Skip to main content
BMJ Open logoLink to BMJ Open
. 2014 May 28;4(5):e005095. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2014-005095

Material, psychosocial and sociodemographic determinants are associated with positive mental health in Europe: a cross-sectional study

Stefanie Dreger 1, Christoph Buck 2, Gabriele Bolte 1
PMCID: PMC4039806  PMID: 24871540

Abstract

Objectives

To investigate the association between psychosocial, sociodemographic and material determinants of positive mental health in Europe.

Design

Cross-sectional analysis of survey data.

Setting

34 European countries.

Participants

Representative Europe-wide sample consisting of 21 066 men and 22 569 women aged 18 years and over, from 34 European countries participating in the third wave of the European Quality of Life Survey (2011–2012).

Outcome

Positive mental health as measured by the WHO-5—Mental Well-being Index, while the lowest 25% centile indicated poor positive mental health.

Results

The prevalence of poor positive mental health was 30% in women and 24% in men. Material, as well as psychosocial, and sociodemographic factors were independently associated with poor positive mental health in a Europe-wide sample from 34 European countries. When studying all factors together, the highest OR for poor positive mental health was reported for social exclusion (men: OR=1.73, 95% CI 1.59 to 1.90; women: OR=1.69, 95% CI 1.57 to 1.81) among the psychosocial factors. Among the material factors, material deprivation had the highest impact (men: OR=1.96, 95% CI 1.78 to 2.15; women: OR=1.93, 95% CI 1.79 to 2.08).

Conclusions

This study gives the first overview on determinants of positive mental health at a European level and could be used as the basis for preventive policies in the field of positive mental health in Europe.

Keywords: Epidemiology, Mental Health, Public Health


Strengths and limitations of this study.

  • Large dataset with comparable data across Europe.

  • Overview of a broad range of material, psychosocial and sociodemographic determinants of positive mental health among people in Europe.

  • Stratified analysis to take potential gender differences into account.

  • No causal interpretation is possible because of cross-sectional nature of study.

  • Response rate of the EQLS was lower than aspired and varied from more than 60% in Bulgaria, Cyprus, Malta, Poland and Slovakia to below 30% in Luxembourg and the UK.

Background

According to the definition of the WHO mental health is a ‘state of well-being in which the individual realises his skills, copes with the normal stresses of life, can work productively and fruitfully, and is able to make a contribution to his community’.1 Studies provide empirical support that mental health consists of two independent dimensions: mental ill-health and positive mental health (PMH) or mental well-being.2 3 Recent studies that have explicitly considered levels of PMH in populations have illustrated that good mental health is more than just the absence of disease,2 4 5 and that people can experience PMH even if diagnosed with a mental illness.3 This is because mental well-being or PMH and mental illness are caused by different factors.6 It has also been shown that low PMH is a risk factor for depression7 8 and absence of PMH has been associated with an increased risk of mortality.2 9

The study of PMH is relatively young and there is still discussion on a common definition of PMH or mental well-being.10 There are two (complementary) traditions in conceptualising well-being: the hedonic approach emphasises feeling good (happiness, pleasant affect, life satisfaction) whereas the eudaimonic approach focuses on optimal social and psychological functioning.5 A valid measure of PMH should include items that assess the hedonic and eudaimonic domain.3 5 11 12 Whereas various studies examined determinants of mental ill health, profound knowledge of determinants of PMH is lacking. PMH can be influenced by sociodemographic, psychosocial or material factors.13–16 However, until now studies that have focused on PMH have investigated only few determinants and looked at one country or at a very limited number of countries. Whereas prevalences of PMH in European countries have been reported before,17 no study so far has analysed a broad set of determinants of PMH considering a high number of European countries. The objective of our study was therefore to examine the association between sociodemographic, psychosocial and material factors and PMH at a European level taking gender differences into account.

Methods

Sample

This study is based on the European Quality of Life Survey (EQLS), which is run every 4 years by the European Foundation for the improvement of living and working conditions. The third wave of the EQLS, which was carried out in 2011–2012, included people aged 18 years and older from 34 countries (EU-27, Croatia, Iceland, Montenegro, former Yugoslav Republic of Macedonia, Serbia, Turkey, Kosovo). In all countries, data were collected via face-to-face interviews at respondents’ home, who were selected by multistage random sampling. The overall response rate was 41%. A more detailed description of the EQLS 2012 can be found elsewhere.18

Positive mental health

Positive mental health was measured with the WHO-5—Mental Well-being Index (WHO-5).19 It is calculated from responses to five items: (1) I have felt cheerful and in good spirits; (2) I have felt calm and relaxed; (3) I have felt active and vigorous; (4) I woke up feeling fresh and rested and (5) my daily life has been filled with things that interest me. The degree to which the aforesaid positive feelings were present in the past 2 weeks is scored on a six-point Likert scale ranging from 0 ‘at no time’ to 5 ‘all of the time’. The scores to these five questions can total to a maximum of 25, which is then multiplied by 4 to get to a maximum of 100, where 0 corresponds with worst thinkable well-being and 100 equals best thinkable well-being. The WHO-5 is considered a valid instrument to evaluate PMH in population-based studies20 and assesses PMH with items covering the eudaimonic perspective on well-being as well as items covering the hedonic dimensions of well-being.17 An average score of the index was calculated for the study population and those with values below the 25% centile were considered to have poor PMH.

Potential determinants of PMH

Three groups of determinants of PMH were studied: sociodemographic, psychosocial and material factors. This classification of determinants was inspired by studies that have used this classification in the field of self-rated health.21–23

Sociodemographic factors were age, educational level (categorised into three groups according to the International Standard Classification of Education), urbanisation level (living in rural/urban area) and citizenship (European/non-European). All these variables were categorical variables. Since potential risk factors might have different meaning for men and women, gender was not considered as a potential risk factor but as a structural variable and thus potential effect modifier. Therefore, all analyses were stratified by gender.24

Psychosocial factors were marital status, presence of children, social support (help from family/friends/neighbour/service provider in case of need for help around the house, advice, looking for a job, feeling depressed, financial problems; 5 items), social network (frequency of contact with family/friends/neighbours; 8 items), political participation (attended a meeting of a trade union/political party/political action group, attended protest or demonstration, signed a petition, contacted a politician/public official; 4 items), trust (in parliament/legal system/press/police/government/local authorities; 6 items), religion (frequency of attending religious services), social exclusion (feelings of lack of recognition/confusion in life/exclusion/inferiority; 4 items). Marital status, presence of children and religion were categorical variables. For social network, social support, political participation, trust and social exclusion, average scores were calculated and the median was used as cut-off point for the creation of dichotomised variables.

Material factors were household tenure, housing problems (shortage of space, rot in windows/doors/floors, damp/leaks in walls/roof, lack of bath or shower/indoor flushing toilet, place to sit outside; 6 items), neighbourhood problems (noise/air pollution/quality of drinking water/crime/violence/vandalism/litter/ traffic; 6 items), material deprivation (not able to afford the following amenities/activities: heating/vacation/furniture/meal with meat, chicken, fish every second day/new clothes/having friends and family for drinks or meals at least once a month; 6 items), financial problems (problems paying bills for rent/informal and consumer loans/electricity; 4 items), quality of public services (health services/education system/public transport/long-term care/child care services/state pension system/social housing; 6 items).

Household tenure was a categorical variable. Housing problems, neighbourhood problems, financial problems, material deprivation and quality of public services were dichotomised at the median of the average score of the items.

Statistical methods

First, the distribution of sociodemographic, psychosocial and material factors was described separately for men and women, and the percentage of poor PMH was reported for each category.

We performed random intercept multilevel logistic regression analyses to examine the association between the potential determinants and PMH.

Multilevel models are particularly appropriate for research designs where data for participants are organised on more than one level to take into account the between-variability and within-variability of these hierarchically organised data (individuals, region, country).25 The model contains a so-called fixed part and a random component. Individual determinants were introduced as fixed effects, and country and region were used as random intercepts in the multilevel analysis taking into account three levels of data: individuals (level 1) nested in 330 regions (level 2), which are nested in 34 countries (level 3). Three separate models for women and men were computed to study the association between the groups of determinants (sociodemographic, psychosocial and material factors) and PMH independently (models 1–3). After that, all variables that were significant at α=0.05 for at least one gender were included in the final model (model 4). Median ORs (MOR) were computed to quantify the country-level variation. MOR is defined as the median value of the OR between the country at highest risk and the country at lowest risk when randomly picking out two countries.26 The MOR equals 1 if there is no variation between countries and gets larger if the between-country variation increases.27 The measure is directly comparable with fixed-effects ORs.27

Although inter-relations between factors were found, no collinearity was detected as the variance inflation factor was never greater than 1.9. Variance inflation factors greater than 2.5 may be problematic.28

Since determinants of PMH have only rarely been studied, no literature on potential interactions was available. However, gender differences have been suggested in this context14 29 and men and women have different life circumstances. Therefore, we studied men and women separately.

All statistical analyses were conducted using SAS statistical software V.9.3. The product of the design weight and post-stratification weight was used as the weighting factor as recommended in the EQLS guidelines. In sensitivity analyses multilevel logistic regressions were conducted without weights and with weights. The parameter estimates were substantially similar. Therefore the unweighted ORs are presented, as advised by Winship and Radbill,30 because they are more efficient and the SE is correct.

Results

Overall, 21 066 men and 22 569 women participated in the study and were considered for the present analysis. Table 1 shows the distribution of sociodemographic, psychosocial and material factors and the percentage of people with poor PMH in each category for men and women separately. Overall, the proportion of poor PMH was higher in women than in men (30% vs 24%). Furthermore, women in the study sample were slightly older, more often had low education, did not work, had children, practiced religion, did not engage in political participation and were affected by material deprivation.

Table 1.

Percentages of men and women with poor positive mental health (PMH) by sociodemographic, psychosocial and material factors*

  Men
Women
N Per cent  Poor PMH (%) N Per cent Poor PMH (%)
PMH
 Good 15 997 76 15 751 70
 Poor 5069 24 6818 30
Sociodemographic factors
 Age (years)
  18–24 2707 13 16 2539 11 22
  25–34 3919 19 21 3742 17 24
  35–49 5847 28 25 5925 26 29
  50–64 4932 23 27 5227 23 32
  65+ 3662 17 28 5136 23 38
 Education
  Primary or less 1971 9 36 3090 14 44
  Secondary 13 945 67 24 13 983 62 30
  Tertiary 5004 24 19 5366 24 22
 Working
  Yes 11 494 55 20 8955 40 24
  No 9573 45 29 13 614 60 34
 Urbanisation level
  Countryside or village 9774 47 25 10 325 46 31
  Town or city 11 247 54 24 12 187 54 30
 Citizenship
  European 20 509 98 24 22 094 98 30
  Non-European 471 2 25 409 2 30
Psychosocial factors
 Marital status
  Living with partner 11 990 57 24 11 678 52 28
  Living alone 8926 43 24 10 749 48 32
 Children
  Present 13 065 62 26 16 272 72 33
  Absent 8001 38 22 6297 28 24
 Religion
  Practicing often 4831 23 25 6854 31 31
  Rarely 6875 33 23 7637 34 29
  Never 9255 44 24 7976 36 31
 Social network
  High 4097 19 24 4563 20 31
  Low 16 969 81 24 18 007 80 30
 Social support
  High 10 070 48 21 10 467 46 26
  Low 10 996 52 27 12 102 54 34
 Political participation
  Yes 5410 26 21 4818 22 25
  No 15 268 74 25 17 380 78 32
 Level of trust
  High 10 359 49 18 10 947 49 24
  Low 10 708 51 30 11 623 52 36
 Social exclusion
  Low 7800 37 16 8200 36 21
  High 13 266 63 29 14 369 64 35
Material factors
 Neighbourhood problems
  Low 8024 38 21 8547 38 27
  High 13 043 62 26 14 022 62 32
 Housing problems
  Absent 13 381 64 20 13 893 62 25
  Present 7499 36 31 8455 38 39
 Household tenure
  Tenant 14 606 75 23 15 997 76 30
  Owner 4832 25 25 5059 24 30
 Material deprivation
  Absent 9843 51 14 8991 43 18
  Present 9592 49 33 11 829 57 38
 Financial problems
  No 16 207 77 21 17 379 77 27
  Yes 4859 23 35 5191 23 41
 Quality of public services
  Good 5699 27 17 6241 28 21
  Poor 15 367 73 27 16 329 72 34

*Product of the design weight and the post-stratification weight was applied.

Models 1–3

Table 2 presents the results for the multilevel logistic regression analyses, with each set of factors being studied separately for men and women. In model 1, which included sociodemographic factors, lower educational level, older age and not working were significantly associated with poor PMH among both genders. Additionally being citizen of a non-European country was associated with poor PMH in women. In model 2, including sociodemographic and psychosocial factors, living without a partner, practicing religion rarely or never, low social support, low levels of trust and high levels of social exclusion were significantly associated with poor PMH among both genders, independent of sociodemographic factors. Having no children was additionally associated with poor PMH in women. The strongest effect in model 2 was seen for high social exclusion with an OR of 1.82 (95% CI 1.68 to 1.98) for men and 1.80 (95% CI 1.68 to 1.92) for women. In model 3, including sociodemographic factors and material factors, all material factors, except household tenure, were associated with poor PMH among both genders, controlling for sociodemographic characteristics. The highest OR was seen for material deprivation in both genders: the OR for men was 2.13 (95% CI 2.00 to 2.41) and for women was 2.17 (95% CI 2.01 to 2.35). Urbanisation level and social network were not associated with poor PMH in both genders in the respective models, and were therefore not included in model 4.

Table 2.

Association between sociodemographic, psychosocial and material factors and poor positive mental health for men and women, results from multilevel logistic regression analyses, showing OR and 95% CI

  Men
Women
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Sociodemographic factors
 Age (years)
  18–24 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
  25–34 1.78 (1.51 to 2.08) 1.65 (1.37 to 1.98) 1.37 (1.20 to 1.56) 1.27 (1.09 to 1.50)
  35–49 2.33 (2.00 to 2.70) 2.26 (1.88 to 2.71) 1.87 (1.65 to 2.11) 1.69 (1.45 to 1.96)
  50–64 2.17 (1.88 to 2.50) 2.44 (2.03 to 2.93) 1.87 (1.65 to 2.11) 1.85 (1.59 to 2.15)
  65+ 1.77 (1.52 to 2.06) 2.47 (2.03 to 3.01) 1.97 (1.74 to 2.24) 2.11 (1.81 to 2.46)
 Education
  Primary or less 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Secondary 0.66 (0.58 to 0.74) 0.73 (0.64 to 0.83) 0.68 (0.62 to 0.74) 0.76 (0.69 to 0.84)
  Tertiary 0.50 (0.43 to 0.57) 0.71 (0.61 to 0.83) 0.47 (0.42 to 0.53) 0.65 (0.58 to 0.73)
 Working
  Yes 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
  No 1.66 (1.52 to 1.81) 1.27 (1.15 to 1.40) 1.27 (1.18 to 1.37) 1.13 (1.05 to 1.23)
 Urbanisation level
  Countryside or village 1.00 1.00
  Town or city 1.01 (0.93 to 1.09) 1.01 (0.95 to 1.07)
 Citizenship
  European 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Non-European 1.22 (0.94 to 1.56) 1.01 (0.77 to 1.33) 1.31 (1.05 to 1.63) 1.02 (0.81 to 1.30)
Psychosocial factors
 Marital status
  Living with partner 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Living alone 1.20 (1.09 to 1.31) 1.18 (1.07 to 1.30) 1.31 (1.23 to 1.40) 1.17 (1.09 to 1.25)
 Children
  Present 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Absent 0.96 (0.86 to 1.08) 1.00 (0.89 to 1.12) 0.83 (0.76 to 0.91) 0.90 (0.82 to 0.98)
 Religion
  Practicing often 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Rarely 1.11 (1.00 to 1.23) 1.27 (1.14 to 1.42) 1.09 (1.01 to 1.17) 1.24 (1.14 to 1.35)
  Never 1.27 (1.15 to 1.41) 1.13 (1.01 to 1.26) 1.27 (1.18 to 1.38) 1.08 (1.00 to 1.17)
 Social network
  High 1.00 1.00
  Low 1.03 (0.93 to 1.13) 1.04 (0.96 to 1.12)
 Social support
  High 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Low 1.30 (1.20 to 1.41) 1.20 (1.10 to 1.31) 1.44 (1.35 to 1.54) 1.29 (1.20 to 1.38)
 Political participation
  Yes 1.00 1.00
  No 0.99 (0.91 to 1.08) 1.03 (0.95 to 1.11)
 Level of trust
  High 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Low 1.66 (1.53 to 1.79) 1.43 (1.31 to 1.55) 1.51 (1.42 to 1.61) 1.32 (1.23 to 1.41)
 Social exclusion
  Low 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
  High 1.82 (1.68 to 1.98) 1.73 (1.59 to 1.90) 1.80 (1.68 to 1.92) 1.69 (1.57 to 1.81)
Material factors
 Neighborhood problems
  Low 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
  High 1.16 (1.07 to 1.27) 1.13 (1.04 to 1.23) 1.12 (1.04 to 1.20) 1.07 (1.00 to 1.15)
 Housing problems
  Absent 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Present 1.46 (1.34 to 1.60) 1.40 (1.30 to 1.52) 1.58 (1.48 to 1.69) 1.52 (1.43 to 1.63)
 Household tenure
  Tenant 1.00 1.00
  Owner 1.00 (0.89 to 1.11) 1.00 (0.91 to 1.08)
 Material deprivation
  Absent 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Present 2.19 (2.00 to 2.41) 1.96 (1.78 to 2.15) 2.17 (2.01 to 2.35) 1.93 (1.79 to 2.08)
 Financial problems
  No 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Yes 1.57 (1.42 to 1.73) 1.50 (1.34 to 1.63) 1.39 (1.29 to 1.51) 1.33 (1.23 to 1.43)
 Quality of public services
  Good 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Poor 1.54 (1.40 to 1.70) 1.39 (1.27 to 1.53) 1.64 (1.51 to 1.77) 1.51 (1.40 to 1.63)
Random effects
 Country level
  Between country variance (SE) 0.1767 (0.05066) 0.1265 (0.03799) 0.08360 (0.02876) 0.07835 (0.02697) 0.1711 (0.04749) 0.1258 (0.03594) 0.08378 (0.02609) 0.07317 (0.02314)
  MOR 1.50 1.40 1.32 1.31 1.48 1.40 1.32 1.30
 Region level
  Between region variance (SE) 0.07319 (0.01670) 0.06726 (0.01644) 0.08601 (0.02034) 0.08965 (0.02038) 0.07009 (0.01378) 0.05600 (0.01303) 0.05915 (0.01401) 0.05245 (0.01312)

Models 2 and 3 have been adjusted for sociodemographic factors of model 1.

MOR, median OR.

Model 4

In model 4 the strongest associations with poor PMH among both genders were observed for higher age, social exclusion (men: OR=1.73, 95% CI 1.59 to 1.90; women: OR=1.69, 95% CI 1.57 to 1.81) and material deprivation (men: OR=1.96, 95% CI 1.27 to 1.53; women: OR=1.93, 95% CI 1.79 to 2.08). Moreover, living without a partner, lower education status, not working, practicing religion rarely or never, low social support, social exclusion and all material factors were significantly associated with poor PMH among both genders. Not having children was independently associated with poor PMH in women only. Being citizen of a non-European country was no longer significant when taking into account all other factors in model 4.

Country-level variation

MOR differed only slightly between men and women, but decreased from model 1 to model 4, where more individual-level information was included. The MOR in model 1, where sociodemographic factors are included, was 1.50 for men and 1.45 for women. However, when studying all factors together in model 4 the MOR was lower, namely 1.31 for men and 1.30 for women. Thus, country-specific variation was larger with regard to effects of sociodemographic factors on mental health, but smaller considering psychosocial (MOR=1.40 for both genders) or material factors (MOR=1.32 for both genders).

Discussion

This is one of the first studies to examine PMH in a large Europe-wide sample and to the best of our knowledge the first to report on a wide range of determinants. We grouped the determinants that have individually been reported in the literature with regard to mental health. Our study found a broad range of risk factors for poor PMH and our results are mainly in line with previous research that showed similar associations in single countries or single correlates, not controlling for other factors. However, most studies so far have looked at mental illness and not at PMH. Other studies covering positive aspects of mental health used single questions about happiness or life satisfaction. This approach is not the same as the concept of PMH, since it only covers the hedonistic perspective of well-being, in the sense of feeling happy.31

A large number of associations between sociodemographic, psychosocial and material risk factors and PMH in citizens from 34 European countries were found in this study. Higher age, lower educational status and not working were associated with poor PMH among both genders. Of the psychosocial factors, practicing religion rarely or never, low social support, low levels of trust and high social exclusion were associated with poor PMH among both genders. Living alone was associated with PMH in both genders. Not having children had a protective effect against poor PMH for women but not for men. All material determinants were associated with poor PMH among men and women.

Our results are in line with previous studies reporting that low educational level,14 32–34 and not working,14 33 are associated with poor mental well-being. The results on age and indicators of mental well-being are controversial, some studies reporting that older age groups are at a higher risk for poor mental well-being,14 16 32 35 which would be in accordance with our results, others finding the opposite.36–38 Associations between living area and mental well-being have been reported; however, the direction of this relationship is not clear: living in a rural area14 and living in a large city16 have been associated with poor PMH. When classifying living area into two categories—urban or rural—we did not find a significant association between living area and PMH. Living alone,16 33 35 low social support,13 14 16 34 39 loneliness14 and exclusion40 have been associated with poor positive mental or emotional health and a study in Russia found associations between high levels of trust and high emotional health.40 We found that not or rarely attending religious services was associated with poor PMH. A previous study reported that frequency of prayer is associated with mental well-being.38 There are some studies investigating the associations of material factors and mental illness. Poor economic condition16 and neighbourhood problems15 39 have been associated with poor mental well-being or PMH before. However, research on the effect of other material factors on PMH is lacking.

In the intermediate models 1–3, age, social exclusion and material deprivation showed the strongest association with poor PMH among men and women. These three factors also appeared to have the strongest association with poor PMH in our final model (model 4), examining the effect of all determinants together. Particularly, all material factors were significantly associated with poor PMH in the separate as well as in the complete model, taking further sociodemographic and psychosocial factors into account. This group of determinants has not been studied extensively yet in the context of PMH but rather with regard to self-rated health21 22 or mental illness.41 The fact that these factors stayed significant throughout all models is in agreement with the belief that material factors may have a direct (through biological pathways) or indirect effect (through eg, behavioural factors) on health outcomes.22 We might not have found a significant association of household tenure and PMH because there are cultural differences between countries in the approaches of buying a house or living on rent. Hence household tenure might not be an indicator for material prosperity in all countries.

One of the limitations of this study is its cross-sectional nature. When interpreting the relationship between the determinants, it needs to be kept in mind that no causal interpretation is possible. The response rate of 41% in the third round of the EQLS was lower than aspired and differed across countries.18 It has been argued that non-participants may be more likely to belong to low social groups and to have poorer health outcomes.42 This would be a selection bias and the prevalence of poor PMH as well as the association between some determinants, especially material determinants, might be underestimated. This study did not take into account (mediating) behavioural factors (eg, physical activity), which may play a role in the association with PMH. Physical activity has a positive effect on PMH43 and it could be hypothesised that living in areas with high neighbourhood problems might hinder leisure-time physical activity, hence physical activity could be a mediating factor in the association between material factors and PMH. For future studies it would be highly desirable to also include behavioural factors. Although the WHO-5 is a validated and relatively short measure of PMH in population surveys, there are more comprehensive measures to assess this complex construct, which should be used in future studies. Moreover, in this study the cut-off point for poor PMH has been set at the 25% centile to look at people who have low levels of PMH. Using medians or quartiles as cut-off points when no official cut-off points are available is common practice. However, a standardised cut-off point for the WHO-5 would be desirable. The study of PMH is relatively young and there is still discussion on a common definition of PMH and different measurements exist. It will take some years to achieve agreement on the appropriate measurement and definition of PMH.10 In this context it would be highly desirable to also test if instruments are gender sensitive. This study, on the other hand, has many strengths. The large dataset with comparable data across Europe, allowed us to study each gender separately and comparability of data between 34 European countries enabled us to give an overall view of determinants of PMH among people in Europe. It used the WHO-5 as a validated measure for PMH and has analysed a broad picture of potential risk factors.

Conclusion

This study showed independent associations between various sociodemographic, psychosocial and material determinants and PMH. Our study provides the first overview of the distribution of determinants and their association with PMH in Europe. Therefore, it can be used as the basis for confirmatory and more specific analysis of determinants of poor PMH as well as for the development of preventive programmes or policies in this context.

Supplementary Material

Author's manuscript
Reviewer comments

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge the European Foundation for the Improvement of Living and Working Conditions as the original data creators and the UK Data Archive. The European Foundation for the Improvement of Living and Working Conditions and the UK Data Archive bear no responsibility for the data analysis or interpretation.

Footnotes

Contributors: SD was in charge of designing, analysing and writing up for the manuscript. CB advised on statistical analyses and helped to draft the manuscript. GB participated in the conceptualisation of the analyses and in the revision of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding: This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Competing interests: None.

Provenance and peer review: Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

Data sharing statement: We analysed data of the European Quality of Life Survey (EQLS). Permission to analyse data of the EQLS can be requested at Eurofond (http://www.eurofound.europa.eu/surveys/faq/index.htm).

References

  • 1.WHO. Strengthening mental health promotion. Fact sheet no 220. Geneva, 2001
  • 2.Huppert FA, Whittington JE. Evidence for the independence of positive and negative well-being: implications for quality of life assessment. Br J Health Psychol 2003;8:107–22 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Keyes CL. The mental health continuum: from languishing to flourishing in life. J Health Soc Behav 2002;43:207–22 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Keyes CLM. Mental illness and/or mental health? Investigating axioms of the complete state model of health. J Consult Clin Psychol 2005;73:539–48 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Hu Y, Stewart-Brown S, Twigg L, et al. Can the 12-item General Health Questionnaire be used to measure positive mental health? Psychol Med 2007;37:1005–13 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Ryff CD, Love GD, Urry HL, et al. Psychological well-being and ill-being: do they have distinct or mirrored biological correlates? Psychother Psychosom 2006;75:85–95 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Wood AM, Joseph S. The absence of positive psychological (eudemonic) well-being as a risk factor for depression: a ten year cohort study. J Affect Disord 2010;122:213–17 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Keyes CL, Dhingra SS, Simoes EJ. Change in level of positive mental health as a predictor of future risk of mental illness. Am J Public Health 2010;100:2366–71 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Keyes CL, Simoes EJ. To flourish or not: positive mental health and all-cause mortality. Am J Public Health 2012;102:2164–72 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Huppert FA, So TT. Flourishing across Europe: application of a new conceptual framework for defining well-being. Soc Indic Res 2013;110:837–61 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ryan RM, Deci EL. On happiness and human potentials: a review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annu Rev Psychol 2001;52:141–66 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Tennant R, Hiller L, Fishwick R, et al. The Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (WEMWBS): development and UK validation. Health Qual Life Outcomes 2007;5:63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bull T. Work life and mental well-being: single and coupled employed mothers in Southern Europe and Scandinavia. Glob Health Promot 2009;16:6–16 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Van Lente E, Barry MM, Molcho M, et al. Measuring population mental health and social well-being. Int J Public Health 2012;57:421–30 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Guite HF, Clark C, Ackrill G. The impact of the physical and urban environment on mental well-being. Public Health 2006;120: 1117–26 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lehtinen V, Sohlman B, Kovess-Masfety V. Level of positive mental health in the European Union: results from the Eurobarometer 2002 survey. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health 2005;1:9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Eurofound. Quality of life in Europe: subjective well-being. Luxembourg: Publications Office of the European Union, 2013 [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Eurofound. Third European Quality of Life Survey—quality of life in Europe: impacts of the crisis. Luxembourg: Publications Office of the European Union, 2012 [Google Scholar]
  • 19.World Health Organization Regional Office Europe. Well-being measures in primary health care: the Depcare project. Stockholm; 1998, Report on a WHO Meeting [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bech P, Olsen LR, Kjoller M, et al. Measuring well-being rather than the absence of distress symptoms: a comparison of the SF-36 mental health subscale and the WHO-Five well-being scale. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 2003;12:85–91 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Niedhammer I, Kerrad S, Schutte S, et al. Material, psychosocial and behavioural factors associated with self-reported health in the Republic of Ireland: cross-sectional results from the SLAN survey. BMJ Open 2013;3:pii: e002797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Schutte S, Chastang JF, Parent-Thirion A, et al. Association between socio-demographic, psychosocial, material and occupational factors and self-reported health among workers in Europe. J Public Health (Oxf) 2013. Published Online First: 23 May 2013. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/fdt050 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Aldabe B, Anderson R, Lyly-Yrjanainen M, et al. Contribution of material, occupational, and psychosocial factors in the explanation of social inequalities in health in 28 countries in Europe. J Epidemiol Community Health 2011;65:1123–31 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Springer KW, Mager Stellman J, Jordan-Young RM. Beyond a catalogue of differences: a theoretical frame and good practice guidelines for researching sex/gender in human health. Soc Sci Med 2012;74:1817–24 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Blakely T, Subramanian SV. Multilevel studies. In: Oakes JM, Kaufman JS, eds. Methods in social epidemiology. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 2006:316–40 [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Merlo J, Chaix B, Ohlsson H, et al. A brief conceptual tutorial of multilevel analysis in social epidemiology: using measures of clustering in multilevel logistic regression to investigate contextual phenomena. J Epidemiol Community Health 2006;60:290–7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Larsen K, Merlo J. Appropriate assessment of neighborhood effects on individual health: integrating random and fixed effects in multilevel logistic regression. Am J Epidemiol 2005;161:81–8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Katz MH. Multilevel analysis. A parctical guide for clinicians and public health researchers. Cambridge University Press, 2011 [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Davoren MP, Fitzgerald E, Shiely F, et al. Positive mental health and well-being among a third level student population. PLoS ONE 2013;8:e74921. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Winship C, Radbill L. Sampling weights and regression-analysis. Sociolog Methods Res 1994;23:230–57 [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Waterman AS. Two conceptions of happiness: contrasts of personal expressiveness (eudaimonia) and hedonic enjoyment. J Pers Soc Psychol 1993;64:678–91 [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Lluch-Canut T, Puig-Llobet M, Sanchez-Ortega A, et al. Assessing positive mental health in people with chronic physical health problems: correlations with socio-demographic variables and physical health status. BMC Public Health 2013;13:928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Cable N, Bartley M, Chandola T, et al. Friends are equally important to men and women, but family matters more for men's well-being. J Epidemiol Community Health 2013;67:166–71 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Hapke U, von der Lippe E, Busch M, et al. Psychische Gesundheit bei Erwachsenen in Deutschland. Daten und Fakten: Ergebnisse der Studie “Gesundheit in Deutschland aktuell 2010”: Robert Koch-Institut, 2012 [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Dear K, Henderson S, Korten A. Well-being in Australia—findings from the National Survey of Mental Health and Well-being. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2002;37:503–9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Keyes CLM, Westerhof GJ. Chronological and subjective age differences in flourishing mental health and major depressive episode. Aging Ment Health 2012;16:67–74 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Esteban MM, Puerto MI, Fernández Cordero X, et al. [Determinants of poor mental health in people aged 16 to 64 residing in a large city]. An Sist Sanit Navar 2012;35:229–40 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Meisenhelder JB, Chandler EN. Prayer and health outcomes in church lay leaders. West J Nurs Res 2000;22:706–16 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Gale CR, Dennison EM, Cooper C, et al. Neighbourhood environment and positive mental health in older people: the Hertfordshire Cohort Study. Health Place 2011;17: 867–74 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Rose R. How much does social capital add to individual health? A survey study of Russians. Soc Sci Med 2000;51:1421–35 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.McKenzie SK, Imlach Gunasekara F, Richardson K, et al. Do changes in socioeconomic factors lead to changes in mental health? Findings from three waves of a population based panel study. J Epidemiol Community Health 2013;68:253–60 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Goldberg M, Chastang JF, Leclerc A, et al. Socioeconomic, demographic, occupational, and health factors associated with participation in a long-term epidemiologic survey: a prospective study of the French GAZEL cohort and its target population. Am J Epidemiol 2001;154:373–84 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Malcolm E, Evans-Lacko S, Little K, et al. The impact of exercise projects to promote mental wellbeing. J Ment Health 2013;22:519–27 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Author's manuscript
Reviewer comments

Articles from BMJ Open are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES