Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2014 May 8;3:e02391. doi: 10.7554/eLife.02391

Re-examining how complexin inhibits neurotransmitter release

Thorsten Trimbuch 1,, Junjie Xu 2,, David Flaherty 2, Diana R Tomchick 2, Josep Rizo 2,3,4,*, Christian Rosenmund 1,*
Editor: Axel T Brunger5
PMCID: PMC4040926  PMID: 24842998

Abstract

Complexins play activating and inhibitory functions in neurotransmitter release. The complexin accessory helix inhibits release and was proposed to insert into SNARE complexes to prevent their full assembly. This model was supported by ‘superclamp’ and ‘poor-clamp’ mutations that enhanced or decreased the complexin-I inhibitory activity in cell–cell fusion assays, and by the crystal structure of a superclamp mutant bound to a synaptobrevin-truncated SNARE complex. NMR studies now show that the complexin-I accessory helix does not insert into synaptobrevin-truncated SNARE complexes in solution, and electrophysiological data reveal that superclamp mutants have slightly stimulatory or no effects on neurotransmitter release, whereas a poor-clamp mutant inhibits release. Importantly, increasing or decreasing the negative charge of the complexin-I accessory helix inhibits or stimulates release, respectively. These results suggest a new model whereby the complexin accessory helix inhibits release through electrostatic (and perhaps steric) repulsion enabled by its location between the vesicle and plasma membranes.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02391.001

Research organism: human, mouse

eLife digest

The instructions sent to, from and within the brain are rapidly transmitted along neurons in the form of electrical signals. These signals cannot pass across the small gaps—called synapses—that separate neighboring neurons. Instead, neurons release chemicals called neurotransmitters into the synapses, and these relay the signal to the next neuron.

The neurotransmitters are stored inside neurons in small bubbles called vesicles. To release these neurotransmitters into the synapse, the membrane that encloses the vesicle fuses with the membrane that surrounds the neuron. To fuse the membranes, proteins embedded in the vesicle membrane interact with similar proteins in the neuron membrane to form a structure called a SNARE complex. Additional proteins control membrane fusion to ensure that the signal is passed to the other neuron at the right time and with the appropriate efficiency.

Among these proteins are the complexins, which are often found attached to SNARE complexes. Although different parts of complexins can both help and hinder membrane fusion, a part known as an accessory helix is thought to have only one role—to stop the membranes from fusing together. Several models have been suggested for how the accessory helix interferes with fusion. However, after performing a range of analyses by diverse biophysical techniques, Trimbuch, Xu et al. suggest these models are unlikely to describe the process accurately.

Instead, Trimbuch, Xu et al. propose a new model based on the electrostatic properties of two molecules that are both negatively charged. An accessory helix taken from a fruit fly complexin was more negatively charged than a mammalian version, and experiments showed it was also better at preventing the release of neurotransmitters. It is thought that the negative charges on the helix hold the membranes apart because the helix is located between the membranes, which are also negatively charged. Consistent with this model, Trimbuch, Xu et al. showed that the membranes fused more easily when some of the negative charges on the accessory helix were replaced with positive charges. The next challenges are to test the model further with additional studies, and to explain how other proteins work with complexins to control neurotransmitter release.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02391.002

Introduction

Neurotransmitter release is crucial for interneuronal communication and is exquisitely regulated by a sophisticated protein machinery (Sudhof, 2013). Great advances have been made in elucidating the mechanism of release (Brunger et al., 2009; Sorensen, 2009; Jahn and Fasshauer, 2012; Rizo and Sudhof, 2012) and basic aspects of this process have been reconstituted with eight central components of the release machinery (Ma et al., 2013), leading to a model with defined roles for each component. In this model, the neuronal soluble N-ethylmaleimide-sensitive factor attachment protein receptors (SNAREs) synaptobrevin, syntaxin-1 and SNAP-25 form a tight four-helix bundle called the SNARE complex (Sollner et al., 1993; Poirier et al., 1998; Sutton et al., 1998) that brings the synaptic vesicle and plasma membranes together (Hanson et al., 1997) and is critical for membrane fusion; N-ethylmaleimide-sensitive factor (NSF) and soluble NSF attachment proteins (SNAPs) disassemble the SNARE complex (Sollner et al., 1993) to recycle the SNAREs for another round of fusion (Mayer et al., 1996), and may favor physiological membrane fusion by disassembling syntaxin-1-SNAP-25 complexes (Ma et al., 2013); Munc18-1 and Munc13s orchestrate SNARE complex assembly in an NSF/SNAP resistant manner (Ma et al., 2013), and may play a direct role in fusion (Dulubova et al., 2007; Li et al., 2011b); and synaptotagmin-1 acts as a Ca2+ sensor (Fernandez-Chacon et al., 2001), likely by bridging the two membranes (Arac et al., 2006; Xue et al., 2008a).

Tight regulation of neurotransmitter release also depends critically on complexins, small soluble proteins that bind to the SNARE complex (McMahon et al., 1995) and play activating and inhibitory functions. Absence of complexins leads to a severe impairment of Ca2+-evoked exocytosis and to varied effects on spontaneous release ranging from small decreases to dramatic increases, depending on the system (Reim et al., 2001; Huntwork and Littleton, 2007; Xue et al., 2008b; Maximov et al., 2009; Hobson et al., 2011; Martin et al., 2011; Yang et al., 2013). These results likely arise from an interplay between stimulatory and inhibitory activities of different regions of complexins (Xue et al., 2007, 2009; Cho et al., 2010; Kaeser-Woo et al., 2012). Complexin I (CpxI) is largely unstructured in solution (Pabst et al., 2000) but forms a central α-helix that binds to the SNARE complex and is preceded by an accessory helix (Bracher et al., 2002; Chen et al., 2002) (Figure 1A). The central helix is crucial for both the activating and inhibitory functions of complexins, while the accessory helix inhibits release (Xue et al., 2007; Maximov et al., 2009); the complexin N-terminus plays an activating function, releasing the inhibition by the accessory helix (Xue et al., 2010; Yang et al., 2010), and the C-terminal sequence has activating and inhibitory roles (Kaeser-Woo et al., 2012).

Figure 1. Models of complexin function.

(A) Domain diagram of CpxI and ribbon diagram of the crystal structure of the CpxI(26-83)/SNARE complex (PDB code 1KIL) (Chen et al., 2002). Selected residue numbers are indicated above the ribbon diagram and on the CpxI(26–83) ribbon in the structure. Synaptobrevin is colored in red, syntaxin-1 in yellow, SNAP-25 in blue and green (N-terminal and C-terminal SNARE motifs, respectively), and CpxI(26-83) in orange (accessory helix) and pink (central helix). The same color code is used in all panels. N and C indicate the N- and C-termini of the SNARE motifs. (B) Ribbon diagram of the crystal structure of the complex between the CpxI(26-83) superclamp mutant and a synaptobrevin truncated SNARE complex (PDB code 3RK3) (Kummel et al., 2011). Three complexes are shown to illustrate the zigzag array present in the crystals. (CE) Models for the inhibitory activity of the complexin accessory helix. In all models, the accessory helix is proposed to prevent C-terminal assembly of a partially assembled SNARE complex either by inserting into the complex (C), by binding to the synaptobrevin SNARE motif (D), or by electrostatic repulsion with both membranes (E).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02391.003

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. The interface between CpxI and the SNARE complex.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

A ribbon diagram of the crystal structure of the CpxI(26-83)/SNARE complex(PDB code 1KIL) (Chen et al., 2002) is shown, with synaptobrevin colored in red, syntaxin-1 in yellow, SNAP-25 in blue and green (N-terminal and C-terminal SNARE motifs, respectively), and CpxI(26-83) in orange (accessory helix) and pink (central helix). The side chains of CpxI(26-83), synaptobrevin and syntaxin-1 are shown, and selected residues are labeled as a guide to see which residues interact and where were the truncations of the different proteins made.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2. High B-factors in the accessory helix in the crystal structure of the CpxI(26-83) superclamp mutant bound to SCΔ60.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(A) Electron density images of the crystal structure of CpxI(26-83) superclamp mutant bound to SCΔ60 (PDB accession code 3RK3; Kummel et al., 2011). Superimposed on the coordinates from 3RK3 is the map with coefficients 2|mF(obs)−DF(calc)|, contoured at the r.m.s.d. level of the map. The phases and map were calculated from the deposited 3RK3 structure factors in the program PHENIX (Afonine et al., 2010). Note that little electron density is observed for the side chains of the accessory helix, even for the hydrophobic residues in the interface with the SNAREs (the native L41 and the mutant L27, F34 and A37; upper panel). In contrast, clear electron density is observed for the side chains of the central helix that contact the SNARE complex (lower panel; some of these residues are labeled). (B) Plot of average atomic displacement parameters (ADP or ‘B-factors’) over the residues of the CpxI(26-83) superclamp molecule in 3RK3. The plot is color coded from blue as the minimum to red as the maximum value of the average B-factors. Note that the B-factors are much higher for the accessory helix than for the residues of the central CpxI helix that contact the truncated SNARE complex (residues 59 to 72).

Cell–cell fusion assays and reconstitution studies also indicated dual roles for complexins that likely recapitulate to some extent their physiological functions (Giraudo et al., 2006; Schaub et al., 2006; Yoon et al., 2008; Malsam et al., 2009, 2012; Diao et al., 2012), but in most cases these studies revealed only stimulatory or inhibitory roles. The activating function has been proposed to arise from stabilization of the SNARE complex by complexin binding (Chen et al., 2002), from interactions of the complexin N-terminus with the C-terminus of the SNARE complex (Xue et al., 2010), and from binding of the complexin C-terminal region to phospholipids (Seiler et al., 2009), but these models remain to be validated. The inhibitory activity of complexins attracted much attention because several studies suggested that complexins prevent exocytosis before Ca2+ influx and synaptotagmin-1 releases the inhibition upon Ca2+ binding by displacing complexins from the SNARE complex (Giraudo et al., 2006; Schaub et al., 2006; Tang et al., 2006; Roggero et al., 2007). Later analyses showed that CpxI is not fully displaced but there is competition between synaptotagmin-1 and part of CpxI for binding to the SNARE complex on membranes (Dai et al., 2007; Xu et al., 2013). Genetic interaction studies in hippocampal neurons showed that complexins regulate release similarly in the absence or presence of synaptotagmin-1, indicating that complexins function independently of synaptotagmin-1 (Xue et al., 2007, 2010), but this finding does not exclude the notion that complexins and synaptotagmin-1 may cooperate in regulating release. For example, the dramatic increase of spontaneous release observed in complexin nulls in Drosophila requires synaptotagmin-1 (Jorquera et al., 2012), and absence of complexins in hippocampal neurons sensitizes release to loss of 50% of synaptotagmin-1 expression (Xue et al., 2010).

While it is clear that the complexin accessory helix inhibits release (Xue et al., 2007; Maximov et al., 2009), a satisfactory model for this activity has not emerged yet. We initially proposed that part of this helix might replace part of the synaptobrevin SNARE motif in trans partially assembled SNARE complexes, hindering C-terminal assembly of the complex (Xue et al., 2007; Figure 1C). A similar insertion model, but envisioning that the entire accessory helix replaces part of synaptobrevin, was proposed later and was supported by the enhanced inhibition in cell–cell fusion assays caused by replacing charged with hydrophobic residues in the accessory helix of CpxI ('superclamp mutants') and by the design of a ‘poor-clamp’ mutation (K26A) that impairs the inhibitory activity (Giraudo et al., 2009). The crystal structure of a fragment of a CpxI superclamp mutant (D27L, E34F, R37A) bound to a SNARE complex with C-terminally truncated synaptobrevin suggested an alternative model whereby the central helix of one CpxI molecule binds to a SNARE complex and the accessory helix inserts into another SNARE complex, resulting in a zigzag array (Kummel et al., 2011; Figure 1B). However, formation of such a complex with wild type (WT) CpxI would be highly unfavorable thermodynamically because three charged residues would be placed into hydrophobic environments.

The study described here was designed to investigate how the complexin accessory helix inhibits neurotransmitter release, testing the insertion and zigzag models as well as additional models that emerged subsequently (Figure 1D,E). Using NMR spectroscopy and isothermal titration calorimetry (ITC), we show that the accessory helix of CpxI does not insert into synaptobrevin-truncated SNARE complexes in solution. Furthermore, in stark contrast with the cell–cell fusion data, rescue experiments in complexin I-III triple knockout (KO) neurons reveal that superclamp mutations in CpxI lead to slightly stimulatory or no effects on neurotransmitter release, while the poor-clamp K26A mutation impairs release. We also find that the accessory helix of complexin from drosophila melanogaster inhibits spontaneous release more strongly than the accessory helix of mammalian CpxI, which may arise from the more negatively charged nature of the former. Indeed, a mutation that increases the negative charge of the CpxI accessory helix inhibits release and a mutation that decreases the negative charge enhances release. These results suggest a model whereby the location of the negatively charged accessory helix between the synaptic vesicle and plasma membranes causes electrostatic and perhaps steric repulsion with the membranes, thus hindering membrane fusion and neurotransmitter release (Figure 1E).

Results

The accessory helix does not insert into synaptobrevin-truncated SNARE complexes: NMR analysis with 2H,15N-labeled CpxI fragments

To analyze interactions between CpxI and soluble truncated SNARE complexes that might mimic trans SNARE complexes partially assembled between two membranes (e.g., Figure 1C, insertion model), we used 1H-15N two-dimensional transverse relaxation optimized spectroscopy (TROSY) heteronuclear single quantum coherence (HSQC) spectra, which provide a powerful tool to study protein–protein interactions. These NMR spectra can be viewed as protein fingerprints with one cross-peak for each non-proline residue in a 15N-labeled protein, and the positions and line widths of the cross-peaks are very sensitive to perturbations caused by binding to an unlabeled protein (Rizo et al., 2012). Flexible and unstructured regions exhibit sharp cross-peaks with poor dispersion whereas structured regions have broader, well-dispersed cross-peaks, as exemplified by 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of a uniformly 2H,15N-labeled CpxI fragment spanning the accessory and central helices [CpxI(26-83)]. As described previously (Chen et al., 2002), the 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectrum of this fragment exhibits sharp cross-peaks and poor dispersion (Figure 2A) because, although partially helical, the fragment is very flexible. Upon binding to a minimal SNARE complex containing the SNAREs motifs of synaptobrevin, syntaxin-1 and SNAP-25 (below referred to as SNARE complex or SC), the 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectrum of CpxI(26-83) reveals strong broadening and a dramatic increase in dispersion for cross-peaks from the central helix (Figure 2B, red contours) due to stable packing of this helix against the synaptobrevin and syntaxin-1 SNARE motifs. Cross-peaks from the accessory helix, which does not contact the SNARE complex, are also perturbed by binding because the stabilization of the central helix propagates toward the accessory helix, but the perturbations are progressively smaller toward the N-terminus due to fraying of the helix and retention of the intrinsic flexibility characteristic of the isolated CpxI(26-83) fragment (Chen et al., 2002).

Figure 2. NMR analysis of interactions between 2H,15N-labeled CpxI fragments and synaptobrevin-truncated SNARE complexes.

(AC) Expansions of 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of 2H,15N-CpxI(26-83) free (black contours), or bound to non-truncated SNARE complex (red contours), to SCΔ68 (blue contours) or SCΔ62 (green contours). Cross-peaks assignments for the free form are based on those described for full-length CpxI (Pabst et al., 2000) and assignments for CpxI(26-83) bound to non-truncated SNARE complex were described previously (Chen et al., 2002). In (B and C), the minimal contour levels of the different spectra were adjusted to enable visualization of the weakest cross-peaks of interest; hence, cross-peak intensities are not directly comparable. (D and E) Expansions of the regions containing the cross-peaks of A30 (D) or Q38 (E) of the spectra shown in panels (AC). The minimal contour levels of the different spectra were adjusted to make the cross-peak intensities directly comparable. (F) Expansions of 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of 2H,15NCpxI(26-83) bound to SCΔ62 (green contours) or SCΔ60 (orange contours). (G) Expansions of 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of WT 2H,15N-CpxI(26-48) in the absence (black contours) or presence (red contours) of SCΔ60. Because the red and black spectra are practically identical, the black spectrum was plotted at slightly lower levels to facilitate observation of the black crosspeaks behind the red ones. However, the intensities of all the cross-peaks were the same in the black and red spectra within experimental error, as illustrated by the one-dimensional traces shown above and on the right of the two-dimensional contour plots (taken at the chemical shifts indicated by the blue arrows).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02391.006

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Additional analysis of the interaction between 2H,15N-labeled CpxI fragments and synaptobrevin-truncated SNARE complexes.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Chemical shift changes in the CpxI central helix of SNARE complex-bound 2H,15N-CpxI(26- 83) caused by truncation of synaptobrevin to residue 68, normalized by the changes caused by binding of 2H,15N-CpxI(26-83) to the SC. The chemical shift changes were calculated as Δδ = [(ΔδHN)2 + (0.17*ΔδN)2]1/2, where ΔδHN and ΔδN are the differences in HN and N chemical shifts, respectively, between the spectra being compared. For ΔδCpx(SCΔ68-SC), we compared 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of 2H,15N-CpxI(26-83) bound to SC and bound to SCΔ68. For ΔδCpx(SC-free), we compared 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of 2H,15N-CpxI(26-83) free and bound to SC. (B) Plot of ΔδCpx(SCΔ68-SC) vs ΔδCpx(SC-free). (C and D) Ratio between the intensities of cross-peaks of 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of 2H,15N-CpxI(26-83) bound to SCΔ68 (C) or SCΔ62 (D) vs those observed for 2H,15N-CpxI(26-83) bound to SC. To correct for small differences in protein concentrations, the cross-peaks intensities measured for each spectra were normalized with a correction factor derived by averaging the cross-peak intensities of the five C-terminal residues (residues 79-83), which were practically unaffected by the synaptobrevin C-terminal truncations. In all the plots shown in AD, comparisons between chemical shifts or cross-peak intensities were made only for cross-peaks that could be identified in all the relevant spectra based on the assignments available for free and SNARE complexbound Cpx(26-83) (Figure 2A,B) (Chen et al., 2002; Pabst et al., 2000) and the progressive movements caused by truncations in the SNARE complex (see Figure 2C).
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. NMR analysis of interactions between 2H,15N-labeled CpxI superclamp mutant fragments and synaptobrevin-truncated SNARE complexes.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(A) Expansions of 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of D27L, E34F, R37A superclamp mutant (supcl) 2H,15N-CpxI(26-48) in the absence (black contours) or presence (red contours) of SCΔ60. Because the red and black spectra are practically identical, the black spectrum was plotted at slightly lower levels to facilitate observation of the black cross-peaks behind the red ones. However, the intensities of all the cross-peaks were the same in the black and red spectra within experimental error, as illustrated by the one-dimensional traces shown above and on the right of the two-dimensional contour plots (taken at the chemical shifts indicated by the blue arrows). (BE) Expansions of 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of WT 2H,15N-CpxI(26-83) free (black contours) or bound to SCΔ60 (orange contours), and of D27L, E34F, R37A superclamp mutant 2H,15N-CpxI(26-83) free (pink contours) or bound to SCΔ60 (blue contours). The minimal contour levels of the different spectra were adjusted to enable visualization of the weakest crosspeaks of interest; hence, cross-peak intensities are not comparable in general. However, the spectra of panel (D) were plotted at the same contour levels to allow direct comparison of crosspeak intensities for free and SCΔ60-bound 2H,15N-CpxI(26-83)supcl. Selected well-resolved cross-peaks are labeled in the different panels. Note that no cross-peak from CpxI(26-83)supcl overlaps closely with the A30 and Q38 cross-peaks of WT Cpx(26-83) due to the mutations (B and C), and three new well-resolved cross-peaks are observed for CpxI(26-83)supcl (labeled N1–N3 in panels BD). Cross-peaks N1–N3 must belong to the accessory helix where the three mutations were made and can be tentatively assigned to Q38, A30 and A37, respectively, based on their proximity to WT cross-peaks or the observed 15N chemical shift (for A37). The overall changes caused by SCΔ60 binding are similar for WT and superclamp mutant CpxI(26-83) (B, D, E), including the effects on the intensities of the cross-peaks from the accessory helix.

To test whether the CpxI accessory helix can replace in part or in full the C-terminus of the synaptobrevin SNARE motif in the SNARE complex, we acquired 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of 2H,15N-labeled CpxI(26-83) bound to SNARE complexes where the synaptobrevin SNARE motif was truncated at residue 62 or 68 (SCΔ62 or SCΔ68). Comparison with the spectrum obtained in the presence of non-truncated SNARE complex (Figure 2C) showed that the well-resolved cross-peaks from several residues of the central helix (e.g., those of E58, E60, M62, R63 and Q64) moved gradually to the center of the spectrum, toward their positions in free CpxI(26-83), as the truncation was more severe. Moreover, some of these cross-peaks exhibited broadening that most likely arises from chemical exchange. In contrast, cross-peaks from more C-terminal residues of the central helix (e.g., those of I66, D68 and K69) were less affected by the truncations. This behavior is illustrated by the chemical shift changes observed in the CpxI(26-83) central helix when comparing SC-bound vs SCΔ68-bound spectra [ΔδCpx(SCΔ68-SC)], normalized by the changes observed between free and SC-bound Cpx(26-83) [ΔδCpx(SC-free)] (Figure 2—figure supplement 1A). These ratios provide a measure of the destabilization of the central helix caused by the synaptobrevin C-terminal truncation. A plot of ΔδCpx(SCΔ68-SC) vs ΔδCpx(SC-free) (Figure 2—figure supplement 1B) also shows that cross-peaks from the C-terminus of the central helix were less affected by the C-terminal truncation.

The differential effects of the truncations show that the movements toward the center of the spectrum do not result simply from incomplete binding of the CpxI(26-83) fragment to the truncated SNARE complexes, which should be almost quantitatively bound based on affinities measured by ITC (see below). Instead, these data indicate that there is exchange between the normal bound state with a stable central helix and one or more states where the C-terminus of the central helix is stably packed against the SNAREs but the N-terminus of the central helix is flexible. These states become more populated for the Δ62 truncation than for Δ68. The effect of the Δ68 truncation can be attributed to an overall destabilization of the synaptobrevin helix beyond R56, the residue in the central polar layer of the SNARE complex that provides an approximate point of separation for two folding units corresponding to the N- and C-terminal halves of the complex (Sorensen et al., 2006; Gao et al., 2012). This destabilization is manifested in the 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of the truncated SNARE complex described below and is transferred to the CpxI central helix, which is not surprising because the central helix makes extensive contacts with synaptobrevin residues spanning from R47 to A69 in the non-truncated complex (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). The stronger effects on the cross-peaks of the CpxI central helix caused by the Δ62 truncation, compared to Δ68, arise naturally from the removal of key synaptobrevin residues that interact with CpxI, including D64, D65 and D68.

Changes in the cross-peaks corresponding to the CpxI accessory helix caused by the Δ62 and Δ68 synaptobrevin truncations were more difficult to monitor because the cross-peaks are mostly located in the crowded center of the spectrum. The truncations did cause some changes in the center of the spectrum, but the number of cross-peaks and their overall distribution remained similar (Figure 2C). Cross-peaks from the accessory helix that could be identified in all the spectra exhibited some shifts in the different complexes, but in all cases they remained close to the position of the cross-peak corresponding to free CpxI(26-83) (illustrated by the A30 and Q38 cross-peaks in Figure 2D,E). Note that these shifts can be induced by changes in the stability of the helix in the different complexes and that insertion of the accessory helix into the truncated SNARE complexes is expected to induce much more dramatic shifts. Moreover, such insertion should cause strong broadening in the cross-peaks from the accessory helix, but the intensities of these cross-peaks actually increased in the spectrum of Cpx(26-83) bound to SCΔ68 with respect to the SC-bound state, and increased somewhat more in the SCΔ62-bound spectrum (Figure 2D,E, Figure 2—figure supplements 1C,D). These data show that the synaptobrevin C-terminal truncations increase the flexibility of the accessory helix, in correlation with the destabilization of N-terminal part of the central helix, and provide very strong evidence against the notion that the accessory helix of CpxI(26-83) inserts into the grove generated by the truncations.

Since the crystal structure of the CpxI(26-83) D27L, E34F, R37A superclamp mutant bound to a SNARE complex with C-terminally synaptobrevin (Figure 1B) was obtained with a complex containing synaptobrevin truncated at residue 60 (SCΔ60), we also acquired 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of 2H,15N-CpxI(26-83) in the presence of SCΔ60. The spectra was similar to that obtained with SCΔ62, but most cross-peaks from the central helix were broadened beyond detection (Figure 2F). This behavior can be attributed to stronger chemical exchange broadening, which is particularly well manifested for the cross-peaks of K73 and K75 (which are adjacent to the central helix). The well-resolved cross-peaks from the accessory helix of CpxI(26-83) bound to SCΔ60 (e.g., those of A30 and Q38) had similar intensities as those observed upon binding to SCΔ62, showing that the accessory helix does not insert into the groove left by the Δ60 truncation. Because the zigzag array observed in the crystal structure of the CpxI(26-83) superclamp/SCΔ60 complex suggested that the accessory helix should be able to bind by itself to SCΔ60, without the central helix, we also acquired 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of a WT 2H,15N-CpxI fragment spanning the accessory helix [CpxI(26-48)]. SCΔ60 did not cause substantial changes in the spectra of CpxI(26-48) (Figure 2G). Because of the very high sensitivity of these spectra to binding to protein complexes such as SCΔ60, particularly for flexible peptides such as CpxI(26-48), even a small percentage of binding should be reflected in some cross-peak broadening. Hence, these results clearly show that CpxI(26-48) does not bind to the synaptobrevin Δ60 truncated SNARE complex in solution under the conditions of our experiments.

We also acquired parallel 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of 2H,15N-labeled fragments of the CpxI(26-83) D27L, E34F, R37A superclamp mutant (supcl) in the presence and absence of SCΔ60. The data acquired with 2H,15N-CpxI(26-48)supcl showed no binding to SCΔ60 (Figure 2—figure supplement 2A), as observed for WT CpxI(26-48). The spectrum obtained for 2H,15N-CpxI(26-83)supcl bound to SCΔ60 was similar to that obtained for WT CpxI(26-83), with a few differences that arise from the mutations (Figure 2—figure supplement 2B) and are also observed in the spectra obtained for the free CpxI fragments (Figure 2—figure supplement 2C). Moreover, the overall effects of SCΔ60 binding to CpxI(26-83)suplc are similar to those observed for WT CpxI(26-83) (Figure 2—figure supplement 2B,D,E) and, as observed for the WT protein, the cross-peaks from the accessory helix do not exhibit dramatic shifts and/or broadening as would be expected for insertion into the groove left by the synaptobrevin truncation. Therefore, we were unable to detect an interaction between the accessory helix of CpxI(26-83)supcl and SCΔ60 under the conditions of our NMR experiments, although we cannot rule out the possibility that there is a weak interaction in solution that becomes stabilized by crystallization.

The accessory helix does not insert into synaptobrevin-truncated SNARE complexes: NMR analysis with 2H,15N-labeled SNARE complexes

To further test the insertion model, we also acquired 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of truncated SNARE complexes that were 2H,15N-labeled at the C-terminal SNARE motif of SNAP-25 (SNC) or at the syntaxin-1 SNARE motif (Syx), since these SNARE motifs were prediced to contact the CpxI accessory helix in synaptobrevin-truncated SNARE complexes. We first compared spectra of 2H,15N-SNC complexes that were non-truncated or truncated at residues 62 or 76 of synaptobrevin (2H,15N-SNC-SCΔ62 or 2H,15N-SNC-SCΔ76) (Figure 3A,B), and found that progressive truncation led to increased appearance of sharp cross-peaks in the center of the spectrum and disappearance of cross-peaks from the SNC C-terminal residues in well-resolved regions, or shifts for residues close to the polar layer (Q174 for SNC). These results show that the synaptobrevin truncations lead to flexibility in the C-terminal half of SNC. For the Δ62 truncation, stable structure appears to remain only up to residue 180.

Figure 3. NMR analysis of interactions between 2H,15N-labeled synaptobrevin-truncated SNARE complexes and CpxI fragments.

(A and B) Expansions of 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of the non-truncated 2H,15N-SNC-SC (black contours), 2H,15N-SNC-SCΔ76 (green contours) and 2H,15N-SNC-SCΔ62 (red contours). (C and D) Expansions of 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of 2H,15N-SNC-SCΔ62 alone (red contours) or in the presence of CpxI(26-83) (light blue; C) or CpxI(26-47) (dark blue; D). (E and F) Expansions of 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of 2H,15N-Syx-SC (black contours) and of 2H,15N-Syx-SCΔ62 in the absence (red contours) or presence (light blue contours) of CpxI(26-83). Cross-peak assignments are based on those described for the non-truncated SNARE complex (Chen et al., 2002; Chen et al., 2005).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02391.009

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Additional NMR analyses of interactions between 2H,15N-labeled synaptobrevin-truncated SNARE complexes and CpxI fragments.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

Expansions of 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of 2H,15N-SNCΔ62 (A and B) or 2H,15N-SNCΔ60 (C and D) in the absence (red contours) or presence (blue contours) of CpxI(26-83) D27L, E34F, R37A superclamp mutant (supcl) (A and D), dmCpx(28-88) (B), or CpxI(26-83) (C).

CpxI(26-83) caused only small shifts in some of the well-resolved cross-peaks of the 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectrum of 2H,15N-SNC-SCΔ62 (Figure 3C), in correlation with the small perturbations observed for the non-truncated SNARE complex because CpxI makes little contact with SNAP-25 (Chen et al., 2002). Moreover, these small shifts can arise from stabilization of the synaptobrevin and syntaxin-1 helix in the truncated SNARE complex upon binding to CpxI(26-83). Importantly, CpxI(26-83) binding induced practically no changes in the cross-peaks corresponding to the flexible C-terminus of SNC in the Δ62 SNARE complex (Figure 3C), showing again that the CpxI accessory helix does not interact with the truncated SNARE complex. We also analyzed perturbations caused by the Cpx(26-83)supcl mutant or a fragment spanning the accessory and central helices of CpxI from Drosophila Melanogaster [dmCpx(28-88)], which inhibits release more strongly than mammalian CpxI (Huntwork and Littleton, 2007; Xue et al., 2009), but the results (Figure 3—figure supplement 1A,B) were analogous to those obtained with WT CpxI(26-83). Since the crystal structure leading to the zig-zag model (Figure 1B) was obtained with synaptobrevin truncated at residue 60, we acquired additional 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra with 2H,15N-SNCΔ60, but similar data were obtained again on addition of WT CpxI(26-83) or Cpx(26-83)supcl mutant (Figure 3—figure supplement 1C,D). All these results suggest that the accessory helix of WT CpxI(26-83), Cpx(26-83)supcl and dmCpx(28-88) do not insert into the SNARE complexes containing C-terminally truncated synaptobrevin. Finally, addition of a CpxI fragment spanning only the accessory helix [CpxI(26-48)] caused practically no changes on the 1H-15N HSQC spectrum of 2H,15N-SNC-SCΔ62 (Figure 3D), confirming that the accessory helix does not bind by itself to the truncated SNARE complex or binds with extremely weak affinity.

We also performed parallel experiments with truncated SNARE complex 2H,15N-labeled at the syntaxin-1 SNARE motif (2H,15N-Syx-SCΔ62). Comparison of the 1H-15N TROSY HSQC spectra of this complex and the non-truncated complex (Figure 3E) revealed that the Δ62 truncation led to disappearance of most of the well-resolved cross-peaks from residues beyond the residue in the polar layer (Q226 for Syx) and an increase in sharp cross-peaks in the center of the spectrum. These results correlated with those obtained with 2H,15N-SNC-SCΔ62 and indicate that the C-terminal half of the syntaxin-1 SNARE motif becomes flexible due to the Δ62 synaptobrevin truncation, although the smaller number of cross-peaks in the middle suggests that there may be broadening due to exchange between structured and flexible conformations. CpxI(26-83) caused multiple changes in the 1H-15N TROSY HSQC spectrum of the 2H,15N-Syx-SCΔ62 complex (Figure 3F) that correlate with those observed for the non-truncated SNARE complex (Chen et al., 2002). Only limited changes were observed for the sharp cross-peaks in the middle of the spectrum, which can arise from partial stabilization of the syntaxin-1 helix. Although these data are less conclusive than those obtained with the 2H,15N-SNC-SCΔ62 complex, it is clear that CpxI(26-83) binding did not yield new well-dispersed cross-peaks that might correspond to structured syntaxin-1 C-terminal residues interacting with the CpxI(26-83) accessory helix. Hence, these data further support the conclusion that the accessory helix does not interact with C-terminally synaptobrevin truncated SNARE complex.

The accessory helix does not insert into synaptobrevin-truncated SNARE complexes: analysis by ITC

The primary evidence reported to support the notion that the accessory helix of WT CpxI(26-83) inserts into the synaptobrevin Δ60 truncated SNARE complex, as observed by crystallography for the CpxI(26-83) D27L, E34F, R37A superclamp mutant, was obtained in competition assays monitored by ITC (Kummel et al., 2011). In these assays, 1.5 equivalents of CpxI lacking the accessory helix [CpxI(47-134)] were used to block the central helix binding site of SCΔ60, and the heat observed on addition of CpxI(26-83) was attributed to binding of the accessory helix of CpxI(26-83) to SCΔ60. This interpretation assumed that 1.5 equivalents for CpxI(47-134) were sufficient to quantitatively saturate SCΔ60, but the validity of this assumption is unclear because removal of multiple synaptobrevin residues that interact with CpxI in the non-truncated SNARE complex is expected to considerably decrease the affinity of SCΔ60 for CpxI.

To address this issue, we measured the affinity of CpxI(47-134) for SCΔ60 and the non-truncated SNARE by ITC. For the latter (Figure 4A), we measured a Kd of 339 ± 9 nM (ΔH = −32.6 kcal/mol; N = 0.95), which is higher than that we obtained for CpxI(26-83) [Kd = 25.3 nM; Xu et al., 2013] and may arise because of favorable long-range electrostatic interactions between the accessory helix and the SNARE complex. Importantly, binding of Cpx(47-134) to SCΔ60 was even weaker, with a Kd of 2.39 ± 0.19 μM (ΔH = −19.5 kcal/mol; N = 0.92) (Figure 4B). This decreased affinity implies that binding of CpxI(47-134) to SCΔ60 is not saturated upon addition of 1.5 equivalents of CpxI(47-134) (arrow in Figure 4B). Hence, the heat observed upon addition of CpxI(26-83) to SCΔ60 blocked with CpxI(27-134) arises from completion of the titration of the central helix binding site, rather than from interactions involving the accessory helix. We confirmed this conclusion by titrating CpxI(47-134) itself on a sample containing SCΔ60 and 1.5 equivalents of CpxI(47-134) (Figure 4C), which logically yielded data similar to those observed in the direct titration experiment of Figure 4B beyond 1.5 equivalents. Moreover, adding CpxI(26-83) to SCΔ60 prebound to 1.5 equivalents of CpxI(47-134) yielded very similar results (Figure 4D), which in turn were also comparable to the data described in Kummel et al. (2011). To test for binding of the accessory helix under conditions where the central helix binding site was more saturated, we performed experiments with SCΔ60 prebound to 3.0 equivalents of CpxI(47-134) (93% binding based on the Kd described above). As expected, addition of Cpx(26-83) yielded only a small amount of heat that again is the natural extension of the direct titration of the central helix binding site (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Hence, no binding of the accessory helix of Cpx(26-83) to SCΔ60 is detected in these experiments.

Figure 4. ITC analysis of binding of CpxI fragments to SNARE complexes.

(A and B) Direct titrations of non-truncated SNARE complex (SC; A) or SCΔ60 (B) with CpxI(47-134). (CE) Competition assays where samples containing SCΔ60 and 1.5 equivalents of CpxI(47-134) were titrated with CpxI(47-134) (C), WT CpxI(26-83) (D) or CpxI(26-83) D27L, E34F, R37A superclamp mutant (supcl) (E). The arrow in panel (B) shows the point of the direct titration where 1.5 equivalents of Cpx(47-134) had been added, and the dashed line shows the heat measured at that point of the titration. The same heat (within experimental error) was measured at the start of the competition experiments of panels (CE). Thus, the heat measurements in the competition assays correspond to the completion of the titration (i.e., the tail of the direct titration of panel B) because 1.5 equivalents of CpxI(47-134) were not sufficient to saturate SCΔ60.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02391.011

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Additional ITC analysis of binding of CpxI fragments to SNARE complexes.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(A) Competition assay where a sample containing SCΔ60 and 3.0 equivalents of CpxI(47-134) was titrated with WT CpxI(26-83). (B) Composite diagram where the ITC data shown in panel (A) (shown here as red circles) was shifted by 3.0 molar ratio units in the x axis and plotted together with ITC data obtained in a direct titration of SCΔ60 with CpxI(47-134), shown by black circles (analogous to that shown in Figure 4B). Note that the data in red constitute a natural continuation of the direct titration of the central helix binding site and hence there is not additional detectable heat caused by binding of the accessory helix of Cpx(26-83) to the groove within SCΔ60.

The equations describing competition data are more complicated than those describing a single-site binding model, but we used this simplified model to fit our ITC data to allow comparison with the results described in Kummel et al. (2011). We obtained apparent Kd = 3.73 ± 0.57 μM and apparent ΔH = −5.5 kcal/mol for competition with CpxI(47-134) (Figure 4C), and apparent Kd = 5.41 ± 1.48 μM and apparent ΔH = −5.0 kcal/mol for competition with CpxI(26-83) (Figure 4D), which are clearly similar. Kummel et al. (2011) reported Kd = 16 μM and ΔH = −5.4 kcal/mol for competition with CpxI(26-83). Although the Kd is somewhat different, we consider that our data do reproduce the results of Kummel et al. (2011) within experimental error, considering the approximation involved in the single-site model. However, our results show that the interpretation of the competition ITC assays needs to be revised and that these assays are unable to detect an interaction between the accessory helix of WT CpxI(26-83) and SCΔ60, in agreement with our NMR results.

We also performed competition assays with CpxI(26-83) D27L, E34F, R37A superclamp mutant and obtained very similar results to those observed with WT CpxI(26-83) (Figure 4E; apparent Kd = 4.03 ± 0.89 μM; apparent ΔH = −5.1 kcal/mol). These results contrast with those described previously (Kummel et al., 2011) but agree with our NMR data and further suggest that even the accessory helix of the CpxI(26-83)supcl mutant does not interact with SNARE complexes containing C-terminally truncated synaptobrevin in solution or, if there is any interaction, it is weak and cannot be detected in our NMR and ITC experiments.

Analysis of interactions between CpxI and syntaxin-1-truncated SNARE complexes

We also investigated whether the complexin accessory helix might inhibit neurotransmitter release by replacing the syntaxin-1 SNARE motif in partially assembled SNARE complexes. For this purpose, we first acquired 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of 2H,15N-labeled synaptobrevin SNARE motif (2H,15N-Syb) free and incorporated into non-truncated SNARE complex (2H,15N-Syb-SC) or SNARE complex with syntaxin-1 truncated at residue 236 (2H,15N-Syb-SCΔ236). Comparison of the spectra obtained for free 2H,15N-Syb and 2H,15N-Syb-SC showed again the dramatic spectral changes that occur when a flexible sequence such as 2H,15N-Syb forms a stable complex (Figure 5A; cross-peak assignments for 2H,15N-Syb based on those of Syb[1-96] [Hazzard et al., 1999] are shown in Figure 5—figure supplement 1A). The truncation of the syntaxin-1 C-terminus in 2H,15N-Syb-SCΔ236 led to disappearance of most well-dispersed cross-peaks from the C-terminal half of the synaptobrevin SNARE motif and appearance of new, sharp cross-peaks in the middle of the spectrum (Figure 5B). We obtained assignments for some of these cross-peaks using triple resonance experiments as described (Chen et al., 2002) and found that they generally were at similar positions to those observed for free 2H,15N-Syb (e.g., for A67, A69, A72, A74, W89 and K91; compare Figure 5B, orange contours, with Figure 5—figure supplement 1A). These results suggest that the C-terminal half of the synaptobrevin SNARE motif is flexible in 2H,15N-Syb-SCΔ236 as a result of the syntaxin-1 truncation. We also acquired 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra for the same truncated complex but with the SNAP-25 C-terminal motif 2H,15N-labeled (2H,15N-SNC-SCΔ236) and again found disappearance of the well-dispersed cross-peaks from the C-terminal half of SNC with concomitant appearance of sharp cross-peaks in the middle of the spectrum (Figure 5—figure supplement 1B). These changes are similar to those caused by the synaptobrevin truncation in 2H,15N-SNC-SCΔ62 (Figure 3B) and show that the C-terminus of SNC also becomes flexible upon truncation of syntaxin-1.

Figure 5. NMR analysis of interactions between 2H,15N-labeled syntaxin-1 truncated SNARE complexes and CpxI fragments.

(A and B) Expansions of 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of free 2H,15N-labeled synaptobrevin SNARE motif (2H,15N-Syb, green contours), the non-truncated 2H,15N-Syb-SC (black contours) and 2H,15N-Syb-SCΔ236 (orange contours). (C) Expansions of 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of 2H,15N-Syb-SC in the absence (black contours) and presence (red contours) of CpxI(26-83). (D) Expansions of 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of 2H,15N-Syb-SCΔ236 in the absence (orange contours) and presence (light blue contours) of CpxI(26-83). (E) Superposition of expansions of 1H-15N TROSY HSQC spectra of 2H,15N-Syb-SC (red contours) and 2H,15N-Syb-SCΔ236 (light blue contours) bound to CpxI(26-83). (F) Expansions of 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of 2H,15N-Syb-SCΔ236 in the absence (orange contours) and presence (dark blue contours) of CpxI(26-47). Cross-peaks assignments for 2H,15N-Syb-SC free and bound to CpxI(26-83) were described previously (Chen et al., 2002). Cross-peaks assignments for 2H,15N-Syb-SCΔ236 that were not immediately clear from those obtained for 2H,15N-Syb-SC were obtained using triple resonance experiments.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02391.013

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Additional NMR analyses of interactions between 2H,15N-labeled syntaxin-1 truncated SNARE complexes and CpxI fragments.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(A) Expansion of a 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectrum of free 2H,15N-labeled synaptobrevin SNARE motif (2H,15N-Syb). Cross-peak assignments are based on those obtained for synaptobrevin(1-96) (Hazzard et al., 1999). (B) 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of the nontruncated 2H,15N-SNC-SC (black contours) and 2H,15N-SNC-SCΔ236 (orange contours). (C) Expansions of 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of 2H,15N-SNC-SCΔ236 in the absence (orange contours) and presence (light blue contours) of CpxI(26-83). Cross-peaks assignments for 2H,15N-Syb-SC free were described previously (Chen et al., 2005).

We next examined the changes induced by CpxI(26-83) on the 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of these complexes. For non-truncated complex (2H,15N-Syb-SC), CpxI(26-83) caused multiple cross-peak shifts, particularly for synaptobrevin residues that contact the CpxI central helix (Figure 5C), as observed previously (Chen et al., 2002). Binding to CpxI(26-83) induced similar shifts for those residues in 2H,15N-Syb-SCΔ236, but in addition caused disappearance of many sharp cross-peaks in the middle of the spectrum that correspond to the flexible synaptobrevin C-terminal half (Figure 5D). As a result, the 1H-15N TROSY HSQC spectrum of 2H,15N-Syb-SCΔ236 bound to CpxI(26-83) is similar to that obtained for the non-truncated SNARE complex except that most cross-peaks corresponding to the synaptobrevin C-terminal half disappeared (Figure 5E). Such disappearance most likely arises from chemical exchange between the flexible conformations characteristic of the synaptobrevin C-terminal half in 2H,15N-Syb-SCΔ236 and a more defined structure(s) induced upon CpxI(26-83) binding. These results contrast with those obtained upon addition of CpxI(26-83) to 2H,15N-SNC-SCΔ236 (Figure 5—figure supplement 1C), which revealed only small cross-peak shifts for a few well-dispersed cross-peaks and no marked changes for the sharp cross-peaks in the middle of the spectrum, as observed for the synaptobrevin-truncated SNARE complex 2H,15N-SNC-SCΔ62 (Figure 3C). We also analyzed the effects of CpxI(26-47) on the 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectrum of 2H,15N-SNC-SCΔ236 but observed practically no changes (Figure 5F), showing that the accessory helix by itself does not bind to the syntaxin-1 truncated SNARE complex.

While the results obtained with 2H,15N-SNC-SCΔ236 show that the accessory helix of CpxI(26-83) does not interact with the SNAP-25 C-terminus in the syntaxin-1 truncated SNARE complex and hence does not insert into the complex, the data acquired with 2H,15N-Syb-SCΔ236 suggested that the accessory helix might interact with the synaptobrevin C-terminal half, which could provide an alternative mechanism to hinder SNARE complex assembly and thus inhibit neurotransmitter release (Figure 1D). To test this possibility, we analyzed the effects of SCΔ236 and other SNARE complexes with stronger truncations in the syntaxin-1 C-terminus (SCΔ232 and SCΔ228) on the 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectrum of 2H,15N-labeled CpxI(26-83). The spectra obtained upon binding to SC or to SCΔ236 revealed only small shifts in a few well-resolved cross-peaks and practically no perturbations of the sharp cross-peaks in the center of the spectrum corresponding to the accessory helix (Figure 6A). When we included the spectra obtained in the presence of SCΔ232 and SCΔ228 in the comparison, it became clear that a few well-resolved cross-peaks (e.g., those of E60, M62, R63, and Q64) shift gradually to the center of the spectrum, toward their positions in free 2H,15N-CpxI(26-83), as the truncation in syntaxin-1 is more severe (Figure 6B). This result is similar to that caused by truncations in the synaptobrevin C-terminus (Figure 2C) and can thus be attributed to increasing destabilization of the CpxI central helix as more residues are deleted in syntaxin-1 (the effects are smaller because most of the residues deleted in syntaxin-1 do not contact CpxI[26-83]; Figure 1—figure supplement 1).

Figure 6. NMR analysis of interactions between 2H,15N-labeled CpxI fragments and syntaxin-1-truncated SNARE complexes.

Figure 6.

(A and B) Expansions of 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of 2H,15N-CpxI(26-83) bound to nontruncated SNARE complex (SC; red contours), to SCΔ236 (blue contours), SCΔ232 (green contours) or SCΔ228 (purple contours). Cross-peaks assignments for CpxI(26-83) bound to nontruncated SNARE complex were described previously (Chen et al., 2002). (C and D) Expansions of the regions containing the cross-peaks of Q38 (C) or A30 (D) of the spectra shown in panels (A and B).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02391.015

As in the case of the synaptobrevin truncations (Figure 2C), the syntaxin-1 truncations in the SNARE complex also caused changes in the center of the 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectrum of the bound 2H,15N-CpxI(26-83), but the number of cross-peaks and their overall distribution remained similar (Figure 6B) and the well-resolved cross-peaks corresponding to A30 and Q38 exhibited small gradual shifts from their positions upon binding to the non-truncated SNARE complex to their free positions as syntaxin-1 was increasingly truncated (Figure 6C,D). These results strongly suggest that the shifts in the CpxI(26-83) accessory helix do not arise from interactions with the C-terminal half of the synaptobrevin SNARE motif but rather because the destabilization of the central helix caused by the truncations in syntaxin-1 is transferred into destabilization of the accessory helix. Note also that no substantial broadening of the cross-peaks from the accessory helix was observed, in contrast to the broadening beyond disappearance of cross-peaks from the synaptobrevin C-terminal half in 2H,15N-Syb-SCΔ236 bound to CpxI(26-83) (Figure 5D,E). It is likely that such disappearance arises from chemical exchange between the flexible conformations of the synaptobrevin C-terminal half in free 2H,15N-Syb-SCΔ236 and formation of a defined helical structure that does not contact CpxI(26-83) but is nucleated by stabilization of the middle of synaptobrevin upon binding of the CpxI(26-83) central helix. Regardless of the validity of this explanation, it is clear from the behavior of the cross-peaks of the accessory helix that this helix does not interact with the synaptobrevin SNARE motif of the syntaxin-1 truncated SNARE complexes, arguing against the model of Figure 1D.

Complexin I superclamp mutations do not inhibit neurotransmitter release

In parallel with our structural studies, we tested the complexin insertion and zigzag models emerging from cell–cell fusion assays (Giraudo et al., 2006) and X-ray crystallography (Kummel et al., 2011) by examining the effects on neurotransmitter release of some of the mutations that were reported to alter the inhibitory activity of CpxI in cell–cell fusion. In particular, we analyzed the effects of the D27L, E34F, R37A superclamp mutation that strongly inhibited cell–cell fusion, and of a 'poor-clamp' mutation that decreased the clamping efficiency of CpxI (K26A) (Figure 7A). Note that rescue studies on complexin knockdown cortical neurons found that the CpxI D27L, E34F, R37A superclamp mutation did not alter evoked neurotransmitter release and appeared to induce a modest decrease in spontaneous release, but it was unclear whether the decrease was significant, in part because of the small nature of the effect and in part because there was no direct comparison with a rescue using WT CpxI (Yang et al., 2010).

Figure 7. Rescue of the complexin KO phenotype with CpxI-superclamp, but not with a clamping deficient CpxI.

(A) Overview of the introduced mutations in CpxI. (BC) Representative traces and summary data of evoked EPSC (B) and synaptic responses to hypertonic sucrose solution (RRP) (C) of T-KO, K26A, D27L E34F R37A and WT-CplxI expressing hippocampal neurons. (D) Bar graph of the calculated vesicular release probability Pvr. (EF) Analysis of short-term plasticity behavior: Example traces of a train of 5 APs at 50 Hz of T-KO, K26A, D27L E34F R37A and WT-CpxI expressing neurons from which the paired pulse ratio was calculated (E) and amplitudes of 50 EPSCs evoked at 10 Hz which were normalized to the first EPSCs and plotted over stimulus number (F). (G) Spontaneous transmitter release: Representative traces of T-KO, K26A, D27L E34F R37A and WT-CpxI expressing neurons and summary data of mEPSC frequency and mEPSC amplitude. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM, *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001. The numbers of neurons analyzed are shown within the bars. Vertical bars in the traces (B and E) represent 2-ms somatic depolarizations; depolarization artifact and action potentials were blanked. Time of sucrose application is indicated as horizontal line (C).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02391.016

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. Expression of Cpx variants in hippocampal CPXI-III triple KO neurons by lentiviral transduction.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

(A) Immunocytochemical detection of cytosolic complexin-FLAG tagged protein variants within presynaptic compartments illustrated by VGlut1 colocalisation. Arrowheads point towards examples of colocalisation. Scale bar: 5 μm. (B) Protein expression of different Cpx variants used in this study is detected by western blotting. The amounts of viruses to reach similar expression levels in individual virus preparations are indicated. TubulinIII served as loading control.

In our studies we performed rescue experiments on hippocampal glutamatergic neurons from complexin I-III triple KO mice. Expression was mediated via lentiviral transduction, and WT and mutant CpxI levels were monitored by immunocytochemistry and western blot analysis (Figure 7—figure supplement 1). CpxI-III-deficient neurons exhibit reduced vesicle released probability (Pvr), increased paired-pulse ratio (PPR), a facilitatory phenotype at high frequency stimulation and reduced spontaneous release frequency, all of which can be rescued with lenti-viral overexpression of WT CpxI (Xue et al., 2008b, 2009). Hence, these synaptic parameters served us as readouts for the rescue behavior of different mutant Cplx variants.

The D27L, E34F, R37A superclamp mutation that introduces hydrophobic residues in the CpxI accessory helix did not change the ability to rescue the complexin I-III KO phenotype. On the contrary the amplitudes of action potential-evoked excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs) tended to be larger that those observed upon rescue with WT CpxI (Figure 7B). As the size of the readily releasable pool (RRP) measured by hypertonic solution (Rosenmund and Stevens, 1996) was unchanged, the calculated Pvr was also slightly increased in the rescue with the superclamp CpxI mutants (Figure 7C,D). The facilitatory synaptic short-term plasticity behavior in complexin I-III triple KO neurons, measured through five EPSCs at 50 Hz and also reflected by calculation of the PPR (EPSC2/EPSC1), could be reversed to a more depressing phenotype in CpxI D27L, E34F, R37A expressing KO neurons compared to the rescue with WT CpxI (Figure 7E). Similarly, depression of EPSC amplitudes during a train of 50 evoked action potentials (AP) at 10 Hz was slightly stronger, and the spontaneous release of vesicles tended to be higher for the super-clamp mutant than for the WT rescue neurons (Figure 7F,G). Conversely, the 'non-clamping' K26A mutation impaired full rescue of the KO phenotype, as the EPSC amplitude and Pvr were reduced, and the PPR was increased (Figure 7B–E). The short-term plasticity experiment applying 50 AP at 10 Hz also revealed weak rescue activity (Figure 7E,F), but spontaneous release was not different from the WT rescue (Figure 7G).

Overall, these results are in contrast with key predictions from the insertion and zigzag models and do not correlate with the data obtained with cell–cell fusion assays (Giraudo et al., 2009), revealing an impairment of evoked release by the K26A mutation that decreases clamping activity in the cell–cell fusion assay and small effects with a tendency to increase evoked release for the superclamp mutant. Hence, our functional data did not support the hypothesis that the inhibitory activity of the accessory helix arises from insertion into the SNARE complex to replace part of the synaptobrevin SNARE motif, in correlation with our NMR and ITC results.

Inhibition of spontaneous release by the accessory helix of dmCpx

In search of an alternative model that could explain the inhibitory function of the complexin accessory helix, we turned our attention to complexin from Drosophila melanogaster (dmCpx) because a dramatic increase in spontaneous release is observed in its absence (Huntwork and Littleton, 2007) and experiments with chimeras of murine CpxI and dmCpx suggested that the accessory helix of dmCpx inhibits release more strongly than that of murine CpxI in mouse hippocampal neurons (Xue et al., 2009). To verify this conclusion, we generated a chimeric complexin with most of the sequence corresponding to murine CpxI but with the accessory helix of dmCpx (dmAcc-CpxI; Figure 8A), and analyzed its influence on neurotransmitter release in complexin triple KO neurons. No significant differences in evoked EPSC amplitudes, RRP charge and Pvr were observed between neurons expressing the dmAcc-CpxI chimera or WT CpxI (Figure 8B–D). The PPR analyzed from trains of five EPSCs at 50 Hz also showed no difference between chimeric and WT rescue (Figure 8E). At longer stimulations, the short-term plasticity characteristics at 10 Hz revealed a slight, but not significant, decrease in depression when the chimeric dmAcc-CpxI was expressed (Figure 8F). Interestingly however, we did observe a significant, ca. 30% reduction in the frequency of spontaneous release in KO neurons expressing dmAcc-CpxI compared to KO neurons expressing WT CpxI, while the amplitudes of the miniature EPSCs (mEPSCs) were not different (Figure 8G). These results indicate that the accessory alpha helix of dmCpx contains some feature(s) that renders it more inhibitory than the accessory helix of mammalian CpxI.

Figure 8. Inhibition of spontaneous release by the accessory alpha helix of dmCpx.

Figure 8.

(A) Overview of the replacement of the accessory helix of CpxI with the accessory helix sequence of drosophila Cpx. (B) Representative traces and summary data of evoked EPSC (B) and synaptic responses to hypertonic sucrose solution (RRP) (C) of T-KO, dmAcc-CpxI and WT-CpxI expressing hippocampal neurons. (D) Bar graph of the calculated Pvr. (EF) Analysis of short-term plasticity behavior: Example traces of a train of 5 APs at 50 Hz of T-KO, dmAcc-CpxI and WT CpxI expressing neurons from which the paired pulse ratio was calculated (E) and amplitudes of 50 EPSCs evoked at 10 Hz which were normalized to the first EPSCs and plotted over stimulus number (F). (G) Spontaneous transmitter release: Representative traces of T-KO, dmAcc-CpxI and WT-CpxI expressing neurons and summary data of mEPSC frequency and mEPSC amplitude. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM, *p<0.05; ***p<0.001. The numbers of neurons analyzed are shown within the bars.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02391.018

Changing the charge of the accessory alpha helix alters neurotransmitter release

The accessory helix of dmCpx contains only one hydrophobic residue, like mammalian CpxI (Figure 8A); therefore, hydrophobicity does not explain the enhanced inhibitory activity of the dmCpx accessory helix. Sequence comparisons suggest that the accessory helix of dmCpx is longer and more negatively charged than that of mammalian CpxI. These observations and the crystal structure of the CpxI(26-83)/SNARE complex (Figure 1A) lead naturally to a simple model whereby the complexin accessory helix inhibits release because it is oriented toward the area where the two membranes need to be brought together for fusion and this action is hindered by electrostatic repulsion between the accessory helix and the two membranes (Figure 1E).

To test this model, we made two mutants of mammalian CpxI where we changed the charge of the accessory helix, one where we added five negative charges (CpxI-5E) and another where we replaced three negatively charged with positive charges (CpxI-3R) (Figure 9A). Lenti-viral expression of the positively charged CpxI-3R and WT CpxI in complexin I-III triple KO neurons did not reveal significant differences in EPSC amplitudes, but rescue with the negatively charged CpxI-5E mutant yielded slightly reduced EPSC amplitudes (Figure 9B). The RRP sizes in the four different groups analyzed were not significantly different (Figure 9C). Calculation of the Pvr showed similar release probabilities, with a tendency in the CpxI-3R mutant towards higher release probability, and a tendency in the CpxI-5E mutant towards lower probability (Figure 9D). Consistent with the Pvr results, trains of five EPSCs at 50 Hz showed that the facilitatory synaptic short-term plasticity characteristic of the complexin I-III triple KO could be rescued in the CpxI-3R expressing neurons to the same level as the WT CpxI expressing neurons (Figure 9E). However, expressing the negatively charged CpxI-5E protein did not rescue this KO phenotype to the same extent, and the PPR in CpxI-5E expressing neurons was significantly increased compared to WT CpxI expressing neurons (Figure 9E). These rescue behaviors of the different charged versions of CpxI were also observed when analyzing the short-term plasticity characteristics from trains of 50 EPSCs at 10 Hz. In this case, CpxI-3R expressing neurons showed a slight increase in depression whereas CpxI-5E expressing KO neurons did not depress to the same extent as WT CpxI expressing neurons (Figure 9F). Importantly, complexin I-III KO neurons expressing the positively charged CpxI-3R exhibited a considerable increase in mEPSC frequency, whereas expression of the negatively charged CpxI-5E resulted in a significant decrease in mEPSC frequency compared to the WT CpxI expressing KO neurons (Figure 9G). The amplitudes of these events were not different (Figure 9G).

Figure 9. Inhibitory effect of the accessory alpha helix is charge dependent.

Figure 9.

(A) Overview of the CpxI accessory alpha helix sequence and the introduced mutations resulting in more positively charged (CpxI-3R) or more negatively charged (CpxI-5E) accessory alpha helix. (BC) Representative traces and summary data of evoked EPSC (B) and synaptic responses to hypertonic sucrose solution (RRP) (C) of T-KO, CpxI-3R, CpxI-5E and WT-CpxI expressing hippocampal neurons. (D) Bar graph of the calculated Pvr. (EF) Analysis of short-term plasticity behavior: example traces of a train of 5 APs at 50 Hz of T-KO, CpxI-3R, CpxI-5E and WT-CpxI expressing neurons from which the paired pulse ratio was calculated (E) and amplitudes of 50 EPSCs evoked at 10 Hz which were normalized to the first EPSCs and plotted over stimulus number (F). (G) Spontaneous transmitter release: Representative traces of T-KO, CpxI-3R, CpxI-5E and WT-CpxI expressing neurons and summary data of mEPSC frequency and mEPSC amplitude. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM, *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001. The numbers of neurons analyzed are shown within the bars.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02391.019

Collectively, these results indicate that the accessory alpha helix of CpxI exerts its inhibitory function at least in part through the presence of negatively charged residues and that changes towards a more positively or more negatively charged nature result in decrease or increase of the inhibitory effect on spontaneous neurotransmitter release, respectively.

Influence of the complexin accessory helix on the fusogenicity of synaptic vesicles

Absence of complexins does not alter the RRP as defined from the release induced by 500 mM hypertonic sucrose in autaptic hippocampal neurons (e.g., Figures 7B, 8C and 9B) but does lead to a decrease in release caused by 250 mM hypertonice sucrose (Xue et al., 2010), showing that complexins increase the propensity of synaptic vesicles to fuse. To examine whether changing charged residues in the accessory helix of CpxI affects fusogenicity, we compared the responses to 250 mM sucrose in complexin I-III KO neurons expressing WT CpxI, CpxI-3R or CpxI-5E. Compared to WT CpxI expressing neurons, expression of the less negatively charged mutant, CpxI-3R, led to clear increases in the fraction of the RRP released by 250 mM sucrose and the peak release rate, as well as a decrease in the response onset latency (Figure 10A–C). Conversely, introduction of negatively charged residues in the CpxI-5E mutant led to responses to 250 mM sucrose that resembled those of KO neurons, with reduced fractions of RRP release and peak release rates, and increased response onset latencies (Figure 10A–C). These data exhibit some correlation with the Pvr (Figure 10C) but correlate best with the results from spontaneous release (Figures 9G, 10C), suggesting that the charge of the accessory helix has a larger influence on the ability of synaptic vesicles to fuse in the absence of Ca2+ than upon Ca2+ influx. It seems likely that the stimulatory effects of CpxI and synaptotagmin-1 in evoked release override at least to some extent the inhibition by the CpxI accessory helix.

Figure 10. Fusogenicity of synaptic vesicles is influenced by the charge of the accessory alpha helix of CpxI.

Figure 10.

(A) Average traces of synaptic responses induced by 250 mM sucrose solution (T-KO n = 33, CpxI-3R n = 49, CpxI-5E n = 39, WT CpxI n = 50). (B) Summary data of 250 mM sucrose solution-induced response onset latency. The numbers of neurons analyzed are shown within the bars. (C) Correlation plot of fraction of RRP released (T-KO n = 33, CpxI-3R n = 49, CpxI-5E n = 39, WT CpxI n = 50) vs peak release rate (T-KO n = 33, CpxI-3R n = 49, CpxI-5E n = 39, WT CpxI n = 50), vesicle release probability (Pvr) (T-KO n = 42, CpxI-3R n = 56, CpxI-5E n = 48, WT CpxI n = 71) and spontaneous release rate (T-KO n = 39, CpxI-3R n = 55, CpxI-5E n = 42, WT CpxI n = 63). Data are expressed as mean ± SEM, *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001. In (C) vertically oriented p values correspond to fraction of RRP and horizontally oriented p values correspond to peak release rate, Pvr and spontaneous release rate compared to WT rescue.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02391.020

Discussion

Complexins are small proteins that play stimulating and inhibitory roles in neurotransmitter release. The inhibitory function was attributed to insertion of the complexin accessory helix into the C-terminus of partially assembled SNARE complexes (Xue et al., 2007; Giraudo et al., 2009; Kummel et al., 2011), but the validity of this model was unclear. In the study presented here, we were unable to detect any interaction between C-terminally-truncated SNARE complexes and the accessory helix of WT CpxI or the CpxI superclamp mutant using highly sensitive biophysical methods in solution. Moreover, we find that the effects of superclamp and poor-clamp CpxI mutations on neurotransmitter release do not correlate with their effects on cell–cell fusion assays, actually pointing in opposite directions. We also show that mutations that increase the negative charge of the accessory helix inhibit neurotransmitter release while mutations that increase its positive charge enhance release. These results strongly argue against the insertion and zigzag models for the inhibitory activity of the complexin accessory helix and suggest a simple, alternative model whereby the negative charges of the accessory helix and perhaps steric hindrance repel both membranes, thus hindering membrane fusion and neurotransmitter release.

A major question that arises from our study is: Can the insertion and zigzag models be now completely ruled out? In addressing this question, it is critical to consider the available data and the arguments that have been used to support these models:

  1. We originally proposed that part of the accessory helix might insert into partially assembled SNARE complexes (Figure 1C) to explain the inhibitory function of this helix (Xue et al., 2007), but this proposal was not based on any biochemical data and the model could be questioned based on the paucity of hydrophobic residues in the accessory helix.

  2. The related model whereby the entire accessory helix inserts into a partially assembled SNARE complex was supported by the effects of superclamp and poor-clamp mutations (e.g., D27L, E34F, R37A, and K26A) in cell–cell fusion assays (Giraudo et al., 2009). However, the model envisioned that the three charged side chains replaced in the superclamp mutant (D27, E34, and R37) insert into the hydrophobic groove left in the SNARE complex, which is very unlikely from a thermodynamic point of view. Moreover, our data show that the effects of these mutations on cell–cell fusion do not correlate with their effects on neurotransmitter release: the superclamp D27L, E34F, R37A mutation that enhanced inhibition of cell–cell fusion had little effect on release, with a slight tendency to stimulate release, while the K26A mutation that diminishes the inhibitory acitivity of CpxI in cell–cell fusion actually impairs release (Figure 7).

  3. It has been argued (e.g., Kummel et al., 2011) that the D27L, E34F, R37A superclamp mutation inhibits neurotransmitter release in vivo based on rescue experiments on complexin knockdown neurons (Yang et al., 2010). However, these rescue experiments revealed no effect of the D27L, E34F, R37A mutation on evoked release, and it was unclear whether a modest inhibitory effect on spontaneous release was significant. Our rescue data with this mutant using complexin I-III triple KO neurons (Figure 7) are consistent with the results of the knockdown rescues considering the small nature of the effects observed, and the experimental differences between the two approaches. Thus, neither of the two studies supports the insertion or zigzag models.

  4. The crystal structure of the CpxI(26-83) D27L, E34F, R37A superclamp mutant with the Δ60 synaptobrevin-truncated SNARE complex was purported to demonstrate an alternative insertion model whereby the accessory and central helices bind to different SNARE complexes (zigzag model) (Kummel et al., 2011) (Figure 1B). However, the binding mode observed for the mutant accessory helix is highly unlikely thermodynamically for WT CpxI for the above mentioned reason that three mutated side chains are charged and hence are unlikely to insert into a hydrophobic groove. Correspondingly, our extensive NMR analyses did not detect any interaction between the WT CpxI accessory helix and synaptobrevin-truncated SNARE complexes in solution (Figures 2, 3, Figure 3—figure supplement 1C). We were also unable to detect any interaction in solution between truncated SNARE complexes and the accessory helix even for CpxI superclamp mutant fragments (Figure 2—figure supplement 2, Figure 3—figure supplement 1A,D, Figure 4E). A plausible explanation for these findings is that the interaction between the superclamp accessory helix and the truncated SNARE complex observed in the crystals is very weak in solution, and hence could not be observed in our assays, but is stabilized by crystallization. Note that, although the interface area with the SNAREs is larger for the accessory helix (ca. 900 Ă2 calculated with PISA; Krissinel and Henrick, 2007) than for the central helix (ca. 540 Ă2), the atomic B-factors of the residues of the accessory helix in the interface with the SNAREs are much larger than those in the central helix interface, with little electronic density for the side chains of the accessory helix interface (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). Interestingly, it has been suggested that motion at a crystal packing interface is intermediate between that of a solvent accessible surface and that of a protein core, even for large interfaces (Carugo and Argos, 1997). It is also worth noting that Kummel et al. (2011) described another structure obtained with a similar CpxI mutant (D27L, E34M, R37A) where four of the eight crystallographically distinct complexes exhibited the same type of interaction with SCΔ60 observed for the D27L, E34F, R37A CpxI mutant, but the other four complexes had an alternative interaction of the accessory helix with the groove of the truncated SNARE complex where the register was shifted by two helical turns (Kummel et al., 2011).

  5. A competition assay monitored by ITC was used to support the conclusion that the accessory helix of WT CpxI inserts into SCΔ60 (Kummel et al., 2011). We can reproduce these data (Figure 4D) but it is clear that the underlying assumption that 1.5 equivalentes of CpxI(47-134) saturate SCΔ60 is incorrect (Figure 4B,C) and that these ITC assays do not detect an interaction of the accessory helix with SCΔ60.

  6. Experiments with a surface force apparatus (SFA) were proposed to support the zigzag model (Li et al., 2011a). However, the effects caused by WT complexin II in these experiments could be subject to more than one interpretation; for instance, they are compatible with both the zigzag model and the electrostatic hindrance model that we propose here.

  7. The zigzag model was also proposed to be supported by FRET measurements showing that the distances between probes placed on CpxI and SNARE complexes increase on truncation of the synaptobrevin C-terminus (Krishnakumar et al., 2011; Kummel et al., 2011), consistent with the fact that the accessory helix remains close to the SNAREs in the CpxI(26-83)-SNARE complex structure (Figure 1A) but points away from the SNAREs in the structure of the CpxI superclamp mutant bound to SCΔ60 (Figure 1B). We believe that the interpretation of the FRET measurements in terms of static structures constitutes an oversimplification because multiple evidence suggests that there is some flexibility in the accessory helix even in the non-truncated complex, including the poor dispersion of the NH cross-peaks of the accessory helix, their sharper line widths compared to cross-peaks of the central helix, the fact that the NH and Cα chemical shifts of the accessory helix change much less than those of the central helix upon SNARE complex binding and the high B factors observed in the crystal structure of the complex (Chen et al., 2002). Indeed, such flexibility is also consistent with the distance of 20 Å measured by FRET with probes placed at residue 38 of CpxI and residue 193 of SNAP-25 of the SNARE complex (Kummel et al., 2011), since the probes would be expected to be much closer according to the structure of the complex (Chen et al., 2002) (the closest distance between the side chains of these residues is 10 Å). Moreover, our NMR data indicate that the truncations in the SNARE complex further increase the flexibility of the accessory helix (Figure 2—figure supplement 1C,D) and produce flexibility in the N-terminus of the CpxI central helix (Figure 2C) as well as in the C-terminus of SNAP-25 where the FRET donor probe was placed (Figure 3B, Figure 3—figure supplement 1C). The loss in FRET efficiency observed by Kummel et al. upon truncation of the SNARE complex can be readily explained by all these increases in flexibility and the fact that SCΔ60 lacks key residues of synaptobrevin that contact CpxI without the truncation (Figure 1—figure supplement 1).

  8. Kummel et al. (2011) concluded that flexibility could not explain the FRET efficiency observed between probes placed at residue 38 of CpxI and residue 193 of SNAP-25 in SCΔ60 because there was no detectable FRET in experiments performed with a CpxI mutant containing a flexible GPGP sequence between the accessory and central helices. However, a small decrease in donor fluorescence was actually observed for this mutant (Figure 4D of Kummel et al., 2011) and, based on other measurements shown in Figure 4C and Supplementary Table 3 in Kummel et al., 2011, such decrease would correspond to a distance of ca. 42 Å, just 8 Å longer than the distance measured without the GPGP insertion. Considering that a flexible sequence of four residues can readily span 8 Å, that the insertion of these residues is expected to push away the fluorescence probes, and that the error in such long distances is expected to be rather large because of the low associated FRET efficiencies, the results obtained with the GPGP mutant are not inconsistent with the conclusion that increased flexbility underlies the decreased FRET caused by the synaptobrevin truncation.

We believe that, although the arguments presented above and the overall available data argue strongly against the validity of the insertion and zigzag models, it might be premature to completely rule out these models given the complexity of this system. Note for instance that our NMR and ITC data were obtained with truncated SNARE complexes in solution and hence do not rule out the possibility that the CpxI accessory helix interacts with trans-SNARE complexes partially assembled between membranes. Moreover, while cell–cell fusion assays do not correlate with our electrophysiological data or with the stimulatory function of complexins in release, these assays were crucial to establish the functional interplay between complexins and synaptotagmin-1 (Giraudo et al., 2006) and did provide support for the insertion model (Giraudo et al., 2009). Hence, we believe that it is advisable to keep an open mind about the insertion or zigzag models, but views considering either of these models proven need to be revised, and alternative models need to be considered.

The model proposed here is attractive because of its simplicity and because it emerges naturally from the realization that the accessory helix of complexins is negatively charged (see Supplementary figure 1 of Huntwork and Littleton (2007)), together with examination of the crystal structure of the WT CpxI(26-83)/SNARE complex. Thus, binding of the central helix to the SNARE complex places the accessory helix right between the membranes at the space where they need to be brought together for fusion (Figure 1E), and such action is very likely to be hindered at least to some extent by steric and electrostatic repulsion between the negatively charged membranes and the accessory helix. Note also that the stronger inhibitory activity of the accessory helix of dmCpx, compared to the mammalian CpxI accessory helix (Figure 8), cannot arise from increased hydrophobicity, but can be explained by our electrostatic hindrance model. The model is also supported by the inhibition of release caused by the 5E mutation, as well as by the increase in release caused by the 3R mutation (Figure 9). Moreover, the impairment in release that we observed for the K26A mutant (Figure 7; charge change = −1), and the increase in spontaneous release caused in rescue experiments on complexin knockdown neurons by a K26E, L41K, E47K mutation (charge change = +1) (Yang et al., 2010) also correlate in general terms with this model. However, these results need to be interpreted with caution, since it is plausible that the 5E and 3R mutations may alter the helical character of the accessary helix, and the considerable magnitude of the effects caused by the K26A and K26E, L41K, E47K mutations suggest that they do not arise only from changes in overall electrostatic potential. Thus, K26 might be involved in some additional, as yet unidentified interaction. Note that K26 does not really form part of the accessory helix in the CpxI(26-83)/SNARE complex and may be involved in releasing the inhibition caused by the accessory helix (Xue et al., 2010).

It is also important to note that the accessory helix does not act on its own and its function depends on the neighboring N-terminus. Our previous studies demonstrate an overall facilitatory effect of complexin for evoked and spontaneous release that requires binding of the complexin N-terminus back onto the SNARE complex (Xue et al., 2010). This proposed loop may serve two functions, namely to further stabilize the trans-SNARE complex and to properly position the accessory helix near the putative fusion area to reduce spontaneous release through its electrostatic repulsion effect. Alternatively, it is plausible that the function of the accessory helix may involve interactions with other components of the release machinery. Clearly, more research will be required to further test the validity of our model, ideally using diverse approaches. Such approaches should include challenging biophysical studies involving trans-SNARE complexes formed between two membranes, reconstitution experiments that have already yielded a wealth of information on complexins (e.g., Schaub et al., 2006; Yoon et al., 2008; Malsam et al., 2009, 2012; Diao et al., 2012), and correlations with additional studies of neurotransmitter release in neurons.

Materials and methods

Recombinant proteins

Expression vectors and protocols for expression and purification of the following protein fragments in E. coli were described previously: rat syntaxin 1A residues 191-253, rat synaptobrevin 2 residues 29-93, human SNAP-25 residues 11-82 and 141-203, full-length rat CpxI and rat CpxI residues 26-83 (Pabst et al., 2000; Chen et al., 2002). Starting from these vectors and from a vector containing the full-length dmCpx sequence (Xue et al., 2009), we used standard recombinant DNA techniques and custom designed primers to generate: C-terminally truncated versions of the synaptobrevin SNARE motif (residues 29-60 [Δ60], residues 29-62 [Δ62], residues 29-68 [Δ68], and residues 29-76 [Δ76]); C-terminally truncated versions of the syntaxin-1 SNARE motif (residues 191-228 [Δ228]; residues 191-232 [Δ232] and residues 191-236 [Δ236]); CpxI fragments corresponding to the CpxI accessory helix (residues 26-47 and 26-48) and the central helix plus C-terminus (residues 47-134); mutant versions of the CpxI(26-83) and CpxI(26-48) fragments; and dmCpx(28-88). Proteins were expressed in E. coli and purified as described (Pabst et al., 2000; Chen et al., 2002). Uniform 15N- or 2H,15N-labeling was performed by growing E. coli BL21(DE3) in minimal medium made with H2O or D2O as the solvent, respectively, and using 15NH4Cl as the sole nitrogen source. Uniform 2H,15N,13C-labeling of synaptobrevin(29-93) for triple resonance experiments acquired on the 2H,15N,13C-Syb-SCΔ236 complex was accomplished by an analogous procedure including 13C6-glucose as the sole carbon source.

SNARE complex assembly

Non-truncated SNARE complexes were formed with SNAP-25 (11-82), SNAP-25 (141-203), syntaxin-1(191-253) and synaptobrevin(29-93). For truncated SNARE complexes, synaptobrevin(29-93) or syntaxin-1(191-253) were replaced by the appropriate C-terminally truncated fragment. In general, complex assembly was accomplished by incubating a mixture of the purified fragments overnight at 4°C and removing remaining unassembled fragments by concentration–dilution cycles with a 30 kDa cutoff, as described for the non-truncated complex (Chen et al., 2002). SDS-PAGE monitored by Coomassie blue staining comparing boiled and non-boiled samples was used to confirm that the complexes were fully formed and the unassembled fragments were removed. For the complexes with the most severe truncations (Δ60, Δ62, Δ228, and Δ232), which are not SDS resistant, full complex formation was monitored by non-denaturing PAGE and Coomassie blue staining.

NMR spectroscopy

All NMR spectra were obtained at 32°C on Agilent (Santa Clara, CA) INOVA800 or INOVA600 spectrometers equipped with triple resonance cold-probes. 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra were acquired with samples that normally contained 25-50 μM 2H,15N-labeled CpxI fragment or SNARE complex 2H,15N-labeled at one of the SNARE motifs in the absence or presence of a 1.2–1.5 equivalents of unlabeled SNARE complex (truncated or non-truncated) or unlabeled CpxI fragment. The particular fragments used for each figure are described in the corresponding figure legend. Samples containing isotopically-labeled SNARE complexes and unlabeled complexin fragments were dissolved in 25 mM Tris (pH 7.4) containing 125 mM NaCl and 8% D2O. Samples containing isotopically labeled complexin fragmens and unlabeled SNARE complexes were dissolved in 25 mM HEPES (pH 7.1) containing 125 mM NaCl and 8% D2O. TROSY-HNCA and TROSY-HNCOCA spectra with 2H decoupling (Yang and Kay, 1999) were used to obtain partial backbone assignments for 2H,15N,13C-Syb-SCΔ236 complex as described (Chen et al., 2002). All the data were processed with NMRPipe (Delaglio et al., 1995) and analyzed with NMRView (Johnson and Blevins, 1994).

Isothermal titration calorimetry

ITC experiments were performed using a VP-ITC system (MicroCal; Northampton, MA) at 37°C in PBS buffer (10 mM Na2HPO4, 2 mM K2HPO4 pH 7.4, 2.7 mM KCl, 137 mM NaCl) containing 0.25 mM TCEP. For Figure 4A,B, CpxI(47-134) (200 μM) was directly titrated into the chamber containing non-truncated SNARE complex or SCΔ60 (10-15 μM). For Figure 4C–E, 200 μM CpxI(47-134), CpxI(26-83) or CpxI(26-83) superclamp mutant (D27L, E34F, R37A) were titrated in the chamber containing 10-15 μM SCΔ60 and 1.5 equivalents of CpxI(47-134). All proteins were dialyzed in the same buffer before the experiments. The data were fitted with a nonlinear least squares routine using a single-site binding model with Origin for ITC v.5.0 (Microcal).

Lentiviral constructs and virus production

For expression of CpxI variants within neuronal cells a modified lentiviral vector (Lois et al., 2002) was used in which a human Synapsin-1 promoter, driving the expression of CpxI, and a second promoter (ubiquitin C), which serves as driver for the reporter gene (EGFP), were employed. WT rat CpxI (GenBank accession number: NM_022864) and Drosophila Cpx (AY121629) cDNAs were used to generate all Cpx variants by standard recombinant DNA techniques. For immunocytochemistry a 3xFLAG epitope (Sigma-Aldrich) was fused at the C-terminus of CpxI. After sequence verification, the cDNAs were cloned into the lentiviral shuttle vector and lentiviral particles were prepared as described (Lois et al., 2002). Briefly, HEK293T cells were cotransfected with 10 µg shuttle vector and the helper plasmids pCMVdR8.9 and pVSV.G (5 µg each) with X-tremeGENE 9 DNA transfection reagent (Roche Diagnostic). After 72 hr the virus containing cell culture supernatant was collected and purified by filtration. Aliquots were flash-frozen in liquid nitrogen and stored at −80°C. Viruses were titrated with WT hippocampal mass-cultured neurons. For infection, about 5 × 105–1 × 106 infectious virus units were pipetted onto 1 DIV hippocampal CpxI-III triple KO neurons per 35 mm-diameter well.

Neuronal culture

Murine microisland cultures were prepared as described (Xue et al., 2007). CpxI-III triple KO neurons were described previously (Xue et al., 2008b). Animals were handled according to the rules of Berlin authorities and the animal welfare committee of the Charité Berlin, Germany. Primary hippocampal neurons were prepared from mice on embryonic day E18 and plated at 300 cm−2 density on WT astrocyte microisland for autaptic neuron electrophysiology. For western blotting and immunocytochemistry hippocampal neurons were plated at 10.000 cm−2 and 5000 cm−2, respectively, on continental WT astrocyte feeder layer.

Electrophysiology of cultured neurons

Whole cell patch-clamp recordings in autaptic neurons were performed as previously described (Xue et al., 2009). The extracellular solution contained (in mM) 140 NaCl, 2.4 KCl, 10 Hepes, 2 CaCl2, 4 MgCl2, 10 Glucose (pH adjusted to 7.3 with NaOH, 300 mOsm). The patch pipette solution contained (in mM) 136 KCl, 17.8 Hepes, 1 EGTA, 0.6 MgCl2, 4 ATP-Mg, 0.3 GTP-Na, 12 phosphocreatine and 50 units/ml phosphocreatine kinase (300mOsm, pH 7.4). Neurons were clamped at −70 mV with a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices; Sunnyvale, CA) under control of Clampex 9 (Molecular Devices) at DIV 11-17. Data were analyzed offline using Axograph X (AxoGraph Scientific; Berkeley, CA) and Prism 5 (GraphPad Software; La Jolla, CA). Statistic significances were tested using one-way analysis of variance followed by a Tukey post hoc test to compare all groups.

EPSCs were evoked by a brief 2 ms somatic depolarization to 0 mV. EPSC amplitude was determined as the average of 5 EPSCs at 0.1 Hz. RRP size was determined by measuring the charge transfer of the transient synaptic current induced by a pulsed 5 s application of hypertonic solution (500 mM sucrose in extracellular solution). Pvr was calculated as the ratio of the charge from an evoked EPSC and the RRP size of the same neuron. Evoking 5 or 50 synaptic responses at 50 or 10 Hz respectively in standard external solution analyzed short-term plasticity. PPR was calculated by dividing the second EPSC amplitude with the first EPSC amplitude from the average of three 50 Hz trains at 0.1 Hz. For analyzing mEPSCs, traces were digitally filtered at 1 kHz offline. Then the last 8 s of 5 traces of EPSCs at 0.1 Hz were analyzed using the template-based mEPSC detection algorithm implemented in Axograph X (AxoGraph Scientific) and substracted from background noise by detecting events in the last 3 s of 5 EPSCs at 0.2 Hz in 3 mM kynurenic acid in extracellular solution.

Synaptic-vesicle fusogenicity was measured by applying 250 mM sucrose solution onto the neuron for 10 s and analyzed as described previously (Xue et al., 2010). Briefly, to obtain the fraction of RRP released at 250 mM sucrose solution, the charge transfer of the transient synaptic current was measured and divided by the RRP size obtained by 500 mM sucrose application (5 s) from the same neuron. The response onset latency was calculated between the open tip control for solution exchange and the onset of the sucrose response. The peak release rate was calculated by dividing peak amplitude of sucrose response with the RRP size of the same neuron. The spontaneous release rate was calculated by dividing the mEPSC frequency with the number of vesicles within the RRP. This number was obtained by multiplying the mEPSC charge with the RRP charge measured by 500 mM sucrose application.

Western blotting and immunocytochemistry

For detection of CpxI protein levels by western blotting, protein lysates were obtained from mass cultures of CpxI-III KO hippocampal neurons (DIV 14) grown on WT astrocyte feeder layers. Briefly, cells were lysed using 50 mM Tris/HCl (pH 7.9), 150 mM NaCl, 5 mM EDTA, 1% Triton-X-100, 1% Nonidet P-40, 1% sodium deoxycholate, and protease inhibitors (complete protease inhibitor cocktail tablet, Roche Diagnostics GmbH; Manheim, Germany). Proteins were separated by SDS-PAGE and transferred to nitrocellulose membranes. After blocking with 5% milk powder (Carl Roth GmbH) for 1 hr at room temperature, membranes were incubated with rabbit anti-CpxI/II (1:1000; Synaptic System) and mouse anti-tubulinIII (1:750; Sigma–Aldrich) antibodies overnight at 4°C. After washing and incubation with corresponding horseradish peroxidase-conjugated goat secondary antibodies (all from Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories), protein expression levels were visualized with ECL Plus Western Blotting Detection Reagents (GE Healthcare Biosciences).

To detect synaptic localization by immunocytochemistry, lentiviral transduced neurons were washed once in PBS, fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 10 min at room temperature and treated 3 times 5 min with 100 mM glycine in PBS. Then cells were blocked with 5% normal goat serum and 0.1% Tween-20 in PBS for 1 hr and incubated with primary antibodies overnight at 4°C in blocking solution. The following antibodies were used: mouse anti-FLAG (1:500; Sigma-Aldrich; Saint Louis, MO), guinea pig anti-VGlut1 (1:4000; Synaptic System). Primary antibodies were labeled with anti-mouse Rhodamine Red and anti-guinea pig Alexa Fluor 405 (each 1:500; Jackson Immunoresearch Laboratories; West Groove, PA) for 1 hr at room temperature. After washing, cover slips were mounted with Mowiol 4-88 antifade medium (Polysciences Europe GmbH; Eppelheim, Germany). Neurons were imaged using an Olympus IX81 microscope.

Acknowledgements

We thank Annegret Felies, Berit Soehl-Kielczynski, Sabine Lenz, Katja Poetschke, Carola Schweynoch and Bettina Brokowski for technical support, members of the Rosenmund laboratory for discussions, Daeho Lee for initial experiments in this project and Yilun Sun for expert technical assistance on protein expression and purification. We also thank the reviewers of the manuscript who have helped to considerably improve the quality of the paper with their constructive criticisms. This work was supported by the German Research Council (DFG Collaborative Research Grant SFB665, 958 to TT and CR), the European Research Foundation (Grant SynVglut to CR), the Excellence Cluster Neurocure Exc257 (to CR), the Welch foundation (grant I-1304 to JR) and the NIH (grant NS37200 to JR).

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft FundRef identification ID: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100001659 SFB665, 958 to Thorsten Trimbuch, Christian Rosenmund.

  • European Research Council FundRef identification ID: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100000781 SynVglut to Christian Rosenmund.

  • Excellence Cluster Neurocure Exc257 to Christian Rosenmund.

  • Welch Foundation FundRef identification ID: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100000928 I-1304 to Josep Rizo.

  • National Institutes of Health FundRef identification ID: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100000002 NS37200 to Josep Rizo.

Additional information

Competing interests

CR: Reviewing editor, eLife.

The other authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

TT, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

JR, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

CR, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

JX, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data.

DRT, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data.

DF, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: Animals were handled according to the animal welfare committee of the Charité-Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Germany. Time pregnant females were anesthetized and euthanized at E18 according to the german animal law (§4 section 1) and permit by the Berlin authorities (Landesamt für Gesundheit und Soziales, LaGeSo) under the permit number T0220/09.

References

  1. Afonine PV, Mustyakimov M, Grosse-Kunstleve RW, Moriarty NW, Langan P, Adams PD. 2010. Joint X-ray and neutron refinement with phenix.refine. Acta Crystallographica Section D, Biological Crystallography 66:1153–1163. doi: 10.1107/S0907444910026582 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arac D, Chen X, Khant HA, Ubach J, Ludtke SJ, Kikkawa M, Johnson AE, Chiu W, Sudhof TC, Rizo J. 2006. Close membrane-membrane proximity induced by Ca(2+)-dependent multivalent binding of synaptotagmin-1 to phospholipids. Nature Structural & Molecular Biology 13:209–217. doi: 10.1038/nsmb1056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bracher A, Kadlec J, Betz H, Weissenhorn W. 2002. X-ray structure of a neuronal complexin-SNARE complex from squid. The Journal of Biological Chemistry 277:26517–26523. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M203460200 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Brunger AT, Weninger K, Bowen M, Chu S. 2009. Single-molecule studies of the neuronal SNARE fusion machinery. Annual Review of Biochemistry 78:903–928. doi: 10.1146/annurev.biochem.77.070306.103621 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Carugo O, Argos P. 1997. Protein-protein crystal-packing contacts. Protein Science 6:2261–2263. doi: 10.1002/pro.5560061021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Chen X, Tang J, Sudhof TC, Rizo J. 2005. Are neuronal SNARE proteins Ca2+ sensors? Journal of Molecular Biology 347:145–158. doi: 10.1016/j.jmb.2005.01.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chen X, Tomchick DR, Kovrigin E, Arac D, Machius M, Sudhof TC, Rizo J. 2002. Three-dimensional structure of the complexin/SNARE complex. Neuron 33:397–409. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00583-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cho RW, Song Y, Littleton JT. 2010. Comparative analysis of Drosophila and mammalian complexins as fusion clamps and facilitators of neurotransmitter release. Molecular and Cellular Neurosciences 45:389–397. doi: 10.1016/j.mcn.2010.07.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Dai H, Shen N, Arac D, Rizo J. 2007. A quaternary SNARE-synaptotagmin-Ca(2+)-phospholipid complex in neurotransmitter release. Journal of Molecular Biology 367:848–863. doi: 10.1016/j.jmb.2007.01.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Delaglio F, Grzesiek S, Vuister GW, Zhu G, Pfeifer J, Bax A. 1995. Nmrpipe - a Multidimensional spectral processing system based on Unix Pipes. Journal of Biomolecular NMR 6:277–293. doi: 10.1007/BF00197809 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Diao J, Grob P, Cipriano DJ, Kyoung M, Zhang Y, Shah S, Nguyen A, Padolina M, Srivastava A, Vrljic M, Shah A, Nogales E, Chu S, Brunger AT. 2012. Synaptic proteins promote calcium-triggered fast transition from point contact to full fusion. eLife 1:e00109. doi: 10.7554/eLife.00109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dulubova I, Khvotchev M, Liu S, Huryeva I, Sudhof TC, Rizo J. 2007. Munc18-1 binds directly to the neuronal SNARE complex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104:2697–2702. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0611318104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fernandez-Chacon R, Konigstorfer A, Gerber SH, Garcia J, Matos MF, Stevens CF, Brose N, Rizo J, Rosenmund C, Sudhof TC. 2001. Synaptotagmin I functions as a calcium regulator of release probability. Nature 410:41–49 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gao Y, Zorman S, Gundersen G, Xi Z, Ma L, Sirinakis G, Rothman JE, Zhang Y. 2012. Single reconstituted neuronal SNARE complexes zipper in three distinct stages. Science 337:1340–1343. doi: 10.1126/science.1224492 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Giraudo CG, Eng WS, Melia TJ, Rothman JE. 2006. A clamping mechanism involved in SNARE-dependent exocytosis. Science 313:676–680. doi: 10.1126/science.1129450 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Giraudo CG, Garcia-Diaz A, Eng WS, Chen Y, Hendrickson WA, Melia TJ, Rothman JE. 2009. Alternative zippering as an on-off switch for SNARE-mediated fusion. Science 323:512–516. doi: 10.1126/science.1166500 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hanson PI, Roth R, Morisaki H, Jahn R, Heuser JE. 1997. Structure and conformational changes in NSF and its membrane receptor complexes visualized by quick-freeze/deep-etch electron microscopy. Cell 90:523–535. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)80512-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hazzard J, Sudhof TC, Rizo J. 1999. NMR analysis of the structure of synaptobrevin and of its interaction with syntaxin. Journal of Biomolecular NMR 14:203–207. doi: 10.1023/A:1008382027065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hobson RJ, Liu Q, Watanabe S, Jorgensen EM. 2011. Complexin maintains vesicles in the primed state in C. elegans. Current Biology 21:106–113. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.12.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Huntwork S, Littleton JT. 2007. A complexin fusion clamp regulates spontaneous neurotransmitter release and synaptic growth. Nature Neuroscience 10:1235–1237. doi: 10.1038/nn1980 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Jahn R, Fasshauer D. 2012. Molecular machines governing exocytosis of synaptic vesicles. Nature 490:201–207. doi: 10.1038/nature11320 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Johnson BA, Blevins RA. 1994. Nmr view - a computer-program for the visualization and analysis of Nmr data. Journal of Biomolecular NMR 4:603–614. doi: 10.1007/BF00404272 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Jorquera RA, Huntwork-Rodriguez S, Akbergenova Y, Cho RW, Littleton JT. 2012. Complexin controls spontaneous and evoked neurotransmitter release by regulating the timing and properties of synaptotagmin activity. The Journal of Neuroscience 32:18234–18245. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3212-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kaeser-Woo YJ, Yang X, Sudhof TC. 2012. C-terminal complexin sequence is selectively required for clamping and priming but not for Ca2+ triggering of synaptic exocytosis. The Journal of Neuroscience 32:2877–2885. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3360-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Krishnakumar SS, Radoff DT, Kummel D, Giraudo CG, Li F, Khandan L, Baguley SW, Coleman J, Reinisch KM, Pincet F, Rothman JE. 2011. A conformational switch in complexin is required for synaptotagmin to trigger synaptic fusion. Nature Structural & Molecular Biology 18:934–940. doi: 10.1038/nsmb.2103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Krissinel E, Henrick K. 2007. Inference of macromolecular assemblies from crystalline state. Journal of Molecular Biology 372:774–797. doi: 10.1016/j.jmb.2007.05.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kummel D, Krishnakumar SS, Radoff DT, Li F, Giraudo CG, Pincet F, Rothman JE, Reinisch KM. 2011. Complexin cross-links prefusion SNAREs into a zigzag array. Nature Structural & Molecular Biology 18:927–933 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Li F, Pincet F, Perez E, Giraudo CG, Tareste D, Rothman JE. 2011a. Complexin activates and clamps SNAREpins by a common mechanism involving an intermediate energetic state. Nature Structural & Molecular Biology 18:941–946. doi: 10.1038/nsmb.2102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Li W, Ma C, Guan R, Xu Y, Tomchick DR, Rizo J. 2011b. The crystal structure of a Munc13 C-terminal module exhibits a remarkable similarity to vesicle tethering factors. Structure 19:1443–1455. doi: 10.1016/j.str.2011.07.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lois C, Hong EJ, Pease S, Brown EJ, Baltimore D. 2002. Germline transmission and tissue-specific expression of transgenes delivered by lentiviral vectors. Science 295:868–872. doi: 10.1126/science.1067081 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Ma C, Su L, Seven AB, Xu Y, Rizo J. 2013. Reconstitution of the vital functions of Munc18 and Munc13 in neurotransmitter release. Science 339:421–425. doi: 10.1126/science.1230473 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Malsam J, Parisotto D, Bharat TA, Scheutzow A, Krause JM, Briggs JA, Sollner TH. 2012. Complexin arrests a pool of docked vesicles for fast Ca2+-dependent release. The EMBO Journal 31:3270–3281. doi: 10.1038/emboj.2012.164 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Malsam J, Seiler F, Schollmeier Y, Rusu P, Krause JM, Sollner TH. 2009. The carboxy-terminal domain of complexin I stimulates liposome fusion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106:2001–2006. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0812813106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Martin JA, Hu Z, Fenz KM, Fernandez J, Dittman JS. 2011. Complexin has opposite effects on two modes of synaptic vesicle fusion. Current Biology 21:97–105. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.12.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Maximov A, Tang J, Yang X, Pang ZP, Sudhof TC. 2009. Complexin controls the force transfer from SNARE complexes to membranes in fusion. Science 323:516–521. doi: 10.1126/science.1166505 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Mayer A, Wickner W, Haas A. 1996. Sec18p (NSF)-driven release of Sec17p (alpha-SNAP) can precede docking and fusion of yeast vacuoles. Cell 85:83–94 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. McMahon HT, Missler M, Li C, Sudhof TC. 1995. Complexins: cytosolic proteins that regulate SNAP receptor function. Cell 83:111–119. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(95)90239-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Pabst S, Hazzard JW, Antonin W, Sudhof TC, Jahn R, Rizo J, Fasshauer D. 2000. Selective interaction of complexin with the neuronal SNARE complex. Determination of the binding regions. The Journal of Biological Chemistry 275:19808–19818. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M002571200 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Poirier MA, Xiao W, Macosko JC, Chan C, Shin YK, Bennett MK. 1998. The synaptic SNARE complex is a parallel four-stranded helical bundle. Nature Structural Biology 5:765–769. doi: 10.1038/1799 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Reim K, Mansour M, Varoqueaux F, McMahon HT, Sudhof TC, Brose N, Rosenmund C. 2001. Complexins regulate a late step in Ca2+-dependent neurotransmitter release. Cell 104:71–81. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(01)00192-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Rizo J, Rosen MK, Gardner KH. 2012. Enlightening molecular mechanisms through study of protein interactions. Journal of Molecular Cell Biology 4:270–283. doi: 10.1093/jmcb/mjs036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Rizo J, Sudhof TC. 2012. The membrane fusion enigma: SNAREs, Sec1/Munc18 proteins, and their accomplices-guilty as charged? Annual Review of Cell and Developmental Biology 28:279–308. doi: 10.1146/annurev-cellbio-101011-155818 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Roggero CM, De Blas GA, Dai H, Tomes CN, Rizo J, Mayorga LS. 2007. Complexin/synaptotagmin interplay controls acrosomal exocytosis. The Journal of Biological Chemistry 282:26335–26343. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M700854200 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Rosenmund C, Stevens CF. 1996. Definition of the readily releasable pool of vesicles at hippocampal synapses. Neuron 16:1197–1207. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80146-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Schaub JR, Lu X, Doneske B, Shin YK, McNew JA. 2006. Hemifusion arrest by complexin is relieved by Ca2+-synaptotagmin I. Nature Structural & Molecular Biology 13:748–750. doi: 10.1038/nsmb1124 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Seiler F, Malsam J, Krause JM, Sollner TH. 2009. A role of complexin-lipid interactions in membrane fusion. FEBS Letters 583:2343–2348. doi: 10.1016/j.febslet.2009.06.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Sollner T, Bennett MK, Whiteheart SW, Scheller RH, Rothman JE. 1993. A protein assembly-disassembly pathway in vitro that may correspond to sequential steps of synaptic vesicle docking, activation, and fusion. Cell 75:409–418 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Sorensen JB. 2009. Conflicting views on the membrane fusion machinery and the fusion pore. Annual Review of Cell and Developmental Biology 25:513–537 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Sorensen JB, Wiederhold K, Muller EM, Milosevic I, Nagy G, de Groot BL, Grubmuller H, Fasshauer D. 2006. Sequential N- to C-terminal SNARE complex assembly drives priming and fusion of secretory vesicles. The EMBO Journal 25:955–966 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Sudhof TC. 2013. Neurotransmitter release: the last millisecond in the life of a synaptic vesicle. Neuron 80:675–690 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Sutton RB, Fasshauer D, Jahn R, Brunger AT. 1998. Crystal structure of a SNARE complex involved in synaptic exocytosis at 2.4 A resolution. Nature 395:347–353. doi: 10.1038/26412 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Tang J, Maximov A, Shin OH, Dai H, Rizo J, Sudhof TC. 2006. A complexin/synaptotagmin 1 switch controls fast synaptic vesicle exocytosis. Cell 126:1175–1187. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2006.08.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Xu J, Brewer KD, Perez-Castillejos R, Rizo J. 2013. Subtle interplay between synaptotagmin and complexin binding to the SNARE complex. Journal of Molecular Biology 425:3461–3475. doi: 10.1016/j.jmb.2013.07.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Xue M, Craig TK, Xu J, Chao HT, Rizo J, Rosenmund C. 2010. Binding of the complexin N terminus to the SNARE complex potentiates synaptic-vesicle fusogenicity. Nature Structural & Molecular Biology 17:568–575. doi: 10.1038/nsmb.1791 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Xue M, Lin YQ, Pan H, Reim K, Deng H, Bellen HJ, Rosenmund C. 2009. Tilting the balance between facilitatory and inhibitory functions of mammalian and Drosophila complexins orchestrates synaptic vesicle exocytosis. Neuron 64:367–380. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Xue M, Ma C, Craig TK, Rosenmund C, Rizo J. 2008a. The Janus-faced nature of the C(2)B domain is fundamental for synaptotagmin-1 function. Nature Structural & Molecular Biology 15:1160–1168. doi: 10.1038/nsmb.1508 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Xue M, Reim K, Chen X, Chao HT, Deng H, Rizo J, Brose N, Rosenmund C. 2007. Distinct domains of complexin I differentially regulate neurotransmitter release. Nature Structural & Molecular Biology 14:949–958. doi: 10.1038/nsmb1292 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Xue M, Stradomska A, Chen H, Brose N, Zhang W, Rosenmund C, Reim K. 2008b. Complexins facilitate neurotransmitter release at excitatory and inhibitory synapses in mammalian central nervous system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105:7875–7880. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0803012105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Yang DW, Kay LE. 1999. TROSY triple-resonance four-dimensional NMR spectroscopy of a 46 ns tumbling protein. Journal of the American Chemical Society 121:2571–2575. doi: 10.1021/ja984056t [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Yang X, Cao P, Sudhof TC. 2013. Deconstructing complexin function in activating and clamping Ca2+-triggered exocytosis by comparing knockout and knockdown phenotypes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 110:20777–20782. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1321367110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Yang X, Kaeser-Woo YJ, Pang ZP, Xu W, Sudhof TC. 2010. Complexin clamps asynchronous release by blocking a secondary Ca(2+) sensor via its accessory alpha helix. Neuron 68:907–920. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Yoon TY, Lu X, Diao J, Lee SM, Ha T, Shin YK. 2008. Complexin and Ca2+ stimulate SNARE-mediated membrane fusion. Nature Structural & Molecular Biology 15:707–713. doi: 10.1038/nsmb.1446 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2014 May 8;3:e02391. doi: 10.7554/eLife.02391.021

Decision letter

Editor: Axel T Brunger1

eLife posts the editorial decision letter and author response on a selection of the published articles (subject to the approval of the authors). An edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the substantive concerns or comments; minor concerns are not usually shown. Reviewers have the opportunity to discuss the decision before the letter is sent (see review process). Similarly, the author response typically shows only responses to the major concerns raised by the reviewers.

Thank you for sending your work entitled “An electrostatic hindrance model for accessory helix-mediated inhibition of neurotransmitter release by complexins” for consideration at eLife. Your article has been generally favorably evaluated by a Senior editor and 3 reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, although there were some concerns.

The following individuals responsible for the peer review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Axel Brunger (Reviewing editor); William Wickner and Ad Bax (peer reviewers).

The Reviewing editor and the other reviewers discussed their comments before we reached this decision, and the Reviewing editor has assembled the following comments to help you prepare a revised submission.

This work questions the interaction between the accessory helix and a truncated ternary SNARE complex. In 2011, multiple experiments by the Rothman, Reinisch, and Tareste groups published: ITC binding experiments, crystal structures with a mutant of complexin in complex with a truncated SNARE complex, solution (bulk) FRET data, and surface force measurements. However, among these four different experiments, the only direct demonstrations of an interaction between the accessory helix of complexin and the SNARE complex were the crystal structures, and the ITC experiment.

In this work, it is shown that the interpretation of ITC experiment may have suffered from an incorrect assumption of the affinity between the complexin and SNARE fragments used, and that the data can be explained in an alternative fashion that does not involve an interaction between the accessory helix of complexin and the truncated SNARE complex. Rizo, Rosenmund and co-workers then present a large body of solution NMR data that does not show any evidence for an interaction between the accessory helix and the truncated SNARE complex, even for the so-called superclamp mutant, suggesting the interaction represents an extremely weak, lowly occupied state. In addition, the authors argue that the bulk FRET data by Kummel et al. can be interpreted in an alternative fashion (but this re-interpretation is questionable, see below).

These in vitro studies are augmented by experiments with cultured neurons that show that mutation of the accessory helix modulates release based on the charge distribution on the accessory helix. The more negative, the more fusion is inhibited. It is suggested that the inhibitory function of complexin is caused by an interaction between the membrane and the accessory helix. Moreover, they show that 'super'-clamp (D27L,E34F,R37A) and a 'no'-clamp (K26A) mutants of complexin do not have the expected effects when introduced into actual neurons.

That being said, support for the newly introduced electrostatic repulsion idea is rather thin and is contradicted by other data (see below). It is even is unlikely that the accessory helix would adopt a helical conformation in the presence of the rather severe mutations of Figure 9. Considering the significant impact of the CpxI K26A mutation, it's more likely that the CpxI role is more subtle, along the lines suggested at the end of the Discussion section.

Regardless if the electrostatic repulsion model is correct (it will certainly require a lot of discussion, or perhaps be removed, in a revised version of this paper), the main point of this paper is that it entirely re-opens the question how complexin really works.

Issues to address:

1) Although the overlaid TROSY-HSQC spectra are reasonably compelling for the general argument the authors are trying to make, it would be helpful if more specific interpretation were supported by an SI figure showing the (composite 1H/15N) amide shift changes as a function of residue number for the delta62 and delta68 truncations. Similarly, a correlation graph of CpxI shift changes (vs. full length minus random coil) for the shift changes in the N-terminus of the central helix would provide stronger support for the “increasingly flexible toward the N-terminus” statement. Alternatively, 15N relaxation measurements would make this more quantitative, but not necessary from my perspective if indeed the shift change is in the random coil direction.

2) The negative charge repulsion model is contradicted by the following experiments: the non-clamping mutants by Kummel et al. (L41E, A44E, A30E, A31E) and (A30E and A31E) are both negatively charged, but yet, they have no effect on spontaneous fusion in their cell-based fusion assay. The poor-clamp mutant (K26E, K41K, E47K, also somewhat negatively charged) by Yang et al. (Neuron, 2010) actually increases spontaneous release in neuronal cultures. These contradictions need to be addressed by additional experiments, or the negative charge model needs to be modified.

3) As mentioned in the Discussion, point 4, Reinish et al. determined a second crystal form (with an additional mutation in complexin, F34M, in addition to the “superclamp” mutations in the accessory helix) where the accessory helix interacts in two different registers. This indicates promiscuity of the interaction between the accessory helix and the 2nd SNARE complex in the crystal structures. This point needs some clarification in the Discussion: first these two alternative forms only occur upon the additional F34M mutation. Second, it is not necessarily that crystallization has induced this interaction. It is possible that this weak interaction is a lowly state that is too infrequent to be observed by NMR, but has been stabilized in the context of the crystal structure.

4) The interaction between the accessory helix and a neighboring partially folded full SNARE complex is not ruled out by the presented experiments since they do not involve partially folded full SNARE complex. This should be noted in the paper.

5) The alternative explanation of the original Kummel et al., ITC data and the additional ITC experiments are convincing. However, these data do not rule out an interaction between the accessory helix and SNARE complex per se, but it may be much weaker than estimated by the original Kummel et al. experiment. It would be prudent to add this caveat to the discussion of the ITC experiments. Did the authors attempt to conduct the competition experiment under actually saturating conditions of CpxI(47-134)?

6) Kummel et al. used bulk fret and measured the FRET between S25 Cterm (residue 193) and CX Nterm (residue 38). They showed that the more VAMP is truncated at the C-terminus, truncation of VAMP up to residue 60, the weaker the FRET efficiency. In this work, the NMR results and the weaker affinity observed by ITC suggests that the VAMP truncations make SNAP-25 and also syntaxin more flexible and unstructured, thus destabilizing the interaction with complexin as well, resulting in lower FRET. Thus, an interpretation of the NMR result is that the lower FRET efficiency is a result of motional averaging of multiple conformations that produces the appearance of a lower FRET “state”. However, Kummel et al. did an experiment to rule out this possibility: They used a 'flexible' CPX construct (CPX-GPGP), in which a helix-breaking GPGP linker was inserted between the central and accessory helices of CPX (Figure 4b,c,d, Supplemental Table 3 in Kummel et al.). To quote Kummel et al. “these experiments indicate that when CPX is bound to a half-zippered form of the SNARE complex, CPXacc has the intrinsic property of extending away from the complex. Because this conformation is maintained in solution, it determines how the complex crystallizes and not vice versa.”

[Editors' note: further clarifications were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for sending your work entitled “An Electrostatic-Steric Hindrance Model for Accessory Helix Mediated Inhibition of Neurotransmitter Release by Complexin” for further consideration at eLife. Your article has been favorably evaluated by a Senior editor and 3 reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors.

The following individuals responsible for the peer review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Axel Brunger (Reviewing editor); William Wickner and Ad Bax (peer reviewers).

The Reviewing editor and the other reviewers discussed their comments before we reached this decision, and the Reviewing editor has assembled the following comments to help you prepare a revised submission.

We thank the authors for addressing some of our concerns and would be ready to recommend acceptance after addressing the following remaining concerns:

1) We are disappointed that the authors did not address the question if the charge-altering mutations of the accessory helix retain the helical character of the accessory helix (suggested by the strong alpha-helical chemical shifts of the accessory helix by Pabst et al., 2000 J.Biol.Chem). It is certainly possible that this happens, but the data presented here do not provide direct (e.g., NMR) evidence for this notion. Moreover, the proposed model of electrostatic-steric hindrance by the accessory helix remains speculative. It is of course fine to provide such a speculative model in the Discussion section of the paper. However, the title is misleading since it suggests that the model has been definitely proven in this work. Rather, the main focus of this paper is a re-examination of the interaction between the accessory helix and the truncated SNARE complex that was previously studied by Kummel et al. Thus, we request a change of title to properly reflect the main focus of this work.

2) The statement in the Results section: “these cross-peaks actually increased in the spectrum of Cpx(26-83) bound to SCdelta68 with respect to the SC-bound state, and increased somewhat more in the SCdelta62-bound spectrum (Figure 2–figure supplements 1C,D) These data show that the synaptobrevin C-terminal truncations increase the flexibility of the accessory helix, in correlation with the destabilization of N terminal part of the central helix,”. The best resolved accessory helix resonances (A30 and Q38) actually do not seem to increase from SCdelta68 to SCdelta62. Thus, the statements about the increase in flexibility of the accessory helix need to be qualified.

3) The discussion of the Kummel at al. crystal structure (point 4 in the Discussion) remains a concern. Firstly, the area of the interface between the accessory helix and the truncated SNARE complex is actually quite reasonable (715 A^2) and this in itself does not suggest that this contact is induced by “crystal packing”. Secondly, the last statement “that the interactions leading to the zigzag array are not specific and are induced by crystallization” implies that crystal packing may induce non-specific interactions. To the contrary, the observation of such an interaction in different crystal forms and for different mutations of residue 34 (albeit with a register shift observed in the second crystal form for some of the non-crystallographic related molecules) actually speaks against an entirely non-specific interaction. Moreover, the crystallization conditions seem not unusual, and the pH is comparable for the crystallization and NMR experiments. There is one clear difference: in the crystal structure, two complexin molecules are interacting with the truncated SNARE complex. It is theoretically possible that binding of the central complexin helix to the SNARE complex is required in order to enable binding of the accessory helix of the second complexin molecule. Thus, we remain concerned about this difference between the crystal structures and the solution NMR studies on the same system, and the discussion must be modified to properly describe this discrepancy, and provide possible alternative explanations.

4) The authors may wish to explicitly leave wiggle room for future discovery of some such an interaction, e.g., with a sub-complex of SNAREs. For example, one could write “While current data do not support the proposed zigzag SNARE:complexin interaction, it remains possible that such an interaction might occur under some biological condition, or with some sub-complex of SNAREs.”

eLife. 2014 May 8;3:e02391. doi: 10.7554/eLife.02391.022

Author response


Thank you for the nice summary of our work and its significance. We agree that the main point of the paper is that it entirely opens the question of how complexin inhibits release, and that the new model we propose is speculative. However, we still would like to propose the model because it provides a testable hypothesis, because it led to the design of the experiments with the 5E and 3R mutants (otherwise we have no rationale for this design), and because it is supported by the electrophysiological data obtained with these mutants (see also response to point 2). Nevertheless, we present the model as a suggestion and make it clear in the discussion that the model will need to be tested with further research. Moreover, we included the term ‘steric’ in the model (in the title, abstract, etc.) because the hindrance caused by the accessory helix could arise from both electrostatic and steric effects, and because this gives further flexibility to the model for testing in the future.

With respect to the comment ‘It is even is unlikely that the accessory helix would adopt a helical conformation in the presence of the rather severe mutations of Figure 9’, glutamate and arginine are generally compatible with helical conformation, and secondary structure predictions with Jpred still predict helical conformation for the accessory helix of the 3R and 5E mutants (see below). In addition, consecutive residues with the same charge occur to some extent in the native accessory helices of mammalian and dm CpxI (Figure 8). Importantly, the 3R and 5E mutants are largely rescuing the phenotype of the complexin I-III triple KO (Figure 9). Therefore, although it is plausible that the large number of consecutive positive or negative charges in the mutants might destabilize the helix, the mutations do not cause a major impairment of the overall function of complexins.

Author response image 1.

Author response image 1.

Issues to address:

1) Although the overlaid TROSY-HSQC spectra are reasonably compelling for the general argument the authors are trying to make, it would be helpful if more specific interpretation were supported by an SI figure showing the (composite 1H/15N) amide shift changes as a function of residue number for the delta62 and delta68 truncations. Similarly, a correlation graph of CpxI shift changes (vs. full length minus random coil) for the shift changes in the N-terminus of the central helix would provide stronger support for the “increasingly flexible toward the N-terminus” statement. Alternatively, 15N relaxation measurements would make this more quantitative, but not necessary from my perspective if indeed the shift change is in the random coil direction.

We agree with the reviewers that it is desirable to provide a more detailed analysis of these data, with some quantification that supports our conclusions. Such detailed analysis is hindered by the fact that multiple cross-peaks of the Cpx(26-83) central helix cannot be unambiguously identified upon truncation of the SNARE complex, but the data for the cross-peaks that can be identified are still informative. In Figure 2–figure supplement 1A,B, we now present plots of chemical shift changes caused by truncation of synaptobrevin to residue 68 compared to the changes observed when comparing free and SNARE complex-bound Cpx(26-83).

In one plot we use the latter changes for normalization to provide a measure of how much the central helix is destabilized, while in the second plot we correlate the first changes with the latter. Based on these plots, we have changed slightly the interpretation, suggesting that ‘the N-terminal half of the accessory helix becomes flexible’ rather than ‘becomes increasingly flexible toward the N-terminus’ Note that we prefer to use the chemical shifts of free Cpx(26-83) rather than random coil chemical shifts because we believe that they provide a better measure of the shifts expected when the central helix is detached from the SNARE complex. Using random coil chemical shifts for the plot presented in Figure 2–figure supplement 1B, would yield the plot shown below and lead to analogous conclusions. We would be glad to use this plot instead if the reviewer believes that this is a better choice.

Author response image 2.

Author response image 2.

We have not performed relaxation experiments, but the cross-peak intensities do provide some information on relaxation properties and we now show plots of cross-peak intensities for Cpx(26-83) bound to SC 68 and SC 62, normalized with those of Cpx(26-83) bound to the SNARE complex (Figure 2–figure supplement 1C,D). These plots show that the intensities of the cross-peaks that can be identified from the accessory helix become stronger upon truncation of the synaptobrevin C- terminus, which provides very strong evidence against insertion of the accessory helix into the truncated complex.

2) The negative charge repulsion model is contradicted by the following experiments: the non-clamping mutants by Kummel et al. (L41E, A44E, A30E, A31E) and (A30E and A31E) are both negatively charged, but yet, they have no effect on spontaneous fusion in their cell-based fusion assay. The poor-clamp mutant (K26E, K41K, E47K, also somewhat negatively charged) by Yang et al. (Neuron, 2010) actually increases spontaneous release in neuronal cultures. These contradictions need to be addressed by additional experiments, or the negative charge model needs to be modified.

At present, there is no clear correlation between the effects of mutations in the cell-cell fusion assay and any physiological data. On the contrary, our electrophysiological data with the superclamp and K26A mutants go in opposite directions from those observed in cell-cell fusion assays. We do not think that this lack of correlation completely rules out the significance of the results obtained with these assays, since at the moment we do not understand the source for the lack of correlation. At the same time, we do not believe that the model emerging from our electrophysiological data and reasonable structural-biophysical arguments should be discarded because of the results obtained with the non-clamping mutants in the cell -cell fusion assays of Kummel et al.

With regard to the K26E, L41K, E47K studied in Yang et al. 2010 (please note that residue 41 of WT CpxI is L), the mutation increases the positive charge by 1 unit and increases spontaneous release. This result in principle correlates with our model, although the increase in spontaneous release is rather large. Overall, we feel that the available electrophysiological data with this mutant described in Yang et al. 2010, our 3R and 5E mutants, and our mutant containing the dmCpxI accessory helix provide support for the electrostatic model, even though we agree that the model is not proven (and this is how we present it in the paper).

3) As mentioned in the Discussion, point 4, Reinish et al. determined a second crystal form (with an additional mutation in complexin, F34M, in addition to the “superclamp” mutations in the accessory helix) where the accessory helix interacts in two different registers. This indicates promiscuity of the interaction between the accessory helix and the 2nd SNARE complex in the crystal structures. This point needs some clarification in the Discussion: first these two alternative forms only occur upon the additional F34M mutation. Second, it is not necessarily that crystallization has induced this interaction. It is possible that this weak interaction is a lowly state that is too infrequent to be observed by NMR, but has been stabilized in the context of the crystal structure.

We would like to clarify that F34M does not really constitute an additional mutation of the WT CpxI sequence but rather replaces one of the side chains that was already mutated in the superclamp mutant (it is D27L,E34M,R37A instead of D27L,E34F,R37A). In the discussion we have tried to explain better what is observed in the crystals of the D27L,E34M,R37A mutant, i.e. two types of interactions in crystallographically distinct complexes, one that is analogous to that observed for the D27L,E34F,R37A mutant and another that has a shift of two helical turns between the accessory helix and the groove of SC 60. We also point out in several parts of the manuscript that the interaction is not observed by NMR but could exist and is not detected because it is very weak.

4) The interaction between the accessory helix and a neighboring partially folded full SNARE complex is not ruled out by the presented experiments since they do not involve partially folded full SNARE complex. This should be noted in the paper.

We agree and have included this suggestion in the Discussion.

5) The alternative explanation of the original Kummel et al., ITC data and the additional ITC experiments are convincing. However, these data do not rule out an interaction between the accessory helix and SNARE complex per se, but it may be much weaker than estimated by the original Kummel et al. experiment. It would be prudent to add this caveat to the discussion of the ITC experiments. Did the authors attempt to conduct the competition experiment under actually saturating conditions of CpxI(47-134)?

We have performed additional ITC experiments where Cpx(26-83) was titrated over a sample of SC 60 containing 3.0 equivalents of CpxI(1-134) to better saturate the binding site. As expected, only a very small amount of heat was observed in these titrations and this heat corresponds to a natural extension of a direct titration that had reached a molar ratio of 3.0 (new Figure 4–figure supplement 1). Note that, given the sensitivity that we observe in these experiments, more full saturation that would completely eliminate any detectable heat would require a large excess of Cpx(47-134) that could promote aggregation. We believe that the overall data clearly show that no interaction of the Cpx(26-83) accessory helix with SC 60 can be detected in these ITC experiments, and this is how we now present our Conclusion.

6) Kummel et al. used bulk fret and measured the FRET between S25 Cterm (residue 193) and CX Nterm (residue 38). They showed that the more VAMP is truncated at the C-terminus, truncation of VAMP up to residue 60, the weaker the FRET efficiency. In this work, the NMR results and the weaker affinity observed by ITC suggests that the VAMP truncations make SNAP-25 and also syntaxin more flexible and unstructured, thus destabilizing the interaction with complexin as well, resulting in lower FRET. Thus, an interpretation of the NMR result is that the lower FRET efficiency is a result of motional averaging of multiple conformations that produces the appearance of a lower FRET “state”. However, Kummel et al. did an experiment to rule out this possibility: They used a 'flexible' CPX construct (CPX-GPGP), in which a helix-breaking GPGP linker was inserted between the central and accessory helices of CPX (Figure 4b,c,d, Supplemental Table 3 in Kummel et al.). To quote Kummel et al. “these experiments indicate that when CPX is bound to a half-zippered form of the SNARE complex, CPXacc has the intrinsic property of extending away from the complex. Because this conformation is maintained in solution, it determines how the complex crystallizes and not vice versa.”

We believe that the FRET data of Kummel et al. need to be reinterpreted for the following reasons:

A. As we explain in points 7 and 8 of the discussion, our NMR data indicate that the synaptobrevin truncation causes increased flexibility in the CpxI accessory helix and also induces flexibility in the CpxI central helix and SNAP-25 C-terminus, which are well structured without the truncation. Such increases in flexibility can indeed explain the decreased FRET observed upon synaptobrevin truncation in Kummel et al. 2011.

B. The R0 for the FRET pair used in Kummel et al. was calculated to be 27.5 Å. Hence, they observed good FRET efficiency for distances measured between CpxI and the non-truncated SNARE complex, which were on the order of 20-27 Å. However, the FRET efficiency decreases considerably for the longer distances measured using the truncated SNARE complex (34-42 Å) (values taken from Supplementary Table 3 of Kummel et al.). Hence, it is much more difficult to obtain reliable distances and strong conclusions for these data.

C. Regardless of the issues mentioned in points A and B, the most important point arises from the interpretation of the data obtained with the flexible CPX-GPGP construct mentioned by the reviewer. Kummel et al. concluded that there was no detectable FRET signal for CPX-GPGP (labeled at residue 38) bound to either the truncated or non-truncated SNARE complex, referring to the data shown in Figure 4d. However, inspection of Figure 4d shows that there was actually a small decrease in the donor fluorescence in the experiments performed with CPX-GPGP, compared to the donor only control. While this decrease is small, it is comparable to that observed for CPX with the acceptor on residue 31 bound to the truncated SNARE complex (cyan curve in Figure 4c), which was used in Supplementary Table 3 to calculate a distance of 42 Å. Note also that the FRET efficiency observed for Cpx labeled at residue 38 (without the flexible GPGP insertion) and bound to the truncated SNARE complex was rather low and yielded a distance of 34 Å (Figures 4b,c and Supplementary Table 3 of Kummel et al.). Hence, the difference in the distances that can be calculated from the FRET data presented for CPX and CPX-GPGP bound to the truncated SNARE complex is just 8 Å, a rather short distance considering the large errors expected for such low FRET efficiencies. Moreover, the insertion of the GPGP sequence between the central and accessory helices is expected to push the fluorescence probe placed in the Cpx accessory helix away from the probe on the truncated SNARE complex, and it is reasonable for a flexible sequence of four residues such as GPGP to span a distance of approximately 8 Å. Hence, the experiments performed with GPGP do not really rule out the flexibility argument.

In the revised paper we have expanded the discussion of the FRET data to explain these points, while trying to shorten the text in other parts to limit the length of the already long discussion.

[Editors' note: further clarifications were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

1) We are disappointed that the authors did not address the question if the charge-altering mutations of the accessory helix retain the helical character of the accessory helix (suggested by the strong alpha-helical chemical shifts of the accessory helix by Pabst et al., 2000 J.Biol.Chem). It is certainly possible that this happens, but the data presented here do not provide direct (e.g., NMR) evidence for this notion. Moreover, the proposed model of electrostatic-steric hindrance by the accessory helix remains speculative. It is of course fine to provide such a speculative model in the Discussion section of the paper. However, the title is misleading since it suggests that the model has been definitely proven in this work. Rather, the main focus of this paper is a re-examination of the interaction between the accessory helix and the truncated SNARE complex that was previously studied by Kummel et al. Thus, we request a change of title to properly reflect the main focus of this work.

We are sorry that we did not properly address the issue of the potential effects of the charge-altering mutations on the helicity of the accessory helix. Since performing NMR analyses of the mutants would unduly delay publication of the paper, to address the concern related to the potential helix disruption by these mutations we have added the following sentence in the Discussion. However, these results need to be interpreted with caution, since it is plausible that the 5E and 3R mutations may alter the helical character of the accessary helix.

In addition, to address the concern regarding the title of the manuscript, we have changed the title to: 'Re-examining how complexin inhibits neurotransmitter release: SNARE complex insertion or electrostatic hindrance?'

2) The statement in the Results section: “these cross-peaks actually increased in the spectrum of Cpx(26-83) bound to SCdelta68 with respect to the SC-bound state, and increased somewhat more in the SCdelta62-bound spectrum (Figure 2–figure supplements 1C,D) These data show that the synaptobrevin C-terminal truncations increase the flexibility of the accessory helix, in correlation with the destabilization of N terminal part of the central helix,”. The best resolved accessory helix resonances (A30 and Q38) actually do not seem to increase from SCdelta68 to SCdelta62. Thus, the statements about the increase in flexibility of the accessory helix need to be qualified.

In the original and first revised manuscript, we had adjusted the contour levels of Figures 2B,C to enable visualization of the weakest cross-peaks of interest. For the expansions showing the cross-peaks of A30 and Q38 in Figures 2D, E, we increased the contour levels proportionately to facilitate observation of the positions of the cross-peaks from the different spectra. Hence, the cross-peak intensities that can be deduced from these plots cannot be directly compared among the spectra. This choice was not meant in any way to bias or confuse the reader, but we realize that it has caused confusion for the review and we are sorry about that. To avoid this confusion in the new revised manuscript and still enable the reader to see the progressive shifts of cross - peaks in Figures 2B, C, without overcrowding the center of the spectra, we have left the same contour levels in Figures 2B, C, but we have adjusted the levels of the expansions of Figures 2D,E to allow direct comparison of cross-peak intensities. We have explained these choices of contour levels in the figure legend. The expansions of Figures 2D, E now show that the cross-peaks of A30 and Q38 are considerably stronger for CpxI(26- 83) bound to SC 62 and SC 68 than for CpxI(26-83) bound to SC, as shown quantitatively in Figure 2–Figure supplements 1C,D.

3) The discussion of the Kummel at al. crystal structure (point 4 in the Discussion) remains a concern. Firstly, the area of the interface between the accessory helix and the truncated SNARE complex is actually quite reasonable (715 A^2) and this in itself does not suggest that this contact is induced by “crystal packing”. Secondly, the last statement “that the interactions leading to the zigzag array are not specific and are induced by crystallization” implies that crystal packing may induce non-specific interactions. To the contrary, the observation of such an interaction in different crystal forms and for different mutations of residue 34 (albeit with a register shift observed in the second crystal form for some of the non-crystallographic related molecules) actually speaks against an entirely non-specific interaction. Moreover, the crystallization conditions seem not unusual, and the pH is comparable for the crystallization and NMR experiments. There is one clear difference: in the crystal structure, two complexin molecules are interacting with the truncated SNARE complex. It is theoretically possible that binding of the central complexin helix to the SNARE complex is required in order to enable binding of the accessory helix of the second complexin molecule. Thus, we remain concerned about this difference between the crystal structures and the solution NMR studies on the same system, and the discussion must be modified to properly describe this discrepancy, and provide possible alternative explanations.

We agree that some of the arguments we used to suggest that the interaction mode observed in the crystals arises from non-specific interactions are not very compelling. Furthermore, it is unclear what can be considered a specific or non-specific interaction in the context of this discussion. Hence, we have removed any mention of non-specific interactions and have discussed the available experimental data on this issue trying to keep an objective perspective.

Among other issues, we now mention that ‘…although the interface area with the SNAREs is larger for the accessory helix (ca. 900 Ă2 calculated with PISA; Krissinel and Henrick, 2007) than for the central helix (ca. 540 Ă2 ), the atomic B-factors of the residues of the accessory helix in the interface with the SNAREs are much larger than those in the central helix interface, with little electronic density for the side chains of the accessory helix interface (Figure 1–figure supplement 2). Interestingly, it has been suggested that motion at a crystal packing interface is intermediate between that of a solvent accessible surface and that of a protein core, even for large interfaces (Carugo and Argos, 1997).’

Since our NMR analyses had focused much more on WT CpxI, but the crystal structure of Kummel et al. was obtained with the superclamp mutant, we have performed additional NMR experiments using the superclamp mutant (supcl). In particular, we have acquired 1H-15N TROSY-HSQC spectra of 2H,15N-CpxI(26-83)supcl in the absence and presence of SC 60 (presented in the new Figure 2–figure supplement 2) . For comparison, we have also acquired parallel spectra with WT 2H,15 N-CpxI(26-83) (shown in Figure 2F). As we describe in the manuscript, we obtained very similar results for the WT and supcl proteins, and we were unable to detect an interaction of the accessory helix with SC 60. However, we cannot rule out the possibility that there is a weak interaction in solution that becomes stabilized by crystallization.

4) The authors may wish to explicitly leave wiggle room for future discovery of some such an interaction, e.g., with a sub-complex of SNAREs. For example, one could write “While current data do not support the proposed zigzag SNARE:complexin interaction, it remains possible that such an interaction might occur under some biological condition, or with some sub-complex of SNAREs.”

We agree that it is desirable to leave such wiggle room. We believe that such wiggle room is left in the following sentences included in the Discussion; it might be premature to completely rule out these models given the complexity of this system. Note for instance that our NMR and ITC data were obtained with truncated SNARE complexes in solution and hence do not rule out the possibility that the CpxI accessory helix interacts with trans-SNARE complexes partially assembled between membranes. We feel that adding the suggested sentence would be redundant, but would be glad to add it if this required for acceptance of the manuscript.


Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

RESOURCES