Abstract
The function and modulation of neural circuits underlying motor skill may involve rhythmic oscillations (Feller, 1999; Marder and Goaillard, 2006; Churchland et al., 2012). In the proposed pattern generator for birdsong, the cortical nucleus HVC, the frequency and power of oscillatory bursting during singing increases with development (Crandall et al., 2007; Day et al., 2009). We examined the maturation of cellular activity patterns that underlie these changes. Single unit ensemble recording combined with antidromic identification (Day et al., 2011) was used to study network development in anesthetized zebra finches. Autocovariance quantified oscillations within single units. A subset of neurons oscillated in the theta/alpha/mu/beta range (8–20 Hz), with greater power in adults compared to juveniles. Across the network, the normalized oscillatory power in the 8–20 Hz range was greater in adults than juveniles. In addition, the correlated activity between rhythmic neuron pairs increased with development. We next examined the functional impact of the oscillators on the output neurons of HVC. We found that the firing of oscillatory neurons negatively correlated with the activity of cortico-basal ganglia neurons (HVCXs), which project to Area X (the song basal ganglia). If groups of oscillators work together to tonically inhibit and precisely control the spike timing of adult HVCXs with coordinated release from inhibition, then the activity of HVCXs in juveniles should be decreased relative to adults due to uncorrelated, tonic inhibition. Consistent with this hypothesis, HVCXs had lower activity in juveniles. These data reveal network changes that shape cortical-to-basal ganglia signaling during motor learning.
Keywords: mu rhythm, oscillation, development, vocal learning, speech
INTRODUCTION
Neural encoding of complex learned motor skills is poorly understood. The best-characterized motor systems are rhythmic and, in these systems, network oscillations play key roles in motor control (Kiehn and Kjaerulff, 1998; Marder and Bucher, 2007; Koch et al., 2011). Network oscillations are also implicated in arrhythmic behaviors, such as reaching (Churchland et al., 2012). What guides the assembly of oscillatory networks (e.g., local or global rules; Prinz et al., 2004) is not well understood.
The classes of cortical neurons and their synaptic interactions are well characterized, particularly in the hippocampus and neocortex (Klausberberger et al., 2003; Buzsaki et al., 2004; Somogyi, 2004; Spruston, 2008; Cardin et al., 2009; Kullmann, 2011). In general, local interneurons sculpt and coordinate the activity of principal neurons that project to more distant targets (Buzsaki and Chrobak, 1995; Cobb et al., 1995; Koos and Tepper, 1999; Royer et al., 2012). However, current understanding of the cellular players in motor control and how specific neuronal sub-types interact to produce and modulate oscillatory activity remains incomplete.
Adult zebra finches produce a highly stereotyped, rhythmic song that is learned during a critical period (Thorpe, 1958; Konishi, 1965; Marler, 1970). Singing is controlled by anatomically distinct brain areas (Nottebohm et al., 1976; Reiner et al., 2004). The cortical song nucleus HVC (HVC is the proper name) drives the production of learned song and appears to be a pattern generator (Nottebohm et al., 1976; Simpson and Vicario, 1990; Vu et al., 1994; Ashmore et al., 2005; Solis and Perkel, 2005; Aronov et al., 2008; Long and Fee, 2008; Aronov and Fee, 2012). Consistent with a role of rhythmic oscillations in song motor control, HVC network oscillation frequency and power during singing increase with song development (Crandall et al., 2007; Day et al., 2009).
In the current study, we investigated the cellular mechanisms underlying maturation of oscillations by comparing HVC activity in juveniles and adults. Four-tetrode recordings were made in the HVCs of anesthetized finches. Replay activity in the song system during anesthesia and sleep resembles that which occurs during singing (Dave et al., 1998; Schmidt and Konishi, 1998; Dave and Margoliash, 2000). We examined oscillations within and between single neurons that were recorded simultaneously. Principal neurons that project to either the cortical motor Robust Nucleus of the Arcopallium (HVCRAs) or to the song basal ganglia (Farries and Perkel, 2002), Area X (HVCXs), were identified in the context of the network using antidromic stimulation. We found a rhythmic population of neurons that had an inverse firing relationship with the cortico-basal ganglia HVCXs. The oscillatory power and coordinated firing of the rhythmic neurons and the spiking activity of the HVCXs was developmentally regulated during the period of song learning.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Animals and Surgical Procedures
Juvenile and adult zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) were reared in our laboratory. Birds were housed under a 14:10-h light:dark cycle and were given food and water ad libitum. The University of Minnesota Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee approved all procedures. Surgical and recording methods were as previously described (Day et al., 2011). Tetrode recordings were obtained from 21 male zebra finches: juveniles were 45–66 days old (n = 9 finches, 12 recording sessions), adults were >138 days old (n = 12 finches, 18 sessions).
All animals were deprived of food and water for a minimum of 1 h before an initial intramuscular injection of 20% urethane (5 μL g–1). Two additional subdoses (not exceeding 30 μL dose–1) were given at 30- to 45-min intervals. The bird was placed in a stereotaxic apparatus (Herb Adams Engineering) and lidocaine (1%, Xylocaine) was injected under the scalp. After resection of the scalp, craniotomies were made over HVC, Area X, and RA.
Bipolar stimulating electrodes were constructed from 200 μm Teflon-coated tungsten rods (A-M Systems, Sequim, WA) and cemented into place with dental acrylic within Area X at a depth of 3.7–4.2 mm. A reference electrode (125 μm bare silver wire) was cemented in place ~3.5 mm anterior and 4 mm lateral of the lambda arterial sinus. A threaded headpost was cemented to the bird's skull. After this preparatory surgery, the animal was moved into a sound-attenuating chamber (Industrial Acoustics Company, New York) on an air table (TMC, Peabody, MA). Once the animal was secured by the headpost, a bipolar stimulating electrode (FHC, Bowdoin, ME) (300–500 kΩ impedance) was inserted into RA.
Voltage Recording and Electrical Stimulation
Ensembles of single units in HVC were recorded extracellularly in 10–15 min recording sessions with a 4-tetrode array (A4×1; Neuronexus Technologies, Ann Arbor, MI) that was amplified by a 16-channel headstage preamplifier (10×) that connected to a Model 3600 16-channel amplifier (A-M Systems). Tetrodes were linearly distributed within HVC and positioned relative to the midsagittal sinus in the rostralcaudal plane (parallel to the sinus) with a spacing of 150 μm. Tetrodes with a recording site area of 312 μm2 were used (0.5–2.0 MΩ impedance). Signals were amplified (1000×), filtered (1 or 300–10,000 Hz), and acquired at 22.05 kHz (PCI 6251; National Instruments, Austin, TX) under the control of custom written Matlab (Mathworks, Natick, MA) software. Activity in HVC was collected on 15-channels; the 4th recording site on one tetrode (#4) was not recorded to enable collection of chamber sound events on our analog-to-digital PCI card, which was limited to 16 channels.
Current stimulation (20–400 μA) was delivered by a stimulus isolation unit triggered by a Master 8 (AMPI, Jerusalem, Israel) to the efferent targets of HVC: Area X and RA. Only one target was stimulated at a time. Single, monophasic pulses of 200 μs duration were delivered at a rate of 0.5 Hz. Stimulation intensity was gradually increased until reliable antidromic spikes with few failures were observed in HVC.
Antidromic collision tests were executed in a subset of experiments to further confirm the antidromicity of the stimulation and to rule out the possibility of intervening synapses. For spike collisions, a two-window discriminator (FHC) was used to identify specific spontaneous spikes and selectively trigger stimulation within 1.5 ms.
Spike Feature Clustering
Spikes from multiple units on the same tetrode were sorted using automatic clustering of waveform features in 9–12 dimensions, depending on the number of recorded channels (tetrode 4 had only 3 channels due to use of the 4th channel for sound), followed by manual checking of each cluster. The 22-point (~1-ms) spike waveforms were used for clustering if they crossed a relatively low, predetermined threshold (median + 4 × standard deviation (STD); calculated independently on each channel for each recording session). Our conservative threshold included numerous “noise” events and clusters, which were subsequently rejected during manual cluster checking. Having a low threshold decreases the possibility of excluding part of a cluster. For each channel, features of thresholded waveforms were calculated (MClust, A.D. Redish) and subjected to unsupervised clustering with a Gaussian mixture model with unconstrained covariance matrices (KlustaKwik; K. Harris, Rutgers, http://klustakwik.sourceforge.net) to obtain a maximum of 30 initial “preclusters” using a classification expectation-maximization (CEM) algorithm. KlustaKwik allows for a variable number of clusters, penalized by the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC). The initial preclusters identified by KlustaKwik were manually checked and occasionally combined or split, using MClust 3.5 (A.D. Redish; http://redishlab.neuroscience.umn.edu/MClust/MClust.html) in the Matlab environment. Clustering was performed either on a Dell PC or blade server with a 64-bit processor.
Cluster quality was assessed using four criteria to ensure that sorted spikes accurately reflected single units. Clusters that did not pass all criteria were discarded. The four criteria for cluster quality were: (1) L-ratio score < 0.1; (2) isolation distance >16; (3) <1% violations of a 1-ms refractory period (Hirabayashi et al., 2013); and (4) visual separation of the cluster from “noise” and other clusters on at least 2 (of 12) dimensions. The L-ratio is a measure of the compactness of a cluster, whereas isolation distance quantifies how well a cluster separates from other clusters (Harris et al., 2000; Schmitzer-Torbert et al., 2005; Jackson et al., 2006). For spike waveform display and spike width analysis, a matrix of 64-point waveforms was created with the same indices as the 22-point waveform matrix that was used for clustering.
Developmental Neurobiology
Identification of Clustered Antidromic Single Units
All clusters used for further analysis, including those identified by antidromic stimulation, met the four criteria described above. Antidromic spikes coclustered with a subset of spontaneous spikes. Spontaneous spikes that triggered spike collisions also coclustered with antidromic spikes. The coclustering of spontaneous spikes with anti-dromic and/or collided spikes enabled the identification of spontaneous events as those of a principal neuron. Precise spike times relative to stimulation enabled the identification of fixed latency spikes and the determination of antidromic latency and its variability. Antidromic latency was defined as the mean time from stimulation to the peak of the action potential for at least 10 stimuli. Latency variability was defined as the STD of the latencies. The latency variability cut-off for identification of unit as a principal neuron was set at <125 μs. To exclude driving effects of stimulation as a mechanism of increased coherency, all spikes that were collected during and 100 ms after stimulation were excluded from analyses of waveform properties and network interactions. Spike width was calculated based on the duration of the positive peak of the action potential waveform at 25% of its maximum (Rauske et al., 2003).
Analyses of Spike Trains
The entire recording between antidromic stimulation epochs (excluding a 100-ms post-stimulus buffer) was used for spike width and spike train measurements. Spike rate for each single unit was defined as the total number of spikes divided by the total time in seconds. Interspike intervals (ISIs) were the time differences between successive spikes. The 95th percentile ISI is a robust measure of the maximal ISI. Burst durations were calculated with a maximum ISI of 10 ms to consider sequential spikes as part of the same burst. Sequential single spikes that had greater than 10 ms between them were each scored a burst duration of 0 ms.
Analyses of Oscillations
The entire recording between antidromic stimulation epochs (excluding a 100-ms post-stimulus buffer) was used for cross-covariance and coherency calculations. The binning window for spike trains was 1 ms. Cross-covariance was used to measure oscillatory activity between neurons
where CA-B(m) is the cross-covariance of two spike trains in the time domain. A and B are the spike train vectors. E is the expected value operator. μA and μB are the mean values.*indicates the complex conjugate. Autocovariance is the cross-covariance when the spike trains are identical and from the same single unit. Cross- and autocovariances were calculated using a Matlab built-in function (xcov.m). The amplitude spectra of the covariances were calculated using fast Fourier transform (fft.m).
Cross-covariance was used for this study because it is sensitive to oscillations. However, for comparison of the spike trains of HVCXs versus oscillatory neurons, we were most interested in fast functional (potentially synaptic) interactions, which are better measured with coherency (Rosenberg et al., 1989; Kimpo et al., 2003). Coherency was calculated as described (Rosenberg et al., 1989; Kimpo et al., 2003) with custom-written Matlab code. Coherency is the cross-covariance of the two spike trains normalized by the autocovariances of both (Rosenberg et al., 1989; Kimpo et al., 2003). Coherency was computed in the frequency domain where CA-B(ω) is the cross-covariance of the two spike trains in the frequency domain and CX-X(ω) is the autocovariance. For further analysis and plotting, γA-Bω was transformed into the time domain (Kimpo et al., 2003). Coherency reflects the relative probability of one neuron firing, given that the other has fired at time t = 0. Coherency values are unitless and range between –1 and 1. A large, positive coherency value at time –5 ms, for example, would indicate that every time one unit fired, the other was highly likely to fire 5 ms earlier. Same tetrode data are contaminated by overlapping waveforms at t = 0 ms for all spike train relationship measures. The jackknife STD was obtained using custom Matlab code (D. Q. Nykamp and T. A. Nick) that calculated the coherency 100 times, each time with a recording segment of 1% length thrown out. The STD was then computed from the resulting 100 coherency vectors. Significant negative coherency was defined as less than 3× jackknife STD.
Statistics
All measures of central tendency and dispersal are shown as median with interquartile range (IQR). The nonparametric Wilcoxon rank sum test was used to compare unmatched data. Significance was defined as a α = 0.05.
RESULTS
Recording Oscillations in the HVC Network
The development of oscillatory activity was examined in the HVC network using a four-tetrode recording array combined with antidromic identification of principal neurons (Fig. 1; Day et al., 2011). With this configuration, multiple single units can be sampled along 450 μm of the rostral-caudal axis of HVC [Fig.1(B,C)]. Consistent with previous population-level findings in the chronically recorded sleeping finch (Crandall et al., 2007), the HVC activity patterns of the juvenile (with plastic song) were subjectively and obviously different from the adult (with stable song). The most striking difference was in synchronous bursts that tended to be extended and involve a greater percentage of the network in the adult (Burst duration, Juv (229): median 6.24 ms, IQR [5.14– 7.49] ms; Adult (227): 6.60 ms, [5.57–8.16] ms; p < 0.007, Wilcoxon Rank Sum). In these same recordings, we also recently found that spike train coherency of HVC neuron pairs increases with development (Day et al., 2013), which indicates increased population synchrony with development.
A Subset of HVC Neurons Oscillate
Neuronal synchrony and extended patterns of activity that are not peripherally driven are thought to arise from network oscillations (Engel et al., 2001). To assess oscillatory activity, the amplitude spectrum of the spike train autocovariance was calculated for each single unit in juveniles and adults. A subset of recorded neurons had oscillations that were readily apparent in the autocovariance. Example spike trains, autocovariances, and amplitude spectra are shown in Figure 2. Juvenile oscillatory neurons tended to have brief periods of firing with a relatively stable ISI intermingled with highly variable ISIs [Fig. 2(A)]. In contrast, adult oscillators fired for longer periods with a relatively fixed ISI [Fig. 2(D)]. These spike train patterns were reflected in the autocovariance in the time domain [Fig. 2(B,E)], which had multiple, regularly spaced peaks and valleys, and in the amplitude spectra of the autocovariances [Fig. 2(C,F)], which had peaks in the range 8–20 Hz. This frequency range includes theta (θ), alpha (α), mu (μ) and low beta (β) ranges. The definition of these ranges varies with animal (including human) and brain area. In the interest of brevity and not any functional interpretation, we will refer to this oscillation range (8–20 Hz) as “α” in this manuscript.
Adult Neurons and Networks Have More Oscillatory Power in the 8–20 Hz Range
To obtain a view of the oscillatory activity of the HVC population, we plotted the frequency of the peak autocovariance versus the power of that peak for each single unit [Fig. 3(A)]. In the range 8–20 Hz, there were 11 (of 227) adult neurons that had a higher peak at a given frequency than any juvenile neuron. The maximum power of α-oscillators (that had their peak power 8–20 Hz) was significantly and substantially (276%) greater in adults relative to juveniles [Fig. 3(B)]. To assess the network effects of the increasing rhythmicity of the α-oscillator neurons, the relative α power was compared across all other HVC neurons (that did not have an 8–20 Hz autoco-variance peak) by taking a power ratio (median power 8–20 Hz/median power 0–200 Hz). This value was also significantly and substantially (173%) greater in adults than juveniles [Fig. 3(C)].
Activities of α-oscillator Neurons Differ from the Rest of the HVC Population and do not Change with Development
Neurons identified as α-oscillators were compared to the rest of the HVC population. In both juveniles [Fig. 4(A)] and adults [Fig. 4(B)], α-oscillators compared to the rest of the population had narrower action potentials, higher spike rates, lower 95th percentile ISIs (a robust assessment of maximal ISI), and shorter high-frequency bursts. Narrower spike widths and higher spike rates are characteristic of interneurons (Mooney, 2000; Rauske et al., 2003). The low 95th percentile ISI measure indicates that these α-oscillator neurons were never quiescent for very long. The shorter high-frequency burst measure indicates that the α-oscillators tend to fire only briefly at high frequencies (≥100 Hz).
In contrast to oscillation power, which increased with development (Fig. 3), spike width and spike train parameters of α-oscillators did not differ between juveniles and adults [Fig. 4(C)]. This indicates that the observed change in oscillation power cannot be attributed to changes in these measures, but rather results from a redistribution of ongoing activity such that ISIs become more stereotyped overall and more invariant in longer spiking sequences: The activity of the α-oscillators becomes more rhythmic with song development.
Pairs of α-oscillator Neurons Become More Correlated During Development
To test if α-oscillator neurons play a role in HVC-wide network synchrony, we measured the correlated activity of pairs of α-oscillators using cross-covariance. If a network of α-oscillators coordinate or transmit network synchronous oscillations, then their oscillations must be correlated. Example spike train pairs, cross-covariances, and amplitude spectra are shown in Figure 5. The α-oscillator neurons in these examples were 300 μm apart. From the juvenile [Fig. 5(A–C)], the best example of α-oscillation in the cross-covariance that we observed shows weak and variable relationships between spike trains. In the cross-covariance [Fig. 5(B)], there is a relatively high peak indicative of synchrony, then a decrease indicative of decreased probability of firing, but the oscillation quickly dies out. In contrast, the adult α-oscillator pair [Fig. 5(D–F)] had relatively reliable spike timing relationships that were reflected in the cross-covariance, which had multiple peaks [Fig. 5(E)]. The amplitude spectrum of the cross-covariance revealed a strong peak in the α range [Fig. 5(F)], indicating that the adult α-oscillator pair fires together in a coordinated pattern in the range 8–20 Hz.
Pairs of Mature α-oscillator Neurons are Significantly More Correlated with Each Other and Other Neurons
The correlated activity of α-oscillator pairs was quantified by taking the peak power of the amplitude spectrum of the cross-covariance [Fig. 6(A)]. Adult pairs were significantly more correlated. Next, the impact of α-oscillator activity on other neurons was addressed by comparing the correlated activity of α-oscillator neurons with all other neurons in the HVC population. The α-oscillation power ratio was significantly greater in adults. This indicates that α-oscillator neurons functionally interact with the broader HVC population in adults, but not or less so in juveniles.
α-Oscillator Neurons Functionally and Tonically Inhibit HVCXs
Do α-oscillator neurons affect the output of HVC? To answer this question, we examined the spike train relationships of α-oscillator neurons with HVC principal neurons: HVCXs and HVCRAs. We were able to obtain a small number of recordings that contained both an antidromically identified HVCX neuron and an α-oscillator neuron (N sessions, Juvenile: 9; Adult: 10). We rarely saw HVCRAs and α-oscillators in the same recording session (N sessions, Juvenile: 2; Adult: 1), possibly due to the differential anatomical distribution of HVCRAs (Wild et al., 2005) and, perhaps, the α-oscillators. Alternatively, RA stimulation necessary for antidromic identification may alter HVC rhythmicity. Previous work that has found only a little evidence of inhibition of HVCRAs (Mooney, 2000; Mooney and Prather, 2005), so there may be no reliable relationship between the activities of α-oscillator neurons and HVCRAs. In contrast, we found three adult oscillator-HVCX pairs and all three pairs showed the same pattern: α-oscillators were reliably silent at least 2 ms prior to the firing of HVCXs [Fig. 7(A–C)]. Both members of each pair were always on the same tetrode, indicating that the strongest (and most detectable) functional interactions between α-oscillators and HVCXs are highly localized. We did not see a similar pattern in any of the juvenile pairs.
The Activity of HVCXs is Developmentally Regulated
Our finding that adult α-oscillator neurons were silent before the firing of HVCXs (Fig. 7) indicates that α-oscillators functionally inhibit HVCXs. This suggests that α-oscillators sculpt HVCX firing times through coordinated release from inhibition. The juvenile data, which indicate no such relationship, could result either from a lack of functional inhibition from α-oscillators or, alternatively, from disorganized inhibition from multiple α-oscillators (which becomes increasingly organized as the animal matures, Figs. 5 and 6). If the former hypothesis is correct (and other factors affecting HVCX activity, such as extrinsic inputs and intrinsic excitability, are relatively constant), then HVCXs should show more activity in the juvenile. Alternatively, if the lack of an oscillator-HVCX relationship in juveniles results from disorganized inhibition from multiple α-oscillators, then HVCXs should show less activity in the juvenile. Consistent with the latter hypothesis, we found that spike rates and burst durations of HVCXs were significantly lower in the juvenile compared to the adult [Fig. 8(A)]. The activity of the other population of HVC principal neurons, HVCRAs, did not significantly change and showed opposite trends in spike rate with development [Fig. 8(B)].
DISCUSSION
A Subclass of HVC Neurons Oscillate and the Power of the Oscillation is Developmentally Regulated
With developmental song learning, the oscillatory bursting activity of the HVC population during singing increases (Crandall et al., 2007; Day et al., 2009). Correspondingly, there are cellular and network oscillations within HVC that increase with developmental song learning: We found that spiking of a subtype of HVC neuron oscillates in the 8–20 Hz range and the power of the oscillation increases with development. At the network level, the correlation of spiking activity in pairs of α-oscillatory neurons increased with development.
Network Oscillations are Important for Function in the Mammalian Brain
Rhythmic oscillations, which are measured in local field potential recordings and the activity of single neurons, shape the firing of principal neurons (Llinas et al., 1991; Klausberberger et al., 2003; Sirota et al., 2008; Burgess and O’Keefe, 2011; Buzs aki and Wang, 2012). Fundamental cortical processes, such as attention, memory, and sensory processing, are regulated by rhythmic oscillations (Benchenane et al., 2011). The role of rhythmic activity in cortical motor control is less clear, but recent evidence highlights the role of oscillations even in arrhythmic behaviors, such as reaching (Churchland et al., 2012). The importance of the HVC oscillations that we describe here is not known. Song is a rhythmic behavior and HVC appears to be its pattern generator (Nottebohm et al., 1976; Simpson and Vicario, 1990; Vu et al., 1994; Ashmore et al., 2005; Solis and Perkel, 2005; Aronov et al., 2008; Long and Fee, 2008). Thus, these oscillations may serve, most simply, to provide a timing cue for the production of learned vocalizations. They may, alternatively or in addition, have roles in the regulation of song plasticity and motor learning (Day et al., 2008, 2009).
Developmental Modulation of Network Oscillations Present yet Another Similarity Between Avian and Mammalian Brains
The finding that rhythmic oscillations are developmentally regulated during the critical period for sensorimotor learning identifies the song system as a good model for studying the mechanisms of oscillations and their role in cortical processing in the context of behavior. Previous work has shown that brain oscillations in humans and other mammals are developmentally regulated (Gramsbergen, 1976; Clarke et al., 2001; Marshall et al., 2002) and that aberrations in brain oscillations correlate with neural disease (Cantor et al., 1986; Stroganova et al., 2007; Uhlhaas and Singer, 2010). Notably, neural oscillations may have a role in speech processing (Giraud and Poeppel, 2012), which, like birdsong, is a learned vocalization (Marler, 1970; Doupe and Kuhl, 1999; Bolhuis et al., 2010). We have previously found cellular and molecular similarities between the forebrain song system and mammalian brain systems that have a critical period in their development: prolonged bursting of some neurons (Fagiolini and Hensch, 2000; Crandall et al., 2007) and developmental regulation of parvalbumin and extracellular matrix perineuronal nets (Hockfield and McKay, 1983; Hensch, 2005; Balmer et al., 2009). It will be interesting to determine whether α-oscillator neurons express parvalbumin or perineuronal nets and if their activity is affected by perineuronal net perturbation, as in mammalian neurons (Dityatev and Schachner, 2006; Dityatev et al., 2007). Understanding the interactions of extracellular matrix, interneurons, and oscillations in the tractable birdsong behavioral model may illuminate the roles of all three in neural plasticity and, potentially, human disease.
Mature α-Oscillator Neurons Resemble Rhythmic Neurons in Mammalian Brain
Certain neurons in the mammalian brain oscillate in the range that we report (8–20 Hz). The theta rhythm generated in the hippocampus (allocortex) appears to result from the interactions of principal neurons and interneurons in the stratum radiatum (Goutagny et al., 2009). Interneurons in the stratum oriens of the hippocampus, oriens-lacunosum moleculare (O-LM) neurons, phase-lock to theta-modulated inputs (Kispersky et al., 2012), and fire in the lower range of the oscillatory neurons described here. O-LM cells may specialize in the transmission and amplification of theta rhythms (Kispersky et al., 2012) and form an anatomical framework for the transmission of theta in the longitudinal axis of the hippocampus (Gloveli et al., 2005). Pyramidal cells of neocortical layer 5 can oscillate within the theta/ alpha range (5–12 Hz; Silva et al., 1991). Mu rhythm (15–25 Hz) suppression may be necessary for the firing of mirror neurons, which belong to a subclass of pyramidal neurons discovered in layer 5 that appear to link perception of an action with the action itself (Hari et al., 1998; Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004; Arnstein et al., 2011). The suppression of mu rhythm is often used as a metric for mirror neuron activity (Pineda, 2005; Nystr€om et al., 2010). These facts are interesting in light of our data that show that the proposed song mirror neurons, the HVCXs (Prather et al., 2008), fire during suppression of mu-range oscillators.
The origin of at least some mammalian oscillations, including neocortical gamma, is not clear. They may be produced solely by interneurons or the interactions of interneurons and principal neurons (Tiesinga and Sejnowski, 2009). We do not know the generator of the 8–20 Hz rhythm detected in the α-oscillatory neurons, which could come from intrinsic and/or extrinsic sources. However, stimulating HVC resets the song (Vu et al., 1994) and cooling results in the slowing of song (Long and Fee, 2008), both indicative of an intrinsic pattern generator. It will be interesting to determine whether α-oscillator neurons, similar to hippocampal O-LM neurons (Gloveli et al., 2005), provide an anatomical structure for theta/alpha transmission and thus define a functional axis within the HVC pattern generator (Nottebohm et al., 1982; Stauffer et al., 2012; Day et al., 2013).
Potential Functional Roles of the Oscillations
Low frequency (theta/alpha/mu/beta) oscillations, such as we describe here, are thought to be important for the transmission of neural information long-distances between brain areas during top-down processing, whereas higher frequency (high beta/gamma) oscillations are thought to reflect local information processing (von Stein and Sarnthein, 2000). In the adult finch, HVC may effectively transmit to downstream areas and thus drive learned song because of the development of low frequency oscillations. In contrast, HVC does not appear to be important for unlearned juvenile vocalizations, since animals can produce unlearned and rambling sounds when HVC is completely ablated (Simpson and Vicario, 1990; Aronov et al., 2008). As song matures, HVC is then free to “try out” various network activity patterns and transmit them during unstructured rambling. Because HVC receives convergent inputs from several auditory areas (Mello et al., 1998; Coleman and Mooney, 2002; Cardin et al., 2004; Coleman et al., 2007; Bauer et al., 2008), it is well positioned to reinforce (or discard) these patterns or to forward the information to downstream areas that ultimately feed back to HVC through midbrain dopaminergic circuits or other pathways (Hamaguchi and Mooney, 2012).
HVC Oscillatory Neurons Functionally Inhibit Cortico-Basal Ganglia Neurons
The observed changes in the oscillation power and coordination of rhythmic neurons directly impacted the output of the HVC circuit: HVCX cortico-basal ganglia neurons were functionally inhibited by the rhythmic α-oscillator neurons. Consistent with an increase in the coordination of inhibition and release of inhibition, HVCXs increased their activity levels with development. In addition to changes in coordinated inhibition, other factors that may affect the development of HVCX activity include the expression patterns of voltage-gated ion channels and extrinsic inputs. The current study does not address these factors. Instead, it shows developmental regulation of spike rates and coordinated activation of neurons that functionally inhibit HVCXs.
We have previously found that activation of the adult, but not juvenile, HVC is rostrocaudally ordered, such that projection neurons fire preferentially with fast-spiking presumed interneurons that are rostral to them. In addition, projection neurons appear to fire in order from rostral to caudal (Day et al., 2013). The results of this previous study are consistent with traveling waves of inhibition within HVC that are led by projection neurons that fire at the leading edge. How would a traveling wave of inhibition lead to the activation of projection neurons? The data in the current study indicate that HVCXs fire during decreases in inhibition. In addition, previous brain slice studies have shown post-inhibitory rebound excitation in HVCXs (Dutar et al., 1998). All of these data, combined with our current finding of developmental regulation of functionally inhibitory neurons, suggest a coordinated disinhibition model of cortico-basal ganglia signaling: In the juvenile, oscillatory inter-neurons are uncoordinated, subjecting HVCXs to continuous, weak inhibition. As oscillatory interneurons mature, they fire more regularly and in a coordinated manner, such that HVCXs are more effectively inhibited and disinhibited during discrete time windows. During disinhibited windows, HVCXs rebound from inhibition and fire. In the adult, waves of inhibition may inhibit the oscillatory interneurons, thus precisely coordinating the activation of HVCXs in a spatiotemporal map.
What would be the impact of precise coordination of cortico-basal ganglia signaling in the song system? We have previously found that patterned bursting activity in HVC predicts song variability millisecond-by-millisecond in chronically recorded, singing juvenile and adult finches (Day et al., 2008, 2009). On the basis of these data, we proposed that HVC limits song plasticity by entraining the activity of the Anterior Forebrain Pathway (AFP) with coordinated bursts (Day et al., 2008, 2009). This model was further supported by a recent study that found that lesions of the basal ganglia target nucleus of HVC within the AFP (Area X) perturbed patterned bursting in the AFP cortical nucleus and blocked auditory modulation of song (Kojima et al., 2013).
We found that cortico-basal ganglia signaling increases with development. What is the general function of cortico-basal ganglia signaling? Previous work has shown that HVCXs have the same activity patterns whether they are hearing or producing (with no auditory feedback) a song (Prather et al., 2008), thus suggesting that these cells are mirror neurons (Hilliard and White, 2009; Fogassi, 2011) and that they function to provide a motor signal that can be compared to auditory feedback in order to perceive and sculpt the learned vocalization. Our current data indicate that these neurons provide weaker signals to the basal ganglia in juveniles. If a weak signal is interpreted by efferent areas as a “mistake” or an indication to continue production of variable songs, then song plasticity may be permitted or enhanced through previously described pathways (Bottjer et al., 1984; Kao et al., 2005; Olveczky et al., 2005; Kao and Brainard, 2006). Whether the proposed motor signal for auditory feedback comparison (Prather et al., 2008) is interpreted based on the performance of an entire song motif (~1 sec) or smaller time segment, is not known. However, previous work suggests that song is learned in relatively short duration segments (smaller than a song syllable; Ravbar et al., 2012) and HVC regulates plasticity millisecond-by-millisecond (Day et al., 2008). Short duration learned fragments would be outlasted by prolonged bursts within the HVC network that we have previously described (Crandall et al., 2007). Thus, sensory feedback from a song fragment could act directly on elements of the network that produced the fragment, shape the coordinated activity of inhibitors and/or disinhibitors of HVCXs, and thereby control cortico-basal ganglia signaling.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank A. Arneja, B. Best, V. Carels, G. Giesler, G. Larson, D. Q. Nykamp, A.D. Redish, and K. Terleski for technical assistance. They thank A. D. Redish for reading a preliminary draft of the manuscript.
Contract grant sponsor: National Science Foundation; contract grant number: IOS/BIO-1025825.
Contract grant sponsor: NIGMS; contract grant number: NI5T32-GM008471-15.
Contract grant sponsor: MN Dissertation Fellowship.
REFERENCES
- Arnstein D, Cui F, Keysers C, Maurits NM, Gazzola V. mu-suppression during action observation and execution correlates with BOLD in dorsal premotor, inferior parietal, and SI cortices. J Neurosci. 2011;31:14243–14249. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0963-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aronov D, Andalman AS, Fee MS. A specialized forebrain circuit for vocal babbling in the juvenile song-bird. Science. 2008;320:630–634. doi: 10.1126/science.1155140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aronov D, Fee MS. Natural changes in brain temperature underlie variations in song tempo during a mating behavior. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e47856. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047856. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashmore RC, Wild JM, Schmidt MF. Brainstem and forebrain contributions to the generation of learned motor behaviors for song. J Neurosci. 2005;25:8543–8554. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1668-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balmer TS, Carels VM, Frisch JL, Nick TA. Modulation of perineuronal nets and parvalbumin during developmental song learning. J Neurosci. 2009;29:12878–12885. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2974-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bauer EE, Coleman MJ, Roberts TF, Roy A, Prather JF, Mooney R. A synaptic basis for auditory-vocal integration in the songbird. J Neurosci. 2008;28:1509–1522. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3838-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benchenane K, Tiesinga PH, Battaglia FP. Oscillations in the prefrontal cortex: A gateway to memory and attention. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2011;21:475–485. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolhuis JJ, Okanoya K, Scharff C. Twitter evolution: Converging mechanisms in birdsong and human speech. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:747–759. doi: 10.1038/nrn2931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bottjer S, Miesner E, Arnold A. Forebrain lesions disrupt development but not maintenance of song in pass-erine birds. Science. 1984;224:901–903. doi: 10.1126/science.6719123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgess N, O'Keefe J. Models of place and grid cell firing and theta rhythmicity. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2011;21:734–744. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsaki G, Chrobak JJ. Temporal structure in spatially organized neuronal ensembles: A role for inter-neuronal networks. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1995;5:504–510. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(95)80012-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsaki G, Geisler C, Henze DA, Wang X-J. Inter-neuron diversity series: Circuit complexity and axon wiring economy of cortical interneurons. TINS. 2004;27:186–193. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsáki G, Wang XJ. Mechanisms of gamma oscillations. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2012;35:203–225. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cantor DS, Thatcher RW, Hrybyk M, Kaye H. Computerized EEG analyses of autistic children. J Autism Dev Disord. 1986;16:169–187. doi: 10.1007/BF01531728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardin JA, Carlen M, Meletis K, Knoblich U, Zhang F, Deisseroth K, Tsai L-H, et al. Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and controls sensory responses. Nature. 2009;459:663–667. doi: 10.1038/nature08002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardin JA, Raksin JN, Schmidt MF. Sensorimotor nucleus NIf is necessary for auditory processing but not vocal motor output in the avian song system. J Neurophysiol. 2004;93:2157–2166. doi: 10.1152/jn.01001.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Churchland MM, Cunningham JP, Kaufman MT, Foster JD, Nuyujukian P, Ryu SI, Shenoy KV. Neural population dynamics during reaching. Nature. 2012;7405:51–56. doi: 10.1038/nature11129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clarke AR, Barry RJ, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M. Age and sex effects in the EEG: Development of the normal child. Clin Neurophys. 2001;112:806–814. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(01)00488-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cobb SR, Buhl EH, Halasy K, Paulsen O, Somogyi P. Synchronization of neuronal activity in hippocampus by individual GABAergic interneurons. Nature. 1995;378:75–78. doi: 10.1038/378075a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coleman MJ, Mooney R. Source of auditory input to the songbird nucleus HVc revealed by pairwise recordings of NIf and HVc. Soc Neurosci Abs. 2002;588:4. [Google Scholar]
- Coleman MJ, Roy A, Wild JM, Mooney R. Thalamic gating of auditory responses in telencephalic song control nuclei. J Neurosci. 2007;27:10024–10036. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2215-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crandall SR, Adam M, Kinnischtzke AK, Nick TA. HVC neural sleep activity increases with development and parallels nightly changes in song behavior. J Neurophysiol. 2007;98:232–240. doi: 10.1152/jn.00128.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crandall SR, Aoki N, Nick TA. Developmental modulation of the temporal relationship between brain and behavior. J Neurophysiol. 2007;97:806–816. doi: 10.1152/jn.00907.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dave AS, Margoliash D. Song replay during sleep and computational rules for sensorimotor vocal learning. Science. 2000;290:812–816. doi: 10.1126/science.290.5492.812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dave AS, Yu AC, Margoliash D. Behavioral state modulation of auditory activity in a vocal motor system. Science. 1998;282:2250–2254. doi: 10.1126/science.282.5397.2250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Day N, Kinnischtzke AK, Adam M, Nick TA. Top-down regulation of plasticity in the birdsong system: “Premotor” activity in the nucleus HVC predicts song variability better than it predicts song features. J Neurophys. 2008;100:2956–2965. doi: 10.1152/jn.90501.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Day N, Kinnischtzke AK, Adam M, Nick TA. Daily and developmental modulation of “premotor” activity in the birdsong system. Dev Neurobiol. 2009;69:796–810. doi: 10.1002/dneu.20739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Day NF, Kerrigan SJ, Aoki N, Nick TA. Identification of single neurons in a forebrain network. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106:3205–3215. doi: 10.1152/jn.00468.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Day NF, Terleski KL, Nykamp DQ, Nick TA. Directed functional connectivity matures with motor learning in a cortical pattern generator. J Neurophysiol. 2013;109:913–923. doi: 10.1152/jn.00937.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dityatev A, Bruckner G, Dityateva G, Grosche J, Kleene R, Schachner M. Activity-dependent formation and functions of chondroitin sulfate-rich extracellular matrix of perineuronal nets. Dev Neurobiol. 2007;67:570–588. doi: 10.1002/dneu.20361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dityatev A, Schachner M. The extracellular matrix and synapses. Cell Tissue Res. 2006;326:647–654. doi: 10.1007/s00441-006-0217-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doupe AJ, Kuhl PK. Birdsong and human speech: Common themes and mechanisms. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1999;22:567–631. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.22.1.567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dutar P, Vu H, Perkel DJ. Multiple cell types distinquished by physiological, pharmacological, and anatomic properties in nucleus HVc of the adult zebra finch. J Neurophys. 1998;80:1828–1838. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.4.1828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engel AK, Fries P, Singer W. Dynamic predictions: Oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2:704–716. doi: 10.1038/35094565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fagiolini M, Hensch TK. Inhibitory threshold for critical-period activation in primary visual cortex. Nature. 2000;404:183–186. doi: 10.1038/35004582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farries MA, Perkel DJ. A telencephalic nucleus essential for song learning contains neurons with physiological characteristics of both striatum and globus pallidus. J Neurosci. 2002;22:3776–3787. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-09-03776.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feller MB. Spontaneous correlated activity in developing neural circuits. Neuron. 1999;22:653. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80724-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fogassi L. The mirror neuron system: How cognitive functions emerge from motor organization. J Econ Behav Organ. 2011;77:66–75. [Google Scholar]
- Giraud A-L, Poeppel D. Cortical oscillations and speech processing: Emerging computational principles and operations. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15:511–517. doi: 10.1038/nn.3063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gloveli T, Nugladze T, Rotstein HG, Traub RD, Monyer H, Heinemann U, Whittington MA, et al. Orthogonal arrangement of rhythm-generating microcircuits in the hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2005;102:13295–13300. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0506259102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goutagny R, Jackson J, Williams S. Self-generated theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Nat Publ Group. 2009;12:1491–1493. doi: 10.1038/nn.2440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gramsbergen A. The development of the EEG in the rat. Dev Psychobiol. 1976;9:501–515. doi: 10.1002/dev.420090604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamaguchi K, Mooney R. Recurrent interactions between the input and output of a songbird cortico-basal ganglia pathway are implicated in vocal sequence variability. J Neurosci. 2012;32:11671–11687. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1666-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hari R, Forss N, Avikainen S, Kirkveskari E, Salenius S, Rizzolatti G. Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: A neuromagnetic study. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95:15061–15065. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.25.15061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris KD, Henze DA, Csicsvari J, Hirase H, Buzsaki G. Accuracy of tetrode spike separation as determined by simultaneous intracellular and extracellular measurements. J Neurophys. 2000;84:401–414. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.1.401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hensch TK. Critical period plasticity in local cortical circuits. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:877–888. doi: 10.1038/nrn1787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hilliard AT, White SA. Evolutionary precursors of syntax. In: Bickerton D, Szathmáry E, Bickerton D, Szathmáry ES, editors. Biological Foundations and Origin of Syntax. MIT Press; Cambridge, MA: 2009. pp. 161–182. [Google Scholar]
- Hirabayashi T, Takeuchi D, Tamura K, Miyashita Y. Functional microcircuit recruited during retrieval of object association memory in monkey perirhinal cortex. Neuron. 2013;77:192–203. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hockfield S, McKay RDG. A surface antigen expressed by a subset of neurons in the vertebrate central nervous system. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1983;80:5758–5761. doi: 10.1073/pnas.80.18.5758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson JC, Johnson A, Redish AD. Hippocampal sharp waves and reactivation during awake states depend on repeated sequential experience. J Neurosci. 2006;26:12415–12426. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4118-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kao MH, Brainard MS. Lesions of an avian basal ganglia circuit prevent context-dependent changes to song variability. J Neurophys. 2006;96:1441–1455. doi: 10.1152/jn.01138.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kao MH, Doupe AJ, Brainard MS. Contributions of an avian basal ganglia-forebrain circuit to real-time modulation of song. Nature. 2005;433:638–643. doi: 10.1038/nature03127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiehn O, Kjaerulff O. Distribution of central pattern generators for rhythmic motor outputs in the spinal cord of limbed vertebrates. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1998;860:110–129. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09043.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimpo RR, Theunissen FE, Doupe AJ. Propagation of correlated activity through multiple stages of a neural circuit. J Neurosci. 2003;23:5750–5761. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-13-05750.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kispersky TJ, Fernandez FR, Economo MN, White JA. Spike resonance properties in hippocampal O-LM cells are dependent on refractory dynamics. J Neurosci. 2012;32:3637–3651. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1361-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klausberberger T, Magill PJ, Marton LF, Roberts JDB, Cobden PM, Buzsaki G, Somogyi P. Brain-state-and cell-type-specific firing of hippocampal interneurons in vivo. Nature. 2003;421:844–848. doi: 10.1038/nature01374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koch H, Garcia AJ, Ramirez JM. Network reconfigu-ration and neuronal plasticity in rhythm-generating networks. Integr Comp Biol. 2011;51:856–868. doi: 10.1093/icb/icr099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kojima S, Kao MH, Doupe AJ. Task-related " cortical" bursting depends critically on basal ganglia input and is linked to vocal plasticity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110:4756–4761. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1216308110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konishi M. The role of auditory feedback in the control of vocalization in the white-crowned sparrow. Z Tierpsychol. 1965;22:770–783. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koos T, Tepper JM. Inhibitory control of neostriatal projection neurons by GABAergic interneurons. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:467–472. doi: 10.1038/8138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kullmann DM. Interneuron networks in the hippo-campus. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2011;21:709–716. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Llinás RR, Grace AA, Yarom Y. In vitro neurons in mammalian cortical layer 4 exhibit intrinsic oscillatory activity in the 10- to 50-Hz frequencyrange. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1991;88:897–901. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.3.897. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Long MA, Fee MS. Using temperature to analyse temporal dynamics in the songbird motor pathway. Nature. 2008;456:189–194. doi: 10.1038/nature07448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marder E, Bucher D. Understanding circuit dynamics using the stomatogastric nervous system of lobsters and crabs. Annu Rev Physiol. 2007;69:291–316. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.69.031905.161516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marder E, Goaillard J-M. Variability, compensation and homeostasis in neuron and network function. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:563–574. doi: 10.1038/nrn1949. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marler P. Birdsong and speech development: Could there be parallels? Am Sci. 1970;58:669–673. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshall PJ, Bar-Haim Y, Fox NA. Development of the EEG from 5 months to 4 years of age. Clin Neurophys. 2002;113:1199–1208. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(02)00163-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mello CV, Vates GE, Okuhata S, Nottebohm F. Descending auditory pathways in the adult male zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). J Comp Neurol. 1998;395:137–160. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mooney R. Different subthreshold mechanisms underlie song selectivity in identified HVc neurons of the zebra finch. J Neurosci. 2000;20:5420–5436. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-14-05420.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mooney R, Prather JF. The HVC microcircuit: the synaptic basis for interactions between song motor and vocal plasticity pathways. J Neurosci. 2005;25:1952–1964. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3726-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nottebohm F, Kelley DB, Paton JA. Connections of vocal control nuclei in the canary telencephalon. J Comp Neurol. 1982;207:344–357. doi: 10.1002/cne.902070406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nottebohm F, Stokes TM, Leonard CM. Central control of song in the canary, Serinus canarius. J Comp Neurol. 1976;165:457–486. doi: 10.1002/cne.901650405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nyström P, Ljunghammar T, Rosander K, Von Hofsten C. Using mu rhythm desynchronization to measure mirror neuron activity in infants. Dev Sci. 2010;14:327–335. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00979.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olveczky BP, Andalman AS, Fee MS. Vocal experimentation in the juvenile songbird requires a basal ganglia circuit. PLOS Biol. 2005;3:0902–0909. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pineda JA. The functional significance of mu rhythms: Translating “seeing” and “hearing” into “doing.”. Brain Res Rev. 2005;50:57–68. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prather JF, Peters S, Nowicki S, Mooney R. Precise auditory-vocal mirroring in neurons for learned vocal communication. Nature. 2008;451:305–310. doi: 10.1038/nature06492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prinz AA, Bucher D, Marder E. Similar network activity from disparate circuit parameters. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:1345–1352. doi: 10.1038/nn1352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rauske PL, Shea SD, Margoliash D. State and neuronal class-dependent reconfiguration in the avian song system. J Neurophys. 2003;89:1688–1701. doi: 10.1152/jn.00655.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ravbar P, Lipkind D, Parra LC, Tchernichovski O. Vocal exploration is locally regulated during song learning. J Neurosci. 2012;32:3422–3432. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3740-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reiner A, Perkel DJ, Mello CV, Jarvis ED. Songbirds and the revised avian brain nomenclature. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1016:77–108. doi: 10.1196/annals.1298.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizzolatti G, Craighero L. The mirror-neuron system. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2004;27:169–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenberg JR, Amjad AM, Breeze P, Brillinger DR, Halliday DM. The Fourier approach to the identification of functional coupling between neuronal spike trains. Prog Biophys Mol Biol. 1989;53:1–31. doi: 10.1016/0079-6107(89)90004-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Royer S, Zemelman BV, Losonczy A, Kim J, Chance F, Magee JC, Buzs aki G. Control of timing, rate and bursts of hippocampal place cells by dendritic and somatic inhibition. Nat Publ Group. 2012;15:769–775. doi: 10.1038/nn.3077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt MF, Konishi M. Gating of auditory responses in the vocal control system of awake songbirds. Nat Neurosci. 1998;1:513–518. doi: 10.1038/2232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmitzer-Torbert N, Jackson J, Henze D, Harris K, Redish AD. Quantitative measures of cluster quality for use in extracellular recordings. Neuroscience. 2005;131:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.09.066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silva LR, Amitai Y, Connors BW. Intrinsic oscillations of neocortex generated by layer 5 pyramidal neurons. Science. 1991;251:432–435. doi: 10.1126/science.1824881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simpson HB, Vicario DS. Brain pathways for learned and unlearned vocalizations differ in zebra finches. J Neurosci. 1990;10:1541–1556. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-05-01541.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sirota A, Montgomery S, Fujisawa S, Isomura Y, Zugaro M, Buzsáki G. Entrainment of neocortical neurons and gamma oscillations by the hippocampal theta rhythm. Neuron. 2008;60:683–697. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solis MM, Perkel DJ. Rhythmic activity in a fore-brain vocal control nucleus in vitro. J Neurosci. 2005;25:2811–2822. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5285-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Somogyi P. Defined types of cortical interneurone structure space and spike timing in the hippocampus. J Physiol. 2004;562:9–26. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.078915. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spruston N. Pyramidal neurons: Dendritic structure and synaptic integration. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:206–221. doi: 10.1038/nrn2286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stauffer TR, Elliott KC, Ross MT, Basista MJ, Hyson RL, Johnson F. Axial organization of a brain region that sequences a learned pattern of behavior. J Neurosci. 2012;32:9312–9322. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0978-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stroganova TA, Nygren G, Tsetlin MM, Posikera IN, Gillberg C, Elam M, Orekhova EV. Abnormal EEG lateralization in boys with autism. Clin Neurophys. 2007;118:1942–1854. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thorpe WH. The learning of song patterns by birds with special reference to the song of the chaffinch, Fringilla coelebs. Ibis. 1958;100:535–642. [Google Scholar]
- Tiesinga P, Sejnowski TJ. Cortical enlightenment: Are attentional gamma oscillations driven by ING or PING? Neuron. 2009;63:727–732. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W. Abnormal neural oscillations and synchrony in schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:100–113. doi: 10.1038/nrn2774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Stein A, Sarnthein J. Different frequencies for different scales of cortical integration: From local gamma to long range alpha. Int J Psychophysiol. 2000;38:301–313. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(00)00172-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vu ET, Mazurek ME, Kuo Y-C. Identification of a forebrain motor programming network for the learned song of zebra finches. J Neurosci. 1994;14:6924–6934. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-11-06924.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wild JM, Williams MN, Howie GJ, Mooney R. Calcium-binding proteins define interneurons in HVC of the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata) J Comp Neurol. 2005;483:76–90. doi: 10.1002/cne.20403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]