Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 May 20.
Published in final edited form as: Brain Res. 2010 Sep 19;1380:229–239. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.09.043

Toward a technology of treatment individualization for young children with autism spectrum disorders

Aubyn C Stahmer a,b,*, Laura Schreibman b, Allison B Cunningham b
PMCID: PMC4028162  NIHMSID: NIHMS578048  PMID: 20858466

Abstract

Although the etiology of autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and early development of the ASD are not yet well understood, recent research in the field of autism has heavily emphasized the importance of early intervention (i.e. treatment before the age of 4 years). Currently, several methods have been demonstrated to be efficacious with some children however no treatment completely ameliorates the symptoms of ASD or works for all children with the disorder. The heterogeneity and developmental nature of the disorder make it unlikely that one specific treatment will be best for all children, or will work for any one child throughout his or her educational career. Thus, this paper examines early research validating different technologies for individualizing treatment. A discussion of current research on pre-treatment characteristics associated with differential outcomes in treatment, including child, family, and practitioner variables; and how specific intervention techniques address each of those pre-treatment characteristics is provided. The ultimate goal of this line of research is to enable practitioners to prospectively tailor treatments to specific children and increase the overall rate of positives outcomes for children with autism. Research that furthers understanding of how to match clients with efficacious treatments will decrease the outcome variability that characterizes early intervention research at present, and provide for the most efficient allocation of resources during the critical early intervention time-period. This type of research is in its infancy, but is imperative if we are to determine a priori which treatment method will be most effective for a specific child.

Keywords: Autism spectrum disorder, Early intervention, Individualization, Treatment, Evidence-based practice

1. Introduction to psychosocial intervention for children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD)

Although the etiology of ASD and early development of the disorder are not yet well understood, recent research in the field of autism has heavily emphasized the importance of early intervention (i.e. treatment before the age of 4 years). This emphasis may be attributed in part to results of treatment studies suggesting substantial gains may be achieved when treatment is provided at a very early age (National Research Council, 2001). Although children with ASD may face significant limitations (i.e. mental retardation, delays in social interaction and communication, challenging behaviors), and the initial causes of the disorder are believed to be biological, it is important to remember that the environment has a significant influence on the outcome of the child, including the development of the brain (Shonkoff and Phillips, 2000). Gains made by children with ASD in early intervention programs may result in a cost savings of nearly one million dollars by the time a person with ASD reaches 55 years of age (Columbia Pacific Consulting, 1999).

Currently, no treatment method completely ameliorates the symptoms of ASD and no specific treatment has emerged as the established standard of care for all children with ASD. However, several methods have been demonstrated to be efficacious with some children in research settings. The most well researched programs are based on the principles of applied behavior analysis (e.g., Dunlap, 1999; National Research Council, 2001; Schreibman, 2000). These range from highly structured programs that are conducted in a one-on-one treatment setting to behaviorally based inclusion programs that include typically developing children as models. Some of these programs are distinguishable by “brand names,” such as Discrete Trial Training (DTT) and Pivotal Response Training (PRT), while other programs use the principles of applied behavior analysis more generally. A few programs that are not based on behavioral principles are beginning to demonstrate effectiveness as well. These include functional techniques that use structured environments, visual cueing, developmental models and other strategies to assist children with ASD in navigating their environments. Case studies and studies of components of these techniques are supportive of treatment efficacy (e.g., Panerai et al., 2002). Developmental and relationship-based models have also shown some promising results (Greenspan and Weider, 1997). In addition, many “model programs” for early intervention have shown success using the techniques described above or a combination of techniques (for a complete description of several model programs, see Handleman and Harris, 2001).

The heterogeneity and developmental nature of the disorder make it unlikely that one specific treatment will be best for all children with ASD, or will work for any one child throughout his or her educational career. Research points to the inadequacy of one single treatment approach for all areas of learning for children with ASD (National Research Council, 2001; Schreibman, 2000, 2005) and, there is now a consensus that there is no “one-size-fits-all” treatment for this population. Differential response to treatment is common for all of the evidence-based approaches, in that up to 50% of children show substantial positive response, and the other 50% make progress at varying rates, some with extremely limited skill development. Moreover, evidence suggests that treatment providers working in community settings do not select just one intervention but rather report using a combination of evidence-based and non-evidence-based interventions to teach these children (Stahmer et al., 2005). Although treatment providers are combining interventions, very little is known about how to individualize treatment protocols or how to best determine a priori which intervention is most likely to benefit individual children (Schreibman, 2000; Sherer and Schreibman, 2005; Yoder and Compton, 2004).

Thus, research validating different technologies for individualizing treatment is important. Such research requires an understanding of the pre-treatment characteristics associated with differential outcomes of intervention, including child, family, and practitioner variables; and how specific intervention techniques address each of those pre-treatment characteristics. In addition, investigation of treatment/behavior interactions is important as different behaviors may be best approached via different treatment protocols. The ultimate goal of this line of research is to enable practitioners to prospectively tailor treatments to specific children and increase the overall rate of positives outcomes for children with ASD.

2. Integration of strategies

Some researchers believe that combining treatments in a systematic way may be the most appropriate way to individualize intervention (e.g., Iovannone et al., 2003; Rogers and Vismara, 2008; Schreibman, 2000; Siegel, 1996), as the exclusive use of one treatment method may ignore important aspects of social, emotional, communicative or pre-academic development. Early studies indicate that combining methods is a promising avenue to pursue (e.g., Dawson et al., 2010; Stahmer and Ingersoll, 2004), however, some researchers feel it may actually be detrimental to learning, confuse the children and reduce the fidelity with which any one treatment is administered (e.g., McGee et al., 1999).

Community programs typically report using more than one method in what are called “eclectic” programs. Indeed, the best practice committees in both New York and California recommend the use of a combination of treatments based on the needs of the child. However, there have been very few studies examining the efficacy of integrating best-practice treatment methods in community settings. Two studies comparing behavioral approaches to general eclectic approaches have found them to be lacking (Eikeseth et al., 2002; Howard et al., 2005), however the eclectic programs were community based and the method of combining various intervention strategies was not measured.

This suggests that a specific, systematic method of combining strategies may be needed to ensure that interventions remain effective when combined in an attempt to individualize for a particular child and family. This may involve: (a) using more than one intervention for teaching multiple skill areas, but varying the proportions of time spent using each; (b) combining interventions into a modified single approach, including components of multiple modalities; (c) varying which intervention is used depending on skill area; (d) varying based on service system variables such as service settings and program availability; or (e) varying service provision based on childor family characteristics.

3. Current systematic methods of individualization and integration

3.1. Combining strategies within and between activities

Some researchers integrate strategies within activities, during different activities and/or based on the level of support a student requires in order to learn. For example, a program might begin with a high level of structure and remove supports as children no longer need them. Alternatively, other programs begin with the use of the most naturalistic strategies and increase structure if a child does not progress. An example of this is the Alexa’s PLAYC inclusion program (formerly Children’s Toddler School or CTS), a community program based in Southern California in which evidence-based, developmental and behavioral techniques are combined to serve young children with autism and their typically developing peers (see Stahmer and Ingersoll, 2004 and Stahmer et al., in press for a detailed description of the program. At Alexa’s PLAYC, children receive a baseline level of structure (e.g., consistent daily routine, transition signals), developmentally appropriate programming and use of developmental strategies. If children have difficulty with this level of support, more structured techniques (e.g., visual schedule, structured behavioral techniques) are added until a skill is learned. These structures are typically systematically faded as the child is able to participate independently. As illustrated in Fig. 1, a clear structure, as well as developmental strategies to enhance engagement and ensure developmentally appropriate goals, is infused throughout all activities. If additional structure is needed to teach a new skill or to maintain attention, teachers use strategies based on the principles of applied behavior analysis, beginning with the least structured, and move to more structure as needed based on individual child characteristics. Additional environmental structure, such as visual support, may be added if needed. These supports may be adjusted within an activity or throughout a child’s program. Supports are faded as quickly as possible to ensure independence, generalization and maintenance of new skills. For example, within activities, teachers may begin by using a less structured strategy and if a child does not respond, they may move to more structured techniques to help the child learn in that moment. This may include a transition back to less structured techniques once the child is responsive. Providers may also make more global changes to a child’s program, for example, adding visual supports for a child who consistently has difficulty with transitions using the usual program procedures, or augmentative communication for a student who is not using spoken language. Data examining child progress are used to make more global program changes, and as children’s learning improves, structured strategies are faded. Alternatively, some programs vary the use of certain strategies based on the specific activities or times of day. Thus, they may use highly structured behavioral strategies during one-on-one times, or select certain strategies to teach parents to use at home. Other programs make these decisions based on systematic decision hierarchies (see below) which help providers determine, based on data over time, when to move to a new (typically more structured) strategy if a child is not making progress.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Strategy Integration at Alexa’s PLAYC. Legend: At Alexa’s PLAYC a structured environmental approach and developmentally appropriate goals are infused throughout all activities. If additional structure is needed teachers add behavioral strategies, beginning with the least structured, and moving to more structure. Supports are faded as quickly as possible.

In general, well-designed, programs that have used these methods to combine interventions have reported good outcomes. The Early Start Denver Model (ESDM; Rogers and Dawson, 2009), for example, uses a combination of developmental and behavioral techniques in both a therapist- and parent-implemented early intervention model. The program includes decision hierarchies to determine when to add more structured strategies or visual supports to a child’s program (see Fig. 2). A recent randomized clinical trial showed significant improvements in IQ, adaptive behavior, and ASD diagnosis for young children receiving ESDM compared with children receiving “usual care” in the community. A parent implemented intervention, Teaching Social Communication to Children with Autism, uses developmental strategies as the base of interactions and intersperses directive behavioral techniques to teach new skills. Parents are taught to intersperse directive techniques to teach new skills while using primarily developmental strategies to increase children’s engagement and initiation skills (see Fig. 3). This method has been shown, through controlled single-subject and quasi-experimental studies, to contribute to progress in the areas of expressive and receptive language and play skills (Ingersoll et al., 2005; Ingersoll and Dvortcsak, 2010).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Early Start Denver Model (ESDM) Alternative Communication Decision Tree. Legend: In the ESDM model the team (which includes a speech language pathologist) follows the decision tree to alter the teaching approach for children who appear unable to produce speech with the typical intervention model. The decision tree is used in decision making about what alternative or augmentative system to use. From: Early Start Denver Model for Young Children with Autism, Rogers and Dawson, 2009. Copyright Guilford Press. Reprinted with permission of The Guilford Press.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Integration of teaching techniques from Teaching Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism. Legend: Teaching Social Communication to Children with Autism calls for the use of both Interactive Techniques and the Direct Teaching Techniques together. The figure illustrates that the interactive techniques provide the background for all of the teaching techniques, with more directive techniques being used within the interactive techniques to teach new social-communication skills. From: Teaching Social Communication to Children with Autism, Ingersoll and Dvorszak, 2010. Copyright Guilford Press. Reprinted with permission of The Guilford Press.

Group inclusion programs that systematically combine strategies have also provided support for combining methods. For example, one study examined outcomes for a group of 102 children diagnosed with an ASD at age 2 who attended an inclusive toddler program, Alexa’s PLAYC until age 3. In quasi-experimental pre/post studies of 100 children who participated in the program, significant improvements, with large effect sizes were found in developmental level, adaptive behavior and communication (Stahmer et al., in press). Thirty-one of the children (31%) were functioning in the typically developing range when they exited the program at age 3, after an average of 8 months of intervention. A recent study of long-term outcome in 29 children who participated in the program (1–5 years after exit) found that initial gains in cognitive and language functioning continued over time and the majority of children (over 60%) were in regular classroom placement at elementary school (Akshoomoff et al., 2010). Project DATA at the University of Washington integrates behavioral strategies with early childhood special education methodologies, providing inclusion as well as individualized instruction, and parent education. Children in this inclusive group program also showed impressive improvement across curriculum areas, including communication, imitation, self-care, and play skills. Eighty one percent of the ten children in a pre–post study of outcomes exited the program with some functional language (Boulware et al., 2006).

All of these programs use data collection to examine progress and changes are made to specific strategies used with each child based on these data. However, the choice of the next strategy to implement and program procedures describing how to move through the various strategies are often based on clinical judgment or recommended best practice, rather than research suggesting systematic steps for making these decisions. Thus it is not clear what would happen if the program providers began with the most structured strategy instead of a less structured approach, or if certain child characteristics could be used to determine the most effective strategies a priori. It must be emphasized, however, that these programs combined strategies in a systematic way, and examined fidelity of implementation of the model, rather than simply using many strategies haphazardly. This is an important distinction from community programs that have used “eclectic” practices. Studies examining eclectic models have not identified the specific components making up the model (Eikeseth et al., 2002; Howard et al., 2005) therefore poor results may simply represent a lack of a specific methodology for combining techniques or limited fidelity of implementation across the approaches used.

3.2. Combining strategies based on curriculum area

Other programs combining interventions have based the use of various strategies on the target skill or curriculum area. This may provide a systematic, yet simpler, way to pre-determine strategy use. One example of this is the STAR program (Arick et al., 2003) that combines structured and naturalistic behavioral methods. In this model, structured behavioral strategies are used to teach receptive language and pre-academic concepts while naturalistic strategies are used to teach play and spontaneous language concepts. This is based on the premise, albeit non-research based, that some strategies are better suited to teaching particular skills. Thus, some naturalistic behavioral strategies rely on a child choosing a particular activity, or having a reward directly related to the task, which may seem difficult when teaching academic skills or receptive language.

While it makes intuitive sense that some strategies may be better suited to specific target areas than others, there are very little data to support specific guidelines. Some recent preliminary data may call these ideas into question, at least for some children. Pilot data evaluating structured versus naturalistic behavioral interventions for teaching expressive and receptive language in younger children with ASD do not support this method. Cunningham et al. (2010) used a single-subject alternating treatments design, whereby three children (28–35 months) received both structured (Discrete Trial Teaching; DTT) and naturalistic interventions (Pivotal Response Training; PRT) for specific expressive and receptive language targets. As has been found in previous research with older children, these younger children learned expressive language in both treatment conditions, but demonstrated greater generalization and maintenance of gains when taught via PRT in the majority of cases. For receptive language, which has not been studied in this manner, treatment conditions were similarly effective in the acquisition of receptive language for two children, but the naturalistic intervention appeared superior for the generalization of these skills. One child demonstrated a distinct pattern of responding, whereby the structured intervention was superior. Children also showed two distinct patterns of disruptive behaviors during treatment sessions, with the children seeming to have different intervention style responses. That is, one child exhibited increased disruptive behavior in the PRT condition, one child had more disruptive behavior in the DTT condition, and the last child showed little disruptive behavior overall. The relationship between child intervention style response and rates of learning did not consistently match, and further analysis of these data will be important in determining the utility of these variables in treatment individualization methods. What these preliminary data do suggest, however, is that observing differences in child response patterns early on in treatment (i.e. by providing two interventions to the same child for comparison) may be more useful in individualizing treatment than using only curriculum guidelines.

4. Individualization based on service setting and system

Another decision that needs to be made when determining individualization of intervention for children with ASD is the intervention setting. Although researchers and educators alike typically agree that children with ASD benefit from early identification and intervention services, disagreement arises regarding the appropriate setting for early intervention (e.g., in-home or inclusion programming). In addition, most intervention researchers recognize the importance of social integration for children with ASD, but there is little consensus as to what point in intervention it should occur. Some researchers would argue that inclusion is appropriate for higher-functioning or older children with ASD, but is not appropriate for preschoolers who may not be “behaviorally ready” to benefit from an inclusion environment (Strain et al., 2001). Others have suggested that preschoolers with ASD will not get appropriate services in inclusion settings or may be socially rejected by their peers (e.g., Lowenthal, 1999). Programs supporting this philosophy typically advocate a period of individualized and small group instruction prior to placement in an inclusive setting (Anderson and Romanczyk, 1999; Harris and Handleman, 1994) in order to work on these prerequisite skills.

Strain et al. (2001) have emphasized the fact that although the commonly held assumption of “behavioral readiness” greatly influences early intervention programming for young children with ASD, there are no data to support this claim. Several inclusive early intervention programs have documented excellent child progress, and report that the majority of children who exit their programs have functional verbal communication skills and are mainstreamed into typical school environments (McGee et al., 1994, 2000; Schwartz et al., 2004; Stahmer et al, in press; Stahmer and Ingersoll, 2004; Strain and Cordisco, 1994). Of course, as is the case for most programs for children with ASD, there is a great deal of variability across children in program outcome in all settings that have been studied and a dearth of data to inform differential placement for individual children. No studies have directly compared in-home or inclusion-based treatment settings in a randomized comparison design.

It is probable that the effectiveness of a particular setting may vary depending upon the characteristics of the children entering the program. One study looked at the effect of peer avoidance at entry in predicting outcome in an inclusion program (Ingersoll et al., 2001). The study looked at six children with ASD entering a toddler inclusion program. Children were matched for language and developmental level. Although no children were interactive with peers at entry, three children were highly avoidant of peers (actively avoiding proximity) and three children were low avoiders (did not avoid proximity). The children demonstrated significant variability in outcome that could not be attributed to mental age equivalent or language level. Pre-treatment peer social avoidance appeared to predict outcome for subsequent peer avoidance and language use. Low peer avoiders made significant progress in language and social interaction while high peer avoiders remained avoidant of peers and made limited progress in their use of language (see Fig. 4). It is possible, then, that peer avoidance may provide one method of determining service setting.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Mean percentage of language use at intake and 6 months for high peer avoiders (HPA), low peer avoiders (LPA) and typical peers (TP). Legend: In the Alexa’s PLAYC program, children who were less avoidant of peers had greater improvement in language skills than children who were avoidant of peers at entry. With kind permission from Springer Science+Business Media: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Differential treatment outcomes for children with autistic spectrum disorder based on level of peer social avoidance, 31, 2001, 347, Brooke Ingersoll, Aubyn C. Stahmer and Laura Schreibman, Fig. 2.

However, a limitation of this study was the lack of a group of children in one-on-one intervention. Previous research has suggested that low initiations (Koegel et al., 1999) and avoidance of therapist (Sherer and Schreibman, 1999) may suggest poor prognosis in any treatment model. Future research that investigates whether peer avoidance is associated with poor outcome in general should be conducted. Additionally, other research looking at other predictors of differential response to service settings would be important.

Service system constraints available in community settings often determine the interventions used. ASD interventions, therefore, need to be individualized and adapted to fit the resources available to the programs in which they will be implemented. However, there have been very few efforts put forth to examine how evidence-based interventions are implemented in the real world. The limited research that has been conducted indicates that practitioners are not implementing interventions developed as intended by researchers. Stahmer et al. (2005) found that 22 practitioners participating in focus groups about early intervention for autism reported using modified versions of interventions, often combining elements of multiple practices or applying adaptations to those practices based on child and/or program characteristics. Practitioners likely are recognizing that interventions designed by researchers in academic settings have to be individualized for use in applied settings.

In fact, individualization research may be informed by methods experienced practitioners use to individualize interventions. Recently, mental health researchers have recognized the need to improve evidence-based practice by complimenting traditional studies with “practice-based evidence” which involves gaining a better understanding of what works in community practice. Garland et al. (2006) suggest that evidence-based practice research would benefit from studies of “practice-based evidence” (Margison et al., 2000), which can be useful for identifying effective and ineffective elements of usual practice. In a qualitative study interviewing 80 teachers regarding their use of evidence-based practices, providers consistently reported the importance of individualizing treatment methods and strategies based on the needs of the child (Stahmer, 2007). The majority of participants reported that they chose specific interventions based on characteristics related to each individual child’s strengths and weaknesses. One participant explained, “Depends on the child. You know, so much of the therapy is responding to the child and not responding the way that the book says to do it.” Because researchers have not yet determined methods for individualized treatment, we feel that this may be an area where providers can contribute significantly through their experiences. A collaborative move toward individualization may be needed to develop a clear roadmap for developing evidence-based interventions designed for specific children.

5. Individualization based on child characteristics

Because of the wide heterogeneity of treatment outcomes in children with ASD, a number of studies have identified child characteristics associated with positive developmental and treatment gains. Developmental studies have used cross-sectional and longitudinal study designs to identify early child variables associated with later child outcomes in general and also in relation to response to a specific intervention. Early language ability and cognitive ability have emerged as the strongest predictors of overall prognosis for ASD during childhood, adolescence, and adulthood (Howlin et al., 2004; Sigman and Ruskin, 1999; Venter et al., 1992). Standardized communication and nonverbal IQ scores from children as young as 3 years old predict later language abilities (Stevens et al., 2000; Tager-Flusberg et al., 2005; Thurm et al., 2007). However, these predictors are not as robust prior to the age of 3 (Charman et al., 2005), making prediction of response to intervention difficult for children receiving this early diagnosis. Also, some studies have reported that early social behaviors such as joint attention, motor imitation and toy play predict outcome in children with ASD (e.g., Anderson et al., 2007; Tager-Flusberg et al., 2005; Thurm et al., 2007). Despite the fact that these data may provide information regarding long term outcome, they do not assist with making specific intervention choices for individual children. Researchers have yet to exam of the influence of physical, genetic or medical conditions on treatment outcomes for children with autism.

More recently, researchers have begun to look for variables that may differentially predict child response to treatment. Studies identifying predictors of treatment response continue to be important, but they are only able to identify child variables associated with response to one specific treatment. This is problematic, as there is no consensus on the specific treatment model of choice for these children (National Research Council, 2001; Schreibman, 2005). While some single-subject and group comparison designs have argued for the superiority of one treatment over the other (Delprato, 2001; Smith et al., 2000), few investigators have examined the possibility that different treatments might be most appropriate for different children and at different points in the treatment process. Improved understanding of child variables associated with differential treatment outcomes may enable researchers and practitioners to prospectively tailor treatments to specific children. This will likely increase the overall rate of treatment effectiveness across children with ASD. This research is becoming even more important as intervention approaches integrating non-behavioral components are gaining support and other treatment options become available (e.g., Dawson et al., 2010).

Two prospective studies have examined child variables associated with differential responsivity to one specific naturalistic behavioral intervention, Pivotal Response Training (PRT). The first study, a multiple baseline design reported that three 3- to 5-year-old children demonstrating high levels of non-verbal stereotypy and avoidance, as well as low levels of verbal stereotypy, toy play, and approach behaviors, were less likely to show positive response to PRT than three children exhibiting the opposite behavioral profile (i.e. low non-verbal stereotypy and avoidance and high levels of verbal stereotypy, toy play and approach behavior) prior to treatment (Sherer and Schreibman, 2005). A follow-up study multiple baseline design study of six children suggested that this predictive profile was specific to PRT and not predictive of response to other more structured, behavioral interventions, such as Discrete Trial Teaching (DTT; Schreibman et al., 2009). Although each of the profile behaviors have not been examined in isolation, toy play and approach behaviors may lead to a better response to a play based intervention that requires interaction with toys and an adult. Verbal stereotypy is a less likely predictor; however it appears that it may lead to shaping of more appropriate verbal skills. More recent data have suggested that the PRT predictive profile may not be predictive of treatment response in a younger-aged sample of children, thereby indicating the need for varying standards with child age (Cunningham, 2007). Together, these initial studies provide evidence that specific behavioral profiles may be useful in identifying which children are likely to respond to particular treatments and therefore in making treatment decisions.

A recent study evaluated the differential effectiveness of two communication training strategies commonly used to teach early communication skills to nonverbal and minimally verbal young children with ASD (Cunningham et al., 2008). Specifically, this randomized trial addressed the differential effects of a language-based approach (PRT) and a visually based approach (the Picture Exchange Communication System, PECS; Bondy and Frost, 2001) on the communication, social, and cognitive functioning of 34 very young (i.e., 2–4 years-old), minimally verbal children with ASD. Both programs were found to result in substantial spoken language gains for approximately 50% of the children. While early word use was highly predictive of verbal gains in both treatment conditions, it was not predictive of augmentative communication gains. Children with some words (i.e. 1–9 words at intake) were equally likely to develop verbal communication skills in PECS or PRT. Children entering treatment with no words were unlikely to develop spoken language. However, over 80% of the PECS participants developed substantial augmentative communication skills. These preliminary results indicate that word use at intake may be an important and parsimonious child variable to consider when deciding between verbal and augmentative communication programs for very young children with ASD.

One other systematic comparison of the differential effects of verbally based and visually based communication training programs for young children with ASD has been reported. Yoder and Stone (2006a, b) conducted a randomized comparison experiment comparing a vocally based naturalistic intervention, Responsive Education and Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching (RPMT), to PECS in 36 children in order to examine how the relative treatment effects varied as a function of child variables. The investigators found that while both PECS and RPMT resulted in an increase in initiating joint attention across treatment, RPMT resulted in more initiating joint attention compared to PECS for those children who had at least some joint attention skills prior to intervention. Alternatively, PECS resulted in more requests in comparison to RPMT and greater gains in initiating joint attention bids for those children with little joint attention skill at intake (2006a). In a different report based on the same experiment, Yoder and Stone (2006b) found that pre-treatment levels of object exploration moderated growth rates in the number of nonimitative words in PECS versus RPMT. Children who began treatment with low object exploration benefited more from RPMT, while children who began treatment with higher levels of object exploration benefited more from PECS. These researchers reasoned that while both PECS and RPMT involve objects as rewards for communicative attempts, only RPMT involves specifically teaching children how to play with objects. Thus, children with low object exploration at pre-treatment were more likely to benefit from intervention after learning to play with objects. Both the PECS versus PRT (Cunningham et al., 2008) and PECS versus RPMT (Yoder and Stone, 2006a,b) studies provide information about the differential effects of visually based and vocally based interventions and illustrate how detailed information about very specific behavioral effects may be identified.

Collectively, these studies serve as the foundation for research aimed at matching treatment approaches to the individual needs of different children with autism. For example, it may be that PECS may be more appropriate as a first line treatment approach for children with fewer prerequisite abilities, including early word use and joint attention initiation skills. Alternatively, verbally based programs, such as RPMT and PRT, may be appropriate for children who enter treatment with some early communication skills. It is important to emphasize that these findings must be replicated and may only apply to early communication treatment. The relative benefits of these interventions for older children or longer term treatment programs, as well as on other areas of development, are yet to be addressed.

6. Individualization based on family characteristics

In addition to child characteristics, it is also important to consider the characteristics of the child’s caregivers. Active parent involvement in intervention is a recommend component of effective intervention for this population (Breiner and Beck, 1984; Ingersoll and Dvortcsak, 2006; National Research Council, 2001; Singh et al., 2006). Yet the field is just beginning to examine how family variables interact with treatment effectiveness. The effects of ethnicity, culture, marital status, parental attitudes, parental age, level of education, socioeconomic status, and other factors all may affect how treatment is best delivered and the ultimate effectiveness of the treatment. To illustrate, a randomized trial with 58 participating families has shown that children of more responsive and educated parents are more likely to benefit from Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching, while children of less responsive and educated mothers benefit more from responsive small group instruction (Yoder and Warren, 1998). In another example, training in self-management may be more successfully implemented by parents for whom child independence is important (Schreibman and Koegel, 1996). However, certain cultures may not place a strong emphasis on child independence; thus, the parents may not choose to use self-management with their child or may use it ineffectively.

Efficacy studies in autism have rarely examined efficacy for ethnic groups separately, and most do not even provide information about the race/ethnicity of the subjects (Brown and Rogers, 2003). This lack of research is a concern given the widespread recognition that culture has a powerful impact on service utilization, treatment attendance, parenting and other service related factors (e.g., Hough et al., 1987; MacPhee et al., 1996; McCabe et al., 1999). Available data suggest that children from ethnic minority backgrounds are more likely to be diagnosed and receive treatment at a later age than white children for reasons that do not appear to be race alone (Croen et al., 2002; Mandell et al., 2002). There is some limited research to indicate that Hispanic children perform similarly to Caucasian children in parent education programs (Baker-Ericzen et al., 2007; McCabe et al., 2005), however, we need much more information to effectively individualize intervention based on cultural needs.

Another important family factor that may require treatment modification is the level of stress in a family. Parents of children with autism report being under high levels of stress (e.g., Koegel et al., 1992). Perhaps parents who are under a good deal of stress at a point in time would be poor candidates to implement training with their child; a clinician may then be the treatment provider of choice. Later, if the stress is reduced, these parents could perhaps very effectively implement the treatment.

Another potential candidate variable for individualization is parent gender. Typically, mothers are the primary trainees in parent education programs; although researchers have found involving fathers in parent education programs can benefit children with problem behaviors (Horton, 1984; Webster-Stratton, 1985). Research indicates that parent education programs may require modifications in order to increase father involvement. Winter (2006) examined factors contributing to increased father participation in a parent education program for children with autism. She found that modifying an existing parent education program by: (1) offering flexible days and times for training, (2) altering location of training by including home visits, and (3) adding a recreational component all influenced father participation. In light of Winter’s findings, ASD researchers and practitioners need to consider parent gender when individualizing parent training programs.

In addition, collaboration between practitioners and parents is important for individualization of intervention for parents. Brookman-Frazee (2004) found when parents and practitioners collaborated on identifying target behaviors and method of treatment implementation for children with problem behaviors, parents showed reduced stress and increased confidence compared to parents who did not collaborate. The children in the collaborative group also showed greater improvement in behavior. Forehand and Kotchick (2002) provide guidelines to assess individual family variables that may influence participation before training to help practitioners determine what level of intervention parents may (or may not) be able to handle: (Level 1) Family discord is not severe: no need to target family variables directly, parent education by itself can sometimes help resolve any problems. (Level 2) Family discord is present and may interfere with treatment: modification of original parent education plan may be helpful to directly target family issues. (Level 3) Family discord is moderate: parent education for the child can be concurrent with family services provided elsewhere. (Level 4) Family discord is severe and will definitely impede any immediate parent education efforts: encourage parents to resolve issues before beginning a parent training plan.

Often, parents have tried many ineffective ways of changing their child’s behavior by the time the parents enter a treatment program. The practitioner can, and should help a parent to have more positive expectations about what the parent can do to increase child outcome, which can increase their feelings of self-efficacy. Assessing parents’ expectations can also assist practitioners in knowing what behavior to expect from parents. Practitioners can use information about parent expectations to individualize interventions to better fit parents’ life circumstances and lifestyles.

7. Conclusions and future research

While positive results have been reported for many treatment methods, there are no ASD treatments that currently meet criteria for well-established or probably efficacious, empirically supported treatment (Lonigan et al., 1998). Additionally, due to the heterogeneity and developmental nature of the disorder, it is unlikely that one specific treatment will emerge as the treatment of choice for all children. Currently, researchers and clinicians must use their judgment and training to choose the most suitable methodology or combination of strategies for a specific child.

The goal, then, is not to find the one perfect treatment for all children with ASD, but to identify the important variables that influence the effectiveness of specific interventions for each child. Research that furthers our understanding of how to match clients with efficacious treatments will enable consumers to make better choices between procedures, decrease the outcome variability that characterizes early intervention research at present, and provide for the most efficient allocation of resources during the critical early intervention time-period. This type of research is in its infancy, but is imperative if we are to determine a priori which treatment method will be most effective for a specific child.

This line of research may lead to guidelines similar to those available for other mental health disorders, such as childhood depression, in which practice guidelines have been developed for both adults and children with the disorder (American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 1998; American Psychiatric Association, 2002). Treatment recommendations vary based of many individual case factors such as the severity of the disorder, age/developmental level of the individual, family involvement, motivation for treatment, and comorbid features. These factors are considered when choosing the first route of treatment as well as in treatment adjustment and maintenance. As is the case with all childhood disorders, numerous child and environmental factors must be examined in order to obtain appropriate guidelines that have a research base, but are also flexible enough to manage complex cases complicate assessment and treatment.

Footnotes

URL: http://www.casrc.org (A.C. Stahmer).

References

  1. Akshoomoff NA, Stahmer AC, Corsello C, Maher N. What happens next? Follow-up outcomes from the Children’s Toddler School. J Posit Behav Interv. 2010;12:245–253. doi: 10.1177/1098300709343724. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Practice parameters for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with depressive disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998;37(10 suppl) doi: 10.1097/00004583-199810001-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Psychiatric Association. Practice Guideline for the Treatment of Patients with Major Depressive Disorder. American Psychiatric Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  4. Anderson SR, Romanczyk RG. Early intervention for young children with autism: continuum-based behavioral models. J Assoc Pers Severe Handicaps. 1999;24:162–173. [Google Scholar]
  5. Anderson DK, Lord C, Risi S, DiLavore PS, Shulman C, Thurm A, Welch K, Pickles A. Patterns of growth in verbal abilities among children with autism spectrum disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2007;75:594–604. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.75.4.594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Arick JR, Young HE, Falco R, Loos LM, Krug D, Gense MH, Johnson SB. Designing an outcome study to monitor the progress of students with autism spectrum disorders. Focus Autism Other Dev Disabil. 2003;18:75–87. [Google Scholar]
  7. Baker-Ericzen M, Stahmer AC, Burns A. Association of child demographics with child outcomes in a community-based pivotal response training program. J Posit Behav Interv. 2007:52–60. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bondy AS, Frost LA. The picture exchange communication system. Behav Modif. 2001;25:725–744. doi: 10.1177/0145445501255004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Boulware G, Schwartz IS, Sandall SR, McBride BJ. Project DATA for toddlers: an inclusive approach to very young children with autism spectrum disorder. Top Early Child Spec Educ. 2006;26:94–105. [Google Scholar]
  10. Breiner J, Beck S. Parents as change agents in the management of their developmentally delayed children’s noncompliant behaviors: a critical review. Appl Res Ment Retard. 1984;5:259–278. doi: 10.1016/s0270-3092(84)80006-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brookman-Frazee L. Using parent/clinician partnerships in parent education programs for children with autism. J Posit Behav Interv. 2004;6:195–213. [Google Scholar]
  12. Brown JR, Rogers S. Cultural issues in autism. In: Ozonoff S, Rogers S, Hendren RL, editors. Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Research Review for Practitioners. American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc; Washington, D.C: 2003. [Google Scholar]
  13. Charman T, Taylor E, Drew A, Cockerill H, Brown JA, Baird G. Outcome at 7 years of children diagnosed with autism at age 2: predictive validity of assessments conducted at 2 and 3 years of age and pattern of symptom change over time. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46:500–513. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00377.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Columbia Pacific Consulting. Preliminary report: cost–benefit analysis of Lovaas treatment for autism and autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Harper Grey Easton Barristor and Soliciters 1999 [Google Scholar]
  15. Croen LA, Grether JK, Selvin S. Descriptive epidemiology of autism in a California population: who is at risk? J Autism Dev Disord. 2002;32:217–224. doi: 10.1023/a:1015405914950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cunningham AB. Unpublished master’s thesis. University of California; San Diego, La Jolla, CA: 2007. Individualization of treatment for young children with autism: a randomized comparison of verbal and pictorial communication training strategies. [Google Scholar]
  17. Cunningham AB, Schreibman L, Stahmer AC, Koegel RL, Koegel LK. Individualization of treatment for young children with autism: a randomized comparison of verbal and pictorial communication systems. Paper presented at the 7th Annual International Meeting for Autism Research; London, England. 2008. May, [Google Scholar]
  18. Cunningham AB, Schreibman L, Stahmer AC. Integrating treatment categories for children with autism. Paper presented at the 9th Annual International Meeting for Autism Research; Philadelphia, PA. 2010. May, [Google Scholar]
  19. Dawson G, Rogers S, Munson J, Smith M, Winter J, Greenson J, Donaldson A, Varley J. Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: the Early Start Denver Model. Pediatrics. 2010;125:e17–e23. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-0958. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Delprato DJ. Comparisons of discrete-trial and normalized behavioral intervention for young children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31:315–325. doi: 10.1023/a:1010747303957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dunlap G. Consensus, engagement, and family involvement for young children with autism. JASH. 1999;24:222–225. [Google Scholar]
  22. Eikeseth S, Smith T, Jahr E, Eldevik S. Intensive behavioral treatment at school for 4- to 7-year-old children with autism: a 1-year comparison controlled study. Behav Modif. 2002;26:49–68. doi: 10.1177/0145445502026001004. (Special Issue: Autism, Part 2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Forehand R, Kotchick BA. Behavioral parent training: current challenges and potential solutions. J Child Fam Stud. 2002;11:377–384. [Google Scholar]
  24. Garland AF, Hurlburt MS, Hawley KM. Examining psychotherapy processes in a services research context. Clin Psychol. 2006;31:30–46. [Google Scholar]
  25. Greenspan SI, Weider S. Developmental patterns and outcomes in infants and children with disorders in relating and communicating: a chart review of 200 cases of children with autistic spectrum diagnosis. J Dev Learn Disord. 1997;1:87–141. [Google Scholar]
  26. Handleman JS, Harris SL. Preschool Education Programs for Children with Autism. PRO-ED, Inc; Austin, TX: 2001. [Google Scholar]
  27. Harris SL, Handleman JS, editors. Preschool Education Programs for Children with Autism. PRO-ED; Austin, TX: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  28. Horton L. The father’s role in behavioral parent training: a review. J Clin Child Psychol. 1984;13:274–279. [Google Scholar]
  29. Hough RL, Landsverk JA, Karno M, Burnam MA. Utilization of health and mental services by Los Angeles Mexican Americans and non-Hispanic Whites. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44:702–709. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800200028005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Howard JS, Sparkman CR, Cohen HG, Green G, Stanislaw H. A comparison of intensive behavior analytic and eclectic treatments for young children with autism. Res Dev Disabil. 2005;26:359–383. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2004.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Howlin P, Goode S, Hutton J, Rutter M. Adult outcome for children with autism. J Child Psych Psychiatry. 2004;45:212–229. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00215.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ingersoll B, Dvortcsak A. Including parent training in the early childhood special education curriculum for children with autism spectrum disorders. J Posit Behav Interv. 2006;8:79–87. [Google Scholar]
  33. Ingersoll B, Dvortcsak A. Teaching Social Communication to Children with Autism: A Practitioner’s Guide to Parent Training. The Guilford Press; New York, NY: 2010. [Google Scholar]
  34. Ingersoll B, Stahmer AC, Schreibman L. Differential treatment outcomes for children with autistic spectrum disorder based on level of peer social avoidance. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31:343–349. doi: 10.1023/a:1010703521704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Ingersoll B, Dvortcsak A, Whalen C, Sikora D. The effects of a developmental, social-pragmatic language intervention on rate of expressive language production in young children with autistic spectrum disorders. Focus Autism Other Dev Disabil. 2005;20:213–222. [Google Scholar]
  36. Iovannone R, Dunlap G, Huber H, Kincaid D. Effective educational practices for students with autism spectrum disorders. Focus Autism Other Dev Disabil. 2003;18:150–165. [Google Scholar]
  37. Koegel RL, Koegel LK, Surratt A. Language intervention and disruptive behavior in preschool children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 1992;22:141–153. doi: 10.1007/BF01058147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Koegel LK, Koegel RL, Shoshan Y, McNerney E. Pivotal response intervention: II. Preliminary long-term outcomes data. J Assoc Pers Severe Handicaps. 1999;24:186–198. [Google Scholar]
  39. Lonigan CJ, Elbert JC, Johnson SB. Empirically supported psychosocial interventions for children: an overview. J Clin Child Psychol. 1998;27:138–145. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2702_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lowenthal B. Early childhood inclusion in the United States. Early Child Dev Care. 1999;150:17–32. [Google Scholar]
  41. MacPhee D, Fritz J, Miller-Heyl J. Ethnic variations in personal social networks and parenting. Child Dev. 1996;67:3278–3295. [Google Scholar]
  42. Mandell DS, Listerud J, Levy SE, Pinto-Martin JA. Race differences in the age of diagnosis among Medicaid-eligible children with autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41:1447–1453. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200212000-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Margison FR, Barkham M, Evans C, McGrath G, Clark JM, Audin K, Connell J. Measurement and psychotherapy: evidence-based practice and practice-based evidence. Br J Psychiatry. 2000;177:123–130. doi: 10.1192/bjp.177.2.123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. McCabe KM, Clark R, Barnett D. Family protective factors among urban African American youths. J Clin Child Psychol. 1999;28:137–150. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2802_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. McCabe K, Yeh M, Garland AF, Lau AS, Chavez G. The GANA program: a tailoring approach to adapting parent child interaction therapy for Mexican Americans. Educ Treat Child. 2005;28:111–129. [Google Scholar]
  46. McGee GG, Daly T, Jacobs HA. The Walden Preschool. In: Harris SL, Handleman JS, editors. Preschool education programs for children with autism. Pro-Ed; Austin, TX: 1994. pp. 127–162. [Google Scholar]
  47. McGee GG, Morrier M, Daly T. An incidental teaching approach to early intervention for toddlers with autism. J Assoc Pers Severe Handicaps. 1999;24:133–146. [Google Scholar]
  48. McGee GG, Morrier MJ, Daly T. The Walden Preschool. In: Handleman JS, Harris SL, editors. Preschool Education Programs for Children with Autism. PRO-ED; Austin, TX: 2000. pp. 157–190. [Google Scholar]
  49. National Research Council. Educating Children with Autism. Committee on Educational Interventions for Children with Autism. Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education, National Academy Press; Washington, DC: 2001. [Google Scholar]
  50. Panerai S, Farrante L, Zingale M. Benefits of the Treatment and Education of Autistic and Communication Handicapped Children (TEACCH) programme as compared with a non-specific approach. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2002;46:318–327. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.2002.00388.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Rogers SJ, Dawson G. Early start Denver model for young children with autism: promoting language, learning, and engagement. The Guildford Press; New York: 2009. [Google Scholar]
  52. Rogers SJ, Vismara LA. Evidence-based comprehensive treatments for early autism. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2008;3:8–38. doi: 10.1080/15374410701817808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Schreibman L. Intensive behavioral/psychoeducational treatments for autism: research needs and future directions. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30:373–378. doi: 10.1023/a:1005535120023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Schreibman L, editor. The Science and Fiction of Autism. Harvard University Press; Cambridge, MA: 2005. [Google Scholar]
  55. Schreibman L, Koegel RL. Fostering self-management: parent delivered pivotal response training for children with autistic disorder. In: Hibbs E, Jensen P, editors. Psychosocial Treatments for Child and Adolescent Disorders: Empirically based Strategies for Clinical Practice. American Psychological Association; Washington, D.C: 1996. pp. 525–552. [Google Scholar]
  56. Schreibman L, Stahmer AC, Cestone Barlett V, Dufek S. Brief report: toward refinement of a predictive behavioral profile for treatment outcome in children with autism. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2009;3:163–172. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2008.04.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Schwartz IS, Sandall SR, McBride BJ, Boulware GL. Project DATA (Developmentally appropriate treatment for autism): an inclusive school based approach to educating young children with autism. Top Early Child Spec Educ. 2004;24:156–168. [Google Scholar]
  58. Sherer MR, Schreibman L. Client treatment matching for children with autism. In: Schreibman L Chair, editor. Linking Assessment to Treatment for Children with Autism; Symposium conducted at the 25th Meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis; Chicago, IL. 1999. May, [Google Scholar]
  59. Sherer MR, Schreibman L. Individual behavioral profiles and predictors of treatment effectiveness for children with autism. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2005;73:525–538. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Shonkoff JP, Phillips PA, editors. From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Science of Early Childhood Development. National Academy Press; Washington, DC: 2000. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Siegel B. The World of the Autistic Child. Oxford University Press; New York: 1996. [Google Scholar]
  62. Sigman M, Ruskin E. Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down Syndrome, and developmental delays. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 1999;64:1–114. doi: 10.1111/1540-5834.00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Singh NN, Lancioni GE, Winton ASW, Fisher BC, Wahler RG, McAleavey K. Mindful parenting decreases aggression, noncompliance, and self-injury in children with autism. J Emot Behav Disord. 2006;14:169–177. [Google Scholar]
  64. Smith T, Groen AD, Wynn JW. Randomized trial of intensive early intervention for children with pervasive developmental disorder. Am J Ment Retard. 2000;105:269–285. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2000)105<0269:RTOIEI>2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Stahmer AC. The basic structure of community early intervention programs for children with autism: provider descriptions. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:1344–1354. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0284-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Stahmer AC, Ingersoll B. Inclusive programming for toddlers with autistic spectrum disorders: outcomes from the Children’s Toddler School. J Posit Behav Interv. 2004;6 [Google Scholar]
  67. Stahmer AC, Collings NM, Palinkas LA. Early intervention practices for children with autism: descriptions from community providers. Focus Autism Other Dev Disabil. 2005;20:66–79. doi: 10.1177/10883576050200020301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Stahmer AC, Akshoomoff NA, Cunningham AB. Inclusion for toddlers with autism spectrum disorders: the first ten years of a community program. Autism. doi: 10.1177/1362361310392253. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Stevens MC, Fein DA, Dunn M, Allen D, Waterhouse LH, Feinstein C, Rapin I. Subgroups of children with autism by cluster analysis: a longitudinal examination. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39:346–352. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200003000-00017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Strain PS, Cordisco LK. LEAP Preschool. In: Harris SL, Handleman JS, editors. Preschool Education Programs for Children with Autism. PRO-ED; Austin, TX: 1994. pp. 225–244. [Google Scholar]
  71. Strain P, McGee G, WKF . Early Childhood Inclusion: Focus on Change. Paul H. Brooks; Baltimore, MD: 2001. Inclusion of children with autism in early intervention settings: an examination of rationale, myths, and procedures. [Google Scholar]
  72. Tager-Flusberg H, Paul R, Lord C. Language and communication in autism. In: Cohen D, Volkmar F, editors. Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders. Vol. 1. John Wiley & Sons, Inc; New Jersey: 2005. pp. 335–364. [Google Scholar]
  73. Thurm A, Lord C, Lee LC, Newschaffer C. Predictors of language acquisition in preschool children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:1721–1734. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0300-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Venter A, Lord C, Schopler E. A follow-up study of high-functioning autistic children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1992;33:489–507. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb00887.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Webster-Stratton C. Predictors of treatment outcome in parent training for conduct disordered children. Behav Ther. 1985;16:223–243. [Google Scholar]
  76. Winter J. Father involvement in parent training interventions for children with autism: effects of tailoring treatment to meet the unique needs of fathers. Dissertation Abstracts International. 2006;67:1751B. [Google Scholar]
  77. Yoder P, Compton D. Identifying predictors of treatment response. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2004;10:162–168. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Yoder P, Stone WL. Randomized comparison of two communication interventions for preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2006a;74:426–435. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.3.426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Yoder P, Stone WL. A randomized comparison of the effect of two prelinguistic communication intervention on the acquisition of spoken communication in preschoolers with ASD. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2006b;49:698–711. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/051). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Yoder PJ, Warren SW. Maternal responsivity predicts the prelinguistic communication intervention that facilitates generalized intentional communication. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1998;41:1207–1219. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4105.1207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES