Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2014 May 30;9(5):e97758. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0097758

Olfactory Deficits in an Alpha-Synuclein Fly Model of Parkinson’s Disease

Alex Y Chen 1,2,3,*, Shouzhen Xia 1, Paul Wilburn 4, Tim Tully 1,2
Editor: Mel B Feany5
PMCID: PMC4039441  PMID: 24879013

Abstract

Parkinson’s disease (PD) is the most common motor neurodegenerative disorder. Olfactory dysfunction is a prevalent feature of PD. It often precedes motor symptoms by several years and is used in assisting PD diagnosis. However, the cellular and molecular bases of olfactory dysfunction in PD are not known. The fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster, expressing human alpha-synuclein protein or its mutant, A30P, captures several hallmarks of PD and has been successfully used to model PD in numerous studies. First, we report olfactory deficits in fly expressing A30P (A30P), showing deficits in two out of three olfactory modalities, tested – olfactory acuity and odor discrimination. The remaining third modality is odor identification/naming. Second, oxidative stress is an important environmental risk factor of PD. We show that oxidative stress exacerbated the two affected olfactory modalities in younger A30P flies. Third, different olfactory receptor neurons are activated differentially by different odors in flies. In a separate experiment, we show that the odor discrimination deficit in A30P flies is general and not restricted to a specific class of chemical structure. Lastly, by restricting A30P expression to dopamine, serotonin or olfactory receptor neurons, we show that A30P expression in dopamine neurons is necessary for development of both acuity and discrimination deficits, while serotonin and olfactory receptor neurons appeared not involved. Our data demonstrate olfactory deficits in a synuclein fly PD model for exploring olfactory pathology and physiology, and for monitoring PD progression and treatment.

Introduction

Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a motor degenerative disease, preferentially affecting the dopamine system. It is characterized by cardinal symptoms of bradykinesia, rigidity, tremor and postural instability. Olfactory impairments in PD were first documented in 1975 [1] and were considered the earliest symptom in the premotor phase of PD, Braak stage 1 [2]. The olfactory impairments can precede the appearance of motor symptoms by years [3]. Indeed, Lewy bodies (LBs) appear first in the olfactory bulb, before spreading to nuclei in brain stem, in amygdala (Stage 2–3) and then spreading into substantia nigra and other regions of the midbrain (stage 4: clinical disease stage), affecting motor functions.

Olfactory impairments in PD patients are prevalent, affecting 80 to 90% of both idiopathic PD patients and familial parkinsonism [4][6]. They comprise deficits in all three functional domains: odor threshold (acuity), identification and discrimination [7][9]. These deficits were shown to predate the onset of motor symptoms by years, particularly for relatives of patients diagnosed with PD [3], [10], [11]. Therapeutic intervention is best to be administrated early during disease progress, prior to significant neurodegeneration. For this reason, early diagnosis and differential diagnosis of PD from other motor disorders, such as vascular parkinsonism, Methylphenyltetrahydropyridine-induced parkinsonism, progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP) and corticobasal degeneration (CBD), are important and can be facilitated by assessing olfactory functions [9], [12][14].

Sporadic cases of PD predominate over heritable ones, and environmental factors such as oxidative stress appear to be involved [15][17]. Indeed, chronic administration of the herbicide paraquat (PQ) alone causes selective loss of nigral dopamine neurons with a concomitant emergence of motor deficits [18][20] and is used as a toxic PD model.

High expression of human α-synuclein (αSyn) directly contributes to PD pathology. Triplication, duplication and point mutations in αSyn are associated with dominantly inherited PD [21], [22]. Allele of synuclein A30P (A30P) is linked to early onset familiar PD. Together with other PD-associated genes, αSyn and A30P transgenic flies were made to model Parkinson’s disease [23]. When A30P is expressed in all neurons, flies show classic hallmarks of PD: the formation of Lewy body-like inclusions in aged animals, progressive and selective loss of dopaminergic neurons and progressive development of motor deficits, which is responsive to L-DOPA treatment [24][28].

In the present study, we examined the motor function in a pre-established A30P fly Parkinson’s disease model. Using A30P flies, we asked whether A30P synuclein flies would develop olfactory impairments. Then, we examined the chronological relationship between the occurrences of olfactory and motor impairments. Separately, we determined whether the olfactory modality of order acuity (OA), odor discrimination (OD) or both, were affected in A30P flies, using T-maze assays [29]. Then, we investigated whether the functions of OA and OD were sensitive to oxidative stress, and whether the OD impairment was odor-specific or non-specific.

Results

Aged A30P Flies Show Deficit of Climbing

To verify A30P protein expression in a pre-established A30P PD fly model, we used immunoblotting and showed that A30P protein was expressed in the heads of A30P flies, carrying both a pan-neural Elav-gal4 driver and an UAS-A30P transgene, but not in Control (CT) flies that carried only UAS-A30P transgenes (Figure 1A). To verify reported motor deficit in A30P flies [25], we next examined the climbing performance of A30P flies, using Countercurrent apparatus. Countercurrent apparatus allowed us to monitor the climbing ability through multiple climbing trials in a test (Figure 1B). Details on the climbing assay can be found in the Methods section. We found a lower percentage of A30P flies reaching the far right fifth and sixth tube, receiving a score of 4 and 5, respectively, while a higher percentage of A30P flies (59.98%) was retained in the first tube after five trials, receiving a score of zero (**P<0.01; Figure 1C, left). A compounded climbing performance index (PI) based on fly distribution between 6 tubes showed 64% decrease of PI in 15 days old A30P (**P = 0.0014, Figure 1C, right), validating that A30P flies exhibit impaired motor function.

Figure 1. A30P flies show climbing deficit.

Figure 1

(A) A30P expression in A30P flies. A30P protein from heads of five days old flies was probed using human-specific αSyn antibody. Pan-actin expression was used as an internal control. (B) Climbing assay. Climbing performance was measured based on negative geotaxis using countercurrent apparatus. By moving the upper five climbing tubes back and forward between each trial per test, flies would distribute between tubes 0 through 5. Each trial is 10 seconds. Details are in materials and methods. (C) Left: Fly distribution based on climbing performance. More CT flies reached the No.5 tube, while more A30P flies stayed in the No.1 tube. The percentage distribution (%) for a tube is [(# of flies in the tube)/(# of flies per test)]×100. The assays were performed with flies of fifteen days old. Fifty to eighty flies were used per trial. Ct vs. A30P: t0 (8) = 3.588, **P 0 = 0.007; t4(8) = 3.770, **P 4 = 0.0054; t5(8) = 3.846, **P 5 = 0.0049. Right: The compounded climbing performance index (PI) from the left. PI = 100%×[Σ5 i = (# of flies)i×i/(# of flies per test)×5]. t(8) = 4.768, **P = 0.0014; CT is +/+: A30P/+; A30P is Elav/+: A30P/+, hereafter unless noted otherwise. Student t-test.

A30P Flies Show Age-accelerated Deficits in Odor Acuity and Odor Discrimination

We asked whether A30P expression would affect olfactory function in Drosophila. Olfactory functions comprised three major domains: odor acuity, odor discrimination and odor identification. Odor acuity (OA) and odor discrimination (OD) assays were performed using fly T-maze [29], allowing flies to make a spontaneous choice between an odor versus air in OA assay or between a mix of foreground and background odors versus a background odor alone in the OD assay (Figure 2) [30]. Detailed description of both assays can be found in figure legend and the methods section.

Figure 2. Fly odor acuity (OA) and odor discrimination (OD) assays.

Figure 2

Fly odor acuity (OA) and odor discrimination (OD) were tested using T-maze apparatus. Flies loaded into T-maze were dropped to a choice point between [BA] and air in the odor acuity assay or between [BA]e and [MCH]e+[BA]e in the odor discrimination assay. To avoid an inherent directional reference in assays, an odor of a pair was delivered from one end of T-maze during the first trial (PI1) and from the opposite end during the second trial (PI2). A test comprised two trials. The performance index (PI) of a test is the average of PI1 and PI2. When the equilibrium concentrations of BA ([BA]e) and MCH ([MCH]e) were delivered from the opposite ends of T-maze, flies would show no preference for either [BA]e or [MCH]e by equally distributing between the two end-tubes. Flies with health odor discrimination would detect the presence of [BA]e, a foreground odor, in the background of [MCH] e and run to [MCH]e tube, avoiding the [MCH]e+[BA]e tube. The formula for PI calculation was expressed as a percentage of the absolute number of flies that were differentially distributed between two end-tubes, divided by the total number of flies in a trial. PI1 or 2 = 100%×|(# of L)- (# of R)|/(# of total flies in a test).

Olfaction is a sensory function that declines with age [31][34]. In OA assay, we measured flies’ ability to detect sub-threshold concentrations of benzaldehyde (BA). We found A30P flies as young as three-days old showed olfactory impairment in detecting 0.05% BA, and such impairment progressed to inability to sense 0.1% BA on day five and fifteen (Figure 3A).

Figure 3. A30P flies show age-accelerated deficit in odor acuity and odor discrimination.

Figure 3

(A) A30P flies showed decreased odor acuity (d3: t(6) = 2.916, *P = 0.0268; d5: t(10) = 3.982, **P = 0.0026; d15: t(5) = 4.046, **P = 0.0099). Odors used are 0.05% BA vs. air on day3 and 0.1% BA versus air on day5 and day 10. (B) Aged A30P flies showed decreased performance in odor discrimination (d3: t(8) = 2.094, *P = 0.0695; d5: t(14) = 3.058, **P = 0.0085; d15: t(6) = 5.915, **P = 0.0010). Odor options were 1.5% BA made in 15% MCH background and 15% MCH. (C) A30P flies showed normal climbing on day 5 (t(14) = 0.7625, P = 0.480). (A–C) Olfactory deficits preceded motor deficit. (D) Aging exacerbated both OA and OD deficits in A30P flies. The PIs for OA and OD were presented as ratios of PIA30P over PICT. Odors used in OA assays were 0.1% BA and air. Odors used in OD assays were the same as in Figure 2B. OA ratio: t(14) = 4.563, ***P = 0.0004; OD ratio: t(7) = 2.680, *P = 0.0316, Student t-test.

In OD assay, we measured flies’ ability to discriminate between 1.5% BA from a background odor of 15% of 4-methyl-cyclohexanol (MCH). The concentrations of both odors used in OD assay were at a bioequivalent concentrations, meaning that while 1.5% BA and 15% MCH were presented at the opposite ended of a T-maze, flies would show equal preference for 1.5% BA and 15% MCH, by distributing 50∶50 at the two T-maze ends [30]. Also, 15% of MCH was pre-determined to induce a saturated behavior response, meaning that higher than 15% of MCH would not produce a higher score [30]. As a result, flies that failed to discriminate BA from MCH would not produce a higher behavior score by misrecognizing BA+15% MCH side containing higher than 15% of MCH. The odor discrimination function appears to initially improve with age in control animals (CT) and starts to show impairment on day five in A30P flies (Figure 3B). Both OA and OD impairments in A30P flies manifested on day five, when the animals still retained normal motor function, suggesting olfactory deficits preceded motor function decline (Figure 3A–C).

Age-dependent pathologies are a key feature of degenerative diseases. To determine whether aging would exacerbate OA and OD deficits, we calculated the PIA30P/PICT ratios in 5 and 10 day old flies. The OA and OD ratios significantly decreased on day 10, compared with day 5. OA decreased 16.31% (***P = 0.0004), and OD decreased 23.25% (*P = 0.024; Figure 3D), suggesting aging worsened both olfactory deficits.

Odor Acuity and Discrimination are Sensitive to Oxidative Stress

Oxidative stress has been linked to Parkinson’s disease by both post-mortem pathological [35][37] and by epidemiological studies [38], [39]. Paraquat (PQ: N,N′-dimethyl-4,4′-bipyridinium dichloride) is one of the most widely used herbicides, and was shown to induce selective dopamine cell death in substantial nigra when systematically administrated in rodent PD models [18], [19], [40]. We asked whether olfactory acuity and discrimination of A30P flies could be affected by oxidative stress. To do so, we fed newly enclosed adult flies with 5 mM PQ mixed into food for the duration of the experiment, and tested OA and OD performances in the animals. Five day old A30P flies showed healthy OA function in detecting 0.05% BA and mild OD impairment (*P = 0.0329). The 0.05% BA used in the PQ OA assay, which was lower than what was used in the OA assay on day5 in Figure 3A, is more challenging. In the PQ OD assay, the same odor pair and concentrations of odors as in Figure 3B were used. We found PQ feeding significantly enhanced both OA and OD deficits in A30P, compared to CT (***P<0.001, Figure 4 A and B), suggesting oxidative stress worsened both olfactory deficits.

Figure 4. Oxidative stress exacerbated olfactory deficits in A30P flies.

Figure 4

(A) Odor acuity (OA) and (B) odor discrimination (OD) assays. Five-days old A30P flies showed normal olfactory acuity (0.05% BA vs. air) and mild discrimination deficit. Paraquat (PQ) feeding enhanced OA and OD deficits in A30P (For CT+PQ vs. A30P+PQ comparison: FOA1,12 = 6.243, *P = 0.0280; FOD1,23 = 5.5152, *P = 0.0329). In OA assays, the mean differences of CT vs. CT+PQ was 10.04% (n.s.: P>0.05) and of A30P vs. A30P+PQ was 27.96% (***P<0.001). In OD assays, the mean differences of CT vs. CT+PQ was 5.97% (n.s.: P>0.05) and of A30P vs. A30P+PQ was 16.25% (***P<0.001). A30P and PQ-feeding were variables significantly interacting with each other (Interaction: *P OA = 0.028, *P OD = 0.033), suggesting an enhancement effect. PQ was 5 mM; wt was an internal control for apparatus assays. Odors used in OA were 0.05%BA and air. Odors used in OD assays were the same as in Figure 3B. Other comparisons: ***P<0.001, **P<0.01, *P<0.05, ns: non significant. Two-way ANOVA followed by Tukey Post-hoc tests among CTs and A30Ps with or without PQ-feeding.

Aged A30P Flies Exhibited Non-odor-specific Discrimination Deficit

Drosophila has a diverse family of odorant receptor (Or) genes. Expression of each Or is restricted to a subset of the olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) that project to the same glomerulus in the antennal lobe, a mammalian olfactory bulb analog, where odor information is first encoded [41], [42]. Studies show that quality, quantity and duration of odor exposure activate ORNs in a specific manner. Aromatic odors often activate fewer ORNs, compared to most alcohols, which activate a broader range of ORNs [43].

We asked whether OD deficit seen in A30P fly is restricted to its ability to discriminate a particular class of odor chemicals. Odor chemical class specificity would suggest that OD deficit in A30P flies affects a specific subset of Or-expressing neurons. We chose a panel of five odors: BA, Methyl Salicylate (MS), 1-Propanol (1-Pro), ethyl acetate (EA), Butyl acetate (ButA), and ethyl hexanoate (EH) to represent three classes of chemical structures: aromatics, alcohol, or esters. Together, the five odorants can activate or inhibit 23 out of the 24 types of functional Or-expressing neurons [43]. We found fifteen day old A30P flies showed non-odor specific discrimination deficit (Figure 5), suggesting that deficit in the odor discrimination is not likely mediated by impairment of any particular ORN type.

Figure 5. A30P flies showed non-odor-specific discrimination deficits.

Figure 5

Fifteen-days old flies were tested for the ability to discriminate the presence of different odors made in 15% MCH versus 15% MCH alone. Chemical structures of each odor are shown and arranged from aromatics on the left, to esters on the right, based on structure similarity to MCH, the background odor. Behavior-equivalent concentrations used were 1.5% of BAe, 20% MSe, 1% of 1-Proe, 20% of EAe, 10% of ButAe and 5% of EHe made in 15% of MCHe. BA: benzaldehyde, MS: methyl salicylate, 1-Pro: 1-propanol, EA: ethyl acetate, ButA: butyl acetate, EH: ethyl hexanoate. For CT vs. A30P comparisons from left to right, tBA(6) = 5.915,**P = 0.001, tMS(7) = 2.,*P = , t1-Pro(6) = 8.986, ***P = 0.0001, tEA(8) = 3.651,**P = 0.0065, tButA(12) = 4.750,***P = 0.0005, and tEH(11) = 8.404,***P<0.0001; Student t-test.

Odor Acuity and Discrimination Deficits in A30P Aged Flies are Dopaminergic

Dopamine neurons are selectively more susceptible to the toxicity of synuclein and A30P overexpression [44], [45]. We asked whether OA and OD deficits in A30P flies were due to: (a) functional impairments of dopamine neurons in olfaction transmission, and/or (b) functional impairments of ORNs. To answer (a), we restricted A30P expression to dopamine neurons, using tyrosine hydroxylase (TH)-gal 4 driver. TH is an enzyme required to convert L-tyrosine into L-DOPA, a precursor to making dopamine. In comparison, we restricted A30P expression to cholinergic neurons, using Cha (choline acetyltransferase)-gal4 driver. Cha is an enzyme required to add acetyl-CoA to choline to produce neurotransmitter acetylcholine. To answer (b), we restricted A30P expression in ORNs, using Or83b-gal4 driver. Or83b is an olfactory co-receptor, broadly expressed in all ORNs to mediate responses to all odors [46]. We measured PIOA and PIOD of fifteen day old A30P and CT flies.

A30P flies showed OA and OD deficits with Elav-driven A30P in all neurons (OAElav: ***P = 1.69×10−5; ODElav: **P = 0.005) and with TH-driven A30P in only dopamine neurons (OATH: **P = 0.002; ODTH: **P = 0.006). In contrast, Cha- and Or83b-driven A30P expression in cholinergic and ORNs, respectively, did not show OA and OD impairments, compared to CT (OAcha: P = 0.398; ODcha: P = 0.700; OAOr83b: P = 0.200; ODOr83b: P = 0.773; Figure 6 A and B). These results suggested that dopamine neurons were the targets of A30P protein toxicity, affecting OA and OD performances as seen in Elav-A30P flies.

Figure 6. A30P expression in dopaminergic neurons causes odor acuity and discrimination deficits in aged A30P flies.

Figure 6

Fifteen-days old A30P flies expressing A30P under Elav-gal4 (Elav), TH-gal4 (TH), Cha-gal4 (Cha) or Or83b-gal4 (Or83b) drivers were tested for odor acuity (A) and the performance of odor discrimination (B). Young wild-type flies of 1–2 days old were used as internal assay control. Only A30P expressed in dopamine neurons, A30PTH, showed olfactory acuity and discrimination deficits as seen in A30PElav. The corresponding controls for each comparison were CTElav (Elav/+; +/+; +/+), CTTH (+/+; +/+; TH/+), CTCha (+/+; +/+; Cha/+), and CTOr83b (Or83b/+; +/+; +/+). OA: tElav(14) = 6.388, ***P<0.0001, tTH(14) = 3.773, **P = 0.0021; OD: tElav(14) = 3.338, **P = 0.0049, tTH(14) = 3.257, **P = 0.0057; Student t-test.

Discussion

Olfactory deficits and misregulation of synuclein expression are prevalent in Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s patients. Prior to our study, olfactory deficits in a synuclein PD fly model were not known. Here, through examining olfactory acuity and odor discrimination, we report impairments in both olfactory domains in Elav-A30P flies that precede appearance of motor dysfunction and are accelerated by oxidative stress with paraquat feeding. Presented with different odor pairs, A30P flies show impairments in odor discrimination in non odor specific manner, as reported in PD human patients [47]. Limiting A30P protein expression to dopamine neurons caused impairments in odor acuity and discrimination similar to those in flies with Elav-driven A30P in all neurons, suggesting impairments are mediate by dysfunction in dopamine neurons. Consistent with our observations, mice expressing wild-type human synuclein, driven by Thy1 promoter, show deficits in both odor acuity and discrimination, tested by buried pellet and block assays [48]. Odor acuity was also impaired in transgenic rats, expressing both A30P and A53T synuclein under TH promoter, prior to appearance of motor symptoms [49], suggesting a direct involvement of dopamine neurons, as this study also indicated.

Olfactory organization is conserved between mammals and insects: axons of olfactory receptor (sensory) neurons are wired and converged based on the type of odorant receptor (Or) expressed. Projection axons are organized into distinctive spherical neuropils, or glomeruli, in the antennal lobe (AL) in Drosophila or the olfactory blub (OB) in mammals, respectively [50].

Whether deterioration of olfactory function is associated with central dopamine deficiency is continuously debated. Olfactory dysfunction in PD patients was thought to plateau without further deterioration to reflect disease progression [47]. However, careful sample selection, closer follow-up and an improved mechanistic understanding have been proposed to reveal the progression olfactory impairments with disease advancement [51]. Common complications are an aging-dependent natural decline in olfactory function and compensatory responses. A30P PD flies provide an excellent model to study the dynamics of olfactory deficits and other PD-related pathologies. Compared to mouse and rat models, flies have a much shorter lifespan and faster disease progression. Olfactory deficits can be detected in as early as five day old adults before motor deficit becomes apparent in 15 day old flies. A fly PD model allows studies with large animal populations to test an array of odor concentrations for different age groups, needed in olfactory studies, and provides the necessary statistical power to draw conclusions.

Whether olfactory deficits in PD originate from sensory, central or motor neuron deficits is not known and is difficult to disentangle in human or mouse studies. Spontaneous, in contrast with conditioned and experience-dependent [52], odor detection and discrimination were proposed to be processed locally within the antennal lobe in insects [53], [54] and the olfactory bulb in mammals [55][60]. Results of OA and OD assays in this study depend on spontaneous responses and, thus, argue against involvement of the central nerve system. Known odor receptor neurons (ORNs) are mostly glutaminergic [61], [62] and acetylcholinergic (ACh) [63], [64]. At the same time, dopamine neurons were found to innervate the center of neurpiles and in the glial layer at intraglomeruli [65], indirectly modulating the outputs of olfactory sensory neurons in flies. Therefore, normal OA and OD function of Or83b-A30P flies and olfactory deficits seen in TH-A30P flies together suggest that olfactory deficits in A30P flies were not due to dysfunction of ORNs but due to local dopamine neurons modulating ORNs in the antennal lobe. A direct involvement of local antenna lobe neurons in olfactory perception was also supported by a recently study, which showed reduced neural activity in electroantennogram and reduced marker for neuronal active zones in PINK null flies [66] - a transgenic fly with autosomal recessive PD.

Deficit in motor neurons was proposed to contribute olfactory impairments in PD patients. PD patients showed significantly decreased sniff airflow and volume. When asked to sniff vigorously, patients’ performances in odor acuity and identification improved [67]. Indeed, active sensing by sniffing (inhaling air into the nasal cavity) is the first step and a requirement for normal olfactory function for human and rodents. It is therefore difficult to disassociate the prime cause of olfactory deficits between the dysfunctions of motor or sensory neurons. On the contrary, flies sample odorants in air directly through odor sensory neurons that reside in the sensillum, and do not require motor function, such as sniffing. Therefore, olfactory deficits in A30P flies support direct involvement of sensory impairment, specifically, mis-modulation of sensory neuron output by A30P expression in dopamine neurons. This is also consistent with observations that increased sniffing effort in PD patients does not restore olfactory function back to normal and is only beneficial to the worst performers that tend to show greatest motor dysfunction [67].

Constitutive neurogenesis in OB is an active event, where activity is highly responsive to injuries. Inhibition of neurogenesis in OB leads to olfactory impairments [68]. Both increase and decrease of neurogenesis in olfactory bulbs were reported Parkinson’s patients and PD animal models [49], [69][73]. The discrepancies may be due to the time points when neurogenesis was examined: early or late during PD progression. Possible compensation mechanism that leads to upregulated neurogenesis was proposed to explain the increased neurogenesis in OB in PD patients. In transgenic PD models, the discrepancies may be also due to methods of expressing synculein or its mutant genes: acute expression by using a conditional system or constitutive expression by using a promoter. The kinetics of synuclein protein expression may affect regulation of neurogenesis. In flies, one prominent site of adult neurogenesis in Drosophila brain is at the conjunction between antennal lobes and antennal nerve bundle [74]. Extensive genetic tools available in flies allow to resolve discrepancies and to investigate the role of neurogenesis in PD pathology and in olfactory function to determine whether altered neurogenesis is a primary or secondary effect of olfaction impairments in s synuclein fly model. By comparing conditional and continuous methods of expressing synuclein in the same genetic background, Drosophila PD model can help delineate the effect of synuclein on neurogenesis.

Materials and Methods

Flies

We used wild-type 2U flies, a pan-neural Elav-gal4 (Elav) promoter, dopaminergic specific Th-gal4 (TH) promoter, olfactory-neuron Or83b-gal4 (Or83b) promoter, cholinergic neuron Cha-gal4 (Cha) promoter from Bloomington Fly center, and a UAS-human αSyn A30P (A30P) from Mel B. Feany Lab. All flies were outcrossed to 2U for six or more generations to equilibrate genetic background before experiments. Flies were raised under a 12∶12 light:dark cycle at 25°C and 70% humidity incubator. PQ was administrated by rearing adult flies in vials with regular food [75] mixed with PQ. PQ food was made by completely melting the regular fly food and allowing it to cool at 57° before adding PQ. PQ-containing fly food was stored up to ten days at 4°C before usage.

Climbing Assay

Startle-induced negative geotactic climbing was used to measure climbing performance with a countercurrent apparatus [76]. During testing, a group of 20–30 flies was placed into the far left “bottom” tube at the loading position. Flies were gently tapped down to the bottom of tube three times with a force of about 1 to 1.5 kg at the frequency of one tap per second. The top “receiver” tubes were quickly moved to the left, allowing flies 10 seconds to climb up into the top receiver tube at a starting position in each trial. Flies that succeeded in climbing up to the receiver tube were transferred to the next bottom tube at the end of the 10 second period by moving the array of top tubes one slot to the right, followed by gentle tapping and prompt placement of top tubes back to the starting position. These steps repeated five times, providing flies five trials to climb into the top tube. At the end of the test, flies in the far right bottom tube that successfully climbed to the top receiver tube in all five trials received a score of 5. Flies that remained in the first bottom tube at the end of five trials failed to climb above a tube’s length in each trial and received a score of 0. A Performance index (PI) was calculated as shown in Figure 1 legend.

Olfactory Assays

Flies were collected and kept in clean bottles for one day before testing. Groups of 50 to 80 flies were transferred into fresh food vials on the test day. All odors were made fresh in mineral oil using odor free glass vials. To eliminate odor and any asymmetric biases in the testing machines, one group of flies was tested with a given odor (or odor mixture) delivered into the left arm of the T-maze (PI1), and a second reciprocal group was tested with the same odor (or odor mixture) delivered into the right arm (PI2). The average of two performance indices (PIs) from these two groups of flies was defined as a score for n = 1. Two olfactory assays performed were odor acuity (OA) and odor discrimination (OD). In the odor acuity (OA) assay, different concentrations of benzaldehyde (BA) were paired with odor-free mineral oil (air). Normally, flies can detect the presence of BA at a very low concentration [77] and show preference towards or away from BA, depending on BA concentration. Flies with OA deficits exhibit a higher threshold of detecting the presence of BA odor and a shifted olfactory response to BA concentration. In odor discrimination (OD) assay, flies were tested for ability to detect the presence of a behavior-equivalent concentration of BA (BAe) odor in the background of methylcyclohexanol (MCHe) odor versus MCHe odor alone. The behavior-equivalent concentrations of two odors are those that produce a PI score close to zero in flies presented with the two choices at the opposite arms of a T-maze [30]. A zero score means that flies would show no odor preference and distribute 50/50 in both arms. Normal flies can identify the presence of BA in the background of MCH and avoid the arm filled with MCHe and BAe odor mixture. However, flies with an OD deficit, would show difficulties in detecting BAe in MCHe background, resulting in a lower PI score, suggesting failure to discriminate the presence of BAe in a strong MCHe odorant background.

Immunoblotting

Thirty fly heads were homogenized in 150 ul 2x Laemmli sample buffer (Bio-Rad, Hercules, CA) and centrifuged for 20 minutes at 13,000×g at 4°C. Protein extract from about five heads was used per lane on SDS PAGE gels for detecting and visualizing the expression transgene, human αSyn protein. Antibodies used were human specific anti-αSyn (1∶1000, Cell Signaling Technology, Inc., Danvers, MA) and a pan-actin antibody (1∶1000; Sigma-Aldrich Corp., St. Louis, MO).

Statistics

Due to the nature of their mathematical derivation, PIs are distributed normally. Data were subjected to Student t-test, one-way or two-way ANOVA test, followed by Tukey post-hoc tests. All data were presented as “mean ± SEM”. All statistics were analyzed using Prism 6 (GraphPad Software, Inc.).

Funding Statement

This work is supported by Dart NeuroScience LLC (http://www.dartneuroscience.com). The funder provided support in the form of salaries for authors AYC, SX and TT, but did not have any additional role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. The specific roles of these authors are articulated in the ‘author contributions’ section.

References

  • 1. Ansari KA, Johnson A (1975) Olfactory function in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Chronic Dis 28: 493–497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Hawkes CH, Del Tredici K, Braak H (2010) A timeline for Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 16: 79–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Ross GW, Petrovitch H, Abbott RD, Tanner CM, Popper J, et al. (2008) Association of olfactory dysfunction with risk for future Parkinson’s disease. Ann Neurol 63: 167–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Berendse HW, Booij J, Francot CM, Bergmans PL, Hijman R, et al. (2001) Subclinical dopaminergic dysfunction in asymptomatic Parkinson’s disease patients’ relatives with a decreased sense of smell. Ann Neurol 50: 34–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Doty RL (2012) Olfactory dysfunction in Parkinson disease. Nat Rev Neurol 8: 329–339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Markopoulou K, Larsen KW, Wszolek EK, Denson MA, Lang AE, et al. (1997) Olfactory dysfunction in familial parkinsonism. Neurology 49: 1262–1267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Hawkes C (2003) Olfaction in neurodegenerative disorder. Mov Disord 18: 364–372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Mesholam RI, Moberg PJ, Mahr RN, Doty RL (1998) Olfaction in neurodegenerative disease: a meta-analysis of olfactory functioning in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases. Arch Neurol 55: 84–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Muller A, Mungersdorf M, Reichmann H, Strehle G, Hummel T (2002) Olfactory function in Parkinsonian syndromes. J Clin Neurosci 9: 521–524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Montgomery EB Jr, Baker KB, Lyons K, Koller WC (1999) Abnormal performance on the PD test battery by asymptomatic first-degree relatives. Neurology 52: 757–762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Ponsen MM, Stoffers D, Booij J, van Eck-Smit BL, Wolters E, et al. (2004) Idiopathic hyposmia as a preclinical sign of Parkinson’s disease. Ann Neurol 56: 173–181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Doty RL, Singh A, Tetrud J, Langston JW (1992) Lack of major olfactory dysfunction in MPTP-induced parkinsonism. Ann Neurol 32: 97–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Katzenschlager R, Lees AJ (2004) Olfaction and Parkinson’s syndromes: its role in differential diagnosis. Curr Opin Neurol 17: 417–423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Wenning GK, Shephard B, Hawkes C, Petruckevitch A, Lees A, et al. (1995) Olfactory function in atypical parkinsonian syndromes. Acta Neurol Scand 91: 247–250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Dhillon AS, Tarbutton GL, Levin JL, Plotkin GM, Lowry LK, et al. (2008) Pesticide/environmental exposures and Parkinson’s disease in East Texas. J Agromedicine 13: 37–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Fong CS, Cheng CW, Wu RM (2005) Pesticides exposure and genetic polymorphism of paraoxonase in the susceptibility of Parkinson’s disease. Acta Neurol Taiwan 14: 55–60. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Giasson BI, Lee VM (2000) A new link between pesticides and Parkinson’s disease. Nat Neurosci 3: 1227–1228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Betarbet R, Sherer TB, MacKenzie G, Garcia-Osuna M, Panov AV, et al. (2000) Chronic systemic pesticide exposure reproduces features of Parkinson’s disease. Nat Neurosci 3: 1301–1306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. McCormack AL, Thiruchelvam M, Manning-Bog AB, Thiffault C, Langston JW, et al. (2002) Environmental risk factors and Parkinson’s disease: selective degeneration of nigral dopaminergic neurons caused by the herbicide paraquat. Neurobiol Dis 10: 119–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Franco R, Li S, Rodriguez-Rocha H, Burns M, Panayiotidis MI (2010) Molecular Mechanisms of Pesticide-induced Neurotoxicity: Relevance to Parkinson’s Disease. Chem Biol Interact. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 21. Singleton AB, Farrer M, Johnson J, Singleton A, Hague S, et al. (2003) alpha-Synuclein locus triplication causes Parkinson’s disease. Science 302: 841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Kruger R, Kuhn W, Muller T, Woitalla D, Graeber M, et al. (1998) Ala30Pro mutation in the gene encoding alpha-synuclein in Parkinson’s disease. Nat Genet 18: 106–108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Botella JA, Bayersdorfer F, Gmeiner F, Schneuwly S (2009) Modelling Parkinson’s disease in Drosophila. Neuromolecular Med 11: 268–280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Auluck PK, Bonini NM (2002) Pharmacological prevention of Parkinson disease in Drosophila. Nat Med 8: 1185–1186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Feany MB, Bender WW (2000) A Drosophila model of Parkinson’s disease. Nature 404: 394–398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Mizuno H, Fujikake N, Wada K, Nagai Y (2010) alpha-Synuclein Transgenic Drosophila As a Model of Parkinson’s Disease and Related Synucleinopathies. Parkinsons Dis 2011: 212706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Park SS, Schulz EM, Lee D (2007) Disruption of dopamine homeostasis underlies selective neurodegeneration mediated by alpha-synuclein. Eur J Neurosci 26: 3104–3112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Trinh K, Moore K, Wes PD, Muchowski PJ, Dey J, et al. (2008) Induction of the phase II detoxification pathway suppresses neuron loss in Drosophila models of Parkinson’s disease. J Neurosci 28: 465–472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Tully T, Quinn WG (1985) Classical conditioning and retention in normal and mutant Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol A 157: 263–277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Xia S, Tully T (2007) Segregation of odor identity and intensity during odor discrimination in Drosophila mushroom body. PLoS Biol 5: e264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Doty RL, Shaman P, Applebaum SL, Giberson R, Siksorski L, et al. (1984) Smell identification ability: changes with age. Science 226: 1441–1443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Grotewiel MS, Martin I, Bhandari P, Cook-Wiens E (2005) Functional senescence in Drosophila melanogaster. Ageing Res Rev 4: 372–397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Mobley AS, Rodriguez-Gil DJ, Imamura F, Greer CA (2014) Aging in the olfactory system. Trends Neurosci 37: 77–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Stevens JC, Cain WS (1987) Old-age deficits in the sense of smell as gauged by thresholds, magnitude matching, and odor identification. Psychol Aging 2: 36–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Sofic E, Paulus W, Jellinger K, Riederer P, Youdim MB (1991) Selective increase of iron in substantia nigra zona compacta of parkinsonian brains. J Neurochem 56: 978–982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Mann VM, Cooper JM, Daniel SE, Srai K, Jenner P, et al. (1994) Complex I, iron, and ferritin in Parkinson’s disease substantia nigra. Ann Neurol 36: 876–881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Montine KS, Quinn JF, Zhang J, Fessel JP, Roberts LJ 2nd, et al. (2004) Isoprostanes and related products of lipid peroxidation in neurodegenerative diseases. Chem Phys Lipids 128: 117–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Liou HH, Tsai MC, Chen CJ, Jeng JS, Chang YC, et al. (1997) Environmental risk factors and Parkinson’s disease: a case-control study in Taiwan. Neurology 48: 1583–1588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Lai BC, Marion SA, Teschke K, Tsui JK (2002) Occupational and environmental risk factors for Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 8: 297–309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Cicchetti F, Lapointe N, Roberge-Tremblay A, Saint-Pierre M, Jimenez L, et al. (2005) Systemic exposure to paraquat and maneb models early Parkinson’s disease in young adult rats. Neurobiol Dis 20: 360–371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Gao Q, Yuan B, Chess A (2000) Convergent projections of Drosophila olfactory neurons to specific glomeruli in the antennal lobe. Nat Neurosci 3: 780–785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Vosshall LB, Wong AM, Axel R (2000) An olfactory sensory map in the fly brain. Cell 102: 147–159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Hallem EA, Carlson JR (2006) Coding of odors by a receptor repertoire. Cell 125: 143–160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Barzilai A, Melamed E (2003) Molecular mechanisms of selective dopaminergic neuronal death in Parkinson’s disease. Trends Mol Med 9: 126–132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Venda LL, Cragg SJ, Buchman VL, Wade-Martins R (2010) alpha-Synuclein and dopamine at the crossroads of Parkinson’s disease. Trends Neurosci 33: 559–568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Larsson MC, Domingos AI, Jones WD, Chiappe ME, Amrein H, et al. (2004) Or83b encodes a broadly expressed odorant receptor essential for Drosophila olfaction. Neuron 43: 703–714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Doty RL, Deems DA, Stellar S (1988) Olfactory dysfunction in parkinsonism: a general deficit unrelated to neurologic signs, disease stage, or disease duration. Neurology 38: 1237–1244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Fleming SM, Tetreault NA, Mulligan CK, Hutson CB, Masliah E, et al. (2008) Olfactory deficits in mice overexpressing human wildtype alpha-synuclein. Eur J Neurosci 28: 247–256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Lelan F, Boyer C, Thinard R, Remy S, Usal C, et al. (2011) Effects of Human Alpha-Synuclein A53T-A30P Mutations on SVZ and Local Olfactory Bulb Cell Proliferation in a Transgenic Rat Model of Parkinson Disease. Parkinsons Dis 2011: 987084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Vosshall LB, Stocker RF (2007) Molecular architecture of smell and taste in Drosophila. Annu Rev Neurosci 30: 505–533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Kranick SM, Duda JE (2008) Olfactory dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease. Neurosignals 16: 35–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Ueno K, Naganos S, Hirano Y, Horiuchi J, Saitoe M (2013) Long-term enhancement of synaptic transmission between antennal lobe and mushroom body in cultured Drosophila brain. J Physiol 591: 287–302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Acebes A, Martin-Pena A, Chevalier V, Ferrus A (2011) Synapse loss in olfactory local interneurons modifies perception. J Neurosci 31: 2734–2745. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Wilson RI (2011) Understanding the functional consequences of synaptic specialization: insight from the Drosophila antennal lobe. Curr Opin Neurobiol 21: 254–260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Daly KC, Carrell LA, Mwilaria E (2008) Characterizing psychophysical measures of discrimination thresholds and the effects of concentration on discrimination learning in the moth Manduca sexta. Chem Senses 33: 95–106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Daly KC, Carrell LA, Mwilaria E (2007) Detection versus perception: physiological and behavioral analysis of olfactory sensitivity in the moth (Manduca sexta). Behav Neurosci 121: 794–807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Escanilla O, Arrellanos A, Karnow A, Ennis M, Linster C (2010) Noradrenergic modulation of behavioral odor detection and discrimination thresholds in the olfactory bulb. Eur J Neurosci 32: 458–468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Hosler JS, Buxton KL, Smith BH (2000) Impairment of olfactory discrimination by blockade of GABA and nitric oxide activity in the honey bee antennal lobes. Behav Neurosci 114: 514–525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Mwilaria EK, Ghatak C, Daly KC (2008) Disruption of GABAA in the insect antennal lobe generally increases odor detection and discrimination thresholds. Chem Senses 33: 267–281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Wei CJ, Linster C, Cleland TA (2006) Dopamine D(2) receptor activation modulates perceived odor intensity. Behav Neurosci 120: 393–400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Trombley PQ, Shepherd GM (1993) Synaptic transmission and modulation in the olfactory bulb. Curr Opin Neurobiol 3: 540–547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Berkowicz DA, Trombley PQ, Shepherd GM (1994) Evidence for glutamate as the olfactory receptor cell neurotransmitter. J Neurophysiol 71: 2557–2561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Kreissl S, Bicker G (1989) Histochemistry of acetylcholinesterase and immunocytochemistry of an acetylcholine receptor-like antigen in the brain of the honeybee. J Comp Neurol 286: 71–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Bicker G (1999) Histochemistry of classical neurotransmitters in antennal lobes and mushroom bodies of the honeybee. Microsc Res Tech 45: 174–183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Kirchhof BS, Homberg U, Mercer AR (1999) Development of dopamine-immunoreactive neurons associated with the antennal lobes of the honey bee, Apis mellifera. J Comp Neurol 411: 643–653. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Poddighe S, Bhat KM, Setzu MD, Solla P, Angioy AM, et al. (2013) Impaired sense of smell in a Drosophila Parkinson’s model. PLoS One 8: e73156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Sobel N, Thomason ME, Stappen I, Tanner CM, Tetrud JW, et al. (2001) An impairment in sniffing contributes to the olfactory impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98: 4154–4159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Bardy C, Pallotto M (2010) What happens to olfaction without adult neurogenesis? Front Neurosci 4: 24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Winner B, Kohl Z, Gage FH (2011) Neurodegenerative disease and adult neurogenesis. Eur J Neurosci 33: 1139–1151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Huisman E, Uylings HB, Hoogland PV (2004) A 100% increase of dopaminergic cells in the olfactory bulb may explain hyposmia in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 19: 687–692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Huisman E, Uylings HB, Hoogland PV (2008) Gender-related changes in increase of dopaminergic neurons in the olfactory bulb of Parkinson’s disease patients. Mov Disord 23: 1407–1413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Hoglinger GU, Rizk P, Muriel MP, Duyckaerts C, Oertel WH, et al. (2004) Dopamine depletion impairs precursor cell proliferation in Parkinson disease. Nat Neurosci 7: 726–735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Marxreiter F, Nuber S, Kandasamy M, Klucken J, Aigner R, et al. (2009) Changes in adult olfactory bulb neurogenesis in mice expressing the A30P mutant form of alpha-synuclein. Eur J Neurosci 29: 879–890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. von Trotha JW, Egger B, Brand AH (2009) Cell proliferation in the Drosophila adult brain revealed by clonal analysis and bromodeoxyuridine labelling. Neural Dev 4: 9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Protocols CSH (2012) Food Media for Drosophila. Cold Spring Harbor Protocols 2012: pdb.rec072488. [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Benzer S (1967) Behavioral Mutants of Drosophila Isolated By Countercurrent Distribution. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 58: 1112–1119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Wang Y, Chiang AS, Xia S, Kitamoto T, Tully T, et al. (2003) Blockade of neurotransmission in Drosophila mushroom bodies impairs odor attraction, but not repulsion. Curr Biol 13: 1900–1904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES