reference group) were given a positive sign. Effect sizes indicating a negative relation between social competence and avoidance, resistance, and disorganization, respectively (lower levels of competence in the resistant group compared to the reference group), were also given a positive sign. Thus, a positive combined effect for the set of studies comparing resistant children with non-resistant children on social competence would mean that across these studies the level of social competence in resistant children was lower than in other children. Using CMA, combined effect sizes were computed. Significance tests and moderator analyses were performed through random effects models, as this approach is considered to be most widely applicable and conservative (Borenstein et al., 2005). Random effects models allow for the possibility that there are random differences between studies that are associated with variations in procedures, measures, settings, that go beyond subject-level sampling error, and thus point to different study populations (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). To test the homogeneity of the overall and specific sets of effect sizes, we computed Q-statistics (Borenstein et al., 2005). In addition, we computed 95% confidence intervals (CIs) around the point estimate of each set of effect sizes. Q-statistics and p-values were also computed to assess differences between combined effect sizes for specific subsets of studies grouped by moderators. Again, the more conservative random effects model tests were used. Contrasts were only tested when at least two of the subsets consisted of at least four studies.
When the children in two sets of studies (partially) overlapped (e.g., some studies reported on multiple types of social competence assessments, and we wanted to compare the combined effects for these sets), it was impossible to directly compare effect sizes across these sets. We computed 85% confidence intervals for the point estimates of the combined effect sizes in the two sets; non-overlapping 85% CIs indicate a significant difference between combined effect sizes. This approach of comparing 85% CIs served as a conservative significance test (Goldstein & Healy, 1995; Van IJzendoorn, Juffer, & Klein Poelhuis, 2005).
We used the “trim and fill” method (Duval & Tweedie, 2000a, 2000b) to calculate the effect of potential data censoring or publication bias on the outcome of the meta-analyses. Using this method, a funnel plot is constructed of each study’s effect size against the sample size or the standard error (usually plotted as 1/SE, or precision). It is expected that this plot has the shape of a funnel, because studies with smaller sample sizes and larger standard errors have increasingly large variation in estimates of their effect size as random variation becomes increasingly influential, whereas studies with larger sample sizes have smaller variation in effect sizes (Duval & Tweedie, 2000b; Sutton, Duval, Tweedie, Abrams, & Jones, 2000). The plots would be expected to be shaped like a funnel if no data censoring is present. However, since smaller non-significant results are less likely to be published (‘file-drawer’ problem, Mullen, 1989), studies in the bottom left hand corner of the plot are often omitted (Sutton et al., 2000). In our meta-analyses, the k right-most studies considered to be symmetrically unmatched were trimmed. The trimmed studies can then be replaced and their missing counterparts imputed or “filled” as mirror images of the trimmed outcomes. This allows for the computation of an adjusted overall effect size and confidence interval (Gilbody, Song, Eastwood, & Sutton, 2000; Sutton et al., 2000).
For each study, Fisher’s Z scores were computed as well-distributed equivalents for the effect size d, and the Z scores were standardized to test for outliers. For the main analyses, no outliers (standardized Z-values outside of +/−3.29; Tabachnik & Fidell, 2001) were found. We also tested for outlying sample sizes. The NICHD SECCYD high SES boy and girl samples had sample sizes that were three SDs above the mean, thus, we winsorized these sample size values. To maintain the ranking of studies by sample size, we added ten to the third largest sample size in the corpus (N = 165; Balentine, 2007) and used that value for the sample size of the second largest sample (high SES girls, N = 175) and added ten to that value for the sample size of the largest sample (high SES boys, N = 185; see Table 1). The pattern and significance of results were nearly identical when the winsorized and non-winsorized values were used in analyses.
Results
Is secure attachment associated with greater social competence?
The first set of meta-analyses concerned the difference in social competence between children rated as secure versus insecure. The insecure group comprised avoidant, resistant, and (if assessed) disorganized children. In eighty independent samples including N = 4,441 children, the association between attachment security and social competence was reported. Any study assessing attachment and social competence was included in this total set, regardless of the type of measures used. In this overall set we found a significant combined effect size of d = 0.39 (see Table 2). Children rated as secure showed higher levels of social competence with peers than children rated as insecure. The trim-and-fill approach showed that eight studies had to be trimmed and filled, and the resulting combined effect size was reduced slightly to d = 0.36, (95% CI 0.30; 0.42). Excluding the two largest independent samples (NICHD SECCYD males high/middle SES, NICHD SECCYD females high/middle SES) had little effect on the combined effect size (d = 0.41, CI 0.33; 0.48). Similarly, when each sample was systematically removed and the effect was re-computed for the k-1 studies, the effect size remained nearly unchanged (ds ranged from 0.38 – 0.40). We computed Orwin’s fail-safe N (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009) using a Fisher Z of .01 as criterion for a ‘trivial’ effect size, and found k = 1,460 studies with null effects needed to bring the observed effect size down to this trivial level.
Table 2.
k | N | d | Confidence Interval 95% |
Homogeneity Q |
Contrast Q1 | Contrast p |
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Secure v. Insecure (A, C, D)2 | 80 | 4,441 | 0.39** | 0.32 – 0.47 | 104.84* | ||
At-risk | 0.58 | .75 | |||||
Not at-risk | 74 | 3,965 | 0.38** | 0.30 – 0.46 | 96.69* | ||
At-risk child | 5 | 311 | 0.47** | 0.18 – 0.77 | 6.90 | ||
Mixed | 1 | 165 | 0.50* | 0.04 – 0.96 | NA | ||
Gender | 2.36 | .31 | |||||
Boys | 11 | 522 | 0.37** | 0.15 – 0.59 | 10.62 | ||
Girls | 12 | 511 | 0.23* | 0.01 – 0.45 | 8.19 | ||
Mixed | 57 | 3,408 | 0.42** | 0.33 – 0.50 | 81.99** | ||
SES | 2.40 | .12 | |||||
Middle/high | 66 | 3,738 | 0.42** | 0.33 – 0.50 | 89.30* | ||
Low | 14 | 703 | 0.26** | 0.08 – 0.44 | 13.58 | ||
Attachment measure | 7.35* | ||||||
SSP | 28 | 1,789 | 0.27** | 0.16 – 0.39 | 24.21 | ||
MSSP | 20 | 1,082 | 0.41** | 0.26 – 0.55 | 36.76** | ||
AQS | 32 | 1,570 | 0.50** | 0.38 – 0.62 | 33.64 | 0.55 | .46 |
Observer-report | 11 | 498 | 0.56** | 0.36 – 0.77 | 11.45 | ||
Outcome by mother | 1 | 33 | 0.58 | −0.24 – 1.40 | NA | ||
Outcome by non-mother | 10 | 465 | 0.57 | 0.34 – 0.80 | 11.45 | ||
Mother-report | 21 | 1,072 | 0.47** | 0.33 – 0.61 | 22.66 | 5.48* | |
Outcome by mother | 4 | 215 | 0.79** | 0.50 – 1.09 | 2.27 | ||
Outcome by non-mother | 17 | 857 | 0.40** | 0.26 – 0.54 | 13.91 |
p < .05
p < .01
subgroups with k < 4 excluded from contrast;
A=Avoidant, C=Resistant, D=Disorganized
We looked for significant moderators that might account for between-study variability in the outcome (see Table 2). Age at which social competence was assessed did not yield a significant regression weight (slope = 0.00; p = .31), indicating that the association between attachment security and social competence did not become stronger or weaker with age. Similarly, time between attachment and social competence assessments did not yield a significant regression weight (slope = 0.00, p = .22), suggesting that the association between attachment security and social competence is similar regardless of whether social competence is assessed contemporaneously or longitudinally.
Next, we examined whether the association between attachment security and social competence was moderated by type of attachment assessment. We conducted these analyses for all studies, as well as with studies that used the mother-reported AQS removed from analyses. The effect size in samples employing the AQS was larger than that of studies employing the Strange Situation (all studies: Q(1) = 10.23, p < .01; mother-reported AQS removed: Q(1) = 7.16, p < .01). The effect size of studies employing the Modified Strange Situation procedure did not significantly differ from those employing the Strange Situation (Q[1] = 2.07, p = .15) or the AQS (Q[1] = 0.48, p = .49). Because the different attachment measures are typically conducted at different ages, we also conducted a meta-regression analysis with assessment age as a predictor. As expected, the regression was significant (all studies: slope = .01, p < .01; mother-reported AQS removed: slope = .01, p < .01). However, when both age of attachment assessment and type of attachment assessment (i.e., SSP v. AQS; SSP v. MSSP) were added to the meta-regression, all effects dropped to non-significance (all studies [age: slope = .00, p = .29; SSP v. AQS: slope = .15, p = .23; SSP v. MSSP: slope = .06, p = .60]; mother-reported AQS removed [age: slope = .00, p = .11; SSP v. AQS: slope = .15, p = .33; SSP v. MSSP: slope = .01, p = .94]).
Among studies that used the AQS, the effect size for samples in which observers completed the AQS did not significantly differ from samples in which mothers completed the AQS (Q[1] = 0.55, p = .46). However, within the sub-set of samples that used the mother-reported AQS, the effect size for samples in which social competence was also assessed using mother reports was significantly larger than the effect size for samples in which social competence was not assessed using mother reports (Q[1] = 5.48, p < .05).
In studies that reported effects separately for boys (k = 11) and girls (k = 12), the effect size for boys was not significantly larger than for girls, Q(1) = 0.97, p = .33. A regression analysis including studies that did not report effects separately for boys and girls did not show a significant regression weight for the percentage of boys in the sample (slope = −0.01, p = .15). Socio-economic status and risk status were not significant moderators of the effect.
Ten papers also reported on outcome data for father-child attachment security. The combined effect size for these studies was d = 0.14 (k = 10, N = 351, 95% CI −0.08; 0.36) in a homogeneous set of outcomes (Q[9] = 7.27, p = .61). The 85% CIs for the effect of father-child (−0.02; 0.30) and mother-child (0.34; 0.45) attachment did not overlap, indicating that mother-child security was more strongly associated with peer competence than father-child security.
Are avoidant, resistant, and disorganized attachments associated with lower levels of social competence?
To examine whether specific patterns of attachment insecurity are associated with lower levels of social competence, we first conducted meta-analyses comparing the social competence of children with each insecure classification to all other children (e.g., avoidant v. not-avoidant [B, C, D]). Next, in a smaller subset of studies with relevant data, we conducted meta-analyses comparing the social competence of children with each insecure classification to secure children (e.g., A v. B) and to children with each of the other insecure classifications (e.g., A v. C).
Insecure Subtype v. All Other Attachment Patterns Combined
In twelve studies involving N = 1,062 children and their mothers, the insecure-avoidant attachment classification was differentiated from the other classifications, and in these studies the combined effect size was significant, d = 0.17 (see Table 3). The trim-and-fill approach indicated that no studies needed to be trimmed and filled, suggesting that asymmetrically unmatched studies were unlikely and providing little evidence for asymmetrical publication bias. Moderator effects for gender, SES, risk status and attachment assessment could not be tested because sub-groups included fewer than four samples.
Table 3.
k | N | D | Confidence Interval 95% |
Homogeneity Q |
Contrast Q1 |
Contrast p |
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Insecure Subtype v. All Other Attachment Patterns Combined | |||||||
Avoidant v. Not-Avoidant (B, C, D)2 | 12 | 1,062 | 0.17** | 0.05 – 0.30 | 7.48 | ||
Gender | |||||||
Boys | 2 | 245 | 0.20 | −0.06 – 0.45 | 1.55 | ||
Girls | 2 | 233 | 0.09 | −0.17 – 0.35 | 0.56 | ||
Mixed | 8 | 584 | 0.20* | 0.02 – 0.37 | 4.92 | ||
SES | |||||||
Middle/high | 9 | 907 | 0.17** | 0.04 – 0.31 | 5.62 | ||
Low | 3 | 155 | 0.17 | −0.16 – 0.50 | 1.87 | ||
Attachment measure | |||||||
SSP | 11 | 958 | 0.18** | 0.05 – 0.31 | 7.44 | ||
MSSP | 1 | 104 | 0.13 | −0.26 – 0.53 | NA | ||
Resistant v. Not-Resistant (A, B, D)2 | 12 | 1,034 | 0.29** | 0.09 – 0.48 | 22.12* | ||
Gender | |||||||
Boys | 2 | 245 | 0.22 | −0.29 – 0.73 | 2.80 | ||
Girls | 3 | 240 | 0.46 | −0.04 – 0.97 | 10.16** | ||
Mixed | 7 | 549 | 0.27 | −0.03 – 0.57 | 8.39 | ||
SES | |||||||
Middle/high | 9 | 879 | 0.13 | 0.00 – 0.27 | 7.49 | ||
Low | 3 | 155 | 0.81** | 0.46 – 1.16 | 2.24 | ||
Attachment measure | |||||||
SSP | 11 | 930 | 0.29** | 0.07 – 0.51 | 21.79* | ||
MSSP | 1 | 104 | 0.33 | −0.31 – 0.97 | NA | ||
Disorganized v. Not-Disorganized (A, B, C)2 | 12 | 1,103 | 0.25** | 0.10 – 0.40 | 15.39 | ||
At-risk | |||||||
Not at-risk | 10 | 910 | 0.23** | 0.06 – 0.40 | 14.71 | ||
At-risk child | 2 | 193 | 0.33 | −0.05 – 0.70 | 0.12 | ||
Gender | |||||||
Boys | 3 | 287 | 0.12 | −0.18 – 0.42 | 8.08* | ||
Girls | 3 | 280 | 0.16 | −0.15 – 0.46 | 1.81 | ||
Mixed | 6 | 536 | 0.35** | 0.14 – 0.56 | 2.05 | ||
SES | 3.27 | .07 | |||||
Middle/high | 8 | 894 | 0.31** | 0.15 – 0.46 | 9.09 | ||
Low | 4 | 209 | 0.00 | −0.30 – 0.30 | 2.91 | ||
Attachment measure | 3.99* | ||||||
SSP | 8 | 802 | 0.16* | 0.01 – 0.31 | 8.75 | ||
MSSP | 4 | 301 | 0.46** | 0.21 – 0.72 | 2.33 | ||
Insecure Subtype v. Secure | |||||||
Avoidant v. Secure | 12 | 914 | 0.27 | 0.11 – 0.43 | 13.63 | ||
Resistant v. Secure | 12 | 847 | 0.41 | 0.16 – 0.66 | 26.85** | ||
Disorganized v. Secure | 9 | 694 | 0.26 | 0.07 – 0.44 | 10.11 | ||
Insecure Subtype v. Other Insecure Subtype | |||||||
Avoidant v. Resistant | 11 | 457 | 0.00 | –0.19 – 0.19 | NA | ||
Avoidant v. Disorganized | 6 | 409 | −0.05 | −0.25 – 0.15 | NA | ||
Resistant v. Disorganized | 6 | 351 | −0.02 | −0.14 – 0.09 | NA |
p < .05
p < .01
subgroups with k < 4 excluded from contrast;
A=Avoidant, B=Secure, C=Resistant, D=Disorganized
In twelve studies involving N = 1,034 children and their mothers, the insecure-resistant attachment classification was differentiated from the other classifications. In this set of studies the combined effect size was significant, d = 0.29 (see Table 3). The trim-and-fill approach indicated that no studies needed to be trimmed and filled, suggesting that asymmetrically unmatched studies were unlikely. Moderator effects for gender, SES, risk status and attachment assessment could not be tested because sub-groups included fewer than four samples.
In twelve studies including N = 1,103 participants, a combined effect size of d = 0.25 was found for the association between disorganization and lower levels of social competence (see Table 3). The trim-and-fill approach showed no symmetrically unmatched studies. The effect size in samples employing the modified Strange Situation procedure was larger than that of studies employing the Strange Situation procedure, Q(1) = 3.99, p < .05. SES did not significantly moderate the effect of disorganization on social competence. Moderator effects for risk status and gender could not be tested because sub-groups included fewer than four samples.
Insecure Subtype v. Secure Attachment
We also compared the secure classification with each of the insecure classifications. Similar to the results from comparisons of each insecure classification to all other classifications, the combined effect size for the association between security versus avoidance and social competence was d = 0.27. For secure versus resistant attachment it was d = 0.41, and for secure versus disorganized attachment it was d = 0.26 (see Table 3). Moderator effects could not be tested for any of these comparisons because sub-groups included fewer than four samples.
Insecure Subtype v. Other Insecure Subtype
Finally, we compared each insecure classification with each of the other insecure classifications. Results indicated that there were no significant differences between insecurity subtypes on social competence (see Table 3), suggesting that avoidance, resistance, and disorganization are similarly associated with lower levels of social competence. Moderator effects could not be tested for any of these comparisons because sub-groups included fewer than four samples.
Type of Relationship
Studies differed in the type of relationship (i.e., friend, non-friend) in which children’s social competence was assessed and some studies reported on children’s social competence within multiple types of relationships. We investigated whether type of relationship moderated the effect of security, avoidance, resistance, or disorganization on social competence. Since it was impossible to directly compare effect sizes across these sets (sometimes including the same children), we computed 85% confidence intervals for the point estimates of the combined effect sizes as a conservative significance test (see Method). As seen in Table 4, relationship type was found to significantly moderate the effect of secure (vs. insecure) attachment and resistant (vs. not-resistant) attachment on social competence. The effect of attachment security and resistance in studies that focused on children’s social competence with friends was smaller than the effect for studies that focused on children’s social competence with non-friends (secure: Q[1] = 9.46, p < .01; resistant: Q[1] = 5.78, p < .05). Type of relationship was not found to significantly moderate the effect of avoidance or disorganization on lower levels of social competence.
Table 4.
k | N | d | Confidence Interval 85% | Homogeneity Q | Contrast Q1 | Contrast p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Secure v. Insecure (A, C, D)2 | |||||||
Relationship | 9.46** | ||||||
Friend | 23 | 1,201 | 0.15* | 0.04 – 0.26 | 28.45 | ||
Not Friend | 68 | 3,888 | 0.41** | 0.35 – 0.47 | 91.19* | ||
Assessment Type | 1.42 | .70 | |||||
Blend | 7 | 517 | 0.40** | 0.22 – 0.58 | 12.09 | ||
Reported Social Skills | 47 | 2,742 | 0.37** | 0.29 – 0.45 | 68.40* | ||
Sociometrics | 22 | 1,403 | 0.39** | 0.28 – 0.50 | 27.71 | ||
Observations | 43 | 2,454 | 0.30** | 0.22 – 0.38 | 66.14** | 6.99* | |
Group | 23 | 1,437 | 0.31** | 0.21 – 0.41 | 34.23* | ||
Dyadic (Friend) | 11 | 685 | 0.10 | −0.04 – 0.24 | 9.99 | ||
Dyadic (Non-Friend) | 9 | 332 | 0.54** | 0.34 – 0.73 | 12.91 | ||
Avoidant v. Not-Avoidant (B, C, D)2 | |||||||
Relationship | 0.15 | .70 | |||||
Friend | 6 | 460 | 0.12 | −0.04 – 0.28 | 10.47 | ||
Not Friend | 11 | 1,011 | 0.17* | 0.06 – 0.28 | 7.44 | ||
Assessment Type | 0.17 | .92 | |||||
Reported Social Skills | 10 | 998 | 0.21** | 0.10 – 0.33 | 12.84 | ||
Sociometrics | 5 | 507 | 0.19 | 0.03 – 0.36 | 0.87 | ||
Observations | 14 | 1,147 | 0.17* | 0.06 – 0.28 | 20.22 | 0.47 | .79 |
Group | 7 | 677 | 0.23 | 0.05 – 0.42 | 8.11 | ||
Dyadic (Friend) | 4 | 397 | 0.09 | −0.16 – 0.33 | 10.32* | ||
Dyadic (Non-Friend) | 3 | 73 | 0.20 | −0.26 – 0.66 | 1.40 | ||
Resistant v. Not-Resistant (A, B, D)2 | |||||||
Relationship | 5.78* | ||||||
Friend | 5 | 448 | −0.08 | −0.27 – 0.11 | 2.24 | ||
Not Friend | 11 | 983 | 0.31** | 0.18 – 0.43 | 18.91* | ||
Assessment Type | 4.72 | .09 | |||||
Reported Social Skills | 10 | 998 | 0.27** | 0.13 – 0.42 | 28.73** | ||
Sociometrics | 5 | 507 | −0.11 | −0.32 – 0.10 | 3.32 | ||
Observations | 13 | 1,122 | 0.10 | −0.04 – 0.24 | 16.07 | 4.35 | .11 |
Group | 6 | 657 | 0.14 | 0.01 – 0.27 | 8.16 | ||
Dyadic (Friend) | 4 | 397 | −0.03 | −0.20 – 0.13 | 1.57 | ||
Dyadic (Non-Friend) | 3 | 68 | 0.56* | 0.16 – 0.96 | 1.41 | ||
Disorganized v. Not-Disorganized (A, B, C)2 | |||||||
Relationship | 3.00 | .08 | |||||
Friend | 5 | 439 | 0.01 | −0.15 – 0.18 | 2.56 | ||
Not Friend | 12 | 1,103 | 0.24** | 0.14 – 0.34 | 15.24 | ||
Assessment Type | 4.93 | .18 | |||||
Blend | 2 | 251 | 0.40** | 0.19 – 0.61 | 0.10 | ||
Reported Social Skills | 8 | 763 | 0.14 | 0.02 – 0.26 | 7.63 | ||
Sociometrics | 6 | 509 | 0.07 | −0.08 – 0.22 | 10.43 | ||
Observations | 8 | 796 | 0.04 | −0.08 – 0.16 | 5.28 | 0.08 | .78 |
Dyadic (Friend) | 4 | 399 | 0.06 | −0.09 – 0.21 | 2.65 | ||
Dyadic (Not Friend) | 4 | 397 | 0.02 | −0.13 – 0.17 | 2.55 |
p < .05
p < .01
subgroups with k < 4 excluded from contrast;
A=Avoidant, B=Secure, C=Resistant, D=Disorganized
Type of Social Competence Assessment
Studies differed in the type of social competence assessment used (e.g., reported social skills) and many studies reported on two or more assessment types. We investigated whether assessment type moderated the effect of security, resistance, avoidance, or disorganization on social competence and we computed 85% confidence intervals for the point estimates because some studies had overlapping samples. Regarding the effect of resistance on lower levels of social competence, the effect for studies including reports of social skills was larger than the effect for studies including sociometric assessments (Q[1] = 4.04, p < .05) but not larger than the effect for observational assessments (Q[1] = 1.51, p = .22). The effect for studies including observational assessments was not significantly different from those including sociometric assessments (Q[1] = 2.38, p = .12). Assessment type was not found to significantly moderate the effect of security, avoidance, or disorganization on social competence (see Table 4).
Studies that employed observational assessments of social competence differed in the type of observation employed (e.g., observations of non-friends) and some studies reported on two or more observation types. We investigated whether the type of observation moderated the effect of security, avoidance, resistance, or disorganization on social competence and we computed 85% confidence intervals for the point estimates because some studies had overlapping samples. As seen in Table 4, type of observation was found to significantly moderate the effect of security versus insecurity on social competence. The effect for studies including observations of non-friends was larger than the effect for studies including observations of friends (Q[1] = 7.53, p < .01). The effect for studies including observations of peer interactions in groups did not significantly differ from those including observations of interactions with friends (Q[1] = 3.03, p = .08) or observations of interactions with non-friends (Q[1] = 1.94, p = .16). The type of observation was not found to significantly moderate the effect of resistance, avoidance, or disorganization on lower levels of social competence.
Social Competence, Externalizing Problems, and Internalizing Problems
Finally, we compared the combined effect sizes for the association between attachment quality and social competence with peers, internalizing psychopathology (Groh et al., 2012), and externalizing psychopathology (Fearon et al., 2010). The combined effect sizes are presented in Figure 1. Similar to externalizing and internalizing symptoms, the effects of secure and avoidant attachment on social competence were significant. Unlike internalizing symptoms, but similar to externalizing symptoms, disorganized attachment was significantly associated with lower levels of social competence. Moreover, and unlike effects for externalizing and internalizing symptoms, resistant attachment was significantly associated with lower levels of social competence.
In order to compare the effect sizes for social competence with peers, internalizing symptoms, and externalizing symptoms for each of the attachment classifications, we computed the 85% confidence intervals for the point estimates of the combined effect sizes. Regarding the effect for security, the 85% confidence intervals for social competence and externalizing symptoms did not overlap with the 85% confidence interval for internalizing symptoms (social competence: k = 80, d = 0.39, 85% CI 0.34; 0.45; externalizing: k = 69, d = 0.31, 85% CI 0.25; 0.37; internalizing: k = 42, d = 0.15; 85% CI 0.08; 0.22). Attachment security was significantly more strongly related to social competence and externalizing problems than to internalizing problems. Similarly, for resistant attachment the 85% confidence interval for social competence did not overlap with the confidence interval for internalizing symptoms, but it did overlap with the confidence interval for externalizing symptoms (social competence: k = 12, d = 0.29, 85% CI 0.14; 0.43; externalizing: k = 35, d = 0.11, 85% CI −0.01; 0.21; internalizing: k = 21, d = 0.03, 85% CI −0.07; 0.13). Avoidant and disorganized attachment were not significantly more strongly associated with social competence than externalizing and internalizing problems (see Figure 1).
Discussion
The current review builds on an earlier quantitative review by Schneider et al. (2001) and extends and complements findings from two recently published meta-analyses on early attachment and externalizing and internalizing symptoms (Fearon et al., 2010; Groh et al., 2010) by providing the meta-analytic estimates of the association between individual differences in attachment in the early life course and social competence with peers. In combination, this series of meta-analyses comprises the most comprehensive set of quantitative reviews of the literature on the predictive significance of early attachment for children’s socioemotional developmental (mal)adaptation, allowing for the evaluation of empirical support across the literature for central claims of attachment theory. The results of the current meta-analysis provide evidence that early attachment is moderately associated with children’s social competence with peers, with early security promoting children’s peer competence and early insecurity, regardless of subtype, undermining children’s peer competence. Moreover, consistent with evidence from Fearon et al. (2010) and Groh et al. (2012), results from the current meta-analysis provide evidence that such predictive effects endured into early adolescence in the sense that they did not decrease in magnitude as a function of the age at which social competence was assessed. Findings from these quantitative reviews also provide evidence that, in comparison to the effect of attachment insecurity on internalizing and externalizing problem behaviors, early security plays a particularly salient role in the evolution of children’s peer competence.
Drawing on data from 80 samples comprising over 4,000 children, the average effect of attachment security (vs. insecurity) on children’s social competence identified in the current meta-analysis was d = 0.39 (CI 0.32; 0.47). This effect is robust in the sense that over 1,400 studies with null effects would need to be added to the database to reduce the combined effect down to a trivial level, largely surpassing the fail-safe N of 514 studies required by Rosenthal’s (1991) criterion. Although the magnitude of this effect is comparable to the effect from Schneider et al.’s (2001) meta-analysis on attachment and peer functioning (d = 0.41) published over a decade ago, the current meta-analysis provides a more precise estimate of the effect of early attachment security on children’s social competence with peers.
In the current meta-analysis, we extended prior meta-analytic work by investigating the contribution of each of the insecurity subtypes to children’s social competence. Consistent with expectations (e.g., Sroufe et al., 2005), avoidance (d = 0.17, CI 0.05; 0.30), resistance (d = 0.29, CI 0.09; 0.48), and disorganization (d = 0.25, CI 0.10; 0.40) were all found to significantly undermine the development of children’s peer competence. Moreover, the magnitudes of these associations were similar across insecurity subtypes, indicating that early avoidance, resistance, and disorganization confer similar risk for the development of children’s peer competence. In contrast to prior meta-analytic evidence in which specific insecurity subtypes were found to place children at significantly heightened risk for developing specific types of psychopathology (Fearon et al., 2010; Groh et al., 2012), findings from the current meta-analysis suggest that developing an insecure attachment relationship in early childhood, regardless of subtype, is negatively associated with children’s peer competence.
Consistent with the idea that early attachment has enduring implications for developmental adaptation (Sroufe et al., 1990), we found that the association between attachment and social competence did not vary with the age at which social competence was assessed, indicating that the association between early attachment security and social competence did not wane in magnitude over the course of development from infancy to early adolescence. Together with meta-analytic evidence that the effect of early attachment on internalizing and externalizing symptoms does not vary in magnitude with the age at which psychopathology is assessed (Fearon et al., 2010; Groh et al., 2012), such evidence provides mounting support for the enduring predictive significance of early attachment for children’s developmental adaptation into early adolescence. As such, there is an urgent need for theory-driven studies that address mediating processes that account for such enduring effects, for example by addressing questions concerning whether such long-term continuities are due to the ongoing supportive function of attachment relationships and/or the early effects of attachment experiences on the construction of stable psychological structures (see Beijersbergen, Juffer, Bakermans-Kranenberg, & Van IJzendoorn, 2012; Belsky & Fearon, 2002; Fraley, Roisman, & Haltigan, 2013).
We also examined the role of potentially important moderators of the association between early attachment variation and social competence that have been highlighted in the literature. Focusing first on type of attachment assessment, the magnitude of the association between attachment and social competence in studies employing the AQS was found to be larger than for those employing the standard SSP, even with studies employing the mother-reported AQS removed from analyses. Interpreting this effect proves challenging due to the fact that the age at which attachment is assessed and the type of attachment assessment are confounded, as indicated by the finding that the effects for age and type of attachment assessment dropped to non-significance when simultaneously added to a meta-regression predicting study effect sizes. This pattern of results is strikingly similar to what we have found in our prior meta-analytic work on attachment and psychopathology (Fearon et al., 2010; Groh et al., 2012), and thus, is emerging as a common theme in the attachment literature. Yet, because it remains unclear if such patterns can be explained by methodological differences (e.g., longer observation time in later attachment assessments) or developmental differences (e.g., attachment representations may not consolidate until early childhood; Bowlby, 1969/1982), it is a topic that warrants further inquiry.
Focusing on the sub-set of studies that employed the AQS, prior meta-analytic evidence suggests that the mother-reported AQS is not as psychometrically valid and reliable as the observer-reported AQS (Van IJzendoorn, Vereijken, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Riksen-Walraven, 2004). In light of such evidence and because a considerable amount of mother-reported AQS data exist in this literature (k = 22), we examined whether AQS reporter influenced study effect sizes. Although AQS reporter was not found to be a significant moderator, within the sub-set of studies employing the mother-reported AQS, those that also used mother reports of social competence had significantly larger effect sizes (d = 0.79) than those that did not (d = 0.40). Thus, the mother-reported AQS may artificially inflate associations between attachment and social competence when mothers are also relied upon to report on their child’s social competence, providing further support for the use of the observer-reported AQS.
Although there are relatively few claims regarding the moderating role of gender on the link between attachment and social competence, Cohn (1990) and Turner (1991) have argued that because insecure boys may be more likely to exhibit externalizing behavior problems than insecure girls, they may be especially likely to be rejected by peers and perceived as less socially competent. Partially supporting this claim, prior meta-analytic evidence indicates that although early insecurity places both boys and girls at heightened risk for developing externalizing behavior problems, this risk is especially heightened in boys (Fearon et al., 2010). In the current meta-analysis, however, gender was not found to significantly moderate the effect of attachment on social competence. Thus, perhaps because of gendered expectations for social behavior (Maccoby, 1990), the implications of exhibiting such behavior problems for insecure girls may be as harmful for their social competence as stronger levels of such behavior in insecure boys.
The impact of early attachment security on children’s socioemotional development has been argued to be amplified under conditions of contextual or psychosocial risk (e.g., DeKlyen & Greenberg, 2008; Belsky & Fearon, 2002). Accordingly, we examined whether risk status moderated the effect of attachment security on children’s social competence. Focusing first on socioeconomic risk, we found that whether studies focused on low versus high/middle SES samples did not moderate the effect of attachment on social competence. Combined with meta-analytic evidence that SES does not moderate the association between attachment and externalizing and internalizing symptomatology (Fearon et al., 2010; Groh et al., 2012), our results cumulatively provide little support for a diathesis-stress model in which the influence of attachment on future adaptation is theorized to be strongest in economically deprived populations. Next, we examined whether other risk factors moderated the impact of attachment security on social competence. Similar to findings regarding socioeconomic risk, whether samples were at-risk (vs. not at-risk) for other contextual (e.g., prenatal exposure to drugs) or psychosocial (e.g., child psychopathology) reasons was not found to moderate the effect of attachment on social competence, again, providing little evidence for a cumulative-risk model.
Despite such evidence, it is important to note that within the literature on risk and resilience, it has been argued that the experience of multiple risk factors may be especially important for heightening children’s risk for developmental maladaptation (Rutter, 1979). In fact, within the NICHD SECCYD it has been found that the association between attachment and social competence is amplified under conditions of multiple risk factors (Belsky & Fearon, 2002). Because so few studies in the literature have included samples with multiple risk factors (k = 3), we were unable to address whether the experience of multiple risk factors compounds the effect of early attachment on social competence. As such, the literature would benefit from more studies focused on samples experiencing multiple types of risk. In addition to focusing on contextual factors, future research might also consider the role of children’s differential susceptibility to context, investigating whether there are some children for whom the predictive significance of attachment variation is amplified, for better and for worse (Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Van IJzendoorn, 2007; Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Van IJzendoorn, 2011; Van IJzendoorn & Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2012).
In light of arguments that friendships may have an attachment component (Ainsworth, 1989) and early attachment variation might be especially predictive of children’s friendships (Belsky & Cassidy, 1995; Sroufe, 1988), we also investigated whether the association between attachment and social competence was stronger when children’s social competence with friends (vs. non-friends) was the outcome. Although we did find that type of relationship moderated the effect of security on peer competence, the magnitude of the effect was strongest when social competence with non-friends was assessed, a pattern consistent across insecurity subtypes.
This finding is surprising given past theorizing and evidence from an earlier meta-analysis in which the influence of security on friendships was found to be stronger than on non-friend peer relationships (Schneider et al., 2001). Although this prior meta-analysis was broader in scope than the current meta-analysis, for this particular contrast, studies in the prior and current meta-analyses largely overlapped in terms of the assessment age of attachment (i.e., with the exception of one study in which attachment was assessed via self-report at age 10 in Schneider et al. [2001], all attachment assessments were administered before 5.5 years) and social competence (assessments conducted between 3 – 11 years), making such factors unlikely contributors to the divergence in findings. However, a number of other factors might have played a role, the most obvious of which is that a number of additional studies on attachment and friendship (k = 6) have been published since the prior meta-analysis, some of which reported weak or non-significant effects (e.g., Agnor, 2009). In addition, the earlier meta-analysis by Schneider et al. (2001) appears to have missed some studies, the majority of which produced weak, non-significant effects (e.g., Clarke-Stewart, 1981; Hubbs-Tait et al., 1994). Moreover, the prior meta-analysis included some articles not included in the current review because they drew on data from the overlapping samples (e.g., Kerns, 1994; Park & Waters, 1989) or the attachment assessment did not meet our inclusion criteria (e.g., self-report, Kerns et al., 1996). Such factors likely contributed to the larger effect for friends reported by Schneider et al. (2001; d = 0.49) than reported here (d = 0.15). Of course, meta-analyses require decisions about ways in which data are collected and coded that might differ between research teams. Explicit decision rules make it possible for researchers and readers to reconcile or interpret divergent outcomes.
A potential factor that might have contributed to the larger effect for non-friends reported here (d = 0.41) than reported previously (d = 0.28), is that in the current meta-analysis, the effect for friends was compared to the effect for non-friends for all studies with relevant data, whereas Schneider and colleagues’ (2001) effect for non-friends reflects a smaller sample of studies that were randomly selected and matched to each study reporting on friends. Thus, we also examined effects within the same type of social competence outcome assessment by comparing the effect of attachment security on observations of friend and non-friend dyads, and found again that the magnitude of the effect for attachment security was stronger for children’s social competence with non-friends than friends. Taken together, while this pattern of findings was unexpected, it is important to emphasize that early attachment security significantly contributed to both children’s social competence with friends and non-friends, highlighting the broad significance of early attachment security for children’s social competence with peers regardless of friendship status.
Providing further evidence for the idea that attachment quality has broad implications for children’s interpersonal functioning with peers, in the current meta-analysis, we found limited evidence that the type of social competence assessment moderated the association between early attachment security and peer competence. That said, the effect for resistant attachment was found to be stronger among studies employing social skills assessments than those employing sociometric assessments. Prior evidence suggests that resistant children may exhibit behavior that might exclude them from the peer group (e.g., chronic contact/attention seeking behavior toward teachers; Sroufe, Fox, & Pancake, 1983). Thus, resistance may be especially associated with lower levels of reported social skills because such dependent behavior may lead them to be perceived as less socially competent by adults, but not actively disliked by peers.
Evidence from this meta-analysis provides a more precise estimate of the magnitude of the effect of early attachment security and insecurity subtypes on children’s peer competence and clarifies the role of potential moderating factors. At the same time, however, this report highlights some of the gaps in the current literature. First, as seen most clearly in Figure 1, in comparison to the literature on early attachment and internalizing and externalizing symptoms, the majority of studies on attachment and social competence exclusively report on data for the insecure group as a whole, resulting in strikingly few studies reporting on data for insecure subtypes. The consequence of this lack of data is that in the current meta-analysis there were too few studies to allow for an investigation of potential moderators of the negative effect of avoidance and resistance on children’s social competence. Clearly, there is a need for future studies to report data for the insecurity subtypes. Second, based on the available data in the literature, father-child attachment security was not found to significantly predict peer competence, suggesting that, unlike mother-child attachment, father-child attachment is not an especially strong predictor of children’s peer competence. Some caution is urged when interpreting this effect, however, because some attachment scholars have argued that the effects of father-child attachment may differ by the gender of the child (e.g., Berlin et al., 2008; Grossmann, Grossmann, & Kindler, 2005; Schneider et al., 2001). Because of the limited number of studies reporting on father-child attachment and the fact that only two of these studies report on effects separately for boys and girls, we were unable to evaluate the empirical evidence in light of this claim, highlighting the need for further research in this area.
Having now taken stock of the literatures on the predictive significance of early attachment for children’s peer competence, externalizing symptomatology (Fearon et al., 2010), and internalizing symptomatology (Groh et al., 2012), we are able to evaluate the empirical evidence in light of claims regarding the relative predictive significance of early attachment across developmental domains. Providing some support for the claim that the early parent-child attachment relationship might be expected to have the strongest implications for subsequent interpersonal relations and important, yet weaker, implications for psychopathology (Belsky & Cassidy, 1995; Sroufe, 1988), in the current meta-analysis the effect of early attachment security was largest in magnitude for children’s social competence with peers and stronger than the effect for internalizing symptoms. However, the meta-analytic association between early security and peer competence was not significantly larger than that of early security and externalizing symptoms. It is important to note, however, that externalizing symptoms includes aspects of maladaptive behavior exhibited within the peer context (e.g., aggression), and due to the social context in which such behavior occurs, it may be especially associated with early security. In fact, the meta-analytic association between early security and observations of children’s externalizing symptoms (d = 0.58), most of which were conducted with peers, was larger than reports of externalizing symptoms by parents (d = 0.22) and teachers (d = 0.30; Fearon et al., 2010), and such effects based on parent and teacher reports were smaller in magnitude than the association between early attachment and peer competence (d = .39). Similarly, a potential reason the meta-analytic effect of security was found to be weakest for internalizing symptoms might be the heavy reliance on observations of internalizing symptoms by mothers and teachers who find it difficult to report on such symptoms. As noted in Groh et al. (2012), further research that makes use of trained observers (e.g., clinicians) of internalizing symptoms is necessary to examine whether the effect of security on internalizing symptoms is larger and more comparable to the effect on externalizing symptoms and social competence when such reports are used.
Finally, we would be remiss if we failed to note that, although we believe that meta-analytic reviews are crucial in advancing our understanding of large, complex literatures, questions remain about the causal implications of early attachment security for subsequent adaptation. The current review, like those of Fearon et al. (2010) and Groh et al. (2012), focused exclusively on bivariate associations between early attachment security and subsequent manifestations of peer competence, as well as the patterning of those predictive relations as a function of when peer competence was assessed and other moderators. Having now taken stock of the literature, the field would certainly benefit from studies that take advantage of the best of nonexperimental and experimental (i.e., intervention) research designs with a focus both on unconfounding effects of shared genes from shared environments in attachment research (Fearon et al., 2006; Roisman & Fraley, 2008) and in identifying the precise mechanisms through which early attachment-relevant experiences have enduring implications for developmental adaptation.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported in part by a postdoctoral fellowship provided by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (T32-HD07376) through the Center for Developmental Science, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, to Ashley M. Groh. Marian J. Bakermans-Kranenburg and Marinus H. van IJzendoorn were supported by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (MJBK: VICI MHvIJ: SPINOZA).
Contributor Information
Ashley M. Groh, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
R. Pasco Fearon, University College London.
Marian J. Bakermans-Kranenburg, Leiden University
Marinus H. van IJzendoorn, Leiden University
Ryan D. Steele, University of Minnesota
Glenn I. Roisman, University of Minnesota
References
References with asterisks are included in the meta-analytic dataset.
- *.Adams BL. Doctoral dissertation. 1994. An investigation of the interrelationships among security of attachment, parenting attitudes, and the development of competence. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- *.Adamson L. Doctoral dissertation. 1990. The relationship between attachment and social competence in peer relations in early childhood. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- *.Agnor CJ. Doctoral dissertation. 2009. A proposed model of friendship quality and attachment in preschool children. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- Ainsworth MDS. Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist. 1989;44(4):709–716. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.44.4.709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ainsworth MDS, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1978. [Google Scholar]
- *.Balentine AC. Doctoral dissertation. 2007. The Relation of Early Attachment with Kindergarten Social Preference: An Examination of Intervening Relational and Behavioral Processes. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- *.Barglow P, Contreras J, Kavesh L, Vaughn BE. Developmental follow-up of 6–7 year old children of mothers employed during their infancies. Child Psychiatry and Human Development. 1998;29:3–20. doi: 10.1023/a:1022636412780. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beijersbergen MD, Juffer F, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH. Remaining or becoming secure: Parental sensitive support predicts attachment continuity from infancy to adolescence in a longitudinal adoption study. Developmental Psychology. 2012;48:1277–1282. doi: 10.1037/a0027442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH. For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2007;16:300–304. [Google Scholar]
- Belsky J, Cassidy J. Attachment theory and evidence. In: Rutter M, Hay D, editors. Development through life. London: Blackwell; 1995. pp. 373–402. [Google Scholar]
- Belsky J, Fearon RMP. Infant-mother attachment security, contextual risk, and early development: A moderational analysis. Development and Psychopathology. 2002;1:293–310. doi: 10.1017/s0954579402002067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berlin LJ, Cassidy J, Appleyard K. The influence of early attachments on other relationships. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2008. pp. 333–347. [Google Scholar]
- *.Bohlin G, Hagekull B, Rydell AM. Attachment and social functioning: A longitudinal study from infancy to middle childhood. Social Development. 2000;9:24–39. [Google Scholar]
- *.Booth CL, Rubin KH, Rose-Krasnor L. Perceptions of emotional support from mother and friend in middle childhood: Links with social-emotional adaptation and preschool attachment security. Child Development. 1998;69:427–442. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JPT, Rothstein HR. Introduction to meta-analysis. London: Wiley; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Borenstein M, Rothstein D, Cohen J. Comprehensive meta-analysis: A computer program for research synthesis [Computer software] Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Biostat.; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- *.Bost KK, Vaughn BE, Washington WN, Cielinski KL, Bradbard MR. Social competence, social support, and attachment: Demarcation of construct domains, measurement, and paths of influence for preschool children attending Head Start. Child Development. 1998;69:192–218. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowlby J. Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books; 1969/1982. [Google Scholar]
- Bowlby J. Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger. New York: Basic Books; 1973. [Google Scholar]
- Bretherton I, Munholland KA. Internal working models in attachment relationships: Elaborating a central construct in attachment theory. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2008. pp. 102–127. [Google Scholar]
- *.Bureau J, Moss E. Behavioural precursors of attachment representations in middle childhood and links with child social adaptation. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2010;28:657–677. doi: 10.1348/026151009x468062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Burmenskaya GV. Child’s attachment to mother as the basis of mental development typology. Psychology in Russia: State of the Art. 2009;2:385–403. [Google Scholar]
- *.Burns SR. Doctoral dissertation. 2002. Attachment security as a predictor of preschoolers’ prosocial responses to mothers and peers. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- *.Bystritsky M. Doctoral dissertation. 1999. Relations among attachment quality, parenting style, quality of family environment, and social adjustment. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertation and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- Carlson EA. A prospective longitudinal study of attachment disorganization/disorientation. Child Development. 1998;69:1107–1128. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Carter AS, Little C, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Kogan N. The Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (ITSEA): Comparing parent ratings to laboratory observations of task mastery, emotional regulation, coping behaviors and attachment status. Infant Mental Health Journal. 1999;20:375–392. [Google Scholar]
- *.Cassibba R, Van IJzendoorn MH, D’Odorico L. Attachment and play in child care centres: Reliability and validity of the Attachment Q-sort for mothers and professional caregivers in Italy. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2000;24:241–255. [Google Scholar]
- *.Cassidy J. Child-mother attachment and the self in six-year-olds. Child Development. 1988;59:121–134. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1988.tb03200.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Cassidy J, Kirsh SJ, Scolton KL, Parke RD. Attachment and representations of peer relationships. Developmental Psychology. 1996;32:892–904. [Google Scholar]
- Cassidy J, Marvin RS the MacArthur Working Group on Attachment. Attachment organization in three and four year olds: Coding guidelines. University of Illinois; 1989. Unpublished scoring manual. [Google Scholar]
- *.Clarke-Stewart K, Allison . Observation and experiment: Complementary strategies for studying day care and social development. In: Kilmer Sally., editor. Advances in early education and day care. Vol. 2. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press; 1981. pp. 227–250. [Google Scholar]
- *.Cohn DA. Doctoral dissertation. 1988. Security of attachment at six years of age and peer social competence in first-grade. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- *.Cohn DA. Child-mother attachment of six-year-olds and social competence at school. Child Development. 1990;61:152–162. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02768.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Cutler KM-Z. Doctoral dissertation. 1995. Parent-child attachment and communication styles: Relations with preschoolers’ peer communication styles. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- DeKlyen M, Greenberg MT. Attachment and psychopathology in Childhood. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research and Clinical Applications. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford; 2008. pp. 637–665. [Google Scholar]
- *.DeMulder EK, Denham S, Schmidt M, Mitchell J. Q-sort assessment of attachment security during the preschool years: Links from home to school. Developmental Psychology. 2000;36:274–282. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.36.2.274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodge KA, Coie JD. Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children's peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1987;53:1146–1158. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.53.6.1146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duval S, Tweedie R. A nonparametric “trim and fill” method for accounting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 2000a;95:89–98. [Google Scholar]
- Duval S, Tweedie R. Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics. 2000b;56:455–463. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341x.2000.00455.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellis BJ, Boyce WT, Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH. Differential susceptibility to the environment: An evolutionary-neurodevelopmental theory. Development & Psychopathology. 2011;23:7–28. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Fagot BI. Attachment, parenting, and peer interactions of toddler children. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33:489–499. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.33.3.489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fearon RP, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, Lapsley A, Roisman GI. The significance of insecure attachment and disorganization in the development of children’s externalizing behavior: A meta-analytic study. Child Development. 2010;81:435–456. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01405.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fearon RMP, Van IJzendoorn MH, Fonagy P, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Schuengel C, Bokhorst CL. In search of shared and nonshared environmental factors in security of attachment: A behavior-genetic study of the association between sensitivity and attachment security. Developmental Psychology. 2006;42:1026–1040. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fraley RC, Roisman GI, Haltigan JD. The legacy of early experiences in development: Formalizing alternative models of how early experiences are carried forward over time. Developmental Psychology. 2013;48:109–126. doi: 10.1037/a0027852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Freitag MK, Belsky J, Grossmann K, Grossmann KE, Scheurer-Englisch H. Continuity in child-parent relationships from infancy to middle childhood and relations with friendship competence. Child Development. 1996;67:1437–1454. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- George C, Kaplan N, Main M. Adult Attachment Interview. Berkeley: University of California; 1985. Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Gilbody SM, Song F, Eastwood AJ, Sutton AJ. The causes, consequences and detection of publication bias in psychiatry. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2000;102:241–249. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2000.102004241.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Goldetsky GL. Doctoral dissertation. 1999. Attachment and the development of peer-related social competency from the toddler period to the preschool period. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- Goldstein H, Healy MJR. The graphical presentation of a collection of means. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series A (Statistics in Society) 1995;158:175–177. [Google Scholar]
- Groh AM, Roisman GI, Van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Fearon RP. The significance of insecure and disorganized attachment for children’s internalizing symptoms: A meta-analytic study. Child Development. 2012;83:591–610. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01711.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossmann K, Grossmann KE, Kindler H. Early care and the roots of attachment and partnership representations: The Bielefeld and Regensburg Longitudinal Studies. In: Grossmann KE, Grossmann K, Waters E, editors. Attachment from Infancy to Adulthood: The Major Longitudinal Studies. New York: Guilford; 2005. pp. 98–136. [Google Scholar]
- Howes C. A comparison of preschool behaviors with peers when children enroll in child-care as infants or older children. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology. 1991;9:105–115. [Google Scholar]
- *.Howes C, Rodning C, Galluzzo DC, Myers L. Attachment and child care: Relationships with mother and caregiver. In: Fox N, Fein G, editors. Infant day care: The current debate. Norwood, NJ: Ablex; 1990. pp. 169–181. [Google Scholar]
- *.Hubbs-Tait L, Osofsky JD, Hann DM, McDonald Culp A. Predicting behavior problems and social competence in children of adolescent mothers. Family Relations. 1994;43:439–446. [Google Scholar]
- *.Jacobson JL, Tianen RL, Wille DE, Aytch DM. Infant-mother attachment and early peer relations: The assessment of behavior in an interactive context. In: Mueller EC, Cooper CR, editors. Process and outcome in peer relationships. Orlando, FL: Academic Press; 1986. pp. 57–78. [Google Scholar]
- *.Kaloustian G. Doctoral dissertation. 2008. Child attachment security, attachment representations and social competence in early school years. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertation and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- Kerns KA. A longitudinal examination of links between mother-child attachment and children's friendships in early childhood. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 1994;11:379–381. [Google Scholar]
- Kerns KA. Attachment in middle childhood. In: Cassidy J, Shaver P, editors. Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2008. pp. 366–382. [Google Scholar]
- *.Kerns KA, Barth JM. Attachment and play: Convergence across components of parent-child relationships and their relations to peer competence. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 1995;12(2):243–260. [Google Scholar]
- Kerns KA, Klepac L, Cole A. Peer relationships and preadolescents' perceptions of security in the child-mother relationship. Developmental Psychology. 1996;32:457–466. [Google Scholar]
- *.Korntheuer P, Lissmann I, Lohaus A. Wandel und Stabilität: Längschnittliche Zusammenhänge zwischen Bindungssicherheit und dem sprachlichen und sozialen Entwicklungsstand. Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht. 2010;57:1–20. [Google Scholar]
- *.Kremmel LM. Doctoral dissertation. 2008. Preschool children’s understanding of love: Its relationship with attachment security and social competence. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertation and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- *.LaFrenière PJ. Doctoral dissertation. 1982. From attachment to peer relations: An analysis of individual differences in social adaptation to the preschool group. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- *.LaFrenière P, Provost M, Dubeau D. From an insecure base: Parent–child relations and internalizing behavior in the preschool. Early Development and Parenting. 1992;1:137–148. [Google Scholar]
- *.Laible D. Maternal emotional expressiveness and attachment security: Links to representations of relationships and social behavior. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 2006;54:645–670. [Google Scholar]
- *.Lamarca PM. Doctoral Dissertation. 1995. An investigation of peer social relationships and self-awareness of preschool age children. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- Lamb ME, Thompson RA, Gardner WP, Charnov EL, Estes D. Security of infantile attachment as assessed in the "strange situation": Its study and biological interpretation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1984;7:127–172. [Google Scholar]
- *.Lewis M, Feiring C. Early predictors of childhood friendship. In: Berndt TJ, Ladd GW, editors. Peer relationships in child development. New York: Wiley; 1989. pp. 246–273. [Google Scholar]
- *.Lieberman AF. Preschoolers' competence with a peer: Relations with attachment and peer experience. Child Development. 1977;48:1277–1287. [Google Scholar]
- Lipsey MW, Wilson DB. Practical meta-analysis. Applied social research methods series. Vol. 49. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Maccoby EE. Gender and relationships: A developmental account. American Psychologist. 1990;45:513–520. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.45.4.513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Madigan S, Atkinson L, Laurin K, Benoit D. Attachment and internalizing behavior in early childhood: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology. 2013;49:672–689. doi: 10.1037/a0028793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Main M, Cassidy J. Categories of response to reunion with the parent at age 6: Predictable from infant attachment classifications and stable over a 1-month period. Developmental Psychology. 1988;24:415–426. [Google Scholar]
- *.Malatesta-Magai C, Leak S, Tesman J, Shepard B, Culver C, Smaggia B. Profiles of emotional development: Individual differences in facial and vocal expression of emotion during the second and third years of life. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 1994;17:239–269. [Google Scholar]
- *.Mallik SA. Doctoral dissertation. 2000. Attachment quality, joint attention, and behavior outcome in infants prenatally exposed to cocaine. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- *.McElwain NL, Volling BL. Attachment security and parental sensitivity during infancy: Associations with friendship quality and false belief understanding at age four. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2004;21:639–667. [Google Scholar]
- *.Milligan KV. Doctoral dissertation. 2004. Attachment security and friendship quality in early childhood: A theoretical and methodological reconceptualization. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- *.Mitchell-Copeland J, Denham SA, DeMulder EK. Q-sort assessment of child–teacher attachment relationships and social competence in the preschool. Early Education and Development. 1997;8:27–39. [Google Scholar]
- Moss E, Parent S, Gosselin C, Rousseau D, St-Laurent D. Attachment and teacher-reported behavior problems during the preschool and early school-age period. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8:511–525. [Google Scholar]
- Mullen B. Advanced BASIC meta-analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Mullen B, Rosenthal R. BASIC meta-analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1985. [Google Scholar]
- *.Nakano S. Does quality of attachment in a strange situation relate to later competence in different situations? Research and Clinical Center for Child Development Annual Report 1982–83. 1984–1985;6:59–70. [Google Scholar]
- *.Neyer FJ, Schäfer M, Asendorph JB. Bindung, Gehemmtheit, soziale Netzwerke und die Entwicklung sozialer Beziehungen im Kindergarten. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie. 1998;30(2):70–79. [Google Scholar]
- *.Pancake VR. Doctoral dissertation. 1988. Quality of attachment in infancy as a predictor of hostility and emotional distance in preschool peer relationships. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- *.Panfile TM, Laible DJ. Attachment security and child’s empathy: The mediating role of emotion regulation. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly: Journal of Development Psychology. 2012;58(1):1–21. [Google Scholar]
- *.Park KA. Preschoolers' reactions to loss of a best friend: Developmental trends and individual differences. Child Study Journal. 1992;22:233–252. [Google Scholar]
- *.Park KA, Waters E. Security of attachment and preschool friendships. Child Development. 1989;60:1076–1081. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1989.tb03538.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Pastor DL. Doctoral dissertation. 1980. The quality of mother-infant attachment and its relationship to toddlers initial sociability with peers. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- *.Pitterle P. Doctoral dissertation. 2003. The relationship between attachment and social-emotional adaptation and self-esteem in the preschool years. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- *.Porter CL. Predicting preschoolers’ social-cognitive play behavior: Attachment, peers, temperament, and physiological regulation. Psychological Reports. 2009;104:517–528. doi: 10.2466/PR0.104.2.517-528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Regan CL. Doctoral dissertation. 1996. Continuity in children’s family and friendship relationships. Systematic biases in perceptions and interpretations as a possible mechanism of transmission: An attachment theory perspective. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- *.Ritchie SA. Doctoral dissertation. 1995. Attachment relationships of substance-exposed children with their caregivers and their teachers. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- Roisman GI, Fraley RC. A behavior-genetic study of parenting quality, infant attachment security, and their covariation in a nationally representative sample. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44:831–839. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenthal R. Meta-analytic procedures for social research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M. Protective factors in children’s responses to stress and disadvantage. In: Kent MW, Rolf JE, editors. Primary prevention in psychopathology: Social competence in children. Hanover, NH: University Press of New England; 1979. pp. 49–74. [Google Scholar]
- Schneider BH, Atkinson L, Tardif C. Child–parent attachment and children’s peer relations: A quantitative review. Developmental Psychology. 2001;37:86–100. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Shulman S, Elicker J, Sroufe LA. Stages of friendship growth in preadolescence as related to attachment history. Journal of Social & Personal Relationships. 1994;11:341–361. [Google Scholar]
- *.Silverman N. Doctoral dissertation. 1990. Attachment, maternal behavior and preschool competence at age three. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- *.Smeekens S, Riksen-Walraven JMA, Van Bakel HJA. The predictive value of different infant attachment measures for socioemotional development at age five. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2009;30:366–383. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Sroufe LA. Infant-caregiver attachment and patterns of adaptation in preschool: The roots of maladaptation and competence. In: Perlmutter M, editor. Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology. Vol. 16. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1983. pp. 41–81. [Google Scholar]
- Sroufe LA. The role of infant-caregiver attachment in development. In: Belsky J, Nezworski T, editors. Clinical implications of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1988. pp. 18–38. [Google Scholar]
- Sroufe LA. Attachment categories as reflections of multiple dimensions: Comment on Fraley and Spieker (2003) Developmental Psychology. 2003;39:413–416. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.39.3.413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Sroufe LA, Egeland B, Carlson EA. One Social World: The integrated development of parent-child and peer relationships. In: Collins WA, Laursen B, editors. Relationships as Developmental Contexts. New Jersey: Erlbaum; 1999. pp. 241–261. [Google Scholar]
- Sroufe LA, Egeland B, Carlson E, Collins WA. The development of the person: The Minnesota study of risk and adaptation from birth to adulthood. New York: Guilford; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Sroufe LA, Egeland B, Kreutzer T. The fate of early experience following developmental change: Longitudinal approaches to individual adaptation in childhood. Child Development. 1990;61:1363–1373. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02867.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sroufe LA, Fox N, Pancake V. Attachment and dependency in developmental perspective. Child Development. 1983;54:1615–1627. [Google Scholar]
- *.Stams GJ, Juffer F, Van IJzendoorn MH. Maternal sensitivity, infant attachment, and temperament in early childhood predict adjustment in middle childhood: The case of adopted children and their biologically unrelated parents. Developmental psychology. 2002;38:806–821. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.38.5.806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Suess GJ, Grossmann K, Sroufe LA. Effects of infant attachment to mother and father on quality of adaptation in preschool: From dyadic to individual organisation of self. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 1992;15:43–65. [Google Scholar]
- *.Sull I. Doctoral dissertation. 1995. Temperament, mother-child attachment, and peer relationships in Korean preschool children. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- Suomi SJ. Gene-Environment Interactions and the Neurobiology of Social Conflict. In: King JA, Ferris CF, Lederhendler II, editors. Roots of mental illness in children. New York, NY: New York Academy of Sciences; 2003. pp. 132–139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sutton AJ, Duval SJ, Tweedie RL, Abrams KR, Jones DR. Empirical assessment of effect of publication bias on meta-analyses. British Medical Journal. 2000;320:1574–1577. doi: 10.1136/bmj.320.7249.1574. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Szewczyk-Sokolowski M, Bost KK, Wainwright AB. Attachment, temperament, and preschool children’s peer acceptance. Social Development. 2005;14:379–397. [Google Scholar]
- Tabachnik BG, Fidell LS. Using multivariate statistics. 4th ed. Boston: Allyn & Bacon; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- *.Thompson SL. Doctoral dissertation. 2000. The social skills of previously institutionalized children adopted from Romania. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- *.Tumer PJ. Relations between attachment, gender, and behavior with peers in preschool. Child Development. 1991;62:1475–1488. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van IJzendoorn HH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Integrating temperament and attachment: The differential susceptibility paradigm. In: Zentner M, Shiner RL, editors. Handbook of Temperament. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2012. pp. 403–424. [Google Scholar]
- Van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F, Klein Poelhuis CW. Adoption and cognitive development: A meta-analytic comparison of adopted and nonadopted children’s IQ and school performance. Psychological Bulletin. 2005;131:301–316. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.2.301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Van IJzendoorn MH, Sagi A, Lambermon MWE. The multiple caretaker paradox: Data from Holland and Israel. In: Pianta RC, editor. Beyond the parent: The role of other adults in children’s lives. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1992. pp. 5–24. [Google Scholar]
- Van IJzendoorn MH, Vereijken CMJ, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Riksen-Walraven JM. Assessing attachment security with the Attachment Q-Sort: Meta-analytic evidence for the validity of the observer AQS. Child Development. 2004;75:1188–1213. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00733.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Vandell DL, Owen MT, Wilson KS, Henderson VK. Social development in infant twins: Peer and child-mother relationships. Child Development. 1988;59:168–177. [Google Scholar]
- *.Veríssimo M, Santos AJ, Vaughn BE, Torres N, Monteiro L, Santos O. Quality of attachment to father and mother and number of reciprocal friends. Early Child Development and Care. 2011;181:27–38. [Google Scholar]
- *.Vondra JI, Shaw DS, Swearingen L, Cohen M, Owens EB. Early relationship quality from home to school: A longitudinal study. Early Education & Development. 1999;10:163–190. [Google Scholar]
- *.Walls ME. Doctoral dissertation. 1997. Multiple predictors of mother-child attachment and peer competency during the preschool years. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- Waters E, Deane KE. Defining and assessing individual differences in attachment relationships: Q-methodology and the organization of behavior in infancy and early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1985;50:41–65. [Google Scholar]
- *.Waters E, Wippman J, Sroufe LA. Attachment, positive affect, and competence in the peer group: Two studies in construct validation. Child Development. 1979;50:821–829. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Weatherill RP. Doctoral dissertation. 2007. Does the mother-child relationship moderate the effects of domestic violence on preschool behavior problems and social competence? Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- *.Wood JJ, Emmerson NA, Cowan PA. Is early attachment security carried forward into relationships with preschool peers? British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2004;22:245–253. [Google Scholar]
- *.Wu F. Doctoral dissertation. 1992. Social competency of only-children in China: Associations among maternal attachment, teacher attachment, and peer relationships. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. [Google Scholar]
- *.Youngblade LM, Belsky J. Child-parent antecedents of 5-year-olds' close friendships: A longitudinal analysis. Developmental Psychology. 1992;28:700–713. [Google Scholar]