Skip to main content
Elsevier Sponsored Documents logoLink to Elsevier Sponsored Documents
. 2014 Jun 1;75(11):901–908. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.003

Variation in Serotonin Transporter Expression Modulates Fear-Evoked Hemodynamic Responses and Theta-Frequency Neuronal Oscillations in the Amygdala

Christopher Barkus a, Samantha J Line a, Anna Huber a, Liliana Capitao a, Joao Lima a, Katie Jennings b, John Lowry c, Trevor Sharp d, David M Bannerman a, Stephen B McHugh a,
PMCID: PMC4032572  PMID: 24120093

Abstract

Background

Gene association studies detect an influence of natural variation in the 5-hydroxytryptamine transporter (5-HTT) gene on multiple aspects of individuality in brain function, ranging from personality traits through to susceptibility to psychiatric disorders such as anxiety and depression. The neural substrates of these associations are unknown. Human neuroimaging studies suggest modulation of the amygdala by 5-HTT variation, but this hypothesis is controversial and unresolved, and difficult to investigate further in humans.

Methods

We used a mouse model in which the 5-HTT is overexpressed throughout the brain and recorded hemodynamic responses (using a novel in vivo voltammetric monitoring method, analogous to blood oxygen level–dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging) and local field potentials during Pavlovian fear conditioning.

Results

Increased 5-HTT expression impaired, but did not prevent, fear learning and significantly reduced amygdala hemodynamic responses to aversive cues. Increased 5-HTT expression was also associated with reduced theta oscillations, which were a feature of aversive cue presentation in controls. Moreover, in control mice, but not those with high 5-HTT expression, there was a strong correlation between theta power and the amplitude of the hemodynamic response.

Conclusions

Direct experimental manipulation of 5-HTT expression levels throughout the brain markedly altered fear learning, amygdala hemodynamic responses, and neuronal oscillations.

Key Words: serotonin transporter, fMRI, amygdala, fear, tissue oxygen, theta oscillations


The serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine; 5-HT) transporter (5-HTT) is a key determinant of brain 5-HT function as it controls 5-HT availability at the synapse. There is a large natural variation in 5-HTT expression in the human population, approximately threefold between individuals (1). Current thinking is that this variation, in large part driven by the 16 or more 5-HTT gene polymorphisms discovered to date, is the source of large individual differences in personality, behavior, and brain disorder susceptibility. In this regard, gene association studies have reported that a common insertion/deletion polymorphism (producing long (l) and short (s) variants, respectively) in the 5-HTT gene upstream promoter region (5-HTTLPR) generates high (l/l) and low (s/s) expressing variants, with the s/s genotype conferring increased risk for anxiety-related traits (2) and affective disorders, especially when combined with environmental factors 3, 4, and the l/l genotype conferring reduced risk. As is common with gene-association studies, these findings are confounded by failed replications 5, 6 and need to be underpinned by a convincing neural substrate.

An attractive theory is that 5-HTT variation has an impact on emotionality and affective disorder susceptibility through modulation of the amygdala. This idea derives largely from human functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies, which detect lower amygdala blood oxygen level–dependent (BOLD) responses to aversive cues in l/l versus s carriers 7, 8. However, a recent meta-analysis of available published and unpublished data sets found that the association between 5-HTT variation and amygdala reactivity was of borderline statistical significance (with no significant genotypic difference in 21 of 34 samples) and resolvable only through further large-scale, and thus impractical, imaging studies to control for study design and subject heterogeneity (9).

At the heart of the 5-HTTLPR debate lie two fundamental and as yet unanswered questions: 1) Does variation in 5-HTT expression influence how aversive cues are processed, and, if so, 2) what are the underlying neuronal mechanisms? Here, as an alternative to gene-association studies, we used a mouse model of genetically altered 5-HTT expression to address these questions. 5-HTT overexpressing mice (5-HTTOE) have two- to threefold greater 5-HTT expression than wild-type (WT) mice (10), mirroring the natural variation in humans 1, 11, 12, with 5-HTTOE mice approximating the human l/l genotype (10). Here we investigated aversive learning and amygdala activity in 5-HTTOE and WT mice during Pavlovian fear conditioning.

We recorded amygdala activity in two ways. First, to allow direct comparison with human neuroimaging, we recorded amygdala hemodynamic responses in behaving 5-HTTOE and WT mice. BOLD fMRI cannot be performed in freely moving rodents but recent advances in tissue oxygen (TO2) voltammetry offer equivalent hemodynamic measurements. TO2 signals are driven by the same neurovascular mechanisms as the BOLD signal and therefore provide a close hemodynamic surrogate, via intracerebrally implanted carbon paste microelectrodes 13, 14, 15, 16. Recently, we have shown that TO2 signals in the amygdala display a brain-region-specific discrimination between aversive and neutral cues during fear conditioning in rats (17). Simultaneously in the same mice, we measured neuronal activity in the form of local field potentials (LFPs).

Here we show that increased 5-HTT expression impairs fear learning and reduces amygdala hemodynamic responses and theta oscillations evoked by aversive cues. Finally, we show that the hemodynamic response amplitude is strongly correlated with theta oscillatory power in WT mice, but not in 5-HTTOE mice, suggesting a plausible neural basis for the 5-HTTLPR-related differences in human BOLD signals.

Methods and Materials

For a full description of the methods, see Supplement 1.

Subjects

Male 5-HTTOE and WT mice were generated on a CBA × C57BL/6J background, as described previously (10). Mice were approximately 5 months old at the time of surgery. The experiments were conducted in accordance with the United Kingdom Animals Scientific Procedures Act (1986) under project license 30/2561.

Surgery

Mice were surgically implanted with a carbon paste electrode (CPE, 200-μm diameter) into the basolateral amygdala to measure TO2 and a silver electrode (125-μm diameter) into the basolateral amygdala of the contralateral hemisphere to measure LFPs, as described previously 14, 17. Right/left electrode positions for TO2/LFP recordings were counterbalanced across mice. Coordinates were −1.35 mm anterior/posterior, ±3.10 mm medial/lateral, and −5.00 mm dorsal/ventral, relative to bregma. Auxiliary and reference electrodes (200-µm diameter silver wire) were implanted into parietal cortex. A pedestal plug (MS363, Plastics One, Roanoke, Virginia) was secured with dental cement and skull screws. Mice were allowed to recover for at least 7 days after surgery.

TO2 Measurements

TO2 signals were measured using constant potential amperometry, as described previously 14, 17, 18. When a constant potential (−650 mV relative to reference) is applied to an electrode implanted into the brain, O2 is electrochemically reduced on the electrode’s surface, inducing a current directly proportional to the local O2 concentration (19). Like the fMRI-BOLD signal, the TO2 signal is determined primarily by changes in local cerebral blood flow 16, 20.

Fear Conditioning Procedures

Two different fear conditioning paradigms were used. A separate cohort of unoperated mice (n = 11 per genotype) were tested on a standard rodent fear conditioning paradigm to see if 5-HTT overexpression affected fear learning. Mice received two training trials (30-second tone followed by .3 mA, .5-second shock) in one context followed 24 hours later by two tone-alone presentations in a novel context.

The operated mice (n = 42; 22 WT, 20 5-HTTOE) were tested on a discriminative fear-conditioning paradigm. This behavioral paradigm differs from the standard fear-conditioning paradigm described above in that mice must learn to discriminate between two distinct auditory cues (tone and white noise), with one cue paired with footshock (conditioned stimulus; CS+) and the other cue never paired with footshock (CS–). This discriminative approach is commonplace in human fMRI 21, 22 and rodent electrophysiologic studies of fear 23, 24. Because any stimulus could potentially evoke amygdala activity, the CS– provides the necessary nonaversive control stimulus with which to compare CS+ evoked responses, akin to a subtraction task in fMRI.

Discriminative fear conditioning was performed over 5 consecutive days. On Day 1 (pre-exposure), mice were presented with the auditory cues (five 2900-Hz tones and five white noise stimuli, both 30 seconds in duration, presented in pseudorandom order), with no shocks administered. On Days 2 through 4 (training), the mice were placed into a different context and presented with the same auditory cues, but now one cue (tone or white noise, counterbalanced across mice) was always paired with coterminating footshock (.3 mA, .5 seconds), whereas the other cue was not. On Day 5 (fear memory recall), mice were placed into a novel context and presented with the auditory cues with no shocks administered. During all days, cue-evoked freezing behavior and amygdala TO2 responses and LFPs were recorded simultaneously in the same mice.

Data Analysis

Behavior was recorded with a video camera and freezing was measured using Videotrack (Viewpoint, Champagne Au Mont D’Or, France) or NIH Image (25). A freezing “difference score” was calculated as follows: percent freezing during the 30-second cue presentation minus percent freezing during the 30 seconds before cue presentation (i.e., positive freezing scores indicate increased freezing to the cue and negative freezing scores indicate decreased freezing to the cue relative to the precue period).

Cue-evoked TO2 responses were calculated by subtracting the mean TO2 signal in the 5 seconds before CS onset (i.e., baseline) from the TO2 signal during the 30-second CS presentation. This yielded a 30-second ΔTO2 signal, which was then divided into fifteen 2-second timebins (i.e., 0–2, 2–4, 4–6 . . . 28–30 seconds), with each data point equal to the mean value during each 2-second timebin (Figure S2 in Supplement 1) (17).

LFPs were band-pass filtered between 1 and 45 Hz. Power spectra were calculated using a fast Fourier transform over the first 10 seconds of CS presentation and were averaged over the five CS+ versus the five CS– trials on each day for each mouse. To compare across mice, spectra were normalized by expressing the power in each frequency bin as a proportion of the total power between 1 and 45 Hz (Figure S2 in Supplement 1).

Histology

Electrode placements were determined at the end of the experiment. Mice were transcardially perfused with physiologic saline (.9% NaCl), followed by 10% formol saline (10% formalin in .9% NaCl). Coronal sections (40 μm) were cut on a freezing microtome and stained with cresyl violet. Only mice with confirmed electrode placements in the basolateral amygdala were used in the TO2 and LFP analyses (Figure S1 in Supplement 1).

Serotonin transporter binding of [3H]citalopram in the amygdala was assessed using autoradiography in naive mice (5-HTTOE: n = 6; WT: n = 5; aged 3–6 months). High-performance liquid chromatography with electrochemical detection was used to measure amygdala tissue levels of 5-HT in a separate cohort of mice (n = 5 per group) (26).

Statistical Procedures

Data were analyzed using t tests, analysis of variance (ANOVA), or Pearson correlation. The familywise error was set at α = .05. Unless otherwise stated, all graphs show the mean ± 1 standard error of the mean (SEM).

Results

Amygdala 5-HTT Expression Is Higher in 5-HTTOE Than WT Mice

First, we established that amygdala 5-HTT expression, measured by [3H]citalopram binding, was 2.7-fold higher in 5-HTTOE compared with WT mice (121.4 ± 4.3 vs. 44.4 ± 5.5 fmol/mg; t9 = 11.2, p < .001). Moreover, high-performance liquid chromatography revealed lower amygdala 5-HT tissue levels in 5-HTTOE compared with WT mice (4.4 ± .9 versus 7.6 ± 1.0 pmol/sample; t8 = 2.5, p < .05; Table S1 in Supplement 1) 10, 27. Thus the genetic modification led to higher 5-HTT expression in the amygdala and this, in turn, resulted in lower 5-HT tissue levels.

5-HTT Overexpression Impairs Conditioned Fear

Next, we investigated whether variation in 5-HTT expression affected aversive learning, using a standard rodent fear-conditioning paradigm. During training, unconditioned responses to the tone and shock did not differ between WT and 5-HTTOE mice (all Fs < 1, p > .5). In addition, WT and 5-HTTOE mice showed indistinguishable acoustic startle responses (see Supplement 1). These results suggest normal ability to hear the tone and normal shock responsivity in 5-HTTOE mice. However, during the fear memory recall test, WT mice froze significantly more during the conditioned tone than the pretone period, whereas this did not reach significance in 5-HTTOE mice (genotype × phase interaction: F1,20 = 8.3, p = .009; tone versus pretone, WT: p < .001; 5-HTTOE: p = .07; Figure 1A). When a difference score was calculated (tone freezing – pretone freezing), WTs froze significantly more than 5-HTTOE mice (t20 = 2.9; p = .01; Figure 1B), thereby demonstrating impaired fear learning in 5-HTTOE mice.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Standard fear conditioning experiment. (A) Wild-type (WT) mice exhibit significantly increased freezing (Fz) to a conditioned auditory tone (compared with the pretone period) during fear memory recall, whereas this did not reach significance for 5-hydroxytryptamine transporter overexpressing (OE) mice (genotype × phase interaction: F1,20 = 8.3, p = .009; pretone versus tone, WT: p < .001; OE: p = .07). (B) When pretone Fz was subtracted from the tone-evoked freezing, WTs exhibited significantly higher Fz than 5-HTTOE mice. *p ≤ .01. Fz diff, freezing difference score.

5-HTT Overexpression Reduces Amygdala Hemodynamic Responses to Aversive Cues

Next, to determine if variation in 5-HTT expression affected amygdala activity in mice as has been suggested by human neuroimaging studies, we investigated TO2 responses and neuronal oscillations during discriminative fear conditioning. Before any cue-shock pairings, the mice were pre-exposed to the auditory cues. Importantly, there were no differences between WT and 5-HTTOE mice in their behavioral, hemodynamic or electrophysiological responses to the auditory cues before training (no effect of genotype or interactions involving genotype; Figure S3 Supplement 1).

Fear conditioning led to marked changes in the amplitude and shape of the cue-evoked TO2 responses in the amygdala, with strikingly different responses in 5-HTTOE compared to WT mice. In WT mice, CS+ evoked TO2 responses increased in amplitude relative to CS– evoked responses over the course of training (Figure 2A). Specifically, the peak CS+ response (occurring approximately 8–10 seconds after CS+ onset) was higher than the peak CS– response. In contrast, in 5-HTTOE mice, peak CS+ and CS– evoked responses did not differ and, by the end of training, CS+ evoked TO2 responses were markedly lower than CS– evoked responses during the last approximately 10 to 15 seconds of CS presentation (Figure 2B). Importantly, CS+ evoked responses were higher in WT than 5-HTTOE mice.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Cue-evoked tissue oxygen (TO2) signals in wild-type (WT) and 5-hydroxytryptamine transporter (5-HTT) overexpressing (OE) mice during discriminative fear conditioning for the pre-exposure (Pre-ex) day, the 3 days of training, and the first trial of fear memory recall. (A) In WT mice, conditioned stimulus (CS)+ evoked responses (red) increased relative to CS– evoked responses (blue) over training. (B) In 5-HTTOE mice, CS+ evoked responses were not higher than CS– evoked responses and had a pronounced “negative tail” by Training Day 3. (C) Mean CS+ (but not CS–) evoked TO2 responses were significantly higher in WTs than 5-HTTOEs by Training Day 3. (D) Mean TO2 responses were significantly higher in WTs than 5-HTTOEs during fear memory recall. *p < .05.

Analysis (ANOVA: genotype2 × day3 × CS type2 × timebin15 × S33) confirmed that TO2 responses were significantly greater in WT than 5-HTTOE mice by Training Day 3 (genotype × CS type × day interaction: F2,60 = 3.9; p = .03; genotype × CS type interaction for day 3: F1,31 = 7.0; p = .01; Figure 2C). This was driven by genotypic differences in CS+ evoked TO2 responses (p < .05), with no genotypic differences in CS- evoked TO2 responses (p = .6; Figure 2C). Thus, fear-evoked TO2 responses in the amygdala were significantly higher in WT than 5-HTTOE mice during training.

A strong genotypic difference was also evident during fear memory recall. WT mice exhibited higher TO2 signals than 5-HTTOE mice (main effect of genotype: F1,31 = 8.9; p = .005), due to higher CS+ evoked responses (genotype × CS type × timebin interaction: F14,434 = 2.1; p = .01; with higher CS+ responses in WT than 5-HTTOE mice 10 to 30 seconds after cue onset, p < .05; Figure 2D). There were no genotypic differences in CS– evoked responses at any time point (Figure S4 in Supplement 1). Thus, 5-HTT overexpression resulted in lower amygdala hemodynamic responses specifically to aversive cues.

5-HTT Overexpression Impairs Discriminative Fear Learning but This Is Ameliorated with Additional Training

Behaviorally, 5-HTTOE mice exhibited impaired learning during discriminative fear conditioning, confirming the behavioral phenotype found with the standard fear-conditioning paradigm. On Training Day 2, WT mice froze significantly more than 5-HTTOE mice during the first CS+ presentation of the session (CS type × genotype interaction: F1,40 = 4.7, p = .04; CS+: WT > 5-HTTOE mice, p = .03; Figure 3A). Thus, the impairment in 5-HTTOE mice was seen at approximately the same time point during the standard and discriminative fear conditioning paradigms (i.e., after 1 day of training).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Cue-evoked freezing behavior in wild-type (WT) and 5-hydroxytryptamine transporter (5-HTT) overexpressing (OE) mice during discriminative fear conditioning. (A) WTs froze more than 5-HTTOE mice during the first conditioned stimulus (CS)+ presentation on Training Day 2. (B) WT mice discriminated between the CS+ and CS– on Training Days 2 and 3 and the fear memory recall day, but discrimination was seen in 5-HTTOE mice on Training Day 3 only, with a trend on the fear memory recall day. Each bar represents the mean freezing difference score (Fz diff) ± 1 SEM. *p < .05; **p = .07. n.s., not significant.

However, with additional training, 5-HTTOE mice learned to discriminate between the CS+ and CS–, and they were indistinguishable from WT mice in terms of freezing levels by Training Day 3 (Figure 3B). Analysis of freezing responses over all training trials (ANOVA: genotype2 × day3 × CS type2 × trial5 × S42) revealed that mice froze significantly more during CS+ than CS– trials overall (main effect of CS type: F1,40 = 6.3; p = .02) and showed greater discrimination later in training (CS type × day interaction: F2,62 = 6.8; p = .002). However, whereas WT mice showed significant discrimination between the CS+ and CS– by Training Day 2, 5-HTTOE mice only discriminated on Training Day 3. Thus, there was a behavioral impairment in the 5-HTTOE mice initially but their CS+/CS– discrimination was comparable to WT mice by Training Day 3. This was in marked contrast to the hemodynamic signals, which were still different between the two genotypes by the end of training.

5-HTT Overexpression Reduces Theta Oscillations to Aversive Cues

The different amygdala hemodynamic responses seen in WT versus 5-HTTOE mice were accompanied by genotypic differences in neuronal oscillations in the simultaneously recorded LFP. In WTs, aversive (CS+) but not neutral (CS–) cues evoked a shift from delta-dominant (1–4 Hz) to theta-dominant (5–10 Hz) neural oscillations (Figure 4A; Figure S5 in Supplement 1). This delta-to-theta shift was significantly reduced in 5-HTTOE mice, with markedly lower theta power between 7 and 10 Hz (see Figure 4BD; Figure S6 in Supplement 1).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Cue-evoked neuronal oscillations in wild-type (WT) and 5-hydroxytryptamine transporter (5-HTT) overexpressing (OE) mice during discriminative fear conditioning. Representative spectrograms for a WT (A) and a 5-HTTOE (B) mouse on Training Day 3 showing the 30 seconds before and the 30 seconds during conditioned stimulus (CS) presentation (CS+: leftmost panel, CS– center left panel), with simultaneously recorded tissue oxygen (TO2) responses overlaid in red (CS+) or green (CS–). The CS+ evoked a shift from delta-dominant to theta-dominant oscillations in WT mice, which was attenuated in 5-HTTOE mice. (C) Power spectra (expressed as a proportion of the total spectral power between 1–45 Hz) for CS+ trials in WT (blue) and 5-HTTOE mice (red), showing lower delta activity (1–4 Hz) and higher theta activity (5–10 Hz) in WT mice. (D) CS+ evoked theta power was higher in WT than 5-HTTOE mice during training (T1–T3) and fear memory recall (FMR) day but not during pre-exposure (PE). (E) As in panel D but showing the summed theta power between 7 and 10 Hz. *p < .05; **p < .01. Freq, frequency; n.s., not significant; PSD (prop.), proportional power spectral density.

Analysis of theta oscillations over all days of training (ANOVA: genotype2 × day5 × CS type2 × S25) revealed significantly higher theta power in WT compared with 5-HTTOE mice (main effect of genotype: F1,23 = 5.2, p = .03. Analyses of individual days revealed that changes in theta power evoked by aversive cues (CS+) were significantly higher in WT than 5-HTTOE mice during the 3 training days and the fear memory recall test (all Fs > 4.8, p < .04; Figure 4D,E), but there were no differences during the pre-exposure session (F < 1.1, p > .3). There were no significant genotypic differences in CS- evoked theta power on any day. Thus, 5-HTT overexpression reduced theta oscillations specifically to aversive cues.

Theta Power Is Correlated with Amygdala TO2 Responses in WT Mice

Finally, to investigate the relationship between theta oscillations and TO2 responses, we plotted CS+ evoked changes in theta power (calculated by dividing the sum of theta power by the sum of delta power) against the mean TO2 response (i.e., the mean TO2 signal during each 30s CS+ presentation), collected over the 3 days of training and the fear memory recall test. The plots revealed that, in WT mice, the mean CS+ evoked TO2 amplitude was significantly correlated with CS+ evoked changes in theta power (r = .52, n = 40 observations; t38 = 3.8; p = .001, Figure 5A). This correlation was strikingly absent in 5-HTTOE mice (r = −.32, n = 48 observations; Figure 5B). This suggests that 1) the shift to theta-dominant oscillations evoked by aversive cues is a key mechanism underlying the amygdala hemodynamic response and 2) this delta-to-theta shift is acutely sensitive to variation in 5-HTT expression.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

(A) For aversive cue (conditioned stimulus [CS]+) evoked responses, the ratio of theta-to-delta oscillatory power was significantly correlated with tissue oxygen (TO2) amplitude in wild-type (WT) mice. (B) This correlation was absent in 5-hydroxytryptamine transporter overexpressing (OE) mice. Note that each data point represents the mean theta:delta ratio versus mean TO2 response amplitude for each mouse on each of the 3 training days and the fear memory recall day.

Discussion

Here we demonstrate that direct, experimental manipulation of 5-HTT expression in mice, genetically engineered to model physiologically relevant increases in 5-HTT expression levels in humans, alters amygdala activity and amygdala-dependent behavior. Increased 5-HTT expression throughout the brain resulted in impaired fear learning, and aversive cues evoked significantly lower amygdala hemodynamic responses and theta oscillations in 5-HTTOE mice compared with WT controls. Moreover, theta oscillatory power and hemodynamic responses were highly correlated in WT mice but this correlation was absent in 5-HTTOE mice. Thus, modulation of theta oscillations provides a potential neuronal mechanism by which 5-HTT expression influences amygdala hemodynamic responses and fear learning.

Increased 5-HTT Expression Is Associated with Reduced Amygdala Activity

Despite more than 30 human neuroimaging studies, the influence of the 5-HTTLPR genotype on amygdala BOLD responses remains equivocal. There may be many reasons why human neuroimaging studies have failed to resolve this issue (9). For example, 5-HTT expression levels are rarely measured in human studies, and polymorphic variation in the 5-HTTLPR is only one of several factors with the potential to influence 5-HTT expression (28). Indeed, although there is a reliable influence of the 5-HTTLPR on 5-HTT expression in vitro, several neuroimaging studies have failed to replicate this finding in vivo 29, 30. The strength of our approach is that we have directly manipulated and measured 5-HTT expression levels, which are two- to threefold higher in 5-HTTOE than WT mice (10). Therefore, our finding of significantly blunted fear-evoked amygdala TO2 responses in 5-HTTOE mice demonstrates unequivocally that 5-HTT expression influences amygdala hemodynamic responses.

Increased 5-HTT Expression Is Associated with Reduced Theta Oscillations

Importantly, the current study found that 5-HTT expression influenced theta oscillations as well as hemodynamic responses. Specifically, 5-HTTOE mice exhibited reduced theta power evoked by fearful stimuli. Theta oscillations are consistently observed in the amygdala during fear conditioning 31, 32, 33 and can be intrinsically generated by amygdala neurons 32, 34. However, it is debated whether amygdala theta, as observed at a population level in the LFP, is locally generated or volume conducted from neighboring regions such as the hippocampus (see Supplement 1 for further discussion). Without concomitant single-unit recordings, caution is merited in attributing the source of these oscillations to the amygdala.

Nevertheless, current thinking is that theta oscillations may enhance plasticity locally or across a wider network of structures to facilitate the encoding and/or subsequent consolidation of fear memories 35, 36. Theta-burst stimulation patterns elicit synaptic plasticity in amygdala tissue slices and, importantly, simultaneous activation of 5-HT receptors during theta burst stimulation transforms short-term into long-term potentiation (37). Moreover, both footshock and aversive conditioned stimuli evoke 5-HT release in the amygdala 38, 39, 40, suggesting that 5-HT input may act to signal aversive events. Thus, during fear conditioning, theta oscillations, at least in part driven by 5-HT input, may facilitate the process whereby an initially neutral stimulus is transformed into an aversive conditioned stimulus, capable of evoking a powerful and long-lasting emotional response.

Increased 5-HTT Expression is Associated with Reduced Fear Learning

Consistent with this, 5-HTTOE mice exhibited markedly impaired fear memory recall in the standard rodent fear conditioning paradigm and were also impaired in the discriminative paradigm after one day of training, although they learned the discrimination with continued training. This fear learning deficit mirrors the observation that human l/l homozygotes show lower skin conductance responses than s carriers following the pairing of simple visual stimuli with electric shock 41, 42. It is also consistent with the reduced amygdala activation seen in the present study. The importance of the amygdala for fear learning is well established in both animal lesion and human imaging studies 43, 44, 45, although the amygdala operates in concert with several other brain areas to mediate fear behavior. Nevertheless, deficits in fear learning in 5-HTTOE mice are consistent with the reduced amygdala activity found in these animals.

However, increased 5-HTT expression did not completely block discriminative fear conditioning but reduced the rate of learning. Notably, there were no behavioral differences between the groups by the end of training. Importantly, at this stage, there were still robust differences in the amygdala hemodynamic responses and theta oscillations between the two genotypes. At one level this is important because it means that the genotypic differences in amygdala activity cannot be explained by differences in the behavior of the two groups at the time of recording. Nevertheless, there is a disconnection between amygdala hemodynamic responses and freezing behavior. However, this result accords with other rodent studies showing that amygdala neuronal activity does not provide a simple readout of freezing behavior 23, 24. Moreover, the 5-HTTLPR-related differences in amygdala BOLD signals in humans are seen in the absence of any behavioral differences in the emotional face matching task or differences in anxiety-related traits 7, 8, 46.

However, note also that amygdala TO2 responses in 5-HTTOE mice clearly discriminated between the aversive (CS+) and nonaversive (CS–) cues by the end of training, except in the opposite direction to WTs. 5-HTTOE mice exhibited markedly lower signals during the last 10 to 15 seconds of the CS+ cue compared with both the pre-CS+ baseline and CS– trials (see Figure 2B). This is a surprising, but robust, result. This is unlikely to be a “ceiling effect” or an artifact of the difference measure used (i.e., the ΔTO2 change from baseline) because the decrease below baseline did not occur on CS- trials. Moreover, it emerged over the course of fear learning and was most evident on Training Day 3, when the 5-HTTOE mice also showed successful behavioral discrimination between the CS+ and CS–, comparable to WT mice. Thus, amygdala TO2 responses in 5-HTTOE mice did discriminate between the cues but in a qualitatively different way from WT mice.

Theta Oscillations Are Correlated with Amygdala TO2 Responses

This is the first study to show a relationship between theta oscillations and the amplitude of the amygdala hemodynamic response. Aversive cue-evoked changes in theta power were significantly correlated with TO2 amplitude in WT but not 5-HTTOE mice. In humans, aversive stimuli evoke increased theta oscillations in the amygdala, as measured by magnetoencephalography (47), as well as amygdala BOLD signals (8), which strengthens the link between theta oscillations and hemodynamic responses. However, the lack of correlation between the theta:delta ratio and the amygdala TO2 response amplitude in 5-HTTOE mice could reflect the possibility that hemodynamic measures are no longer accurately reporting the underlying neuronal activity in 5-HTTOE mice. In other words, in addition to affecting hemodynamic responses and theta oscillations, 5-HTT expression may influence the relationship between them. A detailed analysis of how 5-HTT expression influences neurovascular coupling is beyond the scope of this report but it merits further investigation. Moreover, in any case where neurovascular coupling is altered by a disease-relevant manipulation (such as a genetic polymorphism), this would represent a major challenge for interpreting human neuroimaging studies.

Whole-Brain Versus Amygdala-Specific Effects

Our data demonstrate that variation in 5-HTT expression has a significant effect on amygdala activity and fear learning. However, because 5-HTT expression is upregulated throughout the brains of 5-HTTOE mice (10), the behavioral and physiologic phenotype we report could be due to altered 5-HTT expression in brain regions outside the amygdala. Although this is an important caveat in the interpretation of our data, these mice were specifically generated to model the global variation in 5-HTT expression seen in humans, which is not restricted to the amygdala. Nevertheless, in the present study, the consequences of this whole-brain genetic manipulation are clearly manifest in altered amygdala signals.

Conclusions

To date, there is only equivocal evidence that 5-HTTLPR genotype affects amygdala hemodynamic responses in humans. Here we provide compelling evidence that whole-brain changes in 5-HTT expression influences amygdala activity and aversive cue processing in mice.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the Wellcome Trust (Grant Nos. 074385 and 087736).

We thank Greg Daubney for his assistance with histology.

All authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

Footnotes

Authors DMB and SBM contributed equally to this article.

Appendix A

Supplementary material cited in this article is available online at doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.003.

Appendix A. Supporting information

Supplementary data

mmc1.pdf (1.2MB, pdf)

References

  • 1.Lundberg J., Borg J., Halldin C., Farde L. A PET study on regional coexpression of 5-HT1A receptors and 5-HTT in the human brain. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;195:425–433. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0928-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lesch K.P., Bengel D., Heils A., Sabol S.Z., Greenberg B.D., Petri S., et al. Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science. 1996;274:1527–1531. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5292.1527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Munafo M.R., Clark T.G., Roberts K.H., Johnstone E.C. Neuroticism mediates the association of the serotonin transporter gene with lifetime major depression. Neuropsychobiology. 2006;53:1–8. doi: 10.1159/000089915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Caspi A., Sugden K., Moffitt T.E., Taylor A., Craig I.W., Harrington H., et al. Influence of life stress on depression: Moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science. 2003;301:386–389. doi: 10.1126/science.1083968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Surtees P.G., Wainwright N.W., Willis-Owen S.A., Luben R., Day N.E., Flint J. Social adversity, the serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) polymorphism and major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59:224–229. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.07.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Willis-Owen S.A., Turri M.G., Munafo M.R., Surtees P.G., Wainwright N.W., Brixey R.D., et al. The serotonin transporter length polymorphism, neuroticism, and depression: A comprehensive assessment of association. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58:451–456. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Hariri A.R., Drabant E.M., Munoz K.E., Kolachana B.S., Mattay V.S., Egan M.F., et al. A susceptibility gene for affective disorders and the response of the human amygdala. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:146–152. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.2.146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hariri A.R., Mattay V.S., Tessitore A., Kolachana B., Fera F., Goldman D., et al. Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala. Science. 2002;297:400–403. doi: 10.1126/science.1071829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Murphy S.E., Norbury R., Godlewska B.R., Cowen P.J., Mannie Z.M., Harmer C.J., et al. The effect of the serotonin transporter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) on amygdala function: A meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18:512–520. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Jennings K.A., Loder M.K., Sheward W.J., Pei Q., Deacon R.M., Benson M.A., et al. Increased expression of the 5-HT transporter confers a low-anxiety phenotype linked to decreased 5-HT transmission. J Neurosci. 2006;26:8955–8964. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5356-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Malison R.T., Price L.H., Berman R., van Dyck C.H., Pelton G.H., Carpenter L., et al. Reduced brain serotonin transporter availability in major depression as measured by [123I]-2 beta-carbomethoxy-3 beta-(4-iodophenyl)tropane and single photon emission computed tomography. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;44:1090–1098. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00272-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Mann J.J., Huang Y.Y., Underwood M.D., Kassir S.A., Oppenheim S., Kelly T.M., et al. A serotonin transporter gene promoter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and prefrontal cortical binding in major depression and suicide. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:729–738. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.8.729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Li J., Bravo D.S., Upton A.L., Gilmour G., Tricklebank M.D., Fillenz M., et al. Close temporal coupling of neuronal activity and tissue oxygen responses in rodent whisker barrel cortex. Eur J Neurosci. 2011;34:1983–1996. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07927.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.McHugh S.B., Fillenz M., Lowry J.P., Rawlins J.N., Bannerman D.M. Brain tissue oxygen amperometry in behaving rats demonstrates functional dissociation of dorsal and ventral hippocampus during spatial processing and anxiety. Eur J Neurosci. 2011;33:322–337. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07497.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Logothetis N.K. The ins and outs of fMRI signals. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:1230–1232. doi: 10.1038/nn1007-1230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lowry J.P., Griffin K., McHugh S.B., Lowe A.S., Tricklebank M., Sibson N.R. Real-time electrochemical monitoring of brain tissue oxygen: A surrogate for functional magnetic resonance imaging in rodents. Neuroimage. 2010;52:549–555. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.McHugh S.B., Marques-Smith A., Li J., Rawlins J.N., Lowry J., Conway M., et al. Hemodynamic responses in amygdala and hippocampus distinguish between aversive and neutral cues during Pavlovian fear conditioning in behaving rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2013;37:498–507. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bolger F.B., McHugh S.B., Bennett R., Li J., Ishiwari K., Francois J., et al. Characterisation of carbon paste electrodes for real-time amperometric monitoring of brain tissue oxygen. J Neurosci Methods. 2011;195:135–142. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.11.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hitchman M.L. John Wiley; New York: 1978. Measurement of Dissolved Oxygen. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Lowry J.P., Boutelle M.G., Fillenz M. Measurement of brain tissue oxygen at a carbon past electrode can serve as an index of increases in regional cerebral blood flow. J Neurosci Methods. 1997;71:177–182. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(96)00140-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.LaBar K.S., Gatenby J.C., Gore J.C., LeDoux J.E., Phelps E.A. Human amygdala activation during conditioned fear acquisition and extinction: A mixed-trial fMRI study. Neuron. 1998;20:937–945. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80475-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Phelps E.A., Delgado M.R., Nearing K.I., LeDoux J.E. Extinction learning in humans: Role of the amygdala and vmPFC. Neuron. 2004;43:897–905. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Goosens K.A., Hobin J.A., Maren S. Auditory-evoked spike firing in the lateral amygdala and Pavlovian fear conditioning: Mnemonic code or fear bias? Neuron. 2003;40:1013–1022. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00728-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Herry C., Ciocchi S., Senn V., Demmou L., Muller C., Luthi A. Switching on and off fear by distinct neuronal circuits. Nature. 2008;454:600–606. doi: 10.1038/nature07166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Richmond M.A., Murphy C.A., Pouzet B., Schmid P., Rawlins J.N., Feldon J. A computer controlled analysis of freezing behaviour. J Neurosci Methods. 1998;86:91–99. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(98)00150-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Dalley J.W., Chudasama Y., Theobald D.E., Pettifer C.L., Fletcher C.M., Robbins T.W. Nucleus accumbens dopamine and discriminated approach learning: interactive effects of 6-hydroxydopamine lesions and systemic apomorphine administration. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002;161:425–433. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1078-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Jennings K.A., Lesch K.P., Sharp T., Cragg S.J. Non-linear relationship between 5-HT transporter gene expression and frequency sensitivity of 5-HT signals. J Neurochem. 2010;115:965–973. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.07001.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Murphy D.L., Lesch K.P. Targeting the murine serotonin transporter: Insights into human neurobiology. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:85–96. doi: 10.1038/nrn2284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Murthy N.V., Selvaraj S., Cowen P.J., Bhagwagar Z., Riedel W.J., Peers P., et al. Serotonin transporter polymorphisms (SLC6A4 insertion/deletion and rs25531) do not affect the availability of 5-HTT to [11C] DASB binding in the living human brain. Neuroimage. 2010;52:50–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Shioe K., Ichimiya T., Suhara T., Takano A., Sudo Y., Yasuno F., et al. No association between genotype of the promoter region of serotonin transporter gene and serotonin transporter binding in human brain measured by PET. Synapse. 2003;48:184–188. doi: 10.1002/syn.10204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Narayanan R.T., Seidenbecher T., Kluge C., Bergado J., Stork O., Pape H.C. Dissociated theta phase synchronization in amygdalo-hippocampal circuits during various stages of fear memory. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25:1823–1831. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05437.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Pare D., Collins D.R. Neuronal correlates of fear in the lateral amygdala: Multiple extracellular recordings in conscious cats. J Neurosci. 2000;20:2701–2710. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-07-02701.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Seidenbecher T., Laxmi T.R., Stork O., Pape H.C. Amygdalar and hippocampal theta rhythm synchronization during fear memory retrieval. Science. 2003;301:846–850. doi: 10.1126/science.1085818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Pape H.C., Pare D., Driesang R.B. Two types of intrinsic oscillations in neurons of the lateral and basolateral nuclei of the amygdala. J Neurophysiol. 1998;79:205–216. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.1.205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Pape H.C., Pare D. Plastic synaptic networks of the amygdala for the acquisition, expression, and extinction of conditioned fear. Physiol Rev. 2010;90:419–463. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00037.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Pare D., Collins D.R., Pelletier J.G. Amygdala oscillations and the consolidation of emotional memories. Trends Cogn Sci. 2002;6:306–314. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(02)01924-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Chen A., Hough C.J., Li H. Serotonin type II receptor activation facilitates synaptic plasticity via N-methyl-D-aspartate-mediated mechanism in the rat basolateral amygdala. Neuroscience. 2003;119:53–63. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00076-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kawahara H., Yoshida M., Yokoo H., Nishi M., Tanaka M. Psychological stress increases serotonin release in the rat amygdala and prefrontal cortex assessed by in vivo microdialysis. Neurosci Lett. 1993;162:81–84. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(93)90565-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Yokoyama M., Suzuki E., Sato T., Maruta S., Watanabe S., Miyaoka H. Amygdalic levels of dopamine and serotonin rise upon exposure to conditioned fear stress without elevation of glutamate. Neurosci Lett. 2005;379:37–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.12.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Zanoveli J.M., Carvalho M.C., Cunha J.M., Brandao M.L. Extracellular serotonin level in the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala and dorsal periaqueductal gray under unconditioned and conditioned fear states: An in vivo microdialysis study. Brain Res. 2009;1294:106–115. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.07.074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Garpenstrand H., Annas P., Ekblom J., Oreland L., Fredrikson M. Human fear conditioning is related to dopaminergic and serotonergic biological markers. Behav Neurosci. 2001;115:358–364. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Lonsdorf T.B., Weike A.I., Nikamo P., Schalling M., Hamm A.O., Ohman A. Genetic gating of human fear learning and extinction: Possible implications for gene-environment interaction in anxiety disorder. Psychol Sci. 2009;20:198–206. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02280.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Phelps E.A., LeDoux J.E. Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: from animal models to human behavior. Neuron. 2005;48:175–187. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Campeau S., Davis M. Involvement of the central nucleus and basolateral complex of the amygdala in fear conditioning measured with fear-potentiated startle in rats trained concurrently with auditory and visual conditioned stimuli. J Neurosci. 1995;15:2301–2311. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-03-02301.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Phillips R.G., LeDoux J.E. Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci. 1992;106:274–285. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.106.2.274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Rhodes R.A., Murthy N.V., Dresner M.A., Selvaraj S., Stavrakakis N., Babar S., et al. Human 5-HT transporter availability predicts amygdala reactivity in vivo. J Neurosci. 2007;27:9233–9237. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1175-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Maratos F.A., Mogg K., Bradley B.P., Rippon G., Senior C. Coarse threat images reveal theta oscillations in the amygdala: A magnetoencephalography study. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2009;9:133–143. doi: 10.3758/CABN.9.2.133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary data

mmc1.pdf (1.2MB, pdf)

RESOURCES