Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as: Neuropharmacology. 2013 Jun 4;76(0 0):479–486. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.05.022

Reward, Interrupted: Inhibitory Control and Its Relevance to Addictions

James David Jentsch a,b,c,d, Zachary T Pennington a
PMCID: PMC4023480  NIHMSID: NIHMS490784  PMID: 23748054

Abstract

There are broad individual differences in the ability to voluntarily and effortfully suppress motivated, reward-seeking behaviors, and this review presents the hypothesis that these individual differences are relevant to addictive disorders. On one hand, cumulative experience with drug abuse appears to alter the molecular, cellular and circuit mechanisms that mediate inhibitory abilities, leading to increasingly uncontrolled patterns of drug-seeking and –taking. On the other, native inter-individual differences in inhibitory control are apparently a risk factor for aspects of drug-reinforced responding and substance use disorders. In both cases, the behavioral manifestation of poor inhibitory abilities is linked to relatively low striatal dopamine D2-like receptor availability, and evidence is accumulating for a more direct contribution of striatopallidal neurons to cognitive control processes. Mechanistic research is now identifying genes upstream of dopamine transmission that mediate these relationships, as well as the involvement of other neurotransmitter systems, acting alone and in concert with dopamine. The reviewed research stands poised to identify new mechanisms that can be targeted by pharmacotherapies and/or by behavioral interventions that are designed to prevent or treat addictive behaviors and associated behavioral pathology.

Keywords: impulsivity, compulsivity, motivation, cognition, dopamine, executive function

1. Motivated Actions and Inhibition

Like food/nutrients, water and sexual stimuli, drugs of abuse act as behavioral reinforcers, and humans and non-human animals are motivated to obtain them (Brady, 1991; Johanson, 1978; Spealman and Goldberg, 1978; Weeks, 1962). Since the work of Olds and Milner in the 1950s (Olds and Milner, 1954), an immense amount has been learned about the neural pathways that mediate reinforcement and reward and that allow drugs of abuse to support seeking and taking behaviors (Gardner, 2011; Haber and Knutson, 2010; Kalivas and Volkow, 2005; Kelley et al., 2002; Robbins et al., 1989; Sesack and Grace, 2010; Wise, 2002). Additionally, a recent major advance in the study of drug-reinforced behaviors has been the identification of both goal-directed (“impulsive”) and habitual (“compulsive”) aspects of drug-seeking and –taking and the characterization of the differential neural mechanisms that mediate each (Belin-Rauscent et al., 2012; Everitt et al., 2001).

While the motivation to obtain drugs or to engage in drug-directed responding has received intense study, the phenomenon of inhibitory control – or motivated interruption of reinforced responding – has received much less attention. Human beings are often motivated to completely abstain from taking drugs or to reduce their drug use because of the accumulated aversive consequences of drug consumption, because of the fear of social stigma or because of their own desire to achieve a healthier lifestyle. These attempts to avoid, cut down or terminate drug seeking and consumption depend upon effortful, voluntary inhibition of the conditioned affective and behavioral reactions to drug-related cues and drugs themselves. The engagement in drug-seeking and –taking therefore depends upon the relative strength of both the motivation to use the drug and the motivation (and capacity) to resist it. Thus, while models that propose heightened (“sensitized”) motivation to obtain drugs as a function of drug experience are relevant to addiction (Robinson and Berridge, 1993, 2008), so are models that highlight addiction-related problems with the capacity for inhibitory control (Bechara and Martin, 2004; Garavan and Hester, 2007; Goldstein and Volkow, 2002; Izquierdo and Jentsch, 2012; Jentsch and Taylor, 1999a; Robinson and Berridge, 2003; Volkow et al., 2004). This review aims to discuss literature supporting the hypothesis that inter-individual differences in striatonigral, D2-like receptor expressing neurons – whether genetically or environmentally influenced – predispose individuals to the development of addiction by influencing inhibitory control abilities. Further, in addition to earlier work that predominantly highlighted a role of the prefrontal cortex in executive control processes, we discuss evidence suggesting a direct role of striatal neurons in regulating these processes.

2. Inhibitory Control Deficits in Addiction

It is well-established that addictions are associated with reduced inhibitory control (Ersche et al., 2011; Ersche et al., 2008; Ersche et al., 2012; Fillmore and Rush, 2002, 2006; Lee et al., 2009; Monterosso et al., 2005). These investigations involved the use of a variety of laboratory measures conventionally thought to measure inhibitory control over pre-potent or impulsive responses, including self-report measures of impulsivity (Patton et al., 1995), the stop signal reaction time task, multiple choice serial reaction time tasks and reversal learning procedures (Table 1). While these measures and tasks are conceptually and procedurally distinct, they appear to uncover similar neural and molecular mechanisms and have similar informative value in some cases (discussed below) and are therefore referenced collectively here.

Table 1.

Common Laboratory Measures of Inhibitory Control

Procedure Usual task setup Test of inhibition Species Key References
Stop signal reaction time task Choice reaction time procedure involving making speeded responses after presentation of a “go” cue (often visual) On a minority of trials, a second stimulus is delivered after a response is initiated by the “go” cue. This is a “stop” command that instructs cancellation of the on-going response. Rodents
Non-human primates
Human subjects
(Eagle et al., 2008; Godlove et al., 2011; Verbruggen and Logan, 2008)
Go/No-go Simple reaction time task involving alternative presentations of a “go” cue or “no-go” cue. Subject responds rapidly only when the “go” cue is presented. “No-go” cues are given infrequently, resulting in an overall pre-potent tendency to respond and a need to inhibit that on “no-go” trials. Rodents
Non-human primates
Human subject
(Eagle et al., 2008; Iversen and Mishkin, 1970; Schoenbaum et al., 2002)
Reversal learning Discriminated choice task involving trial-by-trial responses to concurrently presented stimuli. Each stimulus is differentially associated with reinforcing outcomes, and that which is most often associated with the largest size rewards tends to attract the most behavior. Once the task is well learned, a switch in reinforcement contingencies is made. Stimuli initially reinforced at a high rate are now reinforced at a low rate, with the opposite occurring for stimuli initially reinforced at a low rate. Inhibition of the initially trained response must occur to enable new learning. Rodents
Non-human primates
Human subjects
(Boulougouris et al., 2007; Clarke et al., 2007; Ersche et al., 2011a; Groman et al., 2011a; Izquierdo and Jentsch, 2012)
Multiple choice (e.g., 5-choice) serial reaction time tasks Visual cues are presented in a temporally and/or spatially unpredictable manner, and a speeded response must be made in a manner congruent with the cue’s instructional value (e.g., cues may instruct the spatial location of the correct response) Responses made during the inter-cue intervals (before instructional cues are given) reflect inability to wait or to suppress pre-potent actions Rodents
Human subjects
(Bari et al., 2008; Robbins, 2002)

Of importance, however, the extent to which these deficits predate drug use (representing a biobehavioral marker of susceptibility), and/or consequences of experience with the pharmacological effects of the drug, is less clear. Another model, yet to be tested, is that some people are at genetic risk for drug-induced deficits in inhibitory control; a gene by environment (drug) interaction of this sort may reveal itself through escalating neuroadaptations in inhibitory control circuitry with prolonged exposure to the pharmacodynamic effects of drugs of abuse.

2.1 Inhibitory Deficits Result from Drug Use

It is quite clear that long-term exposure to drugs of abuse in animals is sufficient to produce inhibitory control deficits (Calu et al., 2007; Jentsch et al., 2002; Jentsch et al., 1997a; Jentsch et al., 2000; Jentsch et al., 1997b; Krueger et al., 2009; Parsegian et al., 2011; Schoenbaum et al., 2004). Nonetheless, much remains unknown about the details of this phenomenon. For example, do individual drugs of abuse (stimulants vs. alcohol vs. tobacco vs. opiates) vary in their propensity to produce inhibitory control deficits (Ersche et al., 2008)? Are particular patterns of drug intake associated with greater impairment? Do various forms of inhibitory control (suppression of behavior vs. emotions vs. intrusive thoughts) show greater sensitivity to drug-induced deficits (Calu et al., 2007; Parsegian et al., 2011; Schoenbaum et al., 2004)? Though the general idea that chronic drug experience causes these behavioral abnormalities is now unambiguous, a large set of questions must still be answered.

Evidence is mounting that drug-induced deficits in inhibitory control are linked with neuroadaptations in dopamine D2-like receptor signaling (Lee et al., 2009; Volkow et al., 2001; Volkow et al., 1993; Volkow et al., 1996; Wang et al., 1997). D2-like receptor availability was first shown to be decreased within the striatum of cocaine abusers twenty years ago (Volkow et al., 1993). Since then, these findings have been recapitulated in a number of affected populations, including alcohol (Volkow et al., 1996), nicotine (Fehr et al., 2008), methamphetamine (Lee et al., 2009; Volkow et al., 2001), and opiate abusers (Wang et al., 1997), suggesting that these alterations are a common substrate underlying addiction. In support of the notion that these differences represent neuroadaptations produced by drugs of abuse, it has been demonstrated using non-human primates that both the chronic self-administration of cocaine (Moore et al., 1998; Nader et al., 2006) and chronic experimenter administered methamphetamine (Groman et al., 2012) are sufficient to produce long-lasting decreases in striatal D2-like receptor availability.

Pharmacological studies have provided a causal link between striatal D2-like receptors and inhibitory control and suggest that decreased receptor density may directly influence the inhibitory control deficits seen in addiction. The dopamine D2/D3 receptor antagonist raclopride has been found to impair reversal learning performance in monkeys (Lee et al., 2007). Conversely, the performance of cocaine addicts on the reversal learning task was improved by administration of the D2/D3 receptor agonist, pramipexole, and this change was correlated with task-related changes in striatal activity (Ersche et al., 2011). In addition, the systemic injection of the D2-like receptor agonist, quinpirole, has been shown to decrease the number of premature responses that rats make in the 5-chioce serial reaction time task (5-CSRT), possibly reflecting a greater ability to refrain from impulsive actions (Fernando et al., 2012). Lastly, direct manipulations of striatal D2-like receptors alter both 5-CSRT and reversal learning performance (Besson et al., 2010; Haluk and Floresco, 2009).

In summary, the chronic administration of drugs of abuse appears sufficient to produce reductions in both striatal D2-like receptors and inhibitory control processes, and reductions in D2-like receptors may contribute causally to the latter behavioral difference.

2. 2. Inhibitory Control Deficits Index Susceptibility for Addictions

Despite strong evidence that deficits in inhibitory control may result from drug use, longitudinal and family studies have made it increasingly apparent that reduced inhibitory control might also serve as a genetically determined risk factor for addiction. Children less capable of regulating their own behavior appear to be at a heightened risk for developing a substance use disorder later in life. For instance, male three year olds designated as undercontrolled, irritable, and impulsive were more likely to be diagnosed with alcohol dependence at age 21 than well-adjusted children (Caspi et al., 1996), and disinhibition at ages 10–12 has been shown to predict substance use disorders at age 19 in males (Tarter et al., 2003). Additionally, the Eysenck and Cloninger personality traits of psychoticism and novelty seeking – which both comprise impulsive and disinhibited tendencies – were shown to prospectively predict substance use disorders in college students (Sher et al., 2000). Lastly, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, which is in essence a disorder of self-control, has been associated with the development of substance abuse disorders (Groman et al., 2009; Mannuzza et al., 1993, 1998; Wilens et al., 2011).

The idea that reduced inhibitory control is a genetic or familial risk factor for addiction would be quantified by an increased incidence of this trait as a function of genetic proximity to a substance use disorder. A number of studies have now found that measures related to behavioral control are affected in the family members of affected probands (Acheson et al., 2011a; Acheson et al., 2011b; Dawes et al., 1997; Ersche et al., 2012; Nigg et al., 2004). These studies show that both the children of addicts – who are themselves at high risk for developing the disorder – as well as their unaffected siblings, display lower levels of inhibitory control than those that are not in close relation to the disorder.

Data from preclinical research further suggest that measures of inhibitory control predict patterns of drug use and that these differences are genetically determined. Rats with a propensity to engage in high levels of premature responding in a choice reaction time task self-administer greater amounts of cocaine and transition to compulsive patterns of drug intake more readily (Belin et al., 2008; Dalley et al., 2007). Additionally, recent work in our laboratory using inbred mice has shown that performance on the reversal learning task is moderately heritable (Laughlin et al., 2011). Furthermore, inbred mouse strains with poor inhibitory control performance also self-administer greater amounts of cocaine and are more sensitive to cocaine’s locomotor-activating properties, suggesting a genetic link between inhibitory control and substance use (Cervantes et al., 2013).

Hypothetically, these relationships may depend upon the fact that the neural circuits mediating inhibitory control and drug reinforcement are both - perhaps independently - controlled by dopamine D2-like receptors. Recently, it has become clear that individual differences in dopamine D2-like receptor availability, as assessed via PET imaging, predict trait-like differences in inhibitory control in multiple species, and across multiple behavioral tasks. In both rats and mice, a poor inhibitory control phenotype is linked to low dopamine D2-like receptor availability and/or function (Dalley et al., 2007; Laughlin et al., 2011). In non-human primates, reversal learning competency also was positively associated with D2-like receptor availability (Groman et al., 2011). Importantly, unlike the previous rodent studies in which receptor differences were only noted between groups, likely owing to limited genetic variability in rodent lines, this work showed a continuous relationship between the two traits. Lastly, studies on humans have since found that greater D2-like receptor availability predicts greater inhibitory control capabilities using both the stop signal reaction time task and the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (Ghahremani et al., 2012; Lee et al., 2009; Reeves et al., 2012). Taken as a whole, these studies provide exceedingly consistent evidence that decreased D2-like receptor availability predicts a diminished capacity for control over behavior, and further suggest that these differences contribute to the development of substance dependence.

3. The Neural Circuitry of Inhibitory Control

Earlier models linking inhibitory control deficits to addiction proposed a central role for catecholamine transmitters in regulating frontostriatal circuits (Jentsch and Taylor, 1999b). This hypothesis was supported by findings that substance dependent individuals displayed reduced prefrontal glucose utilization (Volkow et al., 1993; Volkow et al., 1991; Volkow et al., 1992), that damage to prefrontal regions in humans and animals resulted in disinhibited and perseverative behaviors (Butter, 1969; Dias et al., 1996, 1997; Iversen and Mishkin, 1970; Milner, 1963; Robbins, 1996), and that pharmacological manipulations that alter catelcholamines in the prefrontal cortex alter indices of executive control (Charrier and Thiébot, 1996; Ridley et al., 1981a; Ridley et al., 1981b; Roberts et al., 1994; Sokolowski and Salamone, 1994; Taylor et al., 1990). Over the course of past decade support for this hypothesis has grown, with an increasingly large number of human imaging studies showing structural abnormalities in the prefrontal cortex of substance dependent individuals (Brody et al., 2004; Franklin et al., 2002; Liu et al., 1998; Matochik et al., 2003; Tanabe et al., 2009; Thompson et al., 2004), in addition to altered recruitment of prefrontal brain regions in tasks measuring response inhibition (Bolla et al., 2003; Courtney et al., 2012; Ersche et al., 2011; Li et al., 2009; Nestor et al., 2011).

Because the striatum remains an important efferent target of prefrontal cortical neurons, and because striatal neurons are themselves regulated by catecholamine transmitters, it remains possible that dysregulation within the prefrontal cortex and/or striatum combine to produce the patterns of inhibitory control problems found in addictions.

Earlier theories of frontostriatal dysfunction in addiction hypothesized that alterations in the striatum were more likely to be involved in motivational components of addiction, whereas those in the prefrontal cortex subserved executive functions (Bolla et al., 1998; Jentsch and Taylor, 1999b). Such ideas are consistent with a rich literature on mesolimbic dopamine systems supporting drug reinforcement and motivational output (Koob and Swerdlow, 1988; Robbins et al., 1989; Roberts et al., 1980; Robinson and Berridge, 1993; Salamone, 1992; Salamone and Correa, 2012), and in the role of the striatum in acquiring and executing skilled motor patterns (Graybiel, 1998; Hikosaka, 1991; Lacourse et al., 2005; Lovinger, 2010; Salamone, 1992; Yin et al., 2009). These sentiments have been expanded into popular Thorndikian learning models wherein the striatum serves to establish behaviors as habitual and compulsive, becoming independent of voluntary initiation (Everitt and Robbins, 2005; Hogarth et al., 2012; Shiflett and Balleine, 2011; Yin and Knowlton, 2006).

While there is a great deal of evidence to support such theories, there is a growing body of evidence that suggests that striatal neurons cooperate with frontal cortical systems in inhibitory control. A number of studies using both non-human primates and rodents have now shown that both the ablation and inactivation of medial portions of the striatum selectively impair inhibitory control in reversal learning tasks (Castañé et al., 2010; Clarke et al., 2008; Ragozzino et al., 2002). Additionally, 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of this same region have been shown to impair reversal learning performance, pointing to the importance of dopaminergic innervation of this region (Clarke et al., 2011; O’Neill and Brown, 2007b). Lastly, a recent study utilizing optogenetic stimulation to elucidate the role of striatal neurons at distinct choice points in a two-choice reversal task showed that activation of dorsomedial striatal neurons is capable of biasing choice behavior, but that this bias is greatest under conditions of uncertainty, immediately following a switch in reward contingencies (Tai et al., 2012). Collectively, these studies highlight a role for the striatum in the ability to suppress prepotent actions and adaptively shift behavior in the face of changing environmental contingencies.

Striatal neurons play a long recognized role in reward, reinforcement and motivational processes (Balleine and O’Doherty, 2010; Everitt and Robbins, 2013; Kelley, 2004; Pennartz et al., 2009; Richard et al., 2012; Salamone and Correa, 2002). A logical question that arises, therefore, is whether striatal function affects inhibitory control abilities through a primary modulation of motivational state or by being the site of action of top-down cortical control over behavior in a manner that is relatively independent of incentive or hedonic processing? Some data supports the notion that dopamine-modulated striatal systems contribute directly to both processes; dopaminergic manipulations of the ventral striatum have been shown to alter both inhibitory control and motivation (Besson et al., 2010; Cousins et al., 1993; Haluk and Floresco, 2009). On the other hand, dissociable control of these two processes has also been revealed. While dopamine depletions of the rodent ventral striatum have been shown to produce robust motivational deficits, depleting dopamine in the medial striatum has been shown to leave motivation to work for a preferred food reward intact (Cousins et al., 1993). Moreover, dopamine depletions of the rodent dorsomedial striatum and homologous primate caudate nucleus impair reversal learning performance but do not concurrently alter response latencies, another way to index motivation during the reversal learning task (Clarke et al., 2011; Eagle et al., 2011; O’Neill and Brown, 2007a). The same has been found following excitotoxic lesions of this brain region (Clarke et al., 2008). These data could be construed as evidence that dopaminergic function in the striatum may, under some circumstances, play a more circumscribed role in response inhibition, whereas altered ventral striatum function might play a broader role in motivational processes.

4. Molecular Influences on Inhibitory Control

4.1. Dopamine

Medium spiny neurons of the striatum have canonically been divided into two populations: those of the striatonigral pathway that express dopamine D1 receptors and those of the striatopallidal pathway that express dopamine D2-like receptors (Gerfen et al., 1990). In line with imaging studies showing decreased striatal D2-like receptor availability in substance dependence, pharmacological and genetic studies have accentuated a role for striatopallidal neurons in inhibitory control. A study performed in our laboratory using primates showed that systemic administration of a D2/D3 receptor antagonist, but not of a D1/D5 receptor antagonist, was able to impair reversal learning performance (Lee et al., 2007). Similarly, in rodents, direct infusions of a D2-like receptor agonist into the nucleus accumbens, but not of a D1-like receptor agonist, also impaired reversal learning performance(Haluk and Floresco, 2009). Further, selectively blocking neurotransmission within the striatopallidal pathway, but not the striatonigal pathway, produced perseverative patterns of responding on the reversal learning task (Yawata et al., 2012). Lastly, mice with selective deletion of striatal adenosine 2A receptors, which are co-expressed with D2-like receptors, maintain goal-directed behavior following training procedures that produce habits in wild-type mice (Yu et al., 2009). These findings highlight a critical role of the striatopallidal pathway in rapid and adaptive shifts in behavior.

It is noteworthy that the contribution of D2-like receptors to inhibitory control is not specific to the reversal learning task, although the dissociation between D2- and D1- expressing neurons admittedly appears to be the most robust in this situation. For instance, striatal infusions of both D1/D5 and D2/D3 receptor antagonists are capable of altering stop signal reaction times, as well as premature responding on the 5-CSRT (Besson et al., 2010; Eagle et al., 2011).

The hypothesis that inhibitory control may be the causal construct through which alterations in striatal dopamine D2-like receptors influence addiction has been outlined above. Given this relationship, pharmacotherapies targeting this system may benefit treatment. That being said, there are a number of substantial issues that must first be addressed. First, while it is a predominantly consistent finding that drugs targeting D2-like receptors alter indices of inhibitory control, the direction of this relationship is sometimes ambiguous, with agonists and antagonists producing both decrements and enhancements in performance (Besson et al., 2010; Boulougouris et al., 2009; Ersche et al., 2011; Haluk and Floresco, 2009; Lee et al., 2007; Mehta et al., 2001). These discrepancies may be the consequence of dose-response functions, compound selectivity for dopamine receptor subtypes, and/or baseline differences in the number and/or distribution of receptors throughout the striatum. Understanding the dynamics by which the activation of receptor subtypes that have hitherto been confounded in drug studies (e.g. D2 and D3 receptors, D2 autoreceptors) interact with individual differences in these receptors must be more thoroughly explored. In addition, genetic and epidemiological studies identifying the source of variation in striatal D2-like receptors may provide predictors of drug response. Lastly, because D2-like receptor agonists have also been shown in animals to reinstate drug taking behaviors (Edwards et al., 2007; Self et al., 1996; Wise et al., 1990), the clinical benefits of enhancing inhibitory control must be weighed against the potential for increasing other processes that might promote recidivism.

Because of these issues, future research should address alternative means for modulating striatopallidal, D2-expressing medium spiny neurons. For example, research could address the interacting proteins that are co-expressed with dopamine D2-like receptors in discrete striatal cell populations to explore these possibilities; amongst the known interacting partners are adenosine A2A receptors and cannabinoid CB1 receptors (Ferre et al., 2009). Moreover, transcriptome profiling of D2-expressing neurons in the striatum have begun to generate lists of genes that are enriched in this cellular compartment, some of which have important actions on motivation and/or impulse control (Lobo et al., 2007; Lobo et al., 2006).

4.2. Serotonin

Though it is the focus of our review, dopamine is not unique in its modulation of inhibitory control; indeed, overwhelming evidence indicates that serotonin, acting on neurons of the orbitofrontal cortex, plays a major role in controlling inhibitory processes, with resulting effects of serotonin depletions and pharmacological manipulations on tasks that measure behavioral and response inhibition (Bari et al., 2009; Boulougouris et al., 2008; Brigman et al., 2010; Clarke et al., 2004; Clarke et al., 2005; Clarke et al., 2007; Eagle et al., 2009; Evenden, 1999; Izquierdo and Jentsch, 2012; Robbins and Roberts, 2007; Vallender et al., 2009). Though both dopamine and serotonin influence inhibitory control, it is not entirely clear if their effects are independent or interactive; some evidence supports the latter hypothesis. For example, the effects of serotonin depletion on impulsive behavior appear to depend, at least in part, upon dysregulated dopaminergic transmission (Winstanley et al., 2003; Winstanley et al., 2005). Moreover, we recently showed that individual differences in inhibitory control abilities in monkeys are explained by the interaction between cortical serotonin and striatal dopamine in a neurochemically and neuroanatomically specific manner (Groman et al., 2013). Though evidence for an interaction is therefore strengthening, the precise mechanistic account of this interaction remains to be delineated.

5. Inhibitory Control and Process Addictions

Mounting evidence suggests that inhibitory control deficits are not unique to addictions to drugs of abuse, but rather, may also play a role in process addictions, such as pathological gambling, compulsive overeating and/or sex addiction (Batterink et al., 2010; Blaszczynski et al., 1997; Cserjési et al., 2007; Jasinska et al., 2012; Leeman and Potenza, 2012; Steel and Blaszczynski, 1998; Verdejo-García et al., 2010; Vitaro et al., 1997). In light of the observation that inhibitory control deficits are found in these conditions, the question emerges as to whether their biological determinants are shared with drug addictions. Notably, people with morbid obesity exhibit, on average, lower striatal D2-like receptor availability, as well as decreased metabolic activity within the prefrontal cortex (Volkow et al., 2008; Wang et al., 2001). In addition, rats given extended exposure to high-sugar/fat foods reveal reduced D2 receptor protein in the striatum, and these changes are further associated with escalating body weight changes (Johnson and Kenny, 2010). Although these findings have not been causally linked to differences in inhibitory control, it is interesting to note that in one study, poor performance on the stop signal reaction time task was positively associated with the magnitude of obesity in children and was negatively associated with weight loss following behavioral treatment (Nederkoorn et al., 2007). Future experiments might directly examine the relationship between D2 receptors, inhibitory control, and treatment outcomes in obesity.

The relationship between pathological gambling and biological markers of inhibitory control is less consistent with that of substance dependence. For instance, although the propensity for gambling-like behavior in rodents has been associated with D2-like receptor availability (Cocker et al., 2012), a study of human pathological gamblers failed to find any group differences in striatal D2-like receptor availability (Clark et al., 2012). Nevertheless, pathological gamblers present altered activity in frontostriatal areas during a monetary delay task (Balodis et al., 2012), possibly suggesting that there are alternative aberrations in the inhibitory control circuitry. As more studies are conducted comparing the biological underpinnings of substance dependence with addictions not involving drugs of abuse, more will be able to be said about what the commonalities are, and further, what might distinguish between them.

6. Conclusions

Over the past dozen years or so, the concept that inhibitory control abilities are crucial to conceptual models of addiction has become well accepted in the field. Moreover, its relationship to addictions – both as a susceptibility factor and mediator of the progressive transition from use, to abuse, to dependence – has also been well established. Important roles of dopamine D2-like and serotonin receptors have also been delineated. Nevertheless, much work remains to be done. Only recently have genome-scale efforts begun to identify the genes that likely influence inhibitory control abilities in animal models (Laughlin et al., 2011), with similar efforts to identify novel loci for inhibitory control in humans as of yet not reported. The identification of neuropharmacological targets for medicines that reliably improve inhibitory control (Bari et al., 2009; Brigman et al., 2010; Floresco and Jentsch, 2011; Robinson et al., 2008; Seu and Jentsch, 2009; Seu et al., 2009) and potentially effectively suppress drug-taking behaviors is just beginning, and the value of already proposed molecules (e.g., atomoxetine) as candidate medications for addictions remains unclear. Finally, the value of biomarkers related to dopamine D2-like receptor function, either neuroimaging -based or proxy measures, in guiding intervention and prevention strategies has not been fully explored. Accordingly, the opportunities are many for deeper mechanistic and translational research into the molecular and systems neuroscience basis of inhibitory control problems in addiction.

  • The inability to effortfully inhibit drug-seeking and –taking actions is central to addictions.

  • Impaired inhibitory control over motivated behaviors occurs as a consequence of drug experience.

  • Natively poor inhibitory control abilities predict addiction susceptibility.

  • Dopamine and serotonin, acting alone and in concert, influence inhibitory control.

  • Improving inhibitory control may be an important new approach for treating addictions.

Acknowledgments

The preparation of this article was supported, in part, by PHS grants R01-DA031852 (JDJ) and T32-DA024635 (Edythe London).

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Acheson A, Richard DM, Mathias CW, Dougherty DM. Adults with a family history of alcohol related problems are more impulsive on measures of response initiation and response inhibition. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2011a;117:198–203. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Acheson A, Vincent AS, Sorocco KH, Lovallo WR. Greater discounting of delayed rewards in young adults with family histories of alcohol and drug use disorders: studies from the Oklahoma family health patterns project. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011b;35:1607–1613. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01507.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Balleine BW, O’Doherty JP. Human and rodent homologies in action control: corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:48–69. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Balodis IM, Kober H, Worhunsky PD, Stevens MC, Pearlson GD, Potenza MN. Diminished frontostriatal activity during processing of monetary rewards and losses in pathological gambling. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71:749–757. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.01.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bari A, Eagle DM, Mar AC, Robinson ES, Robbins TW. Dissociable effects of noradrenaline, dopamine, and serotonin uptake blockade on stop task performance in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2009;205:273–283. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1537-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Batterink L, Yokum S, Stice E. Body mass correlates inversely with inhibitory control in response to food among adolescent girls: an fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2010;52:1696–1703. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bechara A, Martin EM. Impaired decision making related to working memory deficits in individuals with substance addictions. Neuropsychology. 2004;18:152–162. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.1.152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Belin-Rauscent A, Everitt BJ, Belin D. Intrastriatal shifts mediate the transition from drug-seeking actions to habits. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;72:343–345. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Belin D, Mar AC, Dalley JW, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. High impulsivity predicts the switch to compulsive cocaine-taking. Science. 2008;320:1352–1355. doi: 10.1126/science.1158136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Besson M, Belin D, McNamara R, Theobald DE, Castel A, Beckett VL, Crittenden BM, Newman AH, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW, Dalley JW. Dissociable control of impulsivity in rats by dopamine d2/3 receptors in the core and shell subregions of the nucleus accumbens. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:560–569. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Blaszczynski A, Steel Z, McConaghy N. Impulsivity in pathological gambling: the antisocial impulsivist. Addiction. 1997;92:75–87. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bolla KI, Cadet JL, London ED. The neuropsychiatry of chronic cocaine abuse. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1998;10:280–289. doi: 10.1176/jnp.10.3.280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bolla KI, Eldreth DA, London ED, Kiehl KA, Mouratidis M, Contoreggi C, Matochik JA, Kurian V, Cadet JL, Kimes AS, Funderburk FR, Ernst M. Orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in abstinent cocaine abusers performing a decision-making task. Neuroimage. 2003;19:1085–1094. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00113-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Boulougouris V, Castañé A, Robbins TW. Dopamine D2/D3 receptor agonist quinpirole impairs spatial reversal learning in rats: investigation of D3 receptor involvement in persistent behavior. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2009;202:611–620. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1341-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Boulougouris V, Glennon JC, Robbins TW. Dissociable effects of selective 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptor antagonists on serial spatial reversal learning in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:2007–2019. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Brady JV. Animal models for assessing drugs of abuse. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1991;15:35–43. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(05)80089-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Brigman JL, Mathur P, Harvey-White J, Izquierdo A, Saksida LM, Bussey TJ, Fox S, Deneris E, Murphy DL, Holmes A. Pharmacological or Genetic Inactivation of the Serotonin Transporter Improves Reversal Learning in Mice. Cereb Cortex. 2010 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Brody AL, Mandelkern MA, Jarvik ME, Lee GS, Smith EC, Huang JC, Bota RG, Bartzokis G, London ED. Differences between smokers and nonsmokers in regional gray matter volumes and densities. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:77–84. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00610-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Butter CM. Perseveration in extinction and in discrimination reversal tasks following selective frontal ablations in macaca mulatta. Physiology and Behavior. 1969;4:9. [Google Scholar]
  20. Calu DJ, Stalnaker TA, Franz TM, Singh T, Shaham Y, Schoenbaum G. Withdrawal from cocaine self-administration produces long-lasting deficits in orbitofrontal-dependent reversal learning in rats. Learn Mem. 2007;14:325–328. doi: 10.1101/lm.534807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Newman DL, Silva PA. Behavioral observations at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders. Longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53:1033–1039. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830110071009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Castañé A, Theobald DE, Robbins TW. Selective lesions of the dorsomedial striatum impair serial spatial reversal learning in rats. Behav Brain Res. 2010;210:74–83. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.02.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Cervantes MC, Laughlin RE, Jentsch JD. Cocaine self-administration behavior in inbred mouse lines segregating different capacities for inhibitory control. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2013 doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3135-4. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  24. Charrier D, Thiébot MH. Effects of psychotropic drugs on rat responding in an operant paradigm involving choice between delayed reinforcers. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1996;54:149–157. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)02114-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Clark L, Stokes PR, Wu K, Michalczuk R, Benecke A, Watson BJ, Egerton A, Piccini P, Nutt DJ, Bowden-Jones H, Lingford-Hughes AR. Striatal dopamine D2/D3 receptor binding in pathological gambling is correlated with mood-related impulsivity. Neuroimage. 2012;63:40–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Clarke HF, Dalley JW, Crofts HS, Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Cognitive inflexibility after prefrontal serotonin depletion. Science. 2004;304:878–880. doi: 10.1126/science.1094987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Clarke HF, Hill GJ, Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Dopamine, but not serotonin, regulates reversal learning in the marmoset caudate nucleus. J Neurosci. 2011;31:4290–4297. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5066-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Clarke HF, Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Lesions of the medial striatum in monkeys produce perseverative impairments during reversal learning similar to those produced by lesions of the orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2008;28:10972–10982. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1521-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Clarke HF, Walker SC, Crofts HS, Dalley JW, Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Prefrontal serotonin depletion affects reversal learning but not attentional set shifting. J Neurosci. 2005;25:532–538. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3690-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Clarke HF, Walker SC, Dalley JW, Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Cognitive inflexibility after prefrontal serotonin depletion is behaviorally and neurochemically specific. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:18–27. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Cocker PJ, Dinelle K, Kornelson R, Sossi V, Winstanley CA. Irrational choice under uncertainty correlates with lower striatal D(2/3) receptor binding in rats. J Neurosci. 2012;32:15450–15457. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0626-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Courtney KE, Ghahremani DG, Ray LA. Fronto-striatal functional connectivity during response inhibition in alcohol dependence. Addict Biol. 2012 doi: 10.1111/adb.12013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Cousins MS, Sokolowski JD, Salamone JD. Different effects of nucleus accumbens and ventrolateral striatal dopamine depletions on instrumental response selection in the rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1993;46:943–951. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(93)90226-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Cserjési R, Molnár D, Luminet O, Lénárd L. Is there any relationship between obesity and mental flexibility in children? Appetite. 2007;49:675–678. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Dalley JW, Fryer TD, Brichard L, Robinson ES, Theobald DE, Lääne K, Peña Y, Murphy ER, Shah Y, Probst K, Abakumova I, Aigbirhio FI, Richards HK, Hong Y, Baron JC, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Nucleus accumbens D2/3 receptors predict trait impulsivity and cocaine reinforcement. Science. 2007;315:1267–1270. doi: 10.1126/science.1137073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Dawes MA, Tarter RE, Kirisci L. Behavioral self-regulation: correlates and 2 year follow-ups for boys at risk for substance abuse. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1997;45:165–176. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(97)01359-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Dias R, Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts. Nature. 1996;380:69–72. doi: 10.1038/380069a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Dias R, Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Dissociable forms of inhibitory control within prefrontal cortex with an analog of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test: restriction to novel situations and independence from “on-line” processing. J Neurosci. 1997;17:9285–9297. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-23-09285.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Eagle DM, Lehmann O, Theobald DE, Pena Y, Zakaria R, Ghosh R, Dalley JW, Robbins TW. Serotonin depletion impairs waiting but not stop-signal reaction time in rats: implications for theories of the role of 5-HT in behavioral inhibition. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:1311–1321. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Eagle DM, Wong JC, Allan ME, Mar AC, Theobald DE, Robbins TW. Contrasting roles for dopamine D1 and D2 receptor subtypes in the dorsomedial striatum but not the nucleus accumbens core during behavioral inhibition in the stop-signal task in rats. J Neurosci. 2011;31:7349–7356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6182-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Edwards S, Whisler KN, Fuller DC, Orsulak PJ, Self DW. Addiction-related alterations in D1 and D2 dopamine receptor behavioral responses following chronic cocaine self-administration. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32:354–366. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Ersche KD, Roiser JP, Abbott S, Craig KJ, Müller U, Suckling J, Ooi C, Shabbir SS, Clark L, Sahakian BJ, Fineberg NA, Merlo-Pich EV, Robbins TW, Bullmore ET. Response perseveration in stimulant dependence is associated with striatal dysfunction and can be ameliorated by a D(2/3) receptor agonist. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70:754–762. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.06.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Ersche KD, Roiser JP, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Chronic cocaine but not chronic amphetamine use is associated with perseverative responding in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;197:421–431. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-1051-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Ersche KD, Turton AJ, Chamberlain SR, Muller U, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW. Cognitive dysfunction and anxious-impulsive personality traits are endophenotypes for drug dependence. Am J Psychiatry. 2012;169:926–936. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11091421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Evenden JL. Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999;146:348–361. doi: 10.1007/pl00005481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Everitt BJ, Dickinson A, Robbins TW. The neuropsychological basis of addictive behaviour. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2001;36:129–138. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(01)00088-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1481–1489. doi: 10.1038/nn1579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. From the ventral to the dorsal striatum: Devolving views of their roles in drug addiction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Fehr C, Yakushev I, Hohmann N, Buchholz HG, Landvogt C, Deckers H, Eberhardt A, Klager M, Smolka MN, Scheurich A, Dielentheis T, Schmidt LG, Rosch F, Bartenstein P, Grunder G, Schreckenberger M. Association of low striatal dopamine d2 receptor availability with nicotine dependence similar to that seen with other drugs of abuse. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:507–514. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07020352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Fernando AB, Economidou D, Theobald DE, Zou MF, Newman AH, Spoelder M, Caprioli D, Moreno M, Hipólito L, Aspinall AT, Robbins TW, Dalley JW. Modulation of high impulsivity and attentional performance in rats by selective direct and indirect dopaminergic and noradrenergic receptor agonists. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2012;219:341–352. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2408-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Ferre S, Goldberg SR, Lluis C, Franco R. Looking for the role of cannabinoid receptor heteromers in striatal function. Neuropharmacology. 2009;56(Suppl 1):226–234. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.06.076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Fillmore MT, Rush CR. Impaired inhibitory control of behavior in chronic cocaine users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2002;66:265–273. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(01)00206-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Fillmore MT, Rush CR. Polydrug abusers display impaired discrimination-reversal learning in a model of behavioural control. J Psychopharmacol. 2006;20:24–32. doi: 10.1177/0269881105057000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Floresco SB, Jentsch JD. Pharmacological enhancement of memory and executive functioning in laboratory animals. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:227–250. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Franklin TR, Acton PD, Maldjian JA, Gray JD, Croft JR, Dackis CA, O’Brien CP, Childress AR. Decreased gray matter concentration in the insular, orbitofrontal, cingulate, and temporal cortices of cocaine patients. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51:134–142. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01269-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Garavan H, Hester R. The role of cognitive control in cocaine dependence. Neuropsychol Rev. 2007;17:337–345. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9034-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Gardner EL. Addiction and brain reward and antireward pathways. Adv Psychosom Med. 2011;30:22–60. doi: 10.1159/000324065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Gerfen CR, Engber TM, Mahan LC, Susel Z, Chase TN, Monsma FJ, Sibley DR. D1 and D2 dopamine receptor-regulated gene expression of striatonigral and striatopallidal neurons. Science. 1990;250:1429–1432. doi: 10.1126/science.2147780. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Ghahremani DG, Lee B, Robertson CL, Tabibnia G, Morgan AT, De Shetler N, Brown AK, Monterosso JR, Aron AR, Mandelkern MA, Poldrack RA, London ED. Striatal dopamine D2/D3 receptors mediate response inhibition and related activity in frontostriatal neural circuitry in humans. J Neurosci. 2012;32:7316–7324. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4284-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND. Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:1642–1652. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.10.1642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Graybiel AM. The basal ganglia and chunking of action repertoires. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1998;70:119–136. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1998.3843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Groman SM, James AS, Jentsch JD. Poor response inhibition: at the nexus between substance abuse and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009;33:690–698. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Groman SM, James AS, Seu E, Crawford MA, Harpster SN, Jentsch JD. Monoamine Levels Within the Orbitofrontal Cortex and Putamen Interact to Predict Reversal Learning Performance. Biol Psychiatry. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.12.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Groman SM, Lee B, London ED, Mandelkern MA, James AS, Feiler K, Rivera R, Dahlbom M, Sossi V, Vandervoort E, Jentsch JD. Dorsal striatal D2-like receptor availability covaries with sensitivity to positive reinforcement during discrimination learning. J Neurosci. 2011;31:7291–7299. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0363-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Groman SM, Lee B, Seu E, James AS, Feiler K, Mandelkern MA, London ED, Jentsch JD. Dysregulation of D2-mediated dopamine transmission in monkeys after chronic escalating methamphetamine exposure. J Neurosci. 2012;32:5843–5852. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0029-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Haber SN, Knutson B. The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:4–26. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Haluk DM, Floresco SB. Ventral striatal dopamine modulation of different forms of behavioral flexibility. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:2041–2052. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Hikosaka O. Basal ganglia--possible role in motor coordination and learning. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1991;1:638–643. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(05)80042-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Hogarth L, Balleine BW, Corbit LH, Killcross S. Associative learning mechanisms underpinning the transition from recreational drug use to addiction. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2012 doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06768.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Iversen SD, Mishkin M. Perseverative interference in monkeys following selective lesions of the inferior prefrontal convexity. Exp Brain Res. 1970;11:376–386. doi: 10.1007/BF00237911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Izquierdo A, Jentsch JD. Reversal learning as a measure of impulsive and compulsive behavior in addictions. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2012;219:607–620. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2579-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Jasinska AJ, Yasuda M, Burant CF, Gregor N, Khatri S, Sweet M, Falk EB. Impulsivity and inhibitory control deficits are associated with unhealthy eating in young adults. Appetite. 2012;59:738–747. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Jentsch JD, Olausson P, De La Garza R, Taylor JR. Impairments of reversal learning and response perseveration after repeated, intermittent cocaine administrations to monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26:183–190. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00355-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Jentsch JD, Redmond DE, Elsworth JD, Taylor JR, Youngren KD, Roth RH. Enduring cognitive deficits and cortical dopamine dysfunction in monkeys after long-term administration of phencyclidine. Science. 1997a;277:953–955. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5328.953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Jentsch JD, Roth RH, Taylor JR. Object retrieval/detour deficits in monkeys produced by prior subchronic phencyclidine administration: evidence for cognitive impulsivity. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;48:415–424. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00926-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Jentsch JD, Taylor JR. Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse: implications for the control of behavior by reward-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999a;146:373–390. doi: 10.1007/pl00005483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Jentsch JD, Taylor JR. Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse: implications for the control of behavior by reward-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999b;146:373–390. doi: 10.1007/pl00005483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Jentsch JD, Tran A, Le D, Youngren KD, Roth RH. Subchronic phencyclidine administration reduces mesoprefrontal dopamine utilization and impairs prefrontal cortical-dependent cognition in the rat. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1997b;17:92–99. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(97)00034-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Johanson CE. Drugs as reinforcers. In: Blackman DE, Sanger DJ, editors. Contemporary research in behavioral pharmacology. Plenum Press; New York: 1978. pp. 325–390. [Google Scholar]
  80. Johnson PM, Kenny PJ. Dopamine D2 receptors in addiction-like reward dysfunction and compulsive eating in obese rats. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13:635–641. doi: 10.1038/nn.2519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Kalivas PW, Volkow ND. The neural basis of addiction: a pathology of motivation and choice. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:1403–1413. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Kelley AE. Ventral striatal control of appetitive motivation: role in ingestive behavior and reward-related learning. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004;27:765–776. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.11.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Kelley AE, Bakshi VP, Haber SN, Steininger TL, Will MJ, Zhang M. Opioid modulation of taste hedonics within the ventral striatum. Physiol Behav. 2002;76:365–377. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(02)00751-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Koob GF, Swerdlow NR. The functional output of the mesolimbic dopamine system. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1988;537:216–227. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1988.tb42108.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Krueger DD, Howell JL, Oo H, Olausson P, Taylor JR, Nairn AC. Prior chronic cocaine exposure in mice induces persistent alterations in cognitive function. Behav Pharmacol. 2009;20:695–704. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e328333a2bb. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Lacourse MG, Orr EL, Cramer SC, Cohen MJ. Brain activation during execution and motor imagery of novel and skilled sequential hand movements. Neuroimage. 2005;27:505–519. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Laughlin RE, Grant TL, Williams RW, Jentsch JD. Genetic dissection of behavioral flexibility: reversal learning in mice. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69:1109–1116. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.01.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Lee B, Groman S, London ED, Jentsch JD. Dopamine D2/D3 receptors play a specific role in the reversal of a learned visual discrimination in monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32:2125–2134. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Lee B, London ED, Poldrack RA, Farahi J, Nacca A, Monterosso JR, Mumford JA, Bokarius AV, Dahlbom M, Mukherjee J, Bilder RM, Brody AL, Mandelkern MA. Striatal dopamine d2/d3 receptor availability is reduced in methamphetamine dependence and is linked to impulsivity. J Neurosci. 2009;29:14734–14740. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3765-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Leeman RF, Potenza MN. Similarities and differences between pathological gambling and substance use disorders: a focus on impulsivity and compulsivity. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2012;219:469–490. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2550-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Li CS, Luo X, Yan P, Bergquist K, Sinha R. Altered impulse control in alcohol dependence: neural measures of stop signal performance. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2009;33:740–750. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00891.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Liu X, Matochik JA, Cadet JL, London ED. Smaller volume of prefrontal lobe in polysubstance abusers: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1998;18:243–252. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(97)00143-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Lobo MK, Cui Y, Ostlund SB, Balleine BW, Yang XW. Genetic control of instrumental conditioning by striatopallidal neuron-specific S1P receptor Gpr6. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:1395–1397. doi: 10.1038/nn1987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Lobo MK, Karsten SL, Gray M, Geschwind DH, Yang XW. FACS-array profiling of striatal projection neuron subtypes in juvenile and adult mouse brains. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:443–452. doi: 10.1038/nn1654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Lovinger DM. Neurotransmitter roles in synaptic modulation, plasticity and learning in the dorsal striatum. Neuropharmacology. 2010;58:951–961. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2010.01.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Bessler A, Malloy P, LaPadula M. Adult outcome of hyperactive boys. Educational achievement, occupational rank, and psychiatric status. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50:565–576. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820190067007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Bessler A, Malloy P, LaPadula M. Adult psychiatric status of hyperactive boys grown up. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155:493–498. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.4.493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Matochik JA, London ED, Eldreth DA, Cadet JL, Bolla KI. Frontal cortical tissue composition in abstinent cocaine abusers: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroimage. 2003;19:1095–1102. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00244-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Mehta MA, Swainson R, Ogilvie AD, Sahakian J, Robbins TW. Improved short-term spatial memory but impaired reversal learning following the dopamine D(2) agonist bromocriptine in human volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001;159:10–20. doi: 10.1007/s002130100851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Milner B. Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting: The role of the frontal lobes. Archives of Neurology. 1963;9:11. [Google Scholar]
  101. Monterosso JR, Aron AR, Cordova X, Xu J, London ED. Deficits in response inhibition associated with chronic methamphetamine abuse. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2005;79:273–277. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Moore RJ, Vinsant SL, Nader MA, Porrino LJ, Friedman DP. Effect of cocaine self-administration on dopamine D2 receptors in rhesus monkeys. Synapse. 1998;30:88–96. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2396(199809)30:1<88::AID-SYN11>3.0.CO;2-L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Nader MA, Morgan D, Gage HD, Nader SH, Calhoun TL, Buchheimer N, Ehrenkaufer R, Mach RH. PET imaging of dopamine D2 receptors during chronic cocaine self-administration in monkeys. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:1050–1056. doi: 10.1038/nn1737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Nederkoorn C, Jansen E, Mulkens S, Jansen A. Impulsivity predicts treatment outcome in obese children. Behav Res Ther. 2007;45:1071–1075. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Nestor LJ, Ghahremani DG, Monterosso J, London ED. Prefrontal hypoactivation during cognitive control in early abstinent methamphetamine-dependent subjects. Psychiatry Res. 2011;194:287–295. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.04.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Nigg JT, Glass JM, Wong MM, Poon E, Jester JM, Fitzgerald HE, Puttler LI, Adams KM, Zucker RA. Neuropsychological executive functioning in children at elevated risk for alcoholism: findings in early adolescence. J Abnorm Psychol. 2004;113:302–314. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.2.302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. O’Neill M, Brown VJ. The effect of striatal dopamine depletion and the adenosine A2A antagonist KW-6002 on reversal learning in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2007a;88:75–81. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. O’Neill M, Brown VJ. The effect of striatal dopamine depletion and the adenosine A2A antagonist KW-6002 on reversal learning in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2007b;88:75–81. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Olds J, Milner P. Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1954;47:419–427. doi: 10.1037/h0058775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Parsegian A, Glen WB, Lavin A, See RE. Methamphetamine self-administration produces attentional set-shifting deficits and alters prefrontal cortical neurophysiology in rats. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69:253–259. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol. 1995;51:768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Pennartz CM, Berke JD, Graybiel AM, Ito R, Lansink CS, van der Meer M, Redish AD, Smith KS, Voorn P. Corticostriatal Interactions during Learning, Memory Processing, and Decision Making. J Neurosci. 2009;29:12831–12838. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3177-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Ragozzino ME, Jih J, Tzavos A. Involvement of the dorsomedial striatum in behavioral flexibility: role of muscarinic cholinergic receptors. Brain Res. 2002;953:205–214. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(02)03287-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Reeves SJ, Polling C, Stokes PR, Lappin JM, Shotbolt PP, Mehta MA, Howes OD, Egerton A. Limbic striatal dopamine D2/3 receptor availability is associated with non-planning impulsivity in healthy adults after exclusion of potential dissimulators. Psychiatry Res. 2012;202:60–64. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Richard JM, Castro DC, Difeliceantonio AG, Robinson MJ, Berridge KC. Mapping brain circuits of reward and motivation: In the footsteps of Ann Kelley. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.12.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Ridley RM, Baker HF, Haystead TA. Perseverative behaviour after amphetamine; dissociation of response tendency from reward association. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1981a;75:283–286. doi: 10.1007/BF00432439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Ridley RM, Haystead TA, Baker HF. An analysis of visual object reversal learning in the marmoset after amphetamine and haloperidol. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1981b;14:345–351. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(81)90401-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Robbins TW. Dissociating executive functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1996;351:1463–1470. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1996.0131. discussion 1470–1461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Robbins TW, Cador M, Taylor JR, Everitt BJ. Limbic-striatal interactions in reward-related processes. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1989;13:155–162. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(89)80025-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Differential regulation of fronto-executive function by the monoamines and acetylcholine. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17(Suppl 1):i151–160. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Roberts AC, De Salvia MA, Wilkinson LS, Collins P, Muir JL, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. 6-Hydroxydopamine lesions of the prefrontal cortex in monkeys enhance performance on an analog of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test: possible interactions with subcortical dopamine. J Neurosci. 1994;14:2531–2544. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-05-02531.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Roberts DC, Koob GF, Klonoff P, Fibiger HC. Extinction and recovery of cocaine self-administration following 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the nucleus accumbens. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1980;12:781–787. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(80)90166-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Robinson ES, Eagle DM, Mar AC, Bari A, Banerjee G, Jiang X, Dalley JW, Robbins TW. Similar effects of the selective noradrenaline reuptake inhibitor atomoxetine on three distinct forms of impulsivity in the rat. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:1028–1037. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1993;18:247–291. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(93)90013-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. Addiction. Annu Rev Psychol. 2003;54:25–53. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. Review. The incentive sensitization theory of addiction: some current issues. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2008;363:3137–3146. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Salamone JD. Complex motor and sensorimotor functions of striatal and accumbens dopamine: involvement in instrumental behavior processes. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1992;107:160–174. doi: 10.1007/BF02245133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Salamone JD, Correa M. Motivational views of reinforcement: implications for understanding the behavioral functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine. Behav Brain Res. 2002;137:3–25. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00282-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Salamone JD, Correa M. The mysterious motivational functions of mesolimbic dopamine. Neuron. 2012;76:470–485. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Schoenbaum G, Saddoris MP, Ramus SJ, Shaham Y, Setlow B. Cocaine-experienced rats exhibit learning deficits in a task sensitive to orbitofrontal cortex lesions. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;19:1997–2002. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03274.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Self DW, Barnhart WJ, Lehman DA, Nestler EJ. Opposite modulation of cocaine-seeking behavior by D1- and D2-like dopamine receptor agonists. Science. 1996;271:1586–1589. doi: 10.1126/science.271.5255.1586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Sesack SR, Grace AA. Cortico-Basal Ganglia reward network: microcircuitry. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:27–47. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Seu E, Jentsch JD. Effect of acute and repeated treatment with desipramine or methylphenidate on serial reversal learning in rats. Neuropharmacology. 2009;57:665–672. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2009.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Seu E, Lang A, Rivera RJ, Jentsch JD. Inhibition of the norepinephrine transporter improves behavioral flexibility in rats and monkeys. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2009;202:505–519. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1250-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Sher KJ, Bartholow BD, Wood MD. Personality and substance use disorders: a prospective study. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2000;68:818–829. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Shiflett MW, Balleine BW. Molecular substrates of action control in cortico-striatal circuits. Prog Neurobiol. 2011;95:1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2011.05.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Sokolowski JD, Salamone JD. Effects of dopamine depletions in the medial prefrontal cortex on DRL performance and motor activity in the rat. Brain Res. 1994;642:20–28. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90901-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Spealman RD, Goldberg SR. Drug self-administration by laboratory animals: control by schedules of reinforcement. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol. 1978;18:313–339. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pa.18.040178.001525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Steel Z, Blaszczynski A. Impulsivity, personality disorders and pathological gambling severity. Addiction. 1998;93:895–905. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1998.93689511.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Tai LH, Lee AM, Benavidez N, Bonci A, Wilbrecht L. Transient stimulation of distinct subpopulations of striatal neurons mimics changes in action value. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15:1281–1289. doi: 10.1038/nn.3188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Tanabe J, Tregellas JR, Dalwani M, Thompson L, Owens E, Crowley T, Banich M. Medial orbitofrontal cortex gray matter is reduced in abstinent substance-dependent individuals. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65:160–164. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.07.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Tarter RE, Kirisci L, Mezzich A, Cornelius JR, Pajer K, Vanyukov M, Gardner W, Blackson T, Clark D. Neurobehavioral disinhibition in childhood predicts early age at onset of substance use disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:1078–1085. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Taylor JR, Roth RH, Sladek JR, Redmond DE. Cognitive and motor deficits in the performance of an object retrieval task with a barrier-detour in monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus) treated with MPTP: long-term performance and effect of transparency of the barrier. Behav Neurosci. 1990;104:564–576. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.104.4.564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Thompson PM, Hayashi KM, Simon SL, Geaga JA, Hong MS, Sui Y, Lee JY, Toga AW, Ling W, London ED. Structural abnormalities in the brains of human subjects who use methamphetamine. J Neurosci. 2004;24:6028–6036. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0713-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Vallender EJ, Lynch L, Novak MA, Miller GM. Polymorphisms in the 3′ UTR of the serotonin transporter are associated with cognitive flexibility in rhesus macaques. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2009;150B:467–475. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30835. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Verdejo-García A, Pérez-Expósito M, Schmidt-Río-Valle J, Fernández-Serrano MJ, Cruz F, Pérez-García M, López-Belmonte G, Martín-Matillas M, Martín-Lagos JA, Marcos A, Campoy C. Selective alterations within executive functions in adolescents with excess weight. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2010;18:1572–1578. doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Vitaro F, Arseneault L, Tremblay RE. Dispositional predictors of problem gambling in male adolescents. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154:1769–1770. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.12.1769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Volkow ND, Chang L, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Ding YS, Sedler M, Logan J, Franceschi D, Gatley J, Hitzemann R, Gifford A, Wong C, Pappas N. Low level of brain dopamine D2 receptors in methamphetamine abusers: association with metabolism in the orbitofrontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:2015–2021. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.12.2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Hitzemann R, Logan J, Schlyer DJ, Dewey SL, Wolf AP. Decreased dopamine D2 receptor availability is associated with reduced frontal metabolism in cocaine abusers. Synapse. 1993;14:169–177. doi: 10.1002/syn.890140210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Swanson JM. Dopamine in drug abuse and addiction: results from imaging studies and treatment implications. Mol Psychiatry. 2004;9:557–569. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wolf AP, Hitzemann R, Dewey S, Bendriem B, Alpert R, Hoff A. Changes in brain glucose metabolism in cocaine dependence and withdrawal. Am J Psychiatry. 1991;148:621–626. doi: 10.1176/ajp.148.5.621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Volkow ND, Hitzemann R, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Burr G, Pascani K, Dewey SL, Wolf AP. Decreased brain metabolism in neurologically intact healthy alcoholics. Am J Psychiatry. 1992;149:1016–1022. doi: 10.1176/ajp.149.8.1016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Logan J, Hitzemann R, Ding YS, Pappas N, Shea C, Piscani K. Decreases in dopamine receptors but not in dopamine transporters in alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1996;20:1594–1598. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1996.tb05936.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Thanos PK, Logan J, Alexoff D, Ding YS, Wong C, Ma Y, Pradhan K. Low dopamine striatal D2 receptors are associated with prefrontal metabolism in obese subjects: possible contributing factors. Neuroimage. 2008;42:1537–1543. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.06.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Logan J, Abumrad NN, Hitzemann RJ, Pappas NS, Pascani K. Dopamine D2 receptor availability in opiate-dependent subjects before and after naloxone-precipitated withdrawal. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1997;16:174–182. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(96)00184-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Logan J, Pappas NR, Wong CT, Zhu W, Netusil N, Fowler JS. Brain dopamine and obesity. Lancet. 2001;357:354–357. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(00)03643-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Weeks JR. Experimental morphine addiction: method for automatic intravenous injections in unrestrained rats. Science. 1962;138:143–144. doi: 10.1126/science.138.3537.143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Wilens TE, Martelon M, Joshi G, Bateman C, Fried R, Petty C, Biederman J. Does ADHD predict substance-use disorders? A 10-year follow-up study of young adults with ADHD. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;50:543–553. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.01.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Winstanley CA, Dalley JW, Theobald DE, Robbins TW. Global 5-HT depletion attenuates the ability of amphetamine to decrease impulsive choice on a delay-discounting task in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003;170:320–331. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1546-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Winstanley CA, Theobald DE, Dalley JW, Robbins TW. Interactions between serotonin and dopamine in the control of impulsive choice in rats: therapeutic implications for impulse control disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:669–682. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Wise RA. Brain reward circuitry: insights from unsensed incentives. Neuron. 2002;36:229–240. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00965-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Wise RA, Murray A, Bozarth MA. Bromocriptine self-administration and bromocriptine-reinstatement of cocaine-trained and heroin-trained lever pressing in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1990;100:355–360. doi: 10.1007/BF02244606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Yawata S, Yamaguchi T, Danjo T, Hikida T, Nakanishi S. Pathway-specific control of reward learning and its flexibility via selective dopamine receptors in the nucleus accumbens. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109:12764–12769. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1210797109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Yin HH, Knowlton BJ. The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:464–476. doi: 10.1038/nrn1919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Yin HH, Mulcare SP, Hilário MR, Clouse E, Holloway T, Davis MI, Hansson AC, Lovinger DM, Costa RM. Dynamic reorganization of striatal circuits during the acquisition and consolidation of a skill. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:333–341. doi: 10.1038/nn.2261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Yu C, Gupta J, Chen JF, Yin HH. Genetic deletion of A2A adenosine receptors in the striatum selectively impairs habit formation. J Neurosci. 2009;29:15100–15103. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4215-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES