Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2014 May 7;3:e02444. doi: 10.7554/eLife.02444

Complete morphologies of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons in the mouse

Hao Wu 1, John Williams 1, Jeremy Nathans 1,2,3,*
Editor: Franck Polleux4
PMCID: PMC4038840  PMID: 24894464

Abstract

The basal forebrain cholinergic system modulates neuronal excitability and vascular tone throughout the cerebral cortex and hippocampus. This system is severely affected in Alzheimer's disease (AD), and drug treatment to enhance cholinergic signaling is widely used as symptomatic therapy in AD. Defining the full morphologies of individual basal forebrain cholinergic neurons has, until now, been technically beyond reach due to their large axon arbor sizes. Using genetically-directed sparse labeling, we have characterized the complete morphologies of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons in the mouse. Individual arbors were observed to span multiple cortical columns, and to have >1000 branch points and total axon lengths up to 50 cm. In an AD model, cholinergic axons were slowly lost and there was an accumulation of axon-derived material in discrete puncta. Calculations based on published morphometric data indicate that basal forebrain cholinergic neurons in humans have a mean axon length of ∼100 meters.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02444.001

Research organism: mouse

eLife digest

The human brain is made up of roughly 80 to 100 billion neurons, organized into extensive networks. Each neuron consists of a number of components: a cell body, which contains the nucleus; numerous short protrusions from the cell body called dendrites; and a long thin structure called an axon that carries the electrical signals generated in the cell body and the dendrites to the next neuron in the network.

One of the most studied networks in the human brain is the basal forebrain network, which is made up of large neurons that communicate with one another using a chemical transmitter called acetylcholine. This network has a key role in cognition, and its neurons are among the first to degenerate in Alzheimer's disease. However, relatively little is known about the structure of these ‘cholinergic’ neurons because their large size makes them difficult to study using standard techniques.

Now, Wu et al. have visualized, for the first time, the complete 3D structure of cholinergic neurons in the mouse forebrain. The mice in question had been genetically modified so that only ten or so of their many thousands of cholinergic neurons expressed a distinctive ‘marker’ protein. This made it possible to distinguish these neurons from surrounding brain tissue in order to visualize their structures. The resulting pictures clearly illustrate the neurons' complexity, with individual axons in adult mice displaying up to 1000 branches.

Measurements showed that each cholinergic axon in the mouse brain is roughly 30 centimeters long, even though the brain itself is less than 2 centimeters from front to back. Based on measurements by other researchers, Wu et al. calculated that the axons of single cholinergic neurons in the human brain are about 100 meters long on average.

The extreme length and complex branching structure of cholinergic forebrain neurons helps to explain why each neuron is able to modulate the activity of many others in the network. It could also explain their vulnerability to degeneration, as the need to transport materials over such long distances may limit the ability of these neurons to respond to damage.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02444.002

Introduction

The mammalian cerebral cortex and hippocampus are densely innervated by cholinergic fibers that originate in the basal forebrain (Mesulam, 2004). Most, if not all, cortical and hippocampal neurons respond to cholinergic signals using muscarinic and/or nicotinic acetylcholine receptors localized to pre- and/or postsynaptic sites. Activation of postsynaptic nicotinic receptors leads to transient depolarizing currents with a high calcium:sodium ratio and activation of postsynaptic muscarinic receptors leads to a sustained reduction in potassium currents, both effects producing a general elevation in excitability (Lucas-Meunier et al., 2003). Additionally, cholinergic activation of muscarinic receptors in the microvasculature leads to the production of nitric oxide, producing vasodilation (Hamel, 2004).

A wide variety of experiments in primates, cats, and rodents have implicated cholinergic neurotransmission from the basal forebrain to the cortex and hippocampus in attention, memory, and plasticity. In cat primary visual cortex, cholinergic input enhances neuronal responses to a preferred visual stimulus (Sato et al., 1987), and in rat motor cortex, cholinergic input promotes more complex motor sequences in response to electrical stimulation (Berg et al., 2005). In rat barrel cortex and auditory cortex, cholinergic input is required for experience-dependent synaptic plasticity and circuit reorganization (Baskerville et al., 1997; Kilgard and Merzenich, 1998; Zhu and Waite, 1998). In contrast to the high spatial precision of glutamatergic and GABAergic neurotransmission, current evidence indicates that the basal forebrain cholinergic system modulates neuronal excitability and vascular tone over large target areas.

The basal forebrain cholinergic system is of special interest because it degenerates in a variety of common neurologic diseases, including Alzheimer's disease (AD) and Parkinson's disease, to an extent that correlates with the severity of dementia (Schliebs and Arendt, 2011). In advanced AD, the relative loss of cholinergic innervation varies by region, with the temporal lobe showing the greatest loss of fibers and the primary sensory, motor, and anterior cingulate cortices showing the least loss (Geula and Mesulam, 1989). The relative extent of cholinergic fiber loss in different cortical areas appears to correlate inversely with fiber density in the normal brain, suggesting that disease-associated fiber loss progresses at roughly equal rates throughout the cortex and that those regions that began with the fewest fibers are the first to become denuded of cholinergic input. The loss of forebrain cholinergic innervation in AD has stimulated the development of pharmacotherapy to enhance cholinergic signaling as an approach to partially ameliorate cognitive symptoms (Burns et al., 2006).

From the preceding paragraphs it is clear that an accurate anatomic description of the basal forebrain cholinergic system is important for understanding its function and its susceptibility to degeneration. At present, this description, which derives from retrograde and anterograde filling and from histochemical and immunohistochemical staining, provides a view that is accurate as a statistical picture but is incomplete in one critical respect: the morphologies of individual cholinergic axon arbors are unknown because their extraordinarily large size has, thus far, precluded classical tracer filling and reconstruction. In earlier work, we demonstrated the utility of extremely sparse CreER/loxP labeling methods for visualizing axonal and dendritic morphologies of large neurons, including forebrain cholinergic neurons (Rotolo et al., 2008; Badea et al., 2009). In the present work, we use this approach to visualize and quantify the full 3-dimensional axonal morphologies of individual forebrain cholinergic neurons and to define changes in these arbors in response to disease progression in a mouse model of AD.

Results

A genetic system for extremely sparse labeling of cholinergic neurons

In earlier work, we generated an IRES-CreER knock-in in the 3′ untranslated region of the gene coding for choline acetyl transferase (ChAT; Rotolo et al., 2008). This allele expresses relatively low levels of CreER and, consequently, shows no recombination of Cre-activated reporters in the absence of tamoxifen or 4-hydroxytamoxifen (4HT), a prerequisite for visualizing genetically marked neurons at densities of <10 labeled neurons per brain. To visualize large axon arbors in their entirety and to efficiently survey dozens of brains, we chose the highly sensitive histochemical reporter human placental alkaline phosphatase (AP), a GPI-anchored protein that distributes relatively uniformly along dendrites and axons (Rotolo et al., 2008). AP histochemistry works efficiently with relatively thick (300 µm) vibratome sections, which minimizes the number of sections required per brain and thereby simplifies the logistics of staining, imaging, and tracing. To minimize background reporter activity, we used an AP reporter knock-in at the Gt(ROSA)26Sor locus (referred to as R26) in which the 3′ half of the AP coding region is in reverse orientation and Cre-mediated recombination restores this segment to the correct orientation (R26IAP, ‘I’ stands for ‘inverted’; Figure 1A; Badea et al., 2009). In contrast to standard reporters that are maintained in a repressed state by a loxP-stop-loxP cassette, the R26IAP locus shows undetectable reporter activity prior to Cre-mediated recombination.

Figure 1. Cholinergic neuron specificity of Cre-mediated recombination.

Figure 1.

(A) Structure of the R26IAP knock-in. In the absence of Cre-mediated recombination, the 3′ half of the AP coding region is inverted in the germline configuration. It assumes the correct orientation following Cre-mediated recombination between inverted loxP sites. (B) P30 retina from Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP mice treated with 4HT. AP histochemistry labels cholinergic (starburst) amacrine cells. Scale bar, 100 µm. (CF) P30 brain from Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP mice treated with high dose 4HT at P5. AP histochemistry labels numerous axons throughout the cortex (D) and hippocampus (F), as well as cranial motor neurons (E), the axons of which are seen exiting the brain stem (red arrows). Scale bars in DF, 200 µm. (G and H) Coronal sections of P30 forebrain from Chat-IRES-CreER;R26-LSL-nGFP mice treated with high dose 4HT at P4. Approximately 50% of cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain, medial septal nucleus, striatum, and spinal cord (visualized with ChAT immunohistochemistry) are GFP+. Medial to the striatum, a distinctive group of GFP+ cell is ChAT−; these cells presumably expressed Chat (and, therefore, Cre) in the early postnatal period and then repress Chat expression in adulthood. In (H), arrows point to ChAT+;GFP− neurons and arrowheads point to ChAT+; GFP+ neurons. Scale bar, 50 µm.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02444.003

The specificity of the Chat-IRES-CreER driver has been documented by Rotolo et al. (2008) and Badea et al. (2009) and is demonstrated here with Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP mice based on reporter expression in (1) starburst amacrine cells, the only cholinergic retinal neurons (Figure 1B), (2) a uniform network of fibers in the cortex and hippocampus, as expected for the axon arbors of forebrain cholinergic neurons (Figure 1C,D,F), and (3) cranial motor neurons in the brainstem (Figure 1E). Chat-IRES-CreER activation of a nuclear localized GFP reporter (encoded by a R26-loxP-stop-loxP-nGFP knock-in) shows co-localization with ChAT immunoreactivity in the basal forebrain, septal nucleus, and ventral spinal cord as expected (Figure 1G,H). Interestingly, a small population of ChAT-negative forebrain cells, located medial to the striatum, shows GFP expression in the adult, implying that these cells transiently express the ChAT gene at the time of 4HT injection [postnatal day (P)4] but not at later times (Figure 1G).

Morphologies of individual forebrain cholinergic neurons

A series of 4HT titration experiments with Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP mice showed that intraperitoneal (IP) injection of 1–5 µg 4HT at P4-5 resulted in ∼10 forebrain cholinergic neurons labeled per brain. Using this protocol, 67 well-separated forebrain cholinergic neurons were imaged and 12 of these neurons–8 from P12 brains and 4 from P30 brains–were traced (Figures 2, 3, 4B, Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Among the traced arbors, nine were in the cortex, two were in the hippocampus, and one was in the olfactory bulb. For each of the remaining 55 neurons, collected between 1 and 12 months of age, we determined the soma location and the boundaries of the arbor territory.

Figure 2. Axon arbors of forebrain cholinergic neurons from P30 Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP mice visualized with sparse Cre-mediated recombination.

(A) Part of the arbor of a forebrain cholinergic neuron in a P30 hippocampus visualized in a single 300 µm section at three Z-planes and in a Z-stacked image. Bottom, the traced arbor. Scale bar, 200 µm. (B and C) Fifteen consecutive 300 µm sagittal sections from a single P30 hemisphere (C) with two fully traced AP+ forebrain cholinergic neurons, colored red and green. Black arrows in panels l and o, the two cell bodies. Red arrows in g–n, the proximal axon segment for the red neuron trace. (B) An enlarged view of the boxed region of section d in (C). Scale bar in (B), 500 µm (corrected for tissue shrinkage in BBBA).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02444.004

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Dendrite structure among forebrain cholinergic neurons.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Left, Z-stack image of a 300 µm sagittal brain section (‘section 1’) shown in panel ‘o’ of Figure 2C. Middle, trace of AP+ dendrites, part of the soma, and proximal axon arbor for the forebrain cholinergic neuron in section 1. Right, trace of the remainder of the dendritic arbor and the second part of the soma from the adjacent 300 µm sagittal brain section (‘section 2’) shown in panel ‘n’ of Figure 2C. Scale bar, 200 µm. (B) Total dendrite lengths for five P12 and four P30 forebrain cholinergic neurons. Bars show the mean +/− SD. (C) Dendrite arbor volume measured with the polygon method vs dendrite length for the same five P12 and four P30 forebrain cholinergic neurons plotted in panel B. (D) Individual dendrite arbor density (dendrite length divided by dendrite arbor volume) vs dendrite length for the same five P12 and four P30 forebrain cholinergic neurons plotted in panel B.

Figure 3. Axon arbors of forebrain cholinergic neurons from P12 Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP mice visualized with sparse Cre-mediated recombination.

Figure 3.

(A) Enlarged view of the boxed region from panel g in (B). Scale bar, 500 µm (corrected for tissue shrinkage in BBBA). (B) Fourteen consecutive 300 µm sagittal sections from a single P12 hemisphere (a–n) with three traced AP+ cholinergic neurons, colored red, green, and blue. The blue neuron is shown in its entirety, including the cell body and dendrites in the basal forebrain; for the green and red neurons, only the cortical axon arbors are shown.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02444.006

Figure 4. Quantitative analysis of morphologic parameters for cholinergic axon arbors.

(A) The polygon method for estimating the target area for a single cholinergic axon arbor. Traced axon arbor images are shown for each 300 µm sagittal section from the P30 brain in Figure 2C, panels a–e. A minimal convex polygon has been drawn around each trace, providing an upper estimate of the cortical territory that is directly influenced by the arbor. As seen in panels d and e, the polygon method somewhat overestimates the target area by including regions that are relatively far from the axon. P, posterior. A, anterior. (B) Axon arbor locations for the 67 basal forebrain cholinergic neurons analyzed. (C) Quantification of tissue volume shrinkage due to dehydration in ethanol and BBBA. (D) Schematic of a forebrain cholinergic neuron in a dorsal view of the mouse brain showing the mediolateral cell body and arbor locations, the parameters displayed in panels (H) and (I). (E) The number of branch points per mm of axon length for the 12 forebrain cholinergic neurons that were traced. These data were obtained from two 300 µm sections per arbor by measuring the total axon length and counting all branch points for the AP+ arbor within each section. (F) Scatter plot of arbor volume (estimated using the polygon method) vs axon length for the 12 forebrain cholinergic neurons that were traced. (G) Scatter plot of axon density (length divided by arbor volume) vs axon length for the 12 forebrain cholinergic neurons that were traced. (H) Scatter plot of the mediolateral extent (defined in panel D) vs arbor volume (estimated using the polygon method) for the 55 forebrain cholinergic arbors that were not traced. (I) Mediolateral cell body and arbor locations for the 12 traced neurons (left) and the 55 untraced neurons (right). Black dots represent cell body location and the vertical bar represents the mediolateral extent of the axon arbor. OB, olfactory bulb.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02444.007

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Cholinergic axon arbors: Z-stacked images, traces, and branch point locations.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(AD) Axon arbors from single cholinergic neurons visualized in 300 µm sagittal sections from olfactory bulb (A), cortex (B and D), and hippocampus (C): Z-stacked images (left), traces (center), and branch points (right). Z-stacked images (AC) are from Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP brains at P12 or P30 and image (D) is from a Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP;APP/PS1 brain at 9 months. Scale bars, 200 µm.

At both P12 and P30, large and complex axon arbors were observed (Figures 2 and 3). The mean axon length per neuron was 13 cm at P12 (n = 8; range 3–42 cm) and 31 cm at P30 (n = 4; range 11–49 cm), and at both ages, the density of branches averaged 4–5 per mm of axon length (range 3–7 per mm), giving a mean of >1000 branch points per arbor at P30, including occasional branches along the axon's initial segment (Figure 4E–G, Figure 2—figure supplement 1A, Figure 4—figure supplement 1). In a sample of nine basal forebrain cholinergic neurons, the mean dendritic arbor lengths were 9.6 mm at P12 (n = 5 arbors) and 11.5 mm at P30 (n = 4 arbors; Figure 2—figure supplement 1). The territory influenced by each cholinergic arbor was estimated by enclosing the axon traces from each 300 µm tissue section with the smallest possible convex polygon, calculating the area of each polygon, and summing the resulting polygonal volumes (polygon area × 300 µm) across all of the sections populated by the arbor of interest (Figure 4A,C). Using this measure, individual axon density–defined as mm of axon length per mm3 of polygonal volume for an individual axon—was found to vary by a factor of ∼10 among traced arbors at both P12 (n = 8) and at P30 (n = 4; Figure 4F,G), reflecting significant variation in the size and compactness of cholinergic arbors. The comparison between P12 and P30 implies that there is substantial growth of cholinergic axons after the second week of postnatal life. Although the polygon method somewhat over-estimates the territory influenced by an arbor (as illustrated in Figure 4A), it provides a good measure of the linear extent of the arbor, giving a mean value of ∼2 mm in the adult brain, as seen in a compilation of the mediolateral extents of forebrain cholinergic axon arbors (Figure 4H,I).

The coverage factor for forebrain cholinergic axon arbors—defined as the number of arbor territories that encompass any arbitrary point in the cortex and hippocampus—can be calculated based on a mean volume of 1.35 mm3 for axon arbor territories at P30 (n = 4; Figure 4F), a total volume of 130 mm3 for the adult mouse cerebral cortex (109 mm3) and hippocampus (21 mm3; Kovacević et al., 2005), and estimates of the number of forebrain cholinergic neurons of 4500 and 6632 (Boncristiano et al., 2002; Perez et al., 2007). This calculation gives a mean coverage factor of 47–69. The analogous dendrite coverage factor and the density of dendrites in the basal forebrain regions where cholinergic projection neuron cell bodies reside (the medial septal nucleus, ventral diagonal band, and horizontal limb of the diagonal band) can also be calculated based on the volume of these territories (0.667 mm3; Paxinos and Franklin, 2001), the mean dendrite volume per cholinergic neuron at P30 (0.0694 mm3; n = 4; Figure 2—figure supplement 1), the mean dendrite length per cholinergic neuron at P30 (11.5 mm; n = 4; Figure 2—figure supplement 1C,D), and the estimated number of forebrain cholinergic neurons noted above. These calculations give a dendrite coverage factor of 470–690, and a density of cholinergic dendrites in the basal forebrain of 0.078–0.114 µm/µm3.

Numerous retrograde and anterograde labeling studies have investigated the correlation between the territories targeted by forebrain cholinergic arbors and the locations of the corresponding cell bodies (e.g., McKinney et al., 1983; Saper, 1984; Woolf et al., 1986; Baskerville et al., 1993). In our dataset of 67 neurons, there was a clear correlation between soma location and axon arbor position along the mediolateral axis, specifically, cell bodies located more laterally in the basal forebrain give rise to arbors that reside in more lateral cortical, hippocampal, or olfactory bulb territories, supporting the general conclusion that there is a rough topographic map of target territories in the basal forebrain (Figure 4I).

Progressive disruption of forebrain cholinergic axon arbor morphology in an AD model

As noted in the Introduction, loss of forebrain cholinergic innervation is a prominent feature of AD. To visualize AD pathology at the level of single cholinergic axon arbors, we used APPswe/PS1ΔE9 doubly transgenic mice (Jankowsky et al., 2004; referred to hereafter as APP/PS1) in which Aβ plaque rapidly accumulates with age in the cortex and hippocampus (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). This progression is accompanied by microglial reorganization and activation, and premature death (Figure 5—figure supplement 1 and 2). Analysis of AP+ cholinergic arbors in the cortex and hippocampus of 62 Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP;APP/PS1 brains harvested between one and 12 months of age showed fragmentation of axons and a decrease in AP signal strength (Figure 5A–C), consistent with previously described changes in the appearance of ChAT immunoreactive fibers in mouse and human AD brains (Figure 5—figure supplement 1C,D; Gordon et al., 2002; Schliebs and Arendt, 2011). There was also a progressive accumulation of large numbers of AP+ and ChAT immunoreactive puncta with diameters up to ∼10 µm in Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP;APP/PS1 cortex and hippocampus but not in Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP controls (Figure 5, Figure 5—figure supplement 1C,D, Figure 5—figure supplement 3A; Boncristiano et al., 2002). These puncta presumably represent cholinergic axon breakdown products.

Figure 5. Disruption of cholinergic axon arbors in Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP;APP/PS1 mice.

(A) Upper panel, 300 µm sagittal section of a 12 month old Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP;APP/PS1 brain showing part of a single AP+ axon arbor. The olfactory bulb is visible at lower right. Lower panels, three Z-planes enlarged from the region enclosed in the red square in the upper panel. Red arrowheads point to clumps of AP+ material (puncta). Scale bars: upper panel, 500 µm; lower panels, 50 µm. (B) Comparison of representative regions from forebrain cholinergic axon arbors in the cortex of Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP brains (WT; left) and Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP;APP/PS1 brains (right), between one and 12 months of age. Structural heterogeneity, including the clumping of AP + material (puncta) and loss of AP staining intensity, increases with age in the APP/PS1 background. Scale bar, 50 µm. (C) Quantification of AP+ puncta in the cortex and hippocampus of Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP (i.e., WT) and Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP;APP/PS1 mice at different ages. Puncta appear at 3 months in Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP;APP/PS1 mice. The box plots indicate the extreme data points (top and bottom bars), the 25–75% interval (box), and the median (central line). p-values, student's t test. (D) Complete tracing of an AP+ cortical cholinergic arbor (blue) with the locations of AP + puncta (red dots) indicated. Panels a–f show six adjacent 300 µm sagittal sections within which this arbor resides. The three enlarged images above correspond to the boxed regions in panels d–f. Scale bar, 500 µm (corrected for tissue shrinkage in BBBA).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02444.009

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Aβ deposition, microglial reorganization, and disorganization of cholinergic fibers in the APP/PS1 brain.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(A) The hippocampus in WT and APP/PS1 mice at 12 months. Numerous Aβ deposits are seen in the APP/PS1 hippocampus, and are associated with migration of microglia, visualized by ionized calcium binding adaptor molecule 1 (Iba1). Scale bar, 100 µm. (B) The cortex of an APP/PS1 mouse at 12 months. Numerous Aβ deposits are present, along with an irregular distribution of microglia. Scale bar, 100 µm. (C) Enlarged views of the boxed regions in panel (B). Boxed region ‘a’ lacks large Aβ deposits. Boxed region ‘b’ has numerous large Aβ deposits surrounded by microglia. ChAT immunostaining shows numerous puncta in the region corresponding to Aβ deposits. Scale bar, 25 µm. (D) Cholinergic axons in the cortex and hippocampus and cholinergic cell bodies in the basal forebrain in WT and APP/PS1 brains at 12 months. Cholinergic cell bodies in the basal forebrain show little or no difference between the two samples. Cholinergic axons show numerous puncta in the APP/PS1 cortex and hippocampus, some of which are highlighted by red arrows.
Figure 5—figure supplement 2. Characterization of APP/PS1 mice: survival and microglial activation.

Figure 5—figure supplement 2.

(A and B) Sagittal section of a 12 month old WT brain stained with GS-lectin, which visualizes vascular endothelial cells and activated microglia. In the cortex and hippocampus, binding is limited to the vasculature. The boxed region in (A) is enlarged in (B). (C and D) Sagittal section of a 12 month old APP/PS1 brain stained with GS-lectin as for panels (A and B). In the cortex, hippocampus, and olfactory bulb, GS-lectin binding is observed in the vasculature and in numerous clusters of activated microglia. Activated microglia are not present in other brain regions. The boxed region in (C) is enlarged in (D). (E) Kaplan–Meier survival curves for Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP (i.e., WT) and Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP;APP/PS1 mice.
Figure 5—figure supplement 3. Tracing AP+ axons, and locating and quantifying AP+ puncta with and without bexarotene treatment.

Figure 5—figure supplement 3.

(A) Example of a cortical arbor from a Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP;APP/PS1 brain at 9 months of age visualized in a single 300 µm sagittal section at three Z-planes (left three panels) and in a Z-stacked image (fourth panel). Arrowheads point to some of the larger puncta. Fifth panel, the traced arbor (blue) and AP+ puncta (red). Scale bar, 200 µm. (B) Induction of ABCA1 in cerebral cortex by bexarotene treatment, assessed by anti-ABCA1 immunoblotting. 7-month old mice received vehicle (DMSO; n = 3) or 100 mg/kg bexarotene in DMSO (n = 3) once daily for 7 days by oral gavage. The six mice were sacrificed on day 8. (C) Quantification of ABCA1 immunoblot signal intensity (panel B) and the ratio of liver weight to body weight in mice that received vehicle (DMSO; ‘veh’) or 100 mg/kg bexarotene in DMSO (‘bex’) once daily for 7 days by oral gavage. Bexarotene treatment induces a rapid hepatomegaly. (D) Quantification of AP+ puncta in the cortex and hippocampus of untreated Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP (i.e., WT) mice and Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP;APP/PS1 mice treated with daily 100 µl gavages of DMSO for 14 days or bexarotene in DMSO for 14 or 23 days. The box plots indicate the extreme data points (top and bottom bars), the 25–75% interval (box), and the median (central line). There is no statistically significant effect of bexarotene treatment on the density of AP+ puncta. p-value, student's t test.

Bexarotene (Targretin) is a retinoid X receptor (RXR) agonist that has been reported to acutely promote ApoE-dependent clearance of soluble Aβ oligomers and to improve cognitive performance in the APP/PS1 mouse model of AD (Cramer et al., 2012). Whether bexarotene also promotes clearance of insoluble Aβ deposits (plaque) is controversial (Fitz et al., 2013; Landreth et al., 2013; Price et al., 2013; Tesseur et al., 2013; Veeraraghavalu et al., 2013). To determine whether bexarotene treatment alters the destruction of cholinergic axons in the APP/PS1 cortex, nine Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP;APP/PS1 littermates were treated with 50–100 µg 4HT IP at P5 and were divided into three groups of three mice at 7 months of age: the first group received daily DMSO (vehicle) gavages for 14 days; the second group received daily 100 mg/kg bexarotene gavages for 14 days; and the third group received daily 100 mg/kg bexarotene gavages for 23 days, a regimen that activates RXR in the cerebral cortex as determined by the accumulation of ABCA1, a known RXR-inducible protein (Figure 5—figure supplement 3B,C; Schmitz and Langmann, 2005). As observed by others (Fitz et al., 2013; Price et al., 2013; Tesseur et al., 2013; Veeraraghavalu et al., 2013), the size and abundance of insoluble Aβ deposits and the number of activated microglia and astroglia around these deposits appeared to be unaltered by bexarotene treatment (data not shown), although we note that the DMSO formulation that we and Veeraraghavalu et al. used differs from the aqueous suspension of micronized bexarotene particles used by Cramer et al. (2012) (see also Landreth et al., 2013). Quantification of the density of AP+ puncta was also unaltered (Figure 5—figure supplement 3D), suggesting that the pathologic processes leading to the production of these puncta was not appreciably modified by the several week bexarotene treatment.

Extremely large axon arbors in the mammalian brain: estimations and cross-species comparisons

The total length of the axons in a P30 mouse forebrain cholinergic neuron arbor–up to 50 cm–is roughly 25 times the linear dimension of the mouse brain. The large size of these arbors suggested that a systematic analysis of arbor sizes among various types of projection neurons might be of general interest (Table 1).

Table 1.

Axon arbor lengths for diffuse projection neurons

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02444.013

Basal forebrain cholinergic neurons (nucleus basalis of Meynert to cortex)
Species Number of neurons Axon density in cortex Axon length in cortex Cortical volume Mean axon length/neuron
Mouse 6632 (a) 1300 m (b) 109 mm3 (c) 20 cm
4500 (b) 1300 m (b) 109 mm3 (c) 29 cm
Mouse 6632 (a) 0.020–0.044 µm/µm3 (d; this study) 109 mm3 (c) 33–72 cm
4500 (b) 0.020–0.044 µm/µm3 (d; this study) 109 mm3 (c) 48-107 cm
Mouse* 4 traced neurons following CreER/loxP labeling (this study) 31 cm
Rat 7,312 (d) 0.0113 µm/µm3 (e) 400 mm3 (f) 62 cm
Chimp 315,000 (g) 0.066 µm/µm3 (h) 147 cm3 (i) 31 m
Human 435,000 (g) 0.080 µm/µm3 (h) 583 cm3 (i) 107 m
Nigro-striatal dopaminergic neurons
Species Number of neurons Varicosities per axon length Number of TH + varicosities in the striatum Mean axon length/neuron
Rat 7000 (j) 5–7 varicosities/7 µm (j) 3.4 × 109 per side (j) 55–77 cm
Rat* 8 traced neurons following sparse GFP virus infection (k) 47 cm
Serotonergic neurons (dorsal raphe nucleus to cortex)
Species Number of neurons Axon density in cortex Cortical volume mean axon length/neuron
Rat 11,500 (l) 0.023 µm/µm3 (m) 400 mm3 (f) 80 cm
15,191 (n) 0.023 µm/µm3 (m) 400 mm3 (f) 61 cm
Human 80,386 (o) 0.048 µm/µm3 (r) 583 cm3 (i) 348 m
86,565 (p) 0.048 µm/µm3 (r) 583 cm3 (i) 323 m
165,000 (q) 0.048 µm/µm3 (r) 583 cm3 (i) 170 m
Hippocampal CA3 pyramidal neurons
Rat* 1 traced neuron following neurobiotin injection (s) 48 cm
Cutaneous sensory neurons with free endings in back skin
Mouse* 7 traced neurons following CreER/loxP labeling (t) 71 cm

To the best of our knowledge there are only four studies (including the present one) in which individual axon arbors from the largest classes of CNS or PNS neurons have been traced and their lengths quantified. These are: (1) eight nigrostriatal dopaminergic neurons in the rat visualized following sparse infection with a GFP-expressing Sindbis virus (Matsuda et al., 2009), (2) a single CA3 pyramidal neuron in the rat visualized by neurobiotin injection (Wittner et al., 2007), (3) seven cutaneous sensory afferents of the ‘large area, free-ending’ class in mouse skin labeled by sparse CreER activation of an AP reporter (Wu et al., 2012), and (4) the four P30 mouse forebrain cholinergic neurons described here. The mean axon lengths for these four cell types were found to be, respectively: 47 cm (range: 14–78 cm), 48 cm, 71 cm (range: 64–98 cm), and 31 cm (range: 11–49 cm).

The validity of the single-cell axon length measurements for dopaminergic and cholinergic neurons can be independently checked with calculations based on the total volume of the target territory, the density of the particular type of axon (axon length per volume of target territory), and the number of neuronal cell bodies giving rise to that type of axon (Table 1). These population analyses are made possible by the availability of antibodies that localize to different types of axons: anti-ChAT for cholinergic axons (also visualized with acetylcholine esterase histochemistry), anti-tyrosine hydroxylase for striatal dopaminergic axons, and anti-serotonin for serotonergic axons. For example, Anden et al. (1966) estimated the total length of all dopaminergic axons in the rat striatum to be 7900 meters (bilaterally) and the number of mid-brain dopaminergic neurons projecting to the striatum to be 14,000 (bilaterally), giving a calculated mean axon length of 56 cm per dopaminergic neuron, in good agreement with the single cell tracing data of Matsuda et al. (2009) (Table 1).

A similar calculation can be performed for mouse forebrain cholinergic neurons using published estimates of the number of cell bodies in the nucleus basalis of Meynert (6632 and 4500), the volume of the mouse cortex as determined by MRI (109 mm3), and the total length of cholinergic axons in the mouse cortex (1300 m), giving individual axon arbor length estimates of 20 cm and 29 cm (Table 1). (For these calculations we have omitted the volume of the hippocampus, which in mice represents 19% the volume of the cerebral cortex; its inclusion would modestly increase the estimates of axon length per neuron for those calculations based on axon density and cortical volume.) As a check on this calculation, we have independently measured the density of cholinergic axons in the mouse motor cortex (0.044 µm/µm3; Figure 6) and also used the cholinergic axon density measured by Descarries et al. (2005) in parietal cortex (0.020 µm/µm3) to calculate single cholinergic axon arbor lengths of 33–107 cm (Table 1). Given the limitations of the sampling methods, we consider these estimates of axon length to be in reasonably good agreement with the 11–49 cm range for the four P30 cholinergic neurons traced in the present study. An analogous calculation based on published data for rat forebrain cholinergic neurons gives a mean axon length of 62 cm (Table 1).

Figure 6. Quantifying ChAT+ axon density in P30 mouse cortex.

Figure 6.

(A) Coronal section of P30 mouse motor cortex following ChAT immunostaining. The cortical surface is at the top; the base of the cortex is at the bottom. Confocal images of the fluorescently immunostained tissue (converted to grey scale and inverted) were captured at Z-plane separations of 2 µm. Z-stacks encompassing planes 3–6 and 7–10 are shown. The traced axons for both sets of Z-stacks are color-coded with axons in stacks 3–6 in green, axons in stacks 7–10 in red, and regions of overlap in blue. Scale bar, 25 µm. (B) Axon tracings corresponding to the region in the right panel in (A) that is demarcated by the vertical red line. Scale bar, 25 µm. (C) ChAT immunostaining corresponding to the region adjacent to the left panel in (A) that is demarcated by the vertical black line. Scale bar, 25 µm. (D) The red rectangle shows the region of motor cortex analyzed in (A), at approximately Bregma −1.06.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02444.014

The estimates of axon length for large arbors in the rodent brain led us to ask whether it might be possible to perform analogous calculations for neurons in the human CNS. To the best of our knowledge, this simple calculation has not been performed previously. The human data for axon density and neuron counts have been published for forebrain cholinergic neurons and for serotonergic neurons projecting from the dorsal raphe nucleus to the cortex, and cortical volume estimates for humans are available from MRI analyses; forebrain cholinergic neuron data is also available for chimpanzees (Table 1). These calculations lead to axon length estimates of 107 m and 31 m, respectively, for human and chimpanzee forebrain cholinergic neurons, and an axon length estimate of 170–348 m for human serotonergic neurons. For both neurotransmitter systems, the vast majority of the visualized fibers within the cortex are presumed to derive from projection neurons because the density of cortical cell bodies labeled with anti-ChAT or anti-serotonin antibodies is extremely low (Raghanti et al., 2008a, 2008b). Even if we allow for a possible under-estimate in neuron counts or a possible over-estimate in axon density measurements by as much as 2–3-fold, the calculations imply that in the human brain these two classes of projection neurons have axons that are, on average, many tens of meters in length.

Discussion

The present work shows that mammalian forebrain cholinergic neurons are among the largest and most complex neurons described to date, as judged by total axon length and number of branch points. Nigrostriatal dopaminergic neurons appear to be of similar size and complexity, as are the largest cutaneous sensory neurons (Table 1, and references therein). The calculated total axon length for human forebrain cholinergic neurons, ∼100 meters, is larger than the ∼30 meter length estimated for blue whale corticospinal and DRG axons, which are typically cited as the ‘largest’ neurons in the animal kingdom (Smith, 2009). We note, however, that if one considers only the distance from the cell body to the tip of the most distal processes, then human forebrain cholinergic neurons are substantially ‘smaller’ in the sense of being more compact than many neurons with more nearly linear axons.

For forebrain cholinergic neurons, it is instructive to calculate the relative volumes and surface areas of soma and axon. If we take ∼0.3 µm as the mean diameter of a rat cholinergic axon (Vaucher and Hamel, 1995; a value typical for unmyelinated CNS fibers; see; Perge et al., 2012), and temporarily neglect the volume added by varicosities, we obtain an axon volume of ∼70.6 × 103 µm3 per meter of axon length. Along the length of cholinergic fibers in the rat cortex and hippocampus, varicosities occur at a mean frequency of ∼0.4/µm and their mean internal volume is ∼0.06 µm3 (the mean diameter is 0.48 µm; Mechawar et al., 2000, 2002; Aznavour et al., 2005; Descarries et al., 2005). Thus, the varicosities would add a volume of (0.4 × 106) × (0.06 µm3) = 24 × 103 µm3 per meter of axon length. The mean soma volume for rat basal forebrain cholinergic neurons is ∼14 × 103 µm3 (diameters are 18–43 µm; the mean is taken as ∼30 µm; Butcher, 1995). Using the value of 62 cm calculated here for the mean length of rat forebrain cholinergic axons (Table 1), the axon and varicosity volumes (44 × 103 µm and 15 × 103 µm3) sum to ∼59 × 103 µm3, or ∼4.2 times greater than the soma volume. Thus, despite its great length, the axon arbor only increases the neuron's volume several-fold. By contrast, the calculated mean surface area of the axon arbor (∼5.84 × 105 µm2) is ∼210 times greater than the calculated mean surface area of the soma (∼2800 µm2).

To appreciate the relative dimensions of a typical forebrain cholinergic neuron, the reader may find it useful to consider a model in which this neuron has been enlarged by a factor of 104 (Figure 7). (For this model, we use the data on rat cholinergic neurons from the preceding paragraph.) The model neuron would have a cell body ∼30 cm in diameter, a dendritic arbor extending as far as ∼5 meters from the soma, and axons of ∼3 mm diameter divided into a proximal segment ∼200 meters in length connected to a highly branched distal arbor with a total length of 6 kilometers (!). For the human counterpart, the model would have a total axon length of ∼1000 kilometers.

Figure 7. Calculated dimensions of rat and human forebrain cholinergic neurons.

Figure 7.

Actual dimensions of human and rat forebrain cholinergic neurons, calculated from the data in Table 1 (left image in each pair). A macroscopic model in which the linear dimensions of each cholinergic neuron have been multiplied 10,000-fold (right image in each pair). Soma and axons are shown; dendrites have been omitted.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02444.015

Forebrain cholinergic neuron morphology and function

The large sizes of the cholinergic axon arbors described here are consistent with a spatially diffuse modulatory role for cholinergic transmission in the cortex (Descarries et al., 1997; Lucas-Meunier et al., 2003). In the mouse, cortical columns are approximately 150–300 µm across, the diameter of an individual barrel in the barrel cortex (Jan et al., 2008). Since the typical cholinergic axon arbor extends over two millimeters in the plane of the cortex (Figures 2 and 3), each arbor contacts multiple cortical columns. Although locally induced or locally restricted acetylcholine release is possible, it seems likely that signals originating in the dendrites and leading to action potentials at the soma of a forebrain cholinergic neuron would affect synaptic output over the entire arbor. This line of reasoning implies that cholinergic modulation of cortical and hippocampal function in response to information originating in the basal forebrain is likely to have low spatial resolution. A similar argument can be applied to nigrostriatal dopaminergic signaling (Matsuda et al., 2009).

Implications of extremely large axon arbors for neurodegenerative disease

The very large axon lengths calculated for human forebrain cholinergic neurons reflect (1) the enormous evolutionary expansion of the human cerebral cortex, which is ∼5000 times larger than the mouse cerebral cortex, (2) a more modest expansion in the number of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons, which differ between humans and mice by only a factor of 60–100, and (3) a ∼twofold higher density of cholinergic axons in the human cortex (Table 1). The same line of reasoning applies to the rodent vs human difference in the size of individual serotonergic axon arbors (Table 1).

Extremely large axon arbors present a cell biological challenge, as they require a correspondingly large expenditure of resources for growth, maintenance, and repair, especially as related to membrane synthesis and axonal transport. It is possible that the vulnerability of forebrain cholinergic neurons in the context of AD is related, at least in part, to the large size of their axon arbors. For example, vulnerability might be related to a particular sensitivity of the axonal transport machinery to biochemical perturbations associated with Aβ toxicity. Perhaps more significantly, the requirement that all transportation processes between cell body and axon arbor funnel through a single proximal axon segment suggests that trafficking within this segment may limit the efficacy of cellular responses to axonal damage or stress. The general idea that extreme axon length increases vulnerability to neurodegeneration has been discussed in the context of motor neuron disease (Cavanagh, 1984; Ferraiuolo et al., 2011), and it seems reasonable that this concept might apply to a wide variety of neurons with very large axon arbors.

Materials and methods

Mouse lines and neuronal sparse labeling

Experiments unrelated to AD were performed with Chat-IRES-CreER/+;R26IAP/+ mice (referred to in the text as Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP). For AD experiments, R26IAP/R26IAP mice were crossed to Chat-IRES-CreER/Chat-IRES-CreER;APP/PS1/+ mice to obtain R26IAP/+;Chat-IRES-CreER/+ (WT control) and R26IAP/+;Chat-IRES-CreER/+;APP/PS1/+ littermates. The APPswe/PS1ΔE9 line was a gift from Dr Phil Wong (Johns Hopkins University). Cholinergic neuron labeling followed intraperitoneal (IP) delivery of 1–5 µg 4-hydroxytamoxifen (4HT) at P4-P5 with analysis at P12, P30 and later ages, as indicated. A wide range of 4HT doses–from 1 µg to 400 µg– was tested in an initial survey of ∼250 mice to identify the optimal dose for sparse labeling. Mice were handled in accordance with the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) guidelines of the Johns Hopkins Medical Institutions.

AP histochemistry

Mice were deeply anesthetized with ketamine/xylazine and then sacrificed by trans-cardiac perfusion with neutral buffered 10% formalin solution (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO; equivalent to 4% paraformaldehyde). Brains or eyes were heated to 70°C for 90 min to inactivate endogenous phosphatase activity. Serial brain sections of 300 µm thickness were produced with a VT1200 vibratome (Leica, Buffalo Grove, IL). AP histochemistry and clearing in 2:1 benzyl benzoate:benzyl alcohol (BBBA) were performed as described (Wu et al., 2012). For long-term storage, AP-stained brain sections were equilibrated in ethanol and stored at −20°C.

Immunofluorescence and confocal imaging

The following antibodies were used for immunostaining of 50–100 µm thick floating brain sections: goat anti-ChAT, 1:1000 (AB143; Millipore, Billerica, MA); rabbit anti-GFP, 1:1000 (A11122; Invitrogen); mouse anti-β-amyloid (6E10), 1:500 (NE1003; Millipore); rabbit anti-GFAP, 1:1000 (AB5804; Millipore); rabbit anti-Iba1, 1:1000 (019-19741; Wako, Richmond, VA); mouse anti-GFAP 1:1000 (MAB360; Millipore); mouse anti-ABCA1 monoclonal antibody HJ1 (ab66217; Abcam, Cambridge, MA); mouse anti-β-Actin antibody AC-15 (A5441; Sigma); and rabbit anti-TH, 1:1000 (AB152; Millipore). Secondary antibodies were from Invitrogen. GS-lectin staining used Alexa488-IB4, 1:1000 (I21411; Invitrogen, Grand Island, NY). Brain sections were incubated in primary antibodies diluted in PBS, 0.5% Triton X-100, 0.1 mM CaCl2 (PBSTC) + 10% normal goat or donkey serum, washed in PBSTC for 6 hr, and incubated at 4°C overnight in secondary antibodies diluted in PBSTC +10% normal goat or donkey serum. After washing in PBSTC for 4–6 hr, brain sections were mounted in Fluoromount G (17984-25; EM Sciences, Hatfield, PA). Images were captured on a Zeiss LSM700 confocal microscope and processed with Zen software, ImageJ/Fiji, and Adobe Photoshop.

AP image analysis

Only brains with fewer than five AP+ neurons/hemisphere were subjected to detailed analysis. For high-resolution analyses, isolated arbors were imaged in bright-field mode at 10X magnification with Z-planes separated by 3 µm. Grey-scale images were captured with a Zeiss Imager Z1 system in montage mode and assembled with Zeiss AxioVision software. Neurites were traced using Neuromantic neuronal tracing freeware (Darren Myat, http://www.reading.ac.uk/neuromantic) in semi-automatic mode as described (Wu et al., 2012). The total length of an axon arbor was obtained by summing up the lengths of the traces derived from that arbor within each tissue section. (Following dehydration in ethanol and equilibration in BBBA, 300 µm thick vibratome sections of brain undergo an isotropic volume shrinkage of 3.32+/−0.17 [Figure 4C]. Axon lengths reported here have been corrected for that shrinkage.) Branch points were manually scored using ImageJ/Fiji. To estimate arbor volumes, the smallest convex polygon encompassing all axon segments for a given arbor in the Z-stacked image for each 300 µm section was drawn over the image using ImageJ/Fiji, and each polygon area was calculated. Sections that contained only the subcortical axon segment were not included in the polygon analysis. Statistical analyses were performed with Excel and Graph-Pad. Error bars in the figures indicate standard deviation (SD). p-values were calculated with the student's t test.

Quantification of AP puncta

Eight randomly selected 400 µm × 400 µm images of WT or APP/PS1+ axon arbors from each of three mice per genotype and per time point, with or without bexarotene treatments, were acquired with the Zeiss Imager Z1 system as described above. AP deposits >5 pixels in diameter (images were 620 × 620 pixels) were manually counted using ImageJ/Fiji software. No AP axon fragmentation or AP deposits were detected in WT mice younger than 8 months.

Quantification of ChAT immunostained axons in motor cortex

Confocal images at Z-plane intervals of 2 µm within the interior of a 50 µm thick vibratome section were combined in two adjacent Z-stacks of four planes each (i.e., 8 µm thickness per Z-stack) and ChAT+ axon segments were traced using Neuromantic software. To estimate the length of the traced axon segments residing outside of each Z-stack–an artifact that results from the incomplete elimination of out-of-plane signals in the confocal image–the total length of those axon segments that were traced from both of the adjacent Z-stacks was measured, and found to comprise 25.4% of the total axon length traced for each Z-stack. As half of this overlap derives from each Z-stack and as this effect occurs on both surfaces of each Z-stack, the corrected axon length for each Z-stack was calculated by subtracting 25.4% from the total length of the trace from each Z-stack.

Bexarotene treatment of aged APP/PS1+ mice

Bexarotene was purchased from Sigma-Aldrich (SML0282) and dissolved in DMSO. Mice received daily gavages of 100 µl DMSO with or without bexarotene. Nine Chat-IRES-CreER;R26IAP;APP/PS1 littermates at 7 months of age were divided into three groups of three mice per group: group 1 received vehicle only (DMSO) daily for 14 days; group two received 100 mg/kg bexarotene daily for 14 days; and group three received 100 mg/kg bexarotene daily for 23 days. All mice received 50–100 µg 4HT IP at P5. Mice were perfused and analysed by AP histochemistry and immunofluorescent staining as described above.

For ABCA1 immunoblotting, 6 mice at 7 months of age were divided into two groups: 3 mice received vehicle only (DMSO) daily for 7 days, and the 3 mice received 100 mg/kg bexarotene daily for 7 days. On the eighth day, the cerebral cortices were homogenized in ∼1 ml PBS with 0.5% Triton X-100, 1 mM phenylmethylsulfonyl fluoride (PMSF), and complete protease inhibitor cocktail tablet (11697498001, Roche, Indianapolis, IN), to give a total lysate protein concentration of ∼18 µg/µl. Proteins in SDS sample buffer were loaded without boiling onto a 7.5% SDS/polacrylamide gel and immunoblotted with mouse anti-ABCA1 monoclonal antibody HJ1 (ab66217; Abcam). Conveniently, native mouse IgG in the extract runs at lower molecular weight than ABCA1. Monoclonal Anti-β-Actin antibody AC-15 (A5441; Sigma) was used as a loading control.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Drs Glenda Halliday and Mary Ann Raghanti for advice, Dr Philip Wong for the APP/PS1 mice, and Lucas Hua, Dr Amir Rattner, Yulian Zhou, and two reviewers for helpful comments on the manuscript. Supported by the Human Frontiers Science Program, the Howard Hughes Medical Institute, and the Brain Sciences Institute of the Johns Hopkins University.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Human Frontier Science Program FundRef identification ID: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100000854 LT000125/2009-L to Hao Wu.

  • Howard Hughes Medical Institute FundRef identification ID: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100000011 to Hao Wu, John Williams, Jeremy Nathans.

  • Brain Sciences Institute of the Johns Hopkins University to Jeremy Nathans.

Additional information

Competing interests

JN: Reviewing editor, eLife.

The other authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

HW, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article, Contributed unpublished essential data or reagents.

JN, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article, Contributed unpublished essential data or reagents.

JW, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: This study was performed in strict accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health. All of the animals were handled according to approved institutional animal care and use committee (IACUC) protocol MO13M469 of the Johns Hopkins Medical Institutions.

References

  1. Anden N-E, Fuxe K, Hamberger B, Hokfelt T. 1966. A quantitative study on the nigro-striatal dopamine neuron system in the rat. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica 67:306–312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aznavour N, Watkins KC, Descarries L. 2005. Postnatal development of the cholinergic innervation in the dorsal hippocampus of rat: quantitative light and electron microscopic immunocytochemical study. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 486:61–75. doi: 10.1002/cne.20501 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Badea TC, Hua LZ, Smallwood PM, Williams J, Rotolo T, Ye X, Nathans J. 2009. New mouse lines for the analysis of neuronal morphology using CreER(T)/loxP-directed sparse labeling. Public Library of Science One 16:e7859. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0007859 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baker KG, Halliday GM, Hornung JP, Geffen LB, Cotton RG, Törk I. 1991. Distribution, morphology and number of monoamine-synthesizing and substance P-containing neurons in the human dorsal raphe nucleus. Neuroscience 42:757–775. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(91)90043-N [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baskerville KA, Chang HT, Herron P. 1993. Topography of cholinergic afferents from the nucleus basalis of Meynert to representational areas of sensorimotor cortices in the rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 335:552–562. doi: 10.1002/cne.903350407 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baskerville KA, Schweitzer JB, Herron P. 1997. Effects of cholinergic depletion on experience-dependent plasticity in the cortex of the rat. Neuroscience 80:1159–1169. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(97)00064-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Berg RW, Friedman B, Schroeder LF, Kleinfeld D. 2005. Activation of nucleus basalis facilitates cortical control of a brain stem motor program. Journal of Neurophysiology 94:699–711. doi: 10.1152/jn.01125.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Boncristiano S, Calhoun ME, Kelly PH, Pfeifer M, Bondolfi L, Stalder M, Phinney AL, Abramowski D, Sturchler-Pierrat C, Enz A, Sommer B, Staufenbiel M, Jucker M. 2002. Cholinergic changes in the APP23 transgenic mouse model of cerebral amyloidosis. The Journal of Neuroscience 22:3234–3243 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Burns A O'Brien J, BAP Dementia Consensus group Auriacombe S Ballard C Broich K Bullock R Feldman H Ford G Knapp M McCaddon A Iliffe S Jacova C Jones R Lennon S McKeith I Orgogozo JM Purandare N Richardson M Ritchie C Thomas A Warner J Wilcock G Wilkinson D, British Association for Psychopharmacology . 2006. Clinical practice with anti-dementia drugs: a consensus statement from British Association for Psychopharmacology. Journal of Psychopharmacology 20:732–755. doi: 10.1177/0269881106068299 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Butcher LL. 1995. Cholinergic neurons and networks. In: Paxinos G, editor. The rat nervous system. New York: Academic Press; p. 1003–1015 2nd edition [Google Scholar]
  11. Cavanagh JB. 1984. The problems of neurons with long axons. Lancet 1:1284–1287. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(84)92457-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cramer PE, Cirrito JR, Wesson DW, Lee CY, Karlo JC, Zinn AE, Casali BT, Restivo JL, Goebel WD, James MJ, Brunden KR, Wilson DA, Landreth GE. 2012. ApoE-directed therapeutics rapidly clear β-amyloid and reverse deficits in AD mouse models. Science 335:1503–1506. doi: 10.1126/science.1217697 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cunningham MG, Connor CM, Zhang K, Benes FM. 2005. Diminished serotonergic innervation of adult medial prefrontal cortex after 6-OHDA lesions in the newborn rat. Brain Research Developmental Brain Research 157:124–131. doi: 10.1016/j.devbrainres.2005.02.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Descarries L, Gisiger V, Steriade M. 1997. Diffuse transmission by acetylcholine in the CNS. Progress in Neurobiology 53:603–625. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(97)00050-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Descarries L, Aznavour N, Hamel E. 2005. The acetylcholine innervation of cerebral cortex: new data on its normal development and its fate in the hAPP(SW,IND) mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neural Transmission 112:149–162. doi: 10.1007/s00702-004-0186-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Descarries L, Watkins KC, Garcia S, Beaudet A. 1982. The serotonin neurons in nucleus raphe dorsalis of adult rat: a light and electron microscope radioautographic study. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 207:239–254. doi: 10.1002/cne.902070305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Ferraiuolo L, Kirby J, Grierson AJ, Sendtner M, Shaw PJ. 2011. Molecular pathways of motor neuron injury in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Nature Reviews Neurology 7:616–630. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2011.152 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fitz NF, Cronican AA, Lefterov I, Koldamova R. 2013. Comment on “ApoE-directed therapeutics rapidly clear β-amyloid and reverse deficits in AD mouse models”. Science 340:924-c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Geula C, Mesulam MM. 1989. Cortical cholinergic fibers in aging and Alzheimer's disease: a morphometric study. Neuroscience 33:469–481. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(89)90399-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Gordon MN, Holcomb LA, Jantzen PT, DiCarlo G, Wilcock D, Boyett KW, Connor K, Melachrino J, O'Callaghan JP, Morgan D. 2002. Time course of the development of Alzheimer-like pathology in the doubly transgenic PS1+APP mouse. Experimental Neurology 173:183–195. doi: 10.1006/exnr.2001.7754 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hamel E. 2004. Cholinergic modulation of the cortical microvascular bed. Progress in Brain Research 145:171–178. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(03)45012-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Jan TA, Lu L, Li CX, Williams RW, Waters RS. 2008. Genetic analysis of posterior medial barrel subfield (PMBSF) size in somatosensory cortex (SI) in recombinant inbred strains of mice. BMC Neuroscience 9:3. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-9-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Jankowsky JL, Fadale DJ, Anderson J, Xu GM, Gonzales V, Jenkins NA, Copeland NG, Lee MK, Younkin LH, Wagner SL, Younkin SG, Borchelt DR. 2004. Mutant presenilins specifically elevate the levels of the 42 residue beta-amyloid peptide in vivo: evidence for augmentation of a 42-specific gamma secretase. Human Molecular Genetics 13:159–170. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddh019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kilgard MP, Merzenich MM. 1998. Cortical map reorganization enabled by nucleus basalis activity. Science 279:1714–1718. doi: 10.1126/science.279.5357.1714 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kovacević N, Henderson JT, Chan E, Lifshitz N, Bishop J, Evans AC, Henkelman RM, Chen XJ. 2005. A three-dimensional MRI atlas of the mouse brain with estimates of the average and variability. Cerebral Cortex 15:639–645 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Landreth GE, Cramer PE, Lakner MM, Cirrito JR, Wesson DW, Brunden KR, Wilson DA. 2013. Response to comments on “ApoE-directed therapeutics rapidly clear β-amyloid and reverse deficits in AD mouse models”. Science 340:924-g. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lucas-Meunier E, Fossier P, Baux G, Amar M. 2003. Cholinergic modulation of the cortical neuronal network. Pflugers Archiv 446:17–29. doi: 10.1007/s00424-002-0999-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Matsuda W, Furuta T, Nakamura KC, Hioki H, Fujiyama F, Arai R, Kaneko T. 2009. Single nigrostriatal dopaminergic neurons form widely spread and highly dense axonal arborizations in the neostriatum. The Journal of Neuroscience 29:444–453. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4029-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. McKinney M, Coyle JT, Hedreen JC. 1983. Topographic analysis of the innervation of the rat neocortex and hippocampus by the basal forebrain cholinergic system. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 217:103–121. doi: 10.1002/cne.902170109 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Mechawar N, Cozzari C, Descarries L. 2000. Cholinergic innervation in adult rat cerebral cortex: a quantitative immunocytochemical description. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 428:305–318. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20001211)428:23.0.CO;2-Y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Mechawar N, Watkins KC, Descarries L. 2002. Ultrastructural features of the acetylcholine innervation in the developing rat parietal cortex. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 443:250–258. doi: 10.1002/cne.10114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Mengler L, Khmelinskii A, Diedenhofen M, Po C, Staring M, Lelieveldt BP, Hoehn M. 2013. Brain maturation of the adolescent rat cortex and striatum: changes in volume and myelination. Neuroimage 84C:35–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Mesulam MM. 2004. The cholinergic innervation of the human cerebral cortex. Progress in Brain Research 145:67–78. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(03)45004-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Miettinen RA, Kalesnykas G, Koivisto EH. 2002. Estimation of the total number of cholinergic neurons containing estrogen receptor-alpha in the rat basal forebrain. The Journal of Histochemistry and Cytochemistry 50:891–902. doi: 10.1177/002215540205000703 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Paxinos G, Franklin KBJ. 2001. The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates. New York: Academic Press, 2nd edition [Google Scholar]
  36. Perez SE, Dar S, Ikonomovic MD, DeKosky ST, Mufson EJ. 2007. Cholinergic forebrain degeneration in the APPswe/PS1DeltaE9 transgenic mouse. Neurobiology of Disease 28:3–15. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2007.06.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Perge JA, Niven JE, Mugnaini E, Balasubramanian V, Sterling P. 2012. Why do axons differ in caliber? The Journal of Neuroscience 32:626–638. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4254-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Price AR, Xu G, Siemienski ZB, Smithson LA, Borchelt DR, Golde TE, Felsenstein KM. 2013. Comment on “ApoE-directed therapeutics rapidly clear β-amyloid and reverse deficits in AD mouse models”. Science 340:924-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Raghanti MA, Simic G, Watson S, Stimpson CD, Hof PR, Sherwood CC. 2011. Comparative analysis of the nucleus basalis of Meynert among primates. Neuroscience 184:1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.04.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Raghanti MA, Stimpson CD, Marcinkiewicz JL, Erwin JM, Hof PR, Sherwood CC. 2008a. Cholinergic innervation of the frontal cortex: differences among humans, chimpanzees, and macaque monkeys. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 506:409–424. doi: 10.1002/cne.21546 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Raghanti MA, Stimpson CD, Marcinkiewicz JL, Erwin JM, Hof PR, Sherwood CC. 2008b. Differences in cortical serotonergic innervation among humans, chimpanzees, and macaque monkeys: a comparative study. Cerebral Cortex 18:584–597. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Rilling JK, Insel TR. 1999. The primate neocortex in comparative perspective using magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Human Evolution 37:191–223. doi: 10.1006/jhev.1999.0313 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Rotolo T, Smallwood PM, Williams J, Nathans J. 2008. Genetically-directed, cell type-specific sparse labeling for the analysis of neuronal morphology. Public Library of Science One 3:e4099. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004099 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Saper CB. 1984. Organization of cerebral cortical afferent systems in the rat. II. Magnocellular basal nucleus. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 222:313–342. doi: 10.1002/cne.902220302 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Sato H, Hata Y, Masui H, Tsumoto T. 1987. A functional role of cholinergic innervation to neurons in the cat visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 58:765–780 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Schliebs R, Arendt T. 2011. The cholinergic system in aging and neuronal degeneration. Behavioural Brain Research 221:555–563. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.11.058 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Schmitz G, Langmann T. 2005. Transcriptional regulatory networks in lipid metabolism control ABCA1 expression. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta 1735:1–19. doi: 10.1016/j.bbalip.2005.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Smith DH. 2009. Stretch growth of integrated axon tracts: extremes and exploitations. Progress in Neurobiology 89:231–239. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.07.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Tesseur I, Lo AC, Roberfroid A, Dietvorst S, Van Broeck B, Borgers M, Gijsen H, Moechars D, Mercken M, Kemp J, D'Hooge R, De Strooper B. 2013. Comment on “ApoE-directed therapeutics rapidly clear β-amyloid and reverse deficits in AD mouse models”. Science 340:924-e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Underwood MD, Khaibulina AA, Ellis SP, Moran A, Rice PM, Mann JJ, Arango V. 1999. Morphometry of the dorsal raphe nucleus serotonergic neurons in suicide victims. Biological Psychiatry 46:473–483. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00043-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Underwood MD, Mann JJ, Arango V. 2007. Morphometry of dorsal raphe nucleus serotonergic neurons in alcoholism. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research 31:837–845. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00365.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Vaucher E, Hamel E. 1995. Cholinergic basal forebrain neurons project to cortical microvessels in the rat: electron microscopic study with anterogradely transported Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin and choline acetyltransferase immunocytochemistry. The Journal of Neuroscience 15:7427–7441 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Veeraraghavalu K, Zhang C, Miller S, Hefendehl JK, Rajapaksha TW, Ulrich J, Jucker M, Holtzman DM, Tanzi RE, Vassar R, Sisodia SS. 2013. Comment on “ApoE-directed therapeutics rapidly clear β-amyloid and reverse deficits in AD mouse models”. Science 340:924-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Vertes RP, Crane AM. 1997. Distribution, quantification, and morphological characteristics of serotonin-immunoreactive cells of the supralemniscal nucleus (B9) and pontomesencephalic reticular formation in the rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 378:411–424. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19970217)378:33.0.CO;2-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Wittner L, Henze DA, Záborszky L, Buzsáki G. 2007. Three-dimensional reconstruction of the axon arbor of a CA3 pyramidal cell recorded and filled in vivo. Brain Structure & Function 212:75–83. doi: 10.1007/s00429-007-0148-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Woolf NJ, Hernit MC, Butcher LL. 1986. Cholinergic and non-cholinergic projections from the rat basal forebrain revealed by combined choline acetyltransferase and Phaseolus vulgaris leucoaglutinin immunohistochemstry. Neuroscience Letters 66:281–286. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(86)90032-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Wu H, Williams J, Nathans J. 2012. Morphologic diversity of cutaneous sensory afferents revealed by genetically directed sparse labeling. eLife 1:e00181. doi: 10.7554/eLife.00181 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Zhu XO, Waite PM. 1998. Cholinergic depletion reduces plasticity of barrel field cortex. Cerebral Cortex 8:63–72. doi: 10.1093/cercor/8.1.63 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2014 May 7;3:e02444. doi: 10.7554/eLife.02444.016

Decision letter

Editor: Franck Polleux1

eLife posts the editorial decision letter and author response on a selection of the published articles (subject to the approval of the authors). An edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the substantive concerns or comments; minor concerns are not usually shown. Reviewers have the opportunity to discuss the decision before the letter is sent (see review process). Similarly, the author response typically shows only responses to the major concerns raised by the reviewers.

Thank you for sending your work entitled “Complete morphologies of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons in the mouse” for consideration at eLife. Your article has been favorably evaluated by a Senior editor and 2 reviewers, one of whom, Franck Polleux, is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors.

The Reviewing editor and the other reviewer discussed their comments before we reached this decision, and the Reviewing editor has assembled the following comments to help you prepare a revised submission.

Overall, the reviewers were very enthusiastic about the results, the methodology, and the general conclusions of your study. We would like you to address the minor comments listed below that will involve rather small changes in the data presentation and/or text editing. The only main concern listed in the minor comments of Reviewer#1 that we would like you to address concerns the bexarotene data. In particular, you should decide to either (1) provide evidence for bioavailability of your bexarotene treatment (in order to exclude the possibility that your negative results are due to ineffective regimen), or (2) you can decide to remove this data altogether, which we don't think would decrease the main take-home message of the paper. It's your decision.

Please let us know your decision and submit your revised manuscript accordingly with a cover letter detailing the changes you made.

Main issues to address:

Reviewer #1:

1) The only real unknown in the paper concerns the effectiveness of the bexarotene treatment. Because of the negative nature of the results obtained by the authors regarding the effect on axon maintenance in the mouse AD model, the authors need to provide some analysis of the effectiveness of the bexarotene treatment which is provided by oral gavage. Is the dose (solubility?) and regimen used here similar to the original paper by Cramer et al? The authors should provide some measurement of the amount of bexarotene that crosses the BBB or a positive control (biochemical?) of the effectiveness of the treatment since the negative results could simply be due to ineffective dosage and/or brain accessibility compared to the original paper. This is actually an important issue since the present results would further disprove this original study.

Reviewer #2:

2) The use of supplemental figures that have their own independent numbering system that is linked to a main Figure is cumbersome and needlessly complicating. Indeed, as this is presumably an on-line only publication, page space is clearly not a concern. Please eliminate all the supplemental figures, incorporating them into the text as a linear story, which is how you present it all. The bexarotene study, a negative result, could be deleted altogether (it could be described sufficiently in the text).

3) In the Results section, the authors refer to Figure 4H, but this figure does not appear to illustrate this. Perhaps I have misunderstood, but the only 2 mm mentioned in this paragraph that might relate to this Figure is the 2 mm (?) mean breadth of the axon arbor in the M-L dimension.

4) The final Figure 7 includes an example of a rat forebrain neuron and a human forebrain neuron (why not a mouse?); plus the same dimensions scaled up 10,000 times. I found the scaling exercise largely without benefit, and believe the entire figure could probably be eliminated (certainly the right half of each panel), with the discussion text just indicating these relative magnitudes of the real cells, for instance, the total axonal length relative to the somal diameter, or to the axonal diameter, etc. If you do maintain any of Figure 7, the micron symbol should be employed rather than a “u”.

5) As you have the densities of the neurons, are you in a position to comment on the degree of dendritic servitude per unit volume in the basal forebrain, and related to this, can you assess/comment on the extent to which the axonal territories overlap at all? Perhaps you have informative data from occasionally closer pairs of labeled cells that might lend support to calculations based on cell number, average arbor size, and cortical volume, etc.

eLife. 2014 May 7;3:e02444. doi: 10.7554/eLife.02444.017

Author response


We would prefer to keep the bexarotene data in the paper. It is a minor (and negative) part of the study. While we concede that it is not central to the paper, we think that it should be in the public domain.

Reviewer #1:

1) The only real unknown in the paper concerns the effectiveness of the bexarotene treatment. Because of the negative nature of the results obtained by the authors regarding the effect on axon maintenance in the mouse AD model, the authors need to provide some analysis of the effectiveness of the bexarotene treatment which is provided by oral gavage. Is the dose (solubility?) and regimen used here similar to the original paper by Cramer et al? The authors should provide some measurement of the amount of bexarotene that crosses the BBB or a positive control (biochemical?) of the effectiveness of the treatment since the negative results could simply be due to ineffective dosage and/or brain accessibility compared to the original paper. This is actually an important issue since the present results would further disprove this original study.

That is an excellent suggestion. We have fleshed out this issue in the Results section and in an expanded version of Figure 5–figure supplement 3 (panels B and C). In our experiments, we delivered bexarotene dissolved in DMSO by daily oral gavage, which is the regimen used by Veeraraghavalu et al. (2013 Science 340: 924-f), one of three research groups that attempted unsuccessfully to reproduce the findings of Cramer et al. (2012). In their response to the letters from the four groups that failed to reproduce their findings, the authors of Cramer et al (2012) noted that they had used a micronized formulation of bexarotene and suggested that differences in pharmacokinetics may have accounted for the discrepant findings (Landreth et al., 2013 Science 340: 924g). We have not tried to reproduce the data of Cramer et al., so our data does not directly contradict their findings. However, our bexarotene experiment was inspired by their report, so it would be appropriate for us to note that our drug formulation differed from theirs and that the authors of Cramer et al have suggested that such differences could be biologically significant. We have now added a sentence to this effect in the Results section.

We have also added data from an experimental test of bexarotene’s CNS bioavailability using our drug regimen. ABCA1 is a transcriptional target of RXR signaling and Veeraraghavalu et al. (2013) used the increase in ABCA1 protein (monitored by anti-ABCA1 immunoblotting) to assess bexarotene activation of RXR in the brain. We have conducted this type of analysis (using the same mAb used by Veeraraghavalu et al.) to assess the effectiveness of bexarotene treatment in activating RXR in the CNS. Specifically, we compared ABCA1 levels in three vehicle-treated and three bexarotene-treated mice at 7 months of age, using the same drug administration protocol that we used for the cholinergic neuron analysis in AD model mice. The result is a robust induction of ABCA1 in the brain cortex in response to one week of bexarotene treatment, in line with what Veeraraghavalu et al. observed. This new data is now included in Figure 5–figure supplement 3 (panels B and C).

Reviewer #2:

2) The use of supplemental figures that have their own independent numbering system that is linked to a main Figure is cumbersome and needlessly complicating. Indeed, as this is presumably an on-line only publication, page space is clearly not a concern. Please eliminate all the supplemental figures, incorporating them into the text as a linear story, which is how you present it all. The bexarotene study, a negative result, could be deleted altogether (it could be described sufficiently in the text).

The general comment about supplemental figures should probably be directed to the editors of eLife. We think this division into main figures and supplemental figures serves the purpose of permitting data that is supportive but not central to be placed in the public domain. In the present case, the analysis of dendrite length and the immunohistochemical characterization of the AD mouse brain fall into the latter category. We also note that in the final version of the paper on the eLife web site, it is easy to click back and forth between main figures and supplemental figures. We would prefer to keep the bexarotene data in the paper. It is a minor (and negative) part of the study. While we concede that it is not central to the paper, we think that it should be in the public domain.

3) In the Results section, the authors refer to Figure 4H, but this figure does not appear to illustrate this. Perhaps I have misunderstood, but the only 2 mm mentioned in this paragraph that might relate to this Figure is the 2 mm (?) mean breadth of the axon arbor in the M-L dimension.

Yes, the comment referring to “2 mm” does refer to the mean lateral breadth of the axon arbors. This is an example of a linear dimension that illustrates the point we were trying – perhaps unsuccessfully – to make. We have now clarified the writing.

4) The final Figure 7 includes an example of a rat forebrain neuron and a human forebrain neuron (why not a mouse?); plus the same dimensions scaled up 10,000 times. I found the scaling exercise largely without benefit, and believe the entire figure could probably be eliminated (certainly the right half of each panel), with the discussion text just indicating these relative magnitudes of the real cells, for instance, the total axonal length relative to the somal diameter, or to the axonal diameter, etc. If you do maintain any of Figure 7, the micron symbol should be employed rather than a “u”.

We have corrected “u” and changed it to the Greek letter “µ”. We use the rat rather than mouse to illustrate and discuss the dimensions of cholinergic neurons (both in the text and in Figure 7) because there is EM data on rat cholinergic neurons in the scientific literature. The 10,000x scale model presented in Figure 7 is a useful didactic tool. We have found that most people who see this data – including neurobiologists – don’t fully appreciate the extraordinary dimensions of these cells until those dimensions are converted to the scale of everyday experience.

5) As you have the densities of the neurons, are you in a position to comment on the degree of dendritic servitude per unit volume in the basal forebrain, and related to this, can you assess/comment on the extent to which the axonal territories overlap at all? Perhaps you have informative data from occasionally closer pairs of labeled cells that might lend support to calculations based on cell number, average arbor size, and cortical volume, etc.

These are excellent suggestions. We do not have a useful data set of overlapping axon arbors, but we have population data from which these parameters can be calculated. We have now done those calculations and they are now included in the Results section. The calculated redundancy of coverage of forebrain cholinergic axon arbors in the cortex and hippocampus is between 47 and 69, that is on average each point in the cortex and hippocampus resides within the arbor territories of 47–69 forebrain cholinergic arbors. The analogous calculation for dendritic territories within the basal forebrain yields a coverage factor of 470–690. The length density of cholinergic dendrites in the basal forebrain is 0.078–0.114 um/cubic um.

With respect to the calculations of the surface and volume of cholinergic neurons in the Discussion section and the diagram in Figure 7, in the revised manuscript we have changed the estimate of unmyelinated axon diameter from 0.2 um to 0.3 um, a value that more closely approximates the diameters of typical unmyelinated CNS axons (Perge et al., 2012). We have also corrected two typographical errors in the description of the surface area ratio of soma and axon arbor, and a typographical error in Figure 7.


Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

RESOURCES