Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Child Neuropsychol. 2013 Nov 25;20(6):692–699. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2013.856395

Brief Report: Performance on the Dimensional Change Card Sort and Backward Digit Span by young children with autism without intellectual disability

Susan Faja 1, Geraldine Dawson 2
PMCID: PMC4032814  NIHMSID: NIHMS533775  PMID: 24266398

Abstract

The early development of executive function (EF) and its relation to autism symptom expression is of considerable theoretical interest, particularly in children without general cognitive delay. Executive function was tested in 23 children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) without intellectual disability and 20 age- and IQ-matched typically developing children. Even though performance was equivalent between the two groups on tests of general intelligence, flexibility in card sorting was lower for children with ASD. Verbal working memory during the backward digit span did not differ between groups. Among children with ASD, poorer performance on card sorting distinguished a subgroup with worse social-communication functioning above and beyond IQ. With IQ controlled, social and repetitive symptoms of ASD did not differ based on card sorting ability.

Keywords: autism, executive function, working memory, flexibility, symptoms, repetitive behaviors


In order to effectively respond to the environment, executive function (EF) is necessary to hold complex or conflicting details in mind, flexibly adapt to feedback, inhibit inappropriate responses, and strategize about outcomes. These interrelated cognitive processes are impaired among individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD), even in the absence of intellectual disability (see Hill, 2004; Kenworthy, Yerys, Anthony, & Wallace, 2008; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996 for reviews). While not a core symptom of ASD, which is characterized by social communication impairments and restricted repetitive behaviors (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), EF may contribute to social skill development (Pellicano, 2010). Despite much research addressing the executive impairments of individuals with ASD, questions remain.

First, little is known about EF in very young children with ASD without developmental delay. All but three studies of EF in children younger than seven (Pellicano, 2007; Pellicano, Maybery, Durkin, & Maley, 2006; Smithson et al., 2013) are limited by the inclusion of children with ASD who also had general cognitive delays (e.g., Dawson et al., 1998, 2002; McEvoy, Rogers, & Pennington, 1993; Yerys, Hepburn, Pennington, & Rogers, 2007). Thus, although children with ASD are first distinguished from mental age matched typically developing and developmentally delayed children in inhibition, visual working memory and flexibility by preschool (Dawson et al., 1998; McEvoy et al., 1993), it is unclear whether EF difficulties are associated with ASD per se or the combination of ASD and cognitive delay. Two studies of EF among 4–7 year olds with ASD in the absence of cognitive impairment also demonstrated lower inhibition and shifting ability as well as reduced planning capacity. However, both used the same test battery. The third study reported more widespread EF impairments based on parent report and a different pattern of EF than that found among older individuals with ASD (Smithson et al., 2013). Thus, at a period when typically developing children are rapidly gaining EF, knowledge of the early profile in ASD remains limited. Specifically, it is unclear whether shifting, which is commonly impaired among older individuals, is reduced for a variety of tasks. Second, an aspect of EF that is relatively spared among older individuals with ASD is verbal working memory, despite early and persistent impairments in visual working memory (Kenworthy et al., 2008) and parent report of significant working memory difficulties among preschoolers (Smithson et al., 2013). Verbal working memory performance has not been measured in young non-cognitively delayed children with ASD, and doing so would provide information about the stability of a non-unitary profile of EF impairments beginning early in the development of children with ASD.

Second, the relation between EF and functioning in other domains for young children with ASD remains surprisingly poorly understood independent of cognitive delay. Prior work with older children (e.g., Joseph & Tager-Flusberg, 2004; Kenworthy, Black, Harrison, della Rosa, & Wallace, 2009) suggests that EF may relate to social communication symptoms. In addition to direct measurement of symptoms, the relation between EF and more broadly conceptualized social function has also been tested. Cross-sectional and longitudinal evidence suggests EF at 4–7 years predicted later theory of mind performance above and beyond verbal and nonverbal cognitive ability and initial theory of mind ability, while theory of mind did not predict later executive ability (Pellicano, 2007; 2010). Investigations of the relation between EF and repetitive symptoms have included a wide range of ages, executive tasks and measures of repetitive behaviors and, unsurprisingly, have yielded mixed results (Dichter et al., 2010; Lopez, Lincoln, Ozonoff, & Lai, 2005; Mosconi et al., 2009; Reed, Watts, & Truzoli, 2011; South, Ozonoff, & McMahon, 2007; Yerys et al., 2009). Except for Reed et al. (2011), studies that reported a direct relation with EF measured lifetime repetitive symptoms, suggesting that behaviors present earlier in development may relate to current EF. The only examination of EF and symptoms in the social and repetitive domains among 4–7 year olds with ASD without cognitive impairment did not detect relations (Pellicano et al., 2006), but raised the possibility that this could be due to reliance on only parent report of symptoms. Thus, it is important to test this relation. Understanding how EF might relate to symptoms and, more broadly, to social functioning among younger children would benefit from using a wider range of measurements.

The current study had two goals. The first was to compare young children with ASD to a group of age- and IQ-matched typically developing children on two aspects of EF: flexibility (Dimensional Change Card Sort; DCCS) and verbal working memory (backward digit span). These tasks are appropriate for 6–7 year olds with established criteria for passing (Carlson, 2005; Cohen, 1997; Zelazo, 2006) and provide additional information about the uniformity of early EF deficits in ASD. We predicted less flexibility while switching rules during the DCCS task based on prior work with young children with ASD and intellectual impairment and older children with ASD without intellectual impairment (e.g., Colvert, Custance, & Swettenham, 2002; Geurts, Verté, Oosterlaan, Roeyers, & Sergeant, 2004; Reed et al., 2011; but see Dichter et al., 2010). Given the relatively spared verbal working memory ability among older individuals with ASD (Kenworthy et al., 2008), we anticipated spared performance. Second, if EF impairments were detected in the ASD group, the present study examined whether performance related to social communication functioning as well as social and repetitive symptoms. We predicted EF would relate to social communication functioning, given its relation to theory of mind ability (Pellicano, 2007; 2010). Following the suggestion of Pellicano et al (2006), we explored whether the failure to find a relation between symptoms and EF was a result of narrow measurement of symptoms.

Methods

Sixty 6–7 year olds were recruited to obtain a final matched sample of 23 children with idiopathic autism spectrum disorders and 20 children with typical development. Exclusionary criteria for both groups included medical disorders or injuries involving the central nervous system, major physical abnormalities, seizures, and significant sensory or motor impairments. In addition, typically developing children were excluded if they had a family history of ASD, birth or developmental abnormalities, learning or language disability, current or past history of psychiatric or neurological disorders, or regularly used psychoactive medication. The study was conducted in accordance with the university Human Subjects Division.

Groups were matched on age, sex, and intelligence (see Table 1). All children had General Conceptual Ability (GCA) scores above 80 on the school age core of the Differential Ability Scales-2 (DAS-2; Elliott, 1990). Biological parents were well educated and the sample was predominantly white; groups did not differ on these characteristics or on household income. For children with ASD, a previous diagnosis was required and confirmed with the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R; Rutter, Le Couteur, & Lord, 2003) and Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord, Rutter, DiLavore, & Risi, 2002; Gotham, Risi, Pickles, & Lord, 2007) per DSM-IV-TR criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 2000).

Table 1.

Participant descriptive characteristics with Mean, Standard Deviation, and Range

ASD Comparison Signif.(t, p)
Sex; Race 18 male; 18 white 15 male; 18 white
Age in months 82.5 (7.3); 72–94 80.9 (7.6); 73–95 0.69, ns
Family gross annual income 104.6 (34.5); 45–190 120.7 (69.5); 35–300 −.97, ns
Paternal highest education 6; 4–7 6; 4–7 202.5, ns
Maternal highest education 6; 4–7 6; 5–7 195.0, ns
DAS-2 GCA 102.3 (12.4); 84–133 108.0 (5.3); 100–118 −1.9, ns
DAS-2 Verbal Composite 104.1 (14.3); 71–125 108.3 (9.2); 94–123 −1.1, ns

Vineland-2 Communication 93.7 (9.2); 78–108 106.3 (5.9); 93–115 −5.3, <.001
Vineland-2 Socialization 81.0 (8.2); 66–96 99.7 (5.0); 91–110 −8.9, <.001
Social Skills Rating System 78.8 (13.6); 56–104 107.6 (10.6); 84–127 −7.5, <.001
ADOS Social Affect 10.6 (3.9); 5–19
Repetitive Beh. Scale-Revised 23.0 (17.7); 1–65 2.9 (3.0); 0–11 5.0, <.001
ADI-R Higher Order Rep. Beh. 4.0 (2.4); 0–8
ADOS Restricted/Rep. Beh. 3.6 (1.8); 0–8

Note: Household income is reported in thousands of US dollars. Parental education is coded as 4= high school, 5=some college, 6=college, 7=graduate school. Standard scores with an average range of 85–115 are reported for the Differential Ability Scales-2 (DAS-2), Vineland-2, and Social Skills Rating System (SSRS). Raw scores are reported for the Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised (RBS-R), Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) and Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS).

Median score and Mann-Whitney U is reported for this ordinal variable.

Executive function was measured via the Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS; Diamond, Carlson, & Beck, 2005; Zelazo, 2006) and the backward portion of the Children’s Memory Scale Numbers subtest (Cohen, 1997). DCCS cards varied on three dimensions: color, shape, and the presence of a black star. Directions for sorting were repeated and dimensions were labeled for each card as it was presented (e.g., “If it’s blue, then put it here, but if it’s red, put it there. Here is a blue/red one.”). Cards were sorted without feedback into two opaque boxes labeled by cards with the same shapes and colors as the sorting cards but in the opposite combination. That is, the box labels only matched on one dimension––color or shape. In the first, pre-switch phase, children sorted by color or shape, which was counterbalanced across participants. Next, in the post-switch phase the sorting dimension was switched. The passing criterion for these phases was five correct sorts on six consecutive trials. Then, during the mixed sorting phase, the presence or absence of a star indicated whether the color or shape rule was to be followed. Stars appeared on 6 of 12 cards. At least nine correct sorts were required to pass the mixed phase. The dependent variable was the number of phases passed (Zelazo, 2006). During Numbers Backward, children repeated strings of numbers in reverse order. Two items of each span length were administered until two consecutive spans were missed. Each correct span was awarded one point and the dependent variable was the total number of points (i.e., raw score).

Both symptoms and functioning were assessed (see Table 1). Although there may be overlap in social communication symptoms and function, the scope of these measures differs. A social communication functioning composite was created by averaging the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS; Gresham & Elliott, 1990) and the Vineland-2 (Sparrow, Cicchetti, & Balla, 2005) Socialization and Communication scores, Cronbach’s α = .85. These scales measure broad social and communication behaviors including following directions, reading, writing, manners, sportsmanship and personal responsibility. Current clinician-observed ASD symptoms were measured via the ADOS Social Affect and Restricted/Repetitive Behaviors scales. Parent report of current repetitive symptoms was obtained via the Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised (RBS-R; Bodfish, Symons, & Lewis, 1999) and of lifetime higher-order repetitive behaviors via the ADI-R algorithm C1 and C2 (circumscribed interests, compulsive behaviors and rituals). The ADOS, RBS-R and ADI-R are ASD-specific measures and closely linked with symptoms of ASD.

For each measure, groups were first compared. For DCCS phases passed, data were ordinal, so Mann-Whitney U scores were computed. A t-test was computed for Numbers Backward. Second, if groups differed for the DCCS, the ASD group was subdivided and symptoms and functioning were compared between those who failed or passed all phases. If differences were detected for Numbers Backward, Pearson correlations were computed. The IQ range was large, so the relation between EF and IQ was examined and covaried if related.

Results and Discussion

We first compared the performance of children with ASD relative to the comparison group. On the DCCS, all children passed the pre-switch phase. Two children with ASD failed the post-switch phase, which required switching to a new sorting dimension; the remaining 21 passed along with all comparison children. Children who passed the post-switch phase continued to the mixed phase (i.e., sorting on two dimensions): 10 children with ASD failed and 11 passed, while 6 comparison children failed and 16 passed. The total DCCS score (i.e., number of phases of passed) significantly differed by group, Mann-Whitney U = 152, p = .03. Children with ASD passed fewer DCCS phases, which suggests they were less flexible in integrating complex rules for behavior. In contrast, Numbers Backward raw scores did not differ between groups, t(41) = −1.06, p = .30, d = −0.38. This suggests the ability to manipulate information held in working memory was similar for both groups. However, not all aspects of working memory appear spared among young children with ASD including visual working memory (Dawson et al., 1998) and real-world application of working memory to complete tasks and follow instructions over time (Smithson et al., 2013). These results provide support for a non-unitary disruption in EF and spared verbal working memory in ASD, as well as continuity in the pattern of findings between younger and older individuals independent of cognitive impairment. This extends previous investigations of EF in young children with ASD and no intellectual impairment (Pellicano et al., 2006; 2007) by using two novel tasks and demonstrating a differential pattern of impairment between a task that emphasizes flexibility and a task of verbal working memory.

Next, because children with ASD had reduced performance on the DCCS, we examined whether poor performance within this group related to functioning in other domains. We first examined IQ and found it was higher among children with ASD who passed all three DCCS phases, t(21) = 5.21, p < .001, d = 2.2. For this reason, IQ was covaried for all subsequent comparisons. We then examined whether children with ASD who passed all three phases had better social communication function. ANCOVA with GCA covaried indicated the children with ASD who passed all three DCCS phases had better social communication skills above and beyond IQ, F(1, 20) = 7.22, p = .01, ηp2 = 0.27. Finally, we examined whether ASD symptoms differed between children who passed and those who did not. With IQ covaried, no differences were detected for children who passed the three DCCS phases in clinician-observed social symptoms or in clinician-observed or parent reported current or lifetime repetitive behaviors.

This study is limited by a relatively small sample size, which restricted the ability to examine performance on tasks beyond the main dependent variable and may have limited the ability to detect some relations between variables. Our primary goal in recruitment was well-matched groups because age and IQ matching were critical for our goals. In addition, the study used clinician-administered EF tasks, which may underestimate the abilities of children with ASD given that they involve both cognitive and social demands. It is possible that computer tasks may have produced different results. However, while seated across from the child, the same experimenter presented scripted verbal instructions without feedback for both tasks. Differences in performance were detected despite the similarity of social task demands. Finally, it should be noted that the psychometric properties of the scales used to measure social function differed from those used to measure symptoms, which may have contributed to the different relations detected.

Findings suggest that the ability to flexibly follow complex rules is reduced relative to verbal working memory in children with ASD without intellectual disability, and differences in the ability to pass all phases of the DCCS within the ASD group mapped to differing levels of parent reported social communication functioning above and beyond intelligence. The social communication composite captures broad abilities such as flexibility communicating with and responding to others, sharing, playing cooperatively and coping with social demands – all of which may benefit from the ability to manage complex information. This builds on studies linking DCCS performance with theory of mind in children with ASD and comorbid intellectual disability (Zelazo & Müller, 2002) and performance on different executive tasks with theory of mind in children with ASD without cognitive impairment (Pellicano, 2007; 2010).

In contrast, passing all phases of the DCCS did not distinguish children with ASD on their level of clinician observed social-affective symptoms or on three measures of repetitive behavior. These results replicate Pellicano et al. (2006) who also reported no relation between EF and social or repetitive symptoms, and address an unresolved issue raised by that work. In the previous study, only parent report was used to assess symptoms and the authors suggested that direct observation might be more sensitive for detecting relations between symptoms and EF. The current study suggests that this is not the case. Instead, it provides support for a distinction between broadly-construed social communication function and symptoms that are more specific to ASD, which emphasize non-verbal communication and the ability to make and respond to social interactions in a reliable and coordinated way as well as the presence of preoccupations, repetitive actions, and rituals. Yet, it should be noted that our results are inconsistent with work that found better DCCS performance related to more ASD symptoms among older individuals (Dichter et al., 2010). More work will be needed to reconcile these results, but the current study lends support for the contribution of flexibility to meaningful individual differences in the ability to function effectively across a range of environments in ASD. Clinically, this study highlights meaningful differences in shifting between task demands relative to remembering complex information as well as the need to assess individual differences in flexibility for treatment planning – particularly for social skills training that often emphasizes complex, rule-based strategies. In addition, the relation of EF abilities to social and communication function raise the possibility that interventions targeting EF may enhance treatment outcomes for young children with ASD by improving function in their environment.

Acknowledgements

An American Psychological Association Dissertation Award, International Society of Autism Research Dissertation Award, Robert C. Bolles Graduate Fellowship in Psychology, and NINDS Postdoctoral Award (T32NS007413) to the first author supported this project. Additional support was provided through collaboration with Michael Murias (Cure Autism Now Young Investigator Award). The project is the sole responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily reflect the views of the funding agencies. We thank Jasleen Tiwana, Amandeep Kaur Virk and Dana Kamara for research assistance, Stephanie Carlson for helpful comments regarding task selection, Jessica Greenson and Milani Smith for clinical supervision, and we especially thank the participants and their families.

Footnotes

These data represent a portion of the first author’s doctoral dissertation, and the DCCS was presented at the International Meeting for Autism Research, Philadelphia, PA in May 2010.

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Contributor Information

Susan Faja, Department Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences, University of Washington, Box 357920, Seattle, WA, 98195, Phone: 206-685-0217 susfaja@uw.edu.

Geraldine Dawson, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University Durham, NC, geraldine.dawson@duke.edu.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. Text Revision. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bodfish JW, Symons FW, Lewis MH. The Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised. Western Carolina Center Research Reports. 1999 [Google Scholar]
  3. Carlson SM. Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2005;28:595–616. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Cohen MJ. Children’s Memory Scale: Manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  5. Colvert E, Custance D, Swettenham J. Rule-based reasoning and theory of mind in autism: A commentary on the work of Zelazo, Jacques, Burack and Frye. Infant and Child Development. 2002;11:197–200. [Google Scholar]
  6. Dawson G, Meltzoff AN, Osterling J, Rinaldi J. Neuropsychological correlates of early symptoms of autism. Child Development. 1998;69:1276–1285. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Diamond A, Carlson SM, Beck DM. Preschool children’s performance in task switching on the dimensional change card sort task: Separating the dimensions aids the ability to switch. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2005;28:689–729. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Dichter GS, Radonovich KJ, Turner-Brown LM, Lam KSL, Holtzclaw TN, Bodfish JW. Performance of children with autism spectrum disorders on the dimension-change card sort task. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2010;40:448–456. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0886-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Elliott CD. DAS Administration and Scoring Manual. San Antonio TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  10. Geurts HM, Verté S, Oosterlaan J, Roeyers H, Sergeant JA. How specific are executive function deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45:836–854. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00276.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Gotham K, Risi S, Pickles A, Lord C. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule: Revised algorithms for improved diagnostic validity. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2007;37:613–627. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0280-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gresham FM, Elliott SN. Social Skills Rating System. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  13. Hill EL. Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental Review. 2004;24:189–233. [Google Scholar]
  14. Joseph RM, Tager-Flusberg H. The relationship of theory of mind and executive functions to symptom type and severity in children with autism. Development and Psychopathology. 2004;16:137–155. doi: 10.1017/S095457940404444X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Kenworthy LE, Black DO, Harrison B, della Rosa A, Wallace GL. Are executive control functions related to autism symptoms in high-functioning children? Child Neuropsychology. 2009;15:425–440. doi: 10.1080/09297040802646983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Kenworthy LE, Yerys BE, Anthony LG, Wallace GL. Understanding executive control in autism spectrum disorders in the lab and in the real world. Neuropsychological Review. 2008;18:320–338. doi: 10.1007/s11065-008-9077-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Lopez BR, Lincoln AJ, Ozonoff S, Lai Z. Examining the relationship between executive functions and restricted, repetitive symptoms of autistic disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2005;35:445–460. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-5035-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore PC, Risi S. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  19. McEvoy R, Rogers SJ, Pennington BF. Executive function and social communication deficits in young autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1993;34:563–578. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1993.tb01036.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Mosconi MW, Kay M, D’Cruz AM, Seidenfeld A, Guter S, Stanford LD, Sweeney JA. Impaired inhibitory control is associated with higher-order repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders. Psychological Medicine. 2009;39:1559–1566. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Pellicano E. Links between theory of mind and executive function in young children with autism: Clues to developmental primacy. Developmental Psychology. 2007;43:974–990. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Pellicano E. Individual differences in executive function and central coherence predict developmental changes in theory of mind in autism. Developmental Psychology. 2010;46:530–544. doi: 10.1037/a0018287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Pellicano E, Maybery M, Durkin K, Maley A. Multiple cognitive capabilities/deficits in children with autism spectrum disorder: “Weak” central coherence and its relationship to theory of mind and executive control. Development and Psychopathology. 2006;18:77–98. doi: 10.1017/S0954579406060056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Pennington BF, Ozonoff S. Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1996;37:51–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01380.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Reed P, Watts H, Truzoli R. Flexibility in young people with autism spectrum disorders on a card sort task. Autism. 2011;17:162–171. doi: 10.1177/1362361311409599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Rutter M, Le Couteur A, Lord C. ADI-R: The Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  27. Smithson PE, Kenworthy L, Wills MC, Jarrett M, Atmore K, Yerys BE. Real world executive control impairments in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2013;43:1967–1975. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1747-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Sparrow SS, Cicchetti DV, Balla DA. Vineland II: A Revision of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales: I. Survey/Caregiver Form. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  29. South M, Ozonoff S, McMahon W. The relationship between executive functioning, central coherence, and repetitive behaviors in the high-functioning autism spectrum. Autism. 2007;11:437–451. doi: 10.1177/1362361307079606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Yerys B, Hepburn SL, Pennington BF, Rogers SJ. Executive function in preschoolers with autism: Evidence consistent with a secondary deficit. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2007;37:1068–1079. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0250-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Yerys BE, Wallace GL, Harrison B, Celano MJ, Giedd JN, Kenworthy LE. Set-shifting in children with autism spectrum disorders: Reversal shifting deficits on the Intradimensional/Extradimensional Shift Test correlate with repetitive behaviors. Autism. 2009;13:523–538. doi: 10.1177/1362361309335716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Zelazo PD. The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols. 2006;1:297–301. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Zelazo PD, Müller U. Executive function in typical and atypical development. In: Goswami U, editor. Handbook of childhood cognitive development. Oxford: Blackwell; 2002. pp. 445–469. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES