Abstract
Background: Dual process models suggest that the development of addictive behaviors is the result of interplay between impulsive and reflective processes, modulated by boundary conditions such as individual or situational factors. Empirical support for this model has been repeatedly demonstrated in adult samples [for a meta-analysis, see Ref. (1)]. The purpose of this study was to test these processes as they relate to emerging alcohol use in adolescents. Specifically, the interactive effects of several measures of impulsive and reflective processes and working memory capacity (WMC) are examined as predictors of changes in alcohol use among adolescents. It was expected that measures of reflective processes would better predict changes in alcohol use than measures of impulsive processes. Moreover, it was anticipated that WMC would moderate the relation between alcohol-specific impulsive and reflective processes and changes in adolescent alcohol use.
Methods: The sample consisted of 427 adolescents (47.7% male) between 12 and 16 years of age (M = 13.96, SD = 0.78) who reported drinking alcohol at least once. Four measures of impulsive processes were included. Attentional bias for alcohol was assessed with a Visual Probe Test; approach bias toward alcohol was assessed with a Stimulus Response Compatibility (SRC) Test; and memory associations with alcohol were assessed with an Implicit Association Test (IAT) and a Word Association Test. Two measures of reflective measures were included: positive and negative expectancies. WMC was measured using a Self-Ordered Pointing Task.
Results: Results showed that positive expectancies predicted changes in alcohol use, but this effect was qualified by an interaction with IAT scores. Moreover, SRC scores predicted changes in alcohol use only when negative expectancies were low. Attentional bias and word association scores did not predict changes in alcohol use. The relations between alcohol-specific processes or reflective processes and alcohol use were not moderated by WMC.
Conclusion: Although there is empirical evidence for the validity of the model in predicting heavier alcohol use in adolescents, or alcohol abuse and dependence in adults, these observations do not generalize to a sample of normative, early adolescents. More specifically, results indicated that reflective processes are more important predictors of changes in alcohol use than impulsive process during adolescence.
Keywords: implicit cognition, explicit cognition, adolescents, alcohol use, working memory capacity, dual process model
Introduction
Dual process models of addiction state that the development of addictive behaviors is the result of interplay between impulsive and reflective processes, modulated by boundary conditions such as individual or situational factors (2–4). Impulsive processes are automatic, relatively less conscious processes, while reflective processes are deliberate and conscious processes. Impulsive processes become stronger with repeated use due to associative learning and alcohol-induced neural sensitization, while the ability to reflect on behavior is negatively affected by drinking. Support for the applicability of elements of dual process models to various health behaviors, including addictive behaviors, in adults is accumulating [e.g., Ref. (3, 5, 6)]. However, to date much less is known about how these models relate to substance use and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescence. The main aim of the current study is to examine whether the development of young adolescent drinking patterns can be explained by (elements of) the dual process model.
It has been suggested that adolescents are inclined to prefer behaviors that involve a certain risk, such as experimenting with alcohol and other illicit drugs (7). Several models of adolescent brain development have emphasized that risky behaviors are the result of an imbalance in the development of two neural systems: on one hand, an affective system that develops swiftly in parallel with hormonal developments during puberty, and on the other hand a reflective system that matures relatively more slowly and does not reach maturity until young adulthood [e.g., Ref. (8–13)]. The former system is dependent on neural circuits encompassing the striatum/nucleus accumbens [and more limbic areas, cf. the triadic model of Ref. (10)], associated with affective and reward-driven processes, while the latter is associated with more lateral prefrontal regions (9). Imbalance in brain development during adolescence may explain why adolescents are less likely to inhibit the motivation to approach a potentially rewarding, yet risky, stimulus.
These theoretical models are in line with dual process models [e.g., Ref. (3)], suggesting that substances affect related developing neural systems, meaning that repeated alcohol use sensitizes neural systems associated with the attribution of salience to cues previously associated with alcohol use [e.g., Ref. (14)], while alcohol use impairs more lateral prefrontal regions associated with the reflective system. As a result, impulsive processes become stronger, leading to attentional and approach biases toward alcohol (cues), while the reflective system can no longer inhibit these automatic impulses.
To date, most research has focused on reflective processes, measured with questionnaires examining attitudes toward alcohol, expectancies from alcohol, and motives for drinking alcohol. Studies have shown that even before children engage in alcohol use, they already develop expectancies from alcohol (15–18). Before the age of 10, attitudes toward and expectancies about the effects of alcohol are predominantly negative (16–19). After the age of 10, there is an increase in positive expectancies. Whereas some suggest that negative expectancies are replaced by positive ones (17, 20), others suggest that positive and negative expectancies co-exist, with positive expectancies becoming stronger over time (15).
Studies have shown that positive expectancies predict alcohol use concurrently [e.g., Ref. (21)] and longitudinally [e.g., between 6 months and 9 years; (22–24)]. In addition, positive expectancies predicted alcohol use problems in adulthood (25). While many studies have examined positive expectancies and adolescent alcohol use, fewer studies have reported on negative expectancies. Some report that negative expectancies are negatively related to alcohol, such that the more negative expectancies adolescents have, the less they drink [e.g., Ref. (26–28)]. Other studies have not been able to demonstrate a relation between negative expectancies and levels of alcohol use [e.g., Ref. (29)].
Alcohol-specific impulsive processes are assessed using so-called indirect measures. For instance, implicit associations between concepts in memory are often assessed in paper-and-pencil tasks, where individuals are prompted to give their first association with ambiguous words (e.g., draft, which can be related to a manuscript or to beer) or in reaction time tasks such as varieties of the Implicit Association Task (IAT), where the relative strength between concepts in memory is thought to be reflected in reaction times (e.g., alcohol and positive vs. alcohol and negative). Attentional and approach biases can also be measured using reaction time paradigms in which the relative speed of responding to alcohol vs. neutral cues is measured.
The use of indirect measures, in addition to more direct measures, such as traditional surveys assessing expectancies from alcohol use, has several advantages. For instance, indirect measures tap processes that are less easily available to introspection than direct measures. Scholars have indicated that expectancies differ from associations in memory in several ways. For instance, brain networks that are associated with either process do not necessarily overlap to a large extent [e.g., Ref. (30–33)]. In addition, associations develop slowly, whereas expectancies can be formed or changed based on a single occasion (34). Expectancies are also related to a true or false statement, whereas associations are not (35). Furthermore, associations are bidirectional, while expectancies rely more on “if … then” relations (36). Although expectancies sometimes rely on a single association (e.g., drinking alcohol and fun), this is not always the case, as for instance in expectancies related to negative reinforcement (37). Whereas direct measures require conscious introspection, indirect measures tap into associations that influence behavior relatively automatically or unconsciously. Explicit and implicit cognitions have been found to predict a unique part of the variance in alcohol use [meta-analysis, Ref. (1)].
Empirical studies examining the relation between impulsive alcohol-related processes and alcohol use in adolescence are scarce. Regarding young adolescents, approach biases have been shown in drinkers compared to abstainers, especially in adolescents who were less able to inhibit behavior [N = 374; (38)]. In addition, approach bias predicted drinking when inhibition was low over a period of 6 months (39). Yet, the two studies by Peeters et al. (38, 39) investigated adolescents with externalizing problems enrolled in special education. These adolescents are thought to have a higher chance of an early onset of alcohol use or alcohol problems in the first place. Using a normative sample, Willem et al. (40) demonstrated that approach bias was related to alcohol use in older male adolescents (N = 94), with no significant moderation by inhibitory control. Attentional bias was associated with alcohol use, but only in those with low attentional control. Further, in a study among 100 12- and 15-year-olds, it was found that boys with stronger implicit positive associations with alcohol had higher levels of binge drinking (more than five glasses per occasion) 1 year later than those without these positive associations (41). In addition, Thush et al. (42) showed that in a sample of at-risk adolescents, implicit cognitions predicted prospective drinking, over and above the effect of explicit cognitions. Measures of implicit alcohol-related memory associations were shown to be the best predictor of prospective drinking (42). Van Der Vorst and colleagues recently showed that implicit alcohol-related memory associated predicted alcohol use 1 year later in 608 children who never drunk at baseline (43). In addition, Pieters et al. (44), using an IAT, found that children (N = 134) associated alcohol more strongly with negative, compared to positive faces. It is important to note that some studies fail to find the proposed relation between impulsive reactions (approach bias) and alcohol use (45). Studies in adolescents have also shown that when executive control is low, alcohol-related automatic processes drive behavior, while when executive control is high, the reflective system overrides [e.g., Ref. (42, 46)].
To date, research on the dual process model in adolescents has mostly focused on concurrent data, used short-term follow-up assessments, or had small and selective samples [e.g., Ref. (42, 46)]. In addition, most previous research has examined relations between impulsive or reflective processes and alcohol use while there is some evidence suggesting that impulsive and reflective processes contribute to the prediction of adolescent alcohol use [e.g., Ref. (38, 39, 42, 46)]. Thus, the main aim of the current longitudinal study is to examine conditional effects of alcohol-related impulsive processes (e.g., alcohol-related memory associations, attentional bias, and approach bias), reflective processes (i.e., positive and negative expectancies from alcohol use), and working memory capacity (WMC) (as a measure of executive functions) on changes in alcohol use in adolescents who drank alcohol on at least one occasion. WMC was used as an indicator of executive functions, since it has been used in previous research studying the dual process model in adolescents. While there is some debate as to the number and types of executive functions [e.g., Ref. (47–49)], the three functions that are mentioned most often are response inhibition, working memory, and task shifting [e.g., Ref. (48, 49)]. Since there is some overlap between these functions, this phenomenon is called the “unity and diversity of executive functions” (49). We expected that both alcohol-related impulsive processes and positive expectancies would be positively related to alcohol use. WMC and negative expectancies were expected to predict less alcohol use. In addition, it was hypothesized that, when WMC is low, alcohol-related impulsive processes predict alcohol use, while when WMC is high, expectancies would predict alcohol use over time.
Materials and Methods
Participants
The sample consisted of 427 adolescents (47.7% male) between 12 and 16 years of age (M = 13.96, SD = 0.78) who reported drinking alcohol at least once. Roughly equal numbers of participants attended college-preparatory (32.7%), intermediate or basic education (28.2%), and vocational education (28.9%) classrooms. A total of 10.1% did not provide information on educational track, which mainly consisted of first graders who had not yet made a choice.
Procedure
Data were derived from the first two waves of a larger longitudinal project assessing risk factors related to adolescent alcohol use [see also Ref. (50–53)]. Initially, 1215 adolescents attending five schools and their parents were contacted by mail. A total of 725 adolescents (60%) returned consent forms signed by themselves and one of their parents or caregivers, and 556 (77%) participated in at least one of the annual assessments. Of the 556 participants, 129 reported never drinking alcohol, leaving a sample of 427 adolescents who reported drinking alcohol at least once. Participants performed several tasks on a computer, including a Visual Probe Test (VPT), a Stimulus Response Compatibility (SRC) task, an Implicit Association Test (IAT), and a Self-Ordered Pointing Task (SOPT). The following day, adolescents completed an extensive questionnaire assessing alcohol use and expectancies from alcohol use. Both sessions lasted about an hour and a trained research assistant was available in both sessions to ensure that adolescents completed the sessions individually. In addition, the trained research assistant guaranteed confidential handling of the information provided by the adolescents. This study was approved by the review board of the Medical Ethical Committee in The Netherlands.
Measures
Alcohol use
Adolescents completed four items describing the intensity of alcohol use during weekdays, on the weekend, at home and outside home (54). Items were summed to create a measure of weekly alcohol consumption. Alcohol consumption was measured during both annual waves of the study.
Approach tendencies
An SRC task (55) was used to assess alcohol-related approach tendencies. In this task, an alcohol-related picture or a soft-drink-related picture was presented on the screen in a randomized order. A manikin was placed either above or below the picture. Participants were asked to simulate an approach or an avoid movement if the picture was alcohol-related by pressing the arrow pointing upward or downward dependent on the orientation of the manikin. The order of blocks was counterbalanced across subjects, meaning that some participants were first instructed to approach alcohol-related pictures and to avoid alcohol-related pictures in the subsequent block. Each of the two blocks consisted of 40 trials. An SRC score was calculated by subtracting the mean reaction times (correct trials) when approaching alcohol (and avoiding soda) from mean reaction times when avoiding alcohol (and approaching soda) and a positive score indicated faster responses when instructed to approach alcohol. On average, participants made 7.58 errors on this task (SD = 7.51). Scores from participants with more than 33.33% errors in this task were omitted from the analyses. This resulted in 10 omissions for the SRC. Furthermore, Z-scores were computed and scores more than three SD lower or higher than the mean were adjusted to the next lowest or highest value.
Attentional bias
A VPT was used to assess attentional bias toward alcohol-related cues. In this task an alcohol-related and a soft-drink-related picture appear simultaneously on a computer screen. After 1500 ms, a black screen was shown followed by an arrow pointing upward or downward in the location of either the alcohol or soft-drink-related picture. The arrow remained visible until the participant responded to it by pressing the corresponding arrow on the keyboard. Attentional bias is inferred if reaction times to arrows replacing alcohol pictures were faster compared to soft-drink pictures. For this study, we subtracted mean reaction times to arrows replacing alcohol-related pictures from mean reaction times to arrows replacing soft-drink-related pictures. A positive score indicated a relative attentional bias toward alcohol-related photos. On average, adolescents made 7.49 errors on the task (SD = 9.84). Scores from participants with more than 33.33% errors in this task were omitted from the dataset. This resulted in an 18 omissions for the VPT. Furthermore, Z-scores were computed and scores more than three SD lower or higher than the mean were adjusted to the next lowest or highest value.
Memory associations
An IAT and Word Association Test (WAT) were used to assess memory associations. The unipolar IAT was used to assess the strength of associations between “alcohol” and “positive arousal” in memory [see Ref. (42)]. In this task, participants were instructed to categorize pictures and words as quickly as possible in one of four categories displayed in the upper corners of a computer screen by pressing the “E” or “I” button on a keyboard. In the incompatible block, category labels of “alcohol” and “neutral” were placed in one upper corner of the screen, while category labels of “unrelated to alcohol” and “positive arousal” were placed in the other upper corner of the screen. In the compatible block, the pairings were changed so that “alcohol” and “positive arousal” shared one upper corner, and “unrelated to alcohol” and “neutral” shared another. The order of the compatible and incompatible blocks was counterbalanced across participants. A D2SD penalty score was calculated by subtracting mean reaction times in the compatible block from mean reaction times in the incompatible block, meaning that positive scores reflected relatively faster responses in the compatible block. Positive scores were assumed to reflect a relatively better association between “alcohol” and “positive arousal” compared to “alcohol” and “neutral” concepts in memory. On average, participants made 20.2% errors (SD = 11.35). Scores from participants with more than 33.33% errors in this task were omitted from the dataset. This resulted in an omission of 45 participants’ scores on the IAT. Furthermore, Z-scores were computed and scores more than three SD lower or higher than the mean were adjusted to the next lowest or highest value.
In the WAT, participants were asked to write down the first word that came into mind upon reading written verbal cues. Cues consisted of 24 Dutch homographs, of which 6 were related to alcohol. The amount of alcohol-related answers that were given was used as an outcome measure on this task of associative memory [e.g., Ref. (56)].
Working memory capacity
A computerized version of the SOPT was used to assess WMC. This task was based on the original SOPT (57). Participants were instructed to select a picture from a matrix of pictures in each trial. In the next trials, participants had to repeatedly select another picture, in another location. The arrangement of the pictures was changed in every trial. Four versions of the task were utilized: 9 concrete pictures (people performing various sports), 9 abstract pictures (grayscale images), 12 concrete pictures (pictures of farm animals), and 12 abstract pictures (grayscale images). In the case of nine pictures, there were nine trials. For each version, we calculated the percentage of correct trial, and we averaged these percentages over the four versions. Higher scores indicated better WMC. The psychometric properties of the SOPT are sufficient (58) and the task is easy to administer in adolescents. In the current sample, the internal consistency of the four versions was good (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.87). The SOPT has been used in previous studies testing hypotheses regarding the dual process model [e.g., Ref. (42, 46)]. Participants on average responded correctly in 77.20% of the trials (SD = 20.31). We adjusted the scores (% correct) of adolescents with scores lower than three SDs below the mean to the next available lowest value. They were considered to have not taken the task seriously, or to have an extremely insufficient WMC.
Alcohol expectancies
In a paper-and-pencil task, 12 statements were reported regarding the expected consequences (6 items for positive and 6 for negative expectancies) of alcohol use. All statements started by: “Drinking alcohol makes me” followed by a word indicating either a positive or negative consequence. For instance, “Drinking alcohol makes me happy” (i.e., cozy, comfortable, friendly, social, sympathetically, sad, moody, depressed, lonely, unhappy, down). Answers were given on a 10-point scale ranging from (1) not at all to (10) very much. The internal consistency of the scales was good (Cronbach’s alpha was 0.95 for positive and 0.94 for negative).
Results
Descriptives and correlations
Pearson correlations among all study variables are presented in Table 1. Gender was negatively correlated with negative expectancies, with males reporting more negative expectancies than females. Age was positively associated with wave 1 alcohol use and positive expectancies, with older adolescents reporting more drinking and more positive expectancies than younger adolescents. Education level was negatively associated with alcohol use at waves 1 and 2, with students in vocational classrooms reporting more alcohol use than those in university-preparatory classrooms. Alcohol use at wave 1 was negatively correlated with IAT scores, suggesting the more adolescents associate alcohol with arousal, the less they drink. Alcohol use at waves 1 and 2 was positively associated with alcohol-related memory associations and positive expectancies, with more alcohol-related memory associations and positive expectancies being associated with more concurrent and subsequent alcohol use. Positive expectancies were also positively associated with alcohol-related memory associations and negative expectancies. All other correlations were not statistically significant, including the associations between the four measures of impulsive processes.
Table 1.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | Mean | SD | Range | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Gender | 199 Boys, 228 girls | |||||||||||||
2. Age | −0.03 | 14.01 | 0.77 | 11–16 years | ||||||||||
3. Education | 0.07 | −0.05 | ||||||||||||
4. Alcohol 1 | −0.01 | 0.22*** | −0.20*** | 1.52 | 0.82 | 1–6 | ||||||||
5. Alcohol 2 | −0.08 | 0.09 | −0.19** | 0.35*** | 1.79 | 0.98 | 1–6 | |||||||
6. Approach bias | −0.07 | −0.03 | −0.07 | −0.05 | 0.11 | 47.35 | 851.36 | -2988.5 to 3642 | ||||||
7. Attentional bias | −0.03 | 0.06 | −0.08 | −0.02 | 0.03 | 0.12 | −3.13 | 36.25 | -109 to 111.5 | |||||
8. Memory associations (IAT) | 0.04 | 0.01 | 0.07 | −0.16* | −0.08 | −0.02 | −0.07 | 1.01 | 0.71 | -2 to 4.13 | ||||
9. Memory associations (WAT) | −0.06 | 0.16** | −0.00 | 0.20*** | 0.17** | −0.04 | −0.07 | −0.02 | 1.47 | 0.98 | 0–5 | |||
10. WMC | 0.08 | −0.04 | 0.12* | −0.04 | −0.07 | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.11 | −0.01 | 77.04 | 20.19 | 9.72–100 | ||
11. Positive exp. | 0.07 | 0.17** | −0.03 | 0.34*** | 0.26*** | 0.05 | −0.02 | 0.02 | 0.29*** | −0.03 | 4.67 | 2.72 | 1–10 | |
12. Negative exp. | −0.12* | 0.02 | −0.19*** | −0.02 | 0.02 | 0.05 | 0.04 | −0.10 | 0.04 | −0.01 | 0.22*** | 2.15 | 1.73 | 1–10 |
Gender is coded as 0 = male and 1 = female. WMC is working memory capacity. Positive exp. is positive expectancies. Negative exp. is negative expectancies; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
Predicting alcohol use from alcohol-related impulsive processes and working memory capacity
Three multiple linear regression analyses were performed to identify predictors of changes in alcohol use from waves 1 to 2. In the first model, gender, age, educational level, the four impulsive processes (approach tendencies, attentional bias, and memory associations from the WAT and IAT), alcohol use at wave 1, WMC and four interactions between alcohol-related impulsive processes, and WMC were entered as predictors (see Table 2). Changes in alcohol use were used as a dependent variable in this analysis. Overall, the model explained 17% (p < 0.001) of the variance of alcohol use at wave 2. In this model, the only statistically significant predictor of alcohol use in wave 2 was alcohol use in wave 1. All main effects of WMC and the impulsive measures, as well as all four interaction terms were not statistically significant.
Table 2.
Model 1 |
Model 2 |
Model 3 |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | p | β | p | β | p | |
Gender | − 0.053 | 0.277 | − 0.063 | 0.211 | − 0.075 | 0.145 |
Age | 0.008 | 0.893 | − 0.023 | 0.731 | 0.008 | 0.900 |
Level of ed. | − 0.103 | 0.129 | − 0.084 | 0.258 | − 0.050 | 0.609 |
Alcohol use 1 | 0.319 | <0.001 | 0.330 | <0.001 | 0.338 | <0.001 |
SRC | 0.115 | 0.232 | 0.036 | 0.694 | 0.159 | 0.088 |
VP | 0.031 | 0.658 | 0.046 | 0.599 | 0.013 | 0.854 |
IAT | 0.002 | 0.981 | − 0.040 | 0.626 | − 0.007 | 0.942 |
WAT | 0.103 | 0.071 | − 0.012 | 0.874 | 0.116 | 0.053 |
WMC | − 0.040 | 0.537 | − 0.116 | 0.161 | − 0.066 | 0.452 |
PE | 0.148 | 0.002 | ||||
NE | 0.059 | 0.453 | ||||
SRC × WMC | − 0.020 | 0.833 | 0.051 | 0.686 | − 0.146 | 0.201 |
VP × WMC | − 0.021 | 0.713 | − 0.018 | 0.811 | 0.015 | 0.835 |
IAT × WMC | − 0.002 | 0.984 | − 0.107 | 0.364 | 0.001 | 0.997 |
WAT × WMC | 0.031 | 0.538 | 0.159 | 0.179 | − 0.008 | 0.916 |
PE × WMC | 0.063 | 0.452 | ||||
NE × WMC | 0.022 | 0.763 | ||||
SRC × PE | 0.153 | 0.128 | ||||
VP × PE | 0.019 | 0.837 | ||||
IAT × PE | 0.199 | 0.042 | ||||
WAT × PE | 0.053 | 0.385 | ||||
SRC × NE | − 0.238 | 0.035 | ||||
VP × NE | 0.006 | 0.946 | ||||
IAT × NE | 0.180 | 0.051 | ||||
WAT × NE | − 0.066 | 0.466 | ||||
SRC × WMC × PE | − 0.203 | 0.253 | ||||
VP × WMC × PE | − 0.033 | 0.732 | ||||
IAT × WMC × PE | 0.052 | 0.628 | ||||
WAT × WMC × PE | − 0.079 | 0.423 | ||||
SRC × WMC × NE | 0.087 | 0.557 | ||||
VP × WMC × NE | 0.118 | 0.295 | ||||
IAT × WMC × NE | 0.095 | 0.449 | ||||
WAT × WMC × NE | 0.045 | 0.558 |
Gender is coded as 0 = male and 1 = female. SRC is Stimulus Response Compatibility Task. VP is Visual Probe Task. IAT is Implicit Association Test. WAT is Word Association Task. PE is positive expectances. NE is negative expectancies. WMC is working memory capacity.
Predicting alcohol use from alcohol-related impulsive processes, working memory capacity, and positive expectancies
The second model included the same predictors as model 1, as well as the main effect of positive expectancies, the two-way interaction between positive expectancies and WMC, the two-way interactions between positive expectancies and the four impulsive measures, and the three-way interactions between positive expectancies, WMC, and alcohol-related impulsive processes. Changes in alcohol use were used as a dependent variable in this analysis. This model explained 26% (p < 0.001) of the variance of alcohol use in wave 2. Alcohol use in wave 1 and positive expectancies predicted alcohol use in wave 2. The main effect of positive expectancies was qualified by an interaction with alcohol–arousal associations. In order to interpret this interaction simple slopes were calculated for individuals with low (greater than −1 SD), medium (between −1 and 1 SD), and high ( >1 SD) values of positive expectancies [see Ref. (59)]. The results of these post hoc analyses are presented in Figure 1. Specifically, IAT scores were negatively associated with changes in alcohol use for adolescents with low levels of positive expectancies (b = −0.239, SE = 0.122, p = 0.050). The association between IAT scores and changes in alcohol use were not associated for adolescents with average and high levels of positive expectancies (b = −0.041, SE = 0.084, p = 0.626 and b = 0.157, SE = 0.137, p = 0.253, respectively). All other main and interaction effects were not statistically significant.
Predicting alcohol use from alcohol-related impulsive processes, working memory capacity, and negative expectancies
The third model also included the same predictors as model 2, but substituted the main and interaction effects involving positive expectancies with effects involving negative expectancies (R2 = 0.24, p < 0.001). Here too, changes in alcohol use were the dependent variable in this analysis. Aside from the main effect of alcohol use in wave 1, only one effect emerged as a statistically significant predictor: the two-way interaction between approach tendencies (SRC) and negative expectancies. Simple slopes were also calculated to interpret this interaction. Figure 2 presents the results of the association between approach tendencies and changes in alcohol use for high, medium, and low levels of negative expectancies. Specifically, SRC scores were positively associated with changes in alcohol use for adolescents with low levels of negative expectancies (b = 0.457, SE = 0.186, p = 0.014). The association between SRC scores and changes in alcohol use were not associated for adolescents with average and high levels of negative expectancies (b = 0.186, SE = 0.110, p = 0.091 and b = −0.085, SE = 0.121, p = 0.483, respectively). All other main and interaction effects were not statistically significant.
Discussion
This study aimed to predict adolescent alcohol use by implicit cognitions (approach tendencies, attentional bias, and memory associations), explicit cognitions (expectancies), and WMC. It was hypothesized that implicit cognitions and positive expectancies would predict an increase in alcohol use, adjusting for age, gender, and previous alcohol use. Negative expectancies and WMC would predict a decrease in alcohol use.
As expected from earlier studies, positive expectancies predicted alcohol use over time [e.g., Ref. (60, 61)]. Negative expectancies did not predict changes in alcohol use. We found a similar result in a previous study of 10-year-old children, where positive expectancies predicted alcohol use, while negative expectancies and implicit associations did not (44, 50). These findings also agree with other studies showing that positive expectancies are better predictors of increases in alcohol use of adolescents [e.g., Ref. (21, 22)] and young adults (62) than implicit cognitions. This is also in line with previous research suggesting that negative expectancies start to predict alcohol use when alcohol use becomes more common practice when negative effects of alcohol have been encountered before.
Implicit cognitions, and interactions between implicit cognitions and WMC or explicit cognitions, did not consequently predict changes in alcohol use. The interaction effect between the IAT and positive expectancies showed that only in adolescents with low positive expectancies, higher IAT scores were related to less alcohol use. In addition, the interaction effect between the SRC and negative expectancies demonstrated that only in adolescents with low negative expectancies, higher SRC scores were related to more alcohol use.
It was expected that alcohol–arousal associations would be related to alcohol use (41). This study showed that male adolescents who drank alcohol associated alcohol more strongly with arousal than male adolescents who did not drink alcohol. These boys were young adolescents as well, quite similar to the age as the adolescents in the current study. Yet, the sample of adolescents from the Thush et al. (42) study reported heavier drinking patterns than our sample, which could explain the absence of an effect. Related to this issue, it was shown that heavy drinkers had an attentional bias toward alcohol use compared to adolescents who consumed less alcohol (63). In addition, a recent study reported by our lab showed that attentional bias was related to alcohol use only in carriers of the mu-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) G-allele (51). In a study in young adult males who drank heavily, it was also shown that only OPRM1 G-allele carriers had an approach bias toward alcohol (64). From the above, we conclude that the non-significant findings from our study could be explained by the fact that the adolescents in our sample did not have enough experiences with alcohol use. Alternatively, the effects of implicit cognitions might be limited to adolescents with certain genotypes [e.g., Ref. (51)], or those with specific environmental risk factors. Based on previous studies [e.g., Ref. (42, 46)], we also expected that implicit cognitions would be more robustly related to alcohol use in adolescents with relatively poor WMC. Our findings were not in line with these studies [see also Ref. (52)] and therefore fail to support the dual process model of addiction.
Based on our findings, we argue that the dual process model of addiction may not be applicable to alcohol use in normative early adolescents. That is, the model, especially the impulsive route to alcohol use, does not seem to predict emerging alcohol use in these adolescents. There is empirical evidence for the validity of the model in predicting heavier alcohol use in adolescents, or alcohol abuse and dependence in adults. Yet, these observations do not seem to generalize to normative sample of early adolescents. Furthermore, we speculate that alcohol-induced sensitization of dopamine pathways in the brain may require more intense and frequent use. Initial and irregular alcohol use, which is so characteristic of early adolescents, may not be enough to incite sensitization.
Although this is the first longitudinal study to investigate elements of the dual process model and alcohol use in a normative sample of adolescents, it has various limitations. First, implicit cognitions, explicit cognitions, and WMC were assessed only once. As such, we were not able to examine the effect of alcohol use on subsequent implicit cognitions, explicit cognitions, and WMC. It could be that implicit cognitions do not predict increases in alcohol use yet in young adolescents, but that alcohol use does predict alterations in implicit cognitions. Nevertheless, this is beyond the scope of the current research aims. Second, our sample consisted of relatively normative adolescents, while other studies that do find associations between implicit cognitions and alcohol study less normative adolescents, e.g., from continuation high schools. At the same time, our study provides meaningful data in normative adolescents in a relatively large sample. Third, the interval between our assessments was relatively short (i.e., <1 year). Fourth, we used several indirect measures to measure implicit cognitions, but alternatives could be used in future research. In sum, future studies are warranted that employ longitudinal designs, on at-risk and normative samples to test the validity of parts of the dual process model in predicting more advanced stages of alcohol use in adolescents and young adults.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
- 1.Rooke SE, Hine DW, Thorsteinsson EB. Implicit cognition and substance use: a meta-analysis. Addict Behav (2008) 33:1314–28 10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.06.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Deutsch R, Strack F. Reflective and impulsive determinants of addictive behavior. In: Wiers RW, Stacy AW, editors. Handbook of Implicit Cognition and Addiction. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; (2006). p. 45–57 [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wiers RW, Bartholow BD, van den Wildenberg E, Thush C, Engels RC, Sher KJ, et al. Automatic and controlled processes and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescents. Pharmacol Biochem Behav (2007) 86:263–83 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.09.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Wiers RW, Ames SL, Hofmann W, Krank M, Stacy AW. Impulsivity, impulsive and reflective processes and the development of alcohol use and misuse in adolescents and young adults. Front Psychol (2010) 1:144. 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00144 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hofmann W, Friese M, Wiers RW. Impulsive versus reflective influences on health behavior: a theoretical framework and empirical review. Health Psychol Rev (2008) 2:111–37 10.1080/17437190802617668 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Sheeran P, Gollwitzer PM, Bargh JA. Nonconscious processes and health. Health Psychol (2013) 32:460–73 10.1037/a0029203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Arnett J. Reckless behavior in adolescence: a developmental perspective. Dev Rev (1992) 12:339–73 10.1016/0273-2297(92)90013-R18509515 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Casey BJ, Jones RM, Hare TA. The adolescent brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2008) 1124:111–26 10.1196/annals.1440.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Casey BJ, Jones RM. Neurobiology of the adolescent brain and behavior: implications for substance use disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2010) 49:1189–201 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.08.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ernst M, Pine DS, Hardin M. Triadic model of the neurobiology of motivated behavior in adolescence. Psychol Med (2006) 36:299–312 10.1017/S0033291705005891 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Ernst M, Romeo RD, Andersen SL. Neurobiology of the development of motivated behaviors in adolescence: a window into a neural systems model. Pharmacol Biochem Behav (2009) 93:199–211 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.12.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Galvan A, Hare TA, Parra CE, Penn J, Voss H, Glover G, et al. Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in adolescents. J Neurosci (2006) 26:6885–92 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1062-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Somerville LH, Casey BJ. Developmental neurobiology of cognitive control and motivational systems. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2010) 20:236–41 10.1016/j.conb.2010.01.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Rev (1993) 18:247–91 10.1016/0165-0173(93)90013-P [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Cameron CA, Stritzke WG, Durkin K. Alcohol expectancies in late childhood: an ambivalence perspective on transitions toward alcohol use. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2003) 44:687–98 10.1111/1469-7610.00155 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Wiers RW, Sergeant JA, Gunning WB. The assessment of alcohol expectancies in school children: measurement or modification? Addiction (2000) 95:737–46 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2000.9557379.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Dunn ME, Goldman MS. Empirical modeling of an alcohol expectancy memory network in elementary school children as a function of grade. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol (1996) 4:209–17 10.1037/1064-1297.4.2.209 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Jahoda G, Cramond J. Children and Alcohol: A Developmental Study in Glasgow. (Vol. 1). London: Her Majesty’s Stationery Office; (1972). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wiers RW, Gunning WB, Sergeant JA. Do young children of alcoholics hold more positive or negative alcohol-related expectancies than controls? Alcohol Clin Exp Res (1998) 22:1855–63 10.1097/00000374-199811000-00036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Dunn ME, Goldman MS. Age and drinking-related differences in the memory organization of alcohol expectances in 3rd-, 6th-, 9th-, and 12th-grade children. J Consult Clin Psychol (1998) 66:579–85 10.1037/0022-006X.66.3.579 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Cable N, Sacker A. Typologies of alcohol consumption in adolescence: predictors and adult outcomes. Alcohol Alcohol (2008) 43:81–90 10.1093/alcalc/agm146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Gerrard M, Gibbons FX, Benthin AC, Hessling RM. A longitudinal study of the reciprocal nature of risk behaviors and cognitions in adolescents: what you do shapes what you think, and vice versa. Health Psychol (1996) 15:344–54 10.1037/0278-6133.15.5.344 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Aas H, Klepp K, Laberg JC, Edvard L. Predicting adolescents’ intentions to drink alcohol: outcome expectancies and self-efficacy. J Stud Alcohol Drugs (1995) 156:293–9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Leigh BC, Stacy AW. On the scope of alcohol expectancy research: remaining issues of measurement and meaning. Psychol Bull (1991) 110:147–54 10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.147 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Cable N, Sacker A. The role of adolescent social disinhibition expectancies in moderating the relationship between psychological distress and alcohol use and misuse. Addict Behav (2007) 32:282–95 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.04.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Cooper ML. Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: development and validation of a four-factor model. Psychol Assess (1994) 6:117–28 10.1037/1040-3590.6.2.11717716823 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Goldman MS, Roehrich L. Alcohol expectancies and sexuality. Alcohol Health Res World (1991) 15:126–32 [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kuntsche E, Knibbe R, Gmel G, Engels R. Why do young people drink? A review of drinking motives. Clin Psychol Rev (2005) 25:841–61 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Fromme K, Stroot EA, Kaplan D. Comprehensive effects of alcohol: development and psychometric assessment of a new expectancy questionnaire. Psychol Assess (1993) 5:19–26 10.1037/a0028982 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Berridge KC. Reward learning: reinforcement, incentives, and expectations. In: Medin DL, editor. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory. Vol. 40. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; (2001). p. 223–78 [Google Scholar]
- 31.Squire LR, Knowlton B, Musen G. The structure and organization of memory. Annu Rev Psychol (1993) 44:453–95 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Yin HH, Knowlton BJ. The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nat Rev Neurosci (2006) 7:464–76 10.1038/nrn1919 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Yin HH, Knowlton BJ, Balleine BW. Inactivation of dorsolateral striatum enhances sensitivity to changes in the action-outcome contingency in instrumental conditioning. Behav Brain Res (2006) 166:189–96 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.07.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Smith ER, DeCoster J. Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Pers Soc Psychol Rev (2000) 4:108–31 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0402_01 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Gawronski B, Bodenhausen GV. Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: an integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychol Bull (2006) 132:692–731 10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.692 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Wiers RW, Stacy AW. Are alcohol expectancies associations? Comment on Moss and Albery (2009). Psychol Bull (2010) 136:12–6; discussion 17–20 10.1037/a0017769 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Wiers RW. Alcohol and drug expectancies as anticipated changes in affect: negative reinforcement is not sedation. Subst Use Misuse (2008) 43:429–44 10.1080/10826080701203021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Peeters M, Wiers RW, Monshouwer K, Schoot R, Janssen T, Vollebergh WA. Automatic processes in at-risk adolescents: the role of alcohol-approach tendencies and response inhibition in drinking behavior. Addiction (2012) 107:1939–46 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03948.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Peeters M, Monshouwer K, Schoot RA, Janssen T, Vollebergh WA, Wiers RW. Automatic processes and the drinking behavior in early adolescence: a prospective study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2013) 37:1737–44 10.1111/acer.12156 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Willem L, Vasey MW, Beckers T, Claes L, Bijttebier P. Cognitive biases and alcohol use in adolescence and young adulthood: the moderating role of gender, attentional control and inhibitory control. Pers Individ Dif (2013) 54:925–30 10.1016/j.paid.2013.01.015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Thush C, Wiers RW. Explicit and implicit alcohol-related cognitions and the prediction of future drinking in adolescents. Addict Behav (2007) 32:1367–83 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.09.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Thush C, Wiers RW, Ames SL, Grenard JL, Sussman S, Stacy AW. Interactions between implicit and explicit cognition and working memory capacity in the prediction of alcohol use in at-risk adolescents. Drug Alcohol Depend (2008) 94:116–24 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.10.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Van Der Vorst H, Krank M, Engels RC, Pieters S, Burk WJ, Mares SH. The mediating role of alcohol-related memory associations on the relation between perceived parental drinking and the onset of adolescents’ alcohol use. Addiction (2012) 108:526–33 10.1111/add.12042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Pieters S, van der Vorst H, Engels RC, Wiers RW. Implicit and explicit cognitions related to alcohol use in children. Addict Behav (2010) 35:471–8 10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.12.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.van Hemel-Ruiter ME, de Jong PJ, Wiers RW. Appetitive and regulatory processes in young adolescent drinkers. Addict Behav (2011) 36:18–26 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Grenard JL, Ames SL, Wiers RW, Thush C, Sussman S, Stacy AW. Working memory capacity moderates the predictive effects of drug-related associations on substance use. Psychol Addict Behav (2008) 22:426–32 10.1037/0893-164X.22.3.426 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Barkley RA. Executive Functions: What They Are, How They Work, and Why They Evolved. New York, NY: Guilford Press; (2012). [Google Scholar]
- 48.Hofmann W, Schmeichel BJ, Baddeley AD. Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends Cogn Sci (2012) 3:174–80 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol (2000) 41:49–100 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Pieters S, Van Der Vorst H, Burk WJ, Wiers RW, Engels RC. Puberty-dependent sleep regulation and alcohol use in early adolescents. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2010) 34:1512–8 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01235.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Pieters S, Van Der Vorst H, Burk WJ, Schoenmakers TM, Van Den Wildenberg E, Smeets HJ, et al. The effect of the OPRM1 and DRD4 polymorphisms on the relation between attentional bias and alcohol use in adolescence and young adulthood. Dev Cogn Neurosci (2011) 1:591–9 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.07.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Pieters S, Burk WJ, Van der Vorst H, Wiers RW, Engels RC. The moderating role of working memory capacity and alcohol-specific rule-setting on the relation between approach tendencies and alcohol use in young adolescents. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2012) 36:915–22 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01688.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Pieters S, Van Der Zwaluw CS, Van Der Vorst H, Wiers RW, Smeets H, Lambrichs E, et al. The moderating effect of alcohol-specific parental rule-setting on the relation between the dopamine D2 receptor gene (DRD2), the mu-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) and alcohol use in young adolescents. Alcohol Alcohol (2012) 47:663–70 10.1093/alcalc/ags075 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Engels RC, Knibbe RA, Drop MJ. Visiting public drinking places: an explorative study into the functions of pub-going for late adolescents. Subst Use Misuse (1999) 34:1261–80 10.3109/10826089909039408 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Field M, Kiernan A, Eastwood B, Child R. Rapid approach responses to alcohol cues in heavy drinkers. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry (2008) 39:209–18 10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Stacy AW. Memory association and ambiguous cues in models of alcohol and marijuana use. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol (1995) 3:183–94 10.1037/1064-1297.3.2.183 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Petrides M, Milner B. Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia (1982) 20:249–62 10.1016/0028-3932(82)90100-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Ross TP, Hanouskova E, Giarla K, Calhoun E, Tucker M. The reliability and validity of the self-ordered pointing task. Arch Clin Neuropsychol (2007) 22:449–58 10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Aiken LS, West SG. Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; (1991). [Google Scholar]
- 60.Chen MJ, Grube JW, Madden PA. Alcohol expectancies and adolescent drinking: differential prediction of frequency, quantity, and intoxication. Addict Behav (1994) 19:521–9 10.1016/0306-4603(94)90007-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Fromme K, D’Amico EJ. Measuring adolescent alcohol outcome expectancies. Psychol Addict Behav (2000) 14:206–12 10.1037/0893-164X.14.2.206 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Larsen H, Engels RC, Wiers RW, Granic I, Spijkerman R. Implicit and explicit alcohol cognitions and observed alcohol consumption: three studies in (semi) naturalistic drinking settings. Addiction (2012) 107:1420–8 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03805.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Field M, Christiansen P, Cole J, Goudie A. Delay discounting and the alcohol Stroop in heavy drinking adolescents. Addiction (2007) 102:579–86 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.01743.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Wiers RW, Rinck M, Dictus M, Van den Wildenberg E. Relatively strong automatic appetitive action-tendencies in male carriers of the OPRM1 G-allele. Genes Brain Behav (2009) 8:101–6 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2008.00454.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]