SUMMARY
This study compared parent-reported sleep characteristics in 2 to 5 year-old children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) to children with other developmental delays (DD) and typical development (TD). We included 529 children (303 ASD [167 males], 63 DD [46 males], and 163 TD [134 males]) enrolled in the CHARGE study, an ongoing population-based case-control study. The mean age of participants was 3.6 years (standard deviation 0.8 years). ASD diagnosis was confirmed with Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) and Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedules (ADOS). Cognitive and adaptive functioning was assessed using Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) and Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (VABS), respectively. Demographic, medical and sleep history information were ascertained from California birth records, telephone interview, medical assessments at clinic visit, and parent-administered questionnaires. Fifty-three percent of children with ASDs had at least one frequent sleep problem, followed by 46% of children with DDs, and 32% of the TD group (P < 0.0001). Exploratory factor analyses of sleep history data yielded two factors: sleep onset problems and night waking. Children with ASD had marginally higher sleep onset factor scores and significantly higher night waking factor scores compared to the TD group. Factor scores for children with DD were intermediate between the ASD and TD groups. Cognitive or adaptive development did not predict severity of sleep problems in the ASD group.
Keywords: autism, autistic spectrum disorder, sleep problems, developmental delay
INTRODUCTION
Sleep problems are commonly reported by parents of young children. Approximately 25% of typically developing preschool-aged children have sleep problems pertaining to bedtime resistance, sleep onset delays, and night awakenings (Scher et al., 1995; Armstrong et al., 1994; Johnson, 1991; Jenkins et al., 1984). Other less common sleep disturbances that concern parents include night terrors or nightmares and repetitive rhythmic behaviors, which manifest during the toddler-preschool period in some children (Anders et al., 2000). Finally, 1–3% of children experience sleep-disordered breathing (Gislason & Benediktsdottir, 1995; Ali et al., 1993), a medical condition that also contributes to a reduced quality of sleep. Sleep problems have been broadly classified into three major categories (American Psychiatric Association, 1994; American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2001): (1) dyssomnias include sleep onset delays and night awakenings, (2) parasomnias include behaviors involving motor and autonomic activation which intrude on sleep (e.g. night terrors, nightmares, repetitive rhythmic behaviors), and (3) sleep disorders secondary to a physical illness or another psychological disorder. The extant literature has focused primarily on the presence of dyssomnias in both typically developing children as well as clinical samples (Sadeh et al., 2002; Wiggs & Stores, 2001; Malow, 2004).
Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are pervasive developmental disorders characterized by deficits in social interaction, language development and communication, as well as restricted and stereotyped behavior patterns or interests. Children with ASD experience similar sleep problem types to those observed in typically developing children, although the existing literature suggests that the prevalence of sleep problems in autism is markedly higher than expected, occurring in 44–83% of school-age children (Richdale, 2001). Richdale and Prior (1995) compared preschool- and school-aged children with ASD to a normative sample and found significantly higher levels of current general sleep difficulties according to parent report (44% versus 27%). Parents of autistic children commonly express that their children resist going to bed at an appropriate time (Couturier et al., 2005; Wiggs & Stores, 2004), have difficulties in initiating sleep (Richdale & Prior, 1995; Couturier et al., 2005; Wiggs & Stores, 2004; Allik et al., 2006b; Gail Williams et al., 2004; Honomichl et al., 2002; Patzold et al., 1998), and exhibit nighttime (Richdale & Prior, 1995; Wiggs & Stores, 2004; Allik et al., 2006b; Gail Williams et al., 2004; Honomichl et al., 2002; Patzold et al., 1998; Schreck & Mulick, 2000), and early morning waking (Richdale & Prior, 1995; Patzold et al., 1998; Polimeni et al., 2005; Hering et al., 1999).
There is a bidirectional impact between sleep problems and child development. Sleep disturbances increase the parenting burden and family stress (Sadeh et al., 2002; Polimeni et al., 2005; Doo & Wing, 2006; Wiggs & Stores, 1998; Mindell & Durand, 1993). Recently, in a large population-based study, mothers of typically developing preschoolers with a parent-reported sleep problem also endorsed poor general health (Martin et al., 2007). Family stress was associated with poorer sleep quality in school-aged children with typical development and in children with developmental disabilities (Wiggs & Stores, 1998; El-Sheikh et al., 2006).
Other results indicate that parental hassle intensity is associated with sleep problems in children with autism (Richdale et al., 2000). Additionally, there is accumulating evidence for a relationship between sleep and developmental functioning in children (Sadeh et al., 2002; Wolfson et al., 1998). Disturbed sleep in children has been associated with cognitive, verbal and attention impairments (Fallone et al., 2005; Randazzo et al., 1998; Gozal, 1998; Quine, 1991), self-help and daytime behavior problems (Quine, 1991; Allik et al., 2006a) including injury-prone behavior (Owens et al., 2005). Daytime dysfunction due to disturbed sleep patterns may be especially taxing for children with neurodevelopmental disorders such as ASD, who already have difficulties in social interaction and communication.
Most research on sleep problems in children has used subjective measures such as sleep diaries and parent-reported questionnaires. Although biases are introduced when utilizing this method, parent reports remain an established measure of sleep behavior in children. Honomichl et al. (2002) found that children whose parents reported a sleep problem, in contrast to those who did not, consistently displayed more delayed sleep onsets and greater sleep fragmentation, which was reflected in subscale scores of the Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ; Owens et al., 2000), a validated measure of sleep problems and sleep diaries. Parents may be unaware of certain sleep behaviors, and thus, underreport them if their child does not alert them in any way. Several studies have demonstrated parental underreporting of sleep problems using objective measures (e.g. actigraphy, videosomnography) (Allik et al., 2006b; Sadeh, 1996; Minde et al., 1993; Sadeh et al., 1991), although Hering et al. (1999) found that parental reports of sleep problems were overstated when compared to actigraphic recordings. In general, parent reports of their children’s sleep problems appear to be a conservative but reliable measure (Minde et al., 1993).
The underlying cause of poor sleep may include behavioral factors, medical factors or an interaction of both factors. Medical conditions that can impact sleep patterns in children include the presence of seizures, cerebral palsy, sleep disordered breathing, and pharmacologic side effects (Owens et al., 1998; Newman et al., 2006). A recent report indicated a significant association between the presence of insomnia and parasomnias with gastrointestinal symptoms as reported by parents (Liu et al., 2006). Children with ASD in particular may be prescribed medications, e.g. respiridone, that may have a disruptive impact on sleep (Mindell & Owens, 2003).
Research investigating sleep patterns in children with ASD has documented parental concerns, although shortcomings in the existing literature include small sample sizes, inconsistent diagnostic definitions, and convenience samples of controls. None of the studies to date have used a population-based sample for comparison, thus limiting both the validity and the generalizability of their findings. The aim of this report was to compare parent-reported sleep characteristics in young children with ASD between 2 and 5 years of age to typically developing children of the same age as well as children with other developmental delays using a large, well-characterized population-based study sample.
METHOD
Participants
Participants (n= 529) were enrolled in the CHARGE (Childhood Autism Risks from Genetics and the Environment) Study. Eligible children were those who: a) were between the ages of 24 and 60 months, b) were living with at least one biologic parent, c) had a parent who spoke English or Spanish, d) were born in California, and e) were residing in the catchment areas of a specified list of Regional Centers in California. The CHARGE Study is an ongoing population-based case-control study with subjects sampled from three strata: children with autism, children with developmental delay but not autism, and children selected from the general population (Hertz-Picciotto et al., 2006). Children with autism as well as children with other developmental delays were identified through Regional Centers, which are funded by the State of California to provide case management services to children with eligible developmental disorders, regardless of socioeconomic level and racial/ethnic group. Children from the general population were identified from state birth files, and a stratified random sample was generated by frequency-matching to a projected distribution of cases on age, sex, and Regional Center catchment area. Parents were contacted by letter and/or phone to describe the study. Recruitment began in April 2003, and children in this paper include those enrolled from April 2003 through December 2006. During this period of recruitment, clinical assessment and other relevant data were available on 677 children who were examined by the CHARGE study at a clinic site in Sacramento or Los Angeles. From this pool of children, 592 sleep histories were obtained (87.4%). Fifteen children did not complete all developmental assessments to satisfy group definitions in this paper and 48 did not meet the group definition criteria; thus, these 63 children were excluded from all analyses. No further exclusions were made based on genetics, family phenotype, or other characteristics, with the exception of children who had visual, hearing or motor impairments that precluded standardized developmental assessment. This study was approved by institutional review boards of the University of California in Davis and Los Angeles and the State of California Department of Developmental Services. Informed consent was obtained prior to participation. The final sample of 529 participants included 303 children with ASD, 63 with developmental delays, and 163 with typical development.
Diagnostic confirmation
All children with autism were evaluated with the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R; Le Couteur et al., 2003) and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedules (ADOS; Lord et al., 2000) to confirm their diagnosis. Final autism case status was defined as meeting criteria on the communication, social interaction, and repetitive behavior domains of the ADI-R with onset prior to 36 months and scoring at or above the social + communication cutoff for autism on the ADOS module 1 or 2. Children classified with autism spectrum disorders were those who did not meet full criteria for autism on either or both the ADI-R and ADOS, but did meet criteria on either the communication or the social interaction domain of the ADI-R prior to 36 months, were within 2 points of the cut-off on the other domain, and were above the social + communication cutoff for ASD on the ADOS module 1 or 2. Children with autism and autism spectrum disorders were combined into one group (ASD group). Additionally, cases were also administered the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL; Mullen, 1995) and the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (VABS; Sparrow et al., 1984) to determine cognitive and adaptive status.
Parents of children with other developmental delays and those from the general population completed the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ; Rutter et al., 2003) to screen for autism spectrum disorders; children with scores above the screening cut-off of 15 were assessed with the ADI-R and ADOS, and removed from the comparison groups. All children were assessed using MSEL and VABS, and comparison groups were defined using composite scores on both assessments. The delayed development group (DD group) was defined as having a score of 70 or less on MSEL and/or a score of 69 or less on VABS. The typically developing group (TD group) only included children from the general population who had a score of 71 or above on MSEL and a score of 70 or above on VABS.
All CHARGE study clinical assessment personnel have attained research reliability on all developmental assessments (ADI-R, ADOS, MSEL, and VABS); the team includes Spanish bilingual and bicultural staff.
Sleep problems and covariates
Information on current sleep behavior, habits, and patterns was ascertained with the CHARGE Sleep History, a parent-administered questionnaire that includes 39 Likert scale items (0 = never; 1 = rarely; 2 = sometimes; 3 = frequently; 4 = always) describing sleep behavior, habits, and patterns (Table 1). Other information included sleep duration and parental evaluation of their child’s sleep problems on the child and family’s daily functioning. This study only focused on the 30 items that were similar to the items found in the Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ; Owens et al., 2000).
Table 1.
Sleep History items (0 = Never 1=Rarely 2=Sometimes 3=Frequently 4=Always) | |
• Wakes up screaming during the night | • Would stay in bed until gotten up |
• Screams during the second half of the night | • Is sleepy during the day |
• Throws tantrums at bedtime | • Goes to bed at different times |
• Sleeps better in places other than his/her bed | • Walks in sleep |
• Gets less than 6 hours sleep in a 24 hour period | • Rocks body, bangs head, or rocks head in sleep |
• Talks in sleep without knowing it | • Snores |
• Sleeps more than other children his/her age | • Resists going to bed at night |
• Is sluggish or irritable when awakened | • Sleeps less than children his/her age |
• Has trouble falling asleep | • Has frequent let movements while asleep |
• Takes frequent naps during the day | • Sleeps in a family bed |
• Stops breathing during sleep | • Has nightmares |
• Complains of the room or bed being uncomfortable | • Wakes up screaming approximately 2 hours after going to sleep |
• Sleeps longer or shorter periods on the weekends versus the weekdays | • Needs a stuffed animal, toy, blanket, pacifier, night light to fall asleep |
• Engages in violent behavior while asleep (hits, kicks, punches) | • Expresses fear about going to sleep or is afraid to fall asleep |
• Complains of jaw pain during the day or grinds teeth at night | • Takes naps without being told or at an age when naps are not normal |
How many hours does your child sleep per night? | |
How many hours does your child nap during the day? | |
How many hours does you child sleep in 24 hours? |
Do you think your child has a sleeping problem that affects his/her daily functioning? | Yes | No |
Do you think your child has a sleeping problem that affects your family’s daily functioning? | Yes | No |
Demographic (child race/ethnicity, maternal age, paternal age, parental education) and reproductive information (birth weight, gestational age, birth order) was obtained from the CHARGE Environmental Exposure Questionnaire and from California birth records. The Environmental Exposure Questionnaire (EEQ) is a telephone-administered interview with the mother and the primary caregiver designed by the CHARGE study, and includes questions about demographics, maternal medical history, and various environmental exposures such as diet, household product use, residence, and occupational history. Children’s histories of seizures and current use of medications and supplements were obtained from the Child Medical History, a structured questionnaire administered by physicians. Information on current gastrointestinal symptoms was collected using the CHARGE GI History, a parent-administered questionnaire that includes 10 Likert scale items (0 = never; 1 = rarely; 2 = sometimes; 3 = frequently; 4 = always) describing current gastrointestinal symptoms as well as questions related to food allergies and diet restrictions. (All CHARGE study forms available from authors on request.)
Covariates of interest were selected a priori and included child’s age in years, sex, mother’s level of education (college degree versus no degree), history of non-febrile seizures, current use of medications for chronic or recurrent conditions (e.g. ADHD, allergies, anxiety, asthma, constipation, hyperacidity, insomnia, mood disorders, seizures, thyroid abnormalities), current use of dietary supplements (excluding multivitamins and fluoride), current gastrointestinal symptoms (frequent gaseousness/bloating, diarrhea, or constipation), and birth order (first born versus later born).
Statistical Analyses
All analyses were carried out using SAS version 9.1 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC).
Descriptive statistics
Comparisons of categorical variables were statistically evaluated using a Likelihood Ratio Chi-square test, and continuous variables were assessed using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) or Kruskal-Wallis test.
Principal Factor Analysis
To identify clusters of sleep problems on the CHARGE Sleep History, an exploratory factor analysis was performed on all participants (n=529). To be included in the factor analysis, responses to all 30 items must be present. Missing data on item responses on the CHARGE Sleep History questionnaire occurred rarely. Eighty-three of 529 Sleep History surveys (15.7%) had one or more missing responses. Specifically, 49 out of 529 surveys were missing 1 item response (9%), 16 were missing 2 responses (3%), and 18 were missing 3 or more responses (3%). The maximum number of missing responses was 7 out of 30. To avoid excluding subjects from the entire factor analysis because of one or a few missing items, values were imputed using median values of the entire sample of children. Kaiser's measure of sampling adequacy (MSA; Cerny & Kaiser, 1977) was used to quantify the goodness-of-fit of the principal factor model; squared multiple correlations (SMCs) were used as the prior communality estimates. Items with factor loadings greater than 0.40 were included and orthogonal (varimax) rotations were performed. Factor-scoring coefficients were generated to be used in regression models.
Based on the SMC ranges (0.22 – 0.65), we used the exploratory factor analysis instead of the principal components analysis, which assumes all SMCs to be near 1.0. Kaiser’s MSA was 0.82, suggesting an adequate goodness-of-fit of the factor analysis model. (MSA ranges between 1.0 and 0, with values less than 0.50 considered a poor fit.) Varied factor-loading thresholds were examined with the optimal threshold yielding correlations greater than 0.40. The most favorable number of factors was two, based on statistical criteria and factor interpretability. These two factors accounted for 95% of the variance present in the sleep characteristics retained in the model.
Multivariate Linear Regression Analyses
All covariates were assessed for potential confounding by examining associations of each covariate with the primary predictor of interest (group) and with each response variable (factor scores, total amount of sleep); covariates broadly associated (p < 0.20) with both the predictor and the response variable were initially included in the multivariate models. Tolerance was calculated to assess collinearity among variables fitted in each model; a tolerance value of <0.10 was considered as an indicator for collinearity. Child’s sex and age were included in all models to adjust for the study design. Final multivariate linear regression models were fitted to determine whether sleep factors derived from the exploratory factor analysis and total amount of sleep by parent report were associated with the three main diagnostic groups: ASD, developmental delay, and typical development. A secondary set of regression analyses of the ASD group alone was performed to determine whether sleep factors and total amount of sleep were associated with delay in cognitive or adaptive development. Three sets of models were constructed to determine differences comparing: ASD children with delayed versus ASD children with typical cognitive development; ASD children with delayed versus ASD children with typical adaptive development; and ASD children with delayed cognitive and adaptive development versus ASD children with typical development on one or both measures.
RESULTS
The mean age of children included in the analyses (n = 529) was 3.6 years (SD 0.8); TD children were significantly younger compared to the DD (3.3 years versus 3.6 years, P = 0.04) or the ASD (3.3 years versus 3.7 years, P < 0.0001) groups. Males represented 88% (n = 167) in the ASD group, 73 % (n = 46) in the DD group, and 82% (n = 134) in the TD group (P = 0.01). Overall, close to half the children were White, one-third Hispanic, and the remainder of other race/ethnicity (Table 2). Mothers of DD children had the lowest level of education: 27% with college degree, as compared to mothers of ASD (45%, P = 0.01) or of TD (52%, P = 0.001) children (Table 2).
Table 2.
ASD (n=303) |
DD (n=63) |
TD (n=163) |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Characteristic | n | % | n | % | n | % | P value |
Child’s age, yrs | |||||||
2 | 71 | 23.4 | 14 | 22.2 | 60 | 36.8 | <.0001 |
3 | 102 | 33.7 | 28 | 44.5 | 62 | 38.1 | |
4 | 108 | 35.6 | 21 | 33.3 | 41 | 25.1 | |
5 | 22 | 7.3 | 0 | 0.0 | 0 | 0.0 | |
Sex, % male | 267 | 88.1 | 46 | 73.0 | 134 | 82.2 | .01 |
Race/Ethnicity | |||||||
White | 155 | 51.2 | 25 | 39.7 | 78 | 47.9 | .32 |
Hispanic | 98 | 32.3 | 28 | 44.4 | 52 | 31.9 | |
Other | 50 | 16.5 | 10 | 15.9 | 33 | 20.2 | |
Maternal age at birth of child, y | |||||||
<25 | 36 | 11.9 | 13 | 20.6 | 31 | 19.0 | .12 |
25–34 | 187 | 61.7 | 39 | 61.9 | 93 | 57.1 | |
≥35 | 80 | 26.4 | 11 | 17.5 | 39 | 23.9 | |
Paternal age at birth of child, y | |||||||
<25 | 24 | 8.1 | 9 | 15.0 | 22 | 13.7 | .21 |
25–34 | 144 | 48.6 | 31 | 51.7 | 78 | 48.4 | |
≥35 | 128 | 43.2 | 20 | 33.3 | 61 | 37.9 | |
Mother’s level of education | |||||||
≤High school | 38 | 12.5 | 20 | 31.7 | 33 | 20.3 | <.0001 |
Some college | 130 | 42.9 | 26 | 41.3 | 46 | 28.2 | |
≥College degree | 135 | 44.6 | 17 | 27.0 | 84 | 51.5 | |
Highest level of educational attainment in household | |||||||
≤High school | 24 | 7.9 | 14 | 22.2 | 20 | 12.3 | .006 |
Some college | 97 | 32.0 | 25 | 39.7 | 52 | 31.9 | |
≥College degree | 182 | 60.1 | 24 | 38.1 | 91 | 55.8 | |
Birth weight, g | |||||||
<2500 | 22 | 7.3 | 8 | 12.7 | 9 | 5.5 | .21 |
≥2500 | 281 | 92.7 | 55 | 87.3 | 154 | 94.5 | |
Gestational age, wk | |||||||
<37 | 33 | 12.3 | 5 | 8.5 | 18 | 12.2 | .68 |
≥37 | 236 | 87.7 | 54 | 91.5 | 130 | 87.8 | |
Birth order | |||||||
1st born | 135 | 44.6 | 21 | 33.3 | 75 | 46.0 | .19 |
2nd born or later | 168 | 55.4 | 42 | 66.7 | 88 | 54.0 |
Data are given as percentage of each group unless otherwise indicated. Numbers may not add up to total due to sporadic missing data. Percentages may not total 100 because of rounding. P-values were calculated using likelihood ratio Chi-square test.
About one-fifth of children with DD had a history of non-febrile seizures compared to children with ASD (6.6%, P = 0.002) or children with typical development (0%, P < 0.0001); although the proportion of children with ASD and history of non-febrile seizures was also significantly higher than that of children in the TD group (P < 0.0001, Table 3). Current use of medications and/or dietary supplements was significantly higher in children with ASD (34%) and DD (48%) compared to TD controls (17%; P < 0.0001, P < 0.0001, respectively). Similarly, frequent gastrointestinal symptoms such as gaseousness/bloating, diarrhea and/or constipation were reported more frequently in the ASD (30%) and DD (21%) groups compared to the TD group (5.1%; P < 0.0001, P = 0.0008, respectively, Table 3).
Table 3.
ASD (n=303) |
DD (n=63) |
TD (n=163) |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Characteristic | n | % | n | % | n | % | P value |
History of seizures | |||||||
Febrile seizure | 18 | 6.6 | 2 | 3.4 | 3 | 1.9 | .06 |
Non-febrile seizures | 18 | 6.6 | 12 | 20.7 | 0 | 0.0 | <.0001 |
Any seizures | 28 | 10.3 | 12 | 20.7 | 3 | 1.9 | <.0001 |
Current use of medications or supplements for:§ | |||||||
Allergic disorders | 24 | 8.9 | 3 | 5.2 | 10 | 6.4 | .48 |
Central nervous system | 22 | 8.1 | 11 | 19.0 | 1 | 0.6 | <.0001 |
Endocrine system | 1 | 0.4 | 3 | 5.2 | 2 | 1.3 | .04 |
Gastrointestinal tract | 11 | 4.1 | 5 | 8.6 | 2 | 1.3 | .04 |
Respiratory tract | 14 | 5.2 | 10 | 17.2 | 12 | 7.7 | .01 |
Dietary supplements | 46 | 17.0 | 9 | 15.5 | 7 | 4.5 | .0002 |
Any medications/supplements | 93 | 34.3 | 28 | 48.3 | 26 | 16.7 | <.0001 |
Current gastrointestinal symptoms | |||||||
Gaseousness/bloating | 29 | 10.5 | 3 | 4.8 | 3 | 2.0 | .0014 |
Diarrhea | 44 | 14.8 | 5 | 7.9 | 2 | 1.3 | <.0001 |
Constipation | 42 | 14.1 | 10 | 15.9 | 5 | 3.2 | .0002 |
Any gastrointestinal symptoms | 91 | 30.4 | 13 | 20.6 | 8 | 5.1 | <.0001 |
Cognitive function, ≤70 | 237 | 79.8 | 60 | 95.2 | 0 | 0.0 | <.0001 |
Adaptive function, <70 | 220 | 73.1 | 44 | 69.8 | 0 | 0.0 | <.0001 |
Combined low cognitive/adaptive function | 193 | 65.4 | 41 | 65.1 | 0 | 0.0 | <.0001 |
Normal IQ† | 39 | 13.2 | 0 | 0.0 | 163 | 100 | <.0001 |
Data are given as percentage of each group unless otherwise indicated. Numbers may not add up to total due to sporadic missing data. Percentages may not total 100 because of rounding. P-values were calculated using likelihood ratio Chi-square test.
excluded medications for transient infections/conditions; also excluded multivitamins and fluoride
Normal intelligence is defined as Mullen Scales of Early Learning composite score >70 and Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales composite score ≥70
Overall, 53% of children with ASD and 46% of DD children had at least one sleep problem that was reported as occurring “frequently” or “always” compared to 32% of TD children (P < 0.0001, P < 0.05, respectively, Table 4). In the ASD group, 21% of parents perceived that their child’s sleep problem affected the child’s daily functioning and 23% of parents reported that their child’s sleep problem affected the family’s daily functioning. Similarly, 20% of children in the DD group had a sleep problem that affected their daily functioning and 21% of children in this group had a sleep problem that affected their family’s daily functioning. In contrast, 1.2% of children in the TD group had a sleep problem that affected their daily functioning and 2.5% had sleep problems that affected their family’s daily functioning (AU versus TD, P < 0.0001; DD versus TD, P < 0.0001, Table 4). The majority of children slept between 10 and 12 hours in a 24 hour period, but TD children tended to get one additional hour of sleep compared to children with ASD (median: 11 versus 12 hours, P < 0.0001).
Table 4.
ASD (n=303) |
DD (n=63) |
TD (n=163) |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | % | n | % | n | % | P value | |
Summarized sleep patterns | |||||||
Any frequent sleep problems | 159 | 52.5 | 29 | 46.0 | 52 | 31.9 | <.0001 |
Any frequent sleep onset problems | 155 | 51.2 | 29 | 46.0 | 52 | 31.9 | .0003 |
Any frequent night waking problems | 30 | 9.9 | 2 | 3.2 | 3 | 1.8 | .0007 |
Does sleep problem affect child’s daily functioning | 61 | 20.7 | 12 | 19.7 | 2 | 1.2 | <.0001 |
Does sleep problem affect family’s daily functioning | 69 | 23.1 | 13 | 21.3 | 4 | 2.5 | <.0001 |
Factor 1. Sleep onset problems | |||||||
Has trouble falling asleep | 74 | 24.4 | 12 | 19.0 | 14 | 8.6 | <.0001 |
Goes to bed at different times | 67 | 22.1 | 20 | 31.7 | 21 | 12.9 | .0035 |
Resists going to bed at night | 66 | 21.8 | 10 | 15.9 | 26 | 15.9 | .23 |
Sleeps better in places other than his/her bed | 53 | 17.5 | 11 | 17.5 | 14 | 8.6 | .02 |
Sleeps less than children his/her own age | 46 | 15.2 | 6 | 9.5 | 8 | 4.9 | .0018 |
Throws tantrums at bedtime | 30 | 9.9 | 6 | 9.5 | 5 | 3.1 | .01 |
Gets less than 6 hours sleep in a 24 hour period | 7 | 2.3 | 1 | 1.6 | 0 | 0.0 | <.05 |
Factor 2. Night waking | |||||||
Wakes up screaming during the night | 20 | 6.6 | 1 | 1.6 | 1 | 0.6 | .0014 |
Screams during the second half of the night | 19 | 6.3 | 1 | 1.6 | 1 | 0.6 | .0022 |
Has nightmares | 13 | 4.3 | 1 | 1.6 | 1 | 0.6 | .03 |
Wakes up screaming approximately 2 hours after going to sleep | 8 | 2.6 | 1 | 1.6 | 3 | 1.8 | .79 |
Data are given as percentage of each group unless otherwise indicated. Numbers may not add up to total due to sporadic missing data. Percentages may not total 100 because of rounding. P-values were calculated using likelihood ratio Chi-square test.
Exploratory Factor Analysis
The exploratory factor analysis produced two factors involving 11 sleep history items which were retained for further analysis. The factors were: Sleep Onset Problems [7 items] and Night Waking [4 items] (Table 4). Internal consistency (i.e., Cronbach α, an index of reliability for a set of items that indicates the extent to which items measure the same characteristic) was calculated for each factor and was within conventional levels (Sleep Onset Problems α = 0.79 and Night Waking α = 0.81). “Has trouble falling asleep” was the most common item in factor 1, sleep onset problems, reported as occurring frequently in 24% of children with ASD, 19% of DDs, and 9% of TDs (P < 0.0001, Table 4). In factor 2, night waking, parents of 7% of ASDs, 2% of DDs, and 1% of TDs indicated the item “Wakes up screaming during the night” as occurring frequently (P = 0.0014, Table 4).
The item scores were evaluated for a possible cut-off total associated with significantly elevated, or “high”, factor scores (defined as a factor score one standard deviation or higher above the mean). The sum of item scores that corresponded to “high” factor scores in all children were selected as cut-offs. On factor 1, sleep onset problems, the maximum sum of items was 28 points, and 15 or more item score points corresponded to a “high” factor score. Overall, 53 (10%) participants met this cut-off of 15 or more points on sleep onset items. The largest proportion of elevated sleep onset problems was in those with ASD (n = 41, 13%) followed by participants with DD (n = 6, 9.5%) and TD (n = 6, 3.7%). On factor 2, night waking, a sum of items equal to 7 or more points (out of 16 points total) corresponded to a “high” factor score, and 8.5% of participants (n = 45) met this point cut-off. The ASD group had the highest proportion of participants with elevated item scores (n = 34, 11%), followed by 6.3% participants with DD (n = 4), and 4.3% with TD (n = 7).
Multivariate Linear Regression Analyses
The primary set of regression analyses focused on comparing children with ASD, DD and TD on (1) sleep onset factor scores, (2) night waking factor scores, and (3) hours of sleep in a 24-hour period with adjustment for covariates. In a model adjusted for child’s sex, age, current use of allergy, CNS and respiratory medications, and frequent gastrointestinal symptoms, children with ASD had higher sleep onset factor scores compared with TD children, although the difference was of borderline statistical significance (β [coefficient] = 0.177, SE [standard error] = 0.091, P = 0.05). Higher sleep onset scores, indicating more sleep onset problems, were associated with CNS medication use (β = 0.366, SE = 0.153, P = 0.02) and frequent gastrointestinal symptoms (β = 0.385, SE = 0.099, P = 0.0001) in all groups whereas respiratory medication use was significantly associated with lower sleep onset factor scores (β = −0.370, SE = 0.152, P = 0.01). Children with DD did not differ significantly from the ASD or TD groups on sleep onset factor scores. Night waking factor scores were significantly elevated in the ASD group compared to TD children (β = 0.294, SE = 0.090, P = 0.001) after adjusting for child’s sex, age, allergy and respiratory medications, and birth order. Factor scores significantly decreased by 0.16 for each year increase in a child’s age (β = −0.164, SE = 0.050, P = 0.001). Use of respiratory medications was also significantly associated with lower night waking factor scores (β = −0.347, SE = 0.159, P = 0.03). For parent report of average sleep duration in a 24 hour period, a model adjusted for child’s age, sex, mother’s education level, and use of respiratory medications suggested that children with ASD tended to sleep about half an hour less than TD children (β = −0.473, SE = 0.156, P = 0.003). Mother’s education and child’s use of respiratory medications were also strong predictors of increased sleep duration (β = 0.431, SE = 0.140, P = 0.002; β = 0.607, SE = 0.265, P = 0.02, respectively) in all children.
Since cognitive and adaptive development appeared to play a role in the frequency of sleep problems, we examined the entire study sample for associations between cognitive and adaptive scores and sleep onset and night waking factor scores as well as total amount of sleep, adjusted for child’s age and sex. Overall, participants’ higher cognitive and adaptive scores were significantly associated with (1) lower sleep onset factor scores (cognitive: β = −0.049, SE = 0.021, P = 0.02; adaptive: β = −0.080, SE = 0.024, P = 0.0009); (2) lower night waking factor scores (cognitive: β = −0.055, SE = 0.022, P = 0.01; adaptive: β = −0.081, SE = 0.024, P = 0.001); and (3) increase in amount of sleep (cognitive: β = 0.121, SE = 0.036, P = 0.001; adaptive: β = 0.115, SE = 0.042, P = 0.006). However, in secondary analyses of the ASD group alone, sleep disturbances were not associated with cognitive and/or adaptive delays.
DISCUSSION
In this population-based study of parent reported sleep problems in preschool children with ASD, developmental delay without ASD and typical development, parents in all three groups reported very high rates of sleep concerns. Approximately half of the parents of children with ASD and children with developmental delay reported at least one sleep difficulty, while one-third of the parents of children with typical development had similar concerns. These prevalence rates of at least one sleep problem are similar to those reported previously (Richdale & Prior, 1995; Couturier et al., 2005; Wiggs & Stores, 2004; Gail Williams et al., 2004; Polimeni et al., 2005; Hering et al., 1999; Liu et al., 2006; Cotton & Richdale, 2006; Owens et al., 1998). Given that this is the first large sample of children with ASD to be assessed for sleep problems and to be compared with a population-based comparison sample, it is interesting to observe comparable rates.
An exploratory factor analysis of the CHARGE sleep questionnaire yielded two factors indicating two types of sleep problems predominant in this group of young children: problems with sleep onset and with night awakenings. Regression models that adjusted for age, sex and medical factors suggested that children with ASD have a trend towards more problems with sleep onset and have significantly more night awakenings compared to their peers with typical development. Children with developmental delay fell between these two groups, with fewer problems reported in both areas than for children with ASD, but more than for typically developing children. Medical factors, such as use of psychoactive medications and gastrointestinal problems, were also associated with reported sleep problems in all children, although in different ways. Psychoactive medications were associated with increased sleep onset difficulties but not night waking. Likewise, frequent gastrointestinal symptoms were related to more problems with sleep onset but not night waking.
The CHARGE Sleep History factor scores were subsequently dichotomized for two groups of participants: one with “high” sleep problems scores meaning that scores were one standard deviation or more above the mean sleep factor score and another group with “low” scores, meaning less than one standard deviation above the mean. Hence, while all of the children that met the item score cut-off had significantly high factor scores, children with elevated factor scores but item scores below the set cut-offs were not considered as having a “high” or severe sleep problem in this set of analyses. Using this conservative definition, our results suggest that while children with ASD are more likely to have high sleep disturbances (13%) compared to children with other developmental delays (9%) or typical development (5%), all three groups have a lower incidence of more severe sleep disturbance than previously reported (Couturier et al., 2005; Polimeni et al., 2005; Cotton & Richdale, 2006).
In contrast, when we compared a parent report of at least one frequent sleep problem, 52% of children with ASD, 46% of children with other developmental delays, and 32% of typically developing children had a current sleep problem. These findings are in the middle of a range of reported rates of sleep problems in children with ASD (44–86%) (Richdale & Prior, 1995; Couturier et al., 2005; Wiggs & Stores, 2004; Gail Williams et al., 2004; Polimeni et al., 2005; Hering et al., 1999; Liu et al., 2006; Cotton & Richdale, 2006). Likewise, our results in children with other developmental disabilities corresponded to those of Cotton and Richdale (2006) who found a prevalence of 44% for current sleep problems in a group of 43 children with intellectual disabilities. The previously reported prevalence of sleep problems in a typically developing sample of children ranged between 11% and 50% (Couturier et al., 2005; Polimeni et al., 2005; Owens et al., 1998; Cotton & Richdale, 2006); our findings were consistent with the mid-range of the reported rates. Direct comparison of the frequency of sleep problems across studies is difficult because of variations in instruments used to ascertain the data on sleep problems or in the definition of sleep problems. Nevertheless, our results approximated previously reported prevalence of sleep problems as reported by parents.
The relationships between cognitive and adaptive development and sleep patterns in young children with autism spectrum disorders have not been previously investigated with such a large sample. In our combined analyses with all diagnostic groups together, lower cognitive and adaptive function was associated with increased difficulties with sleep onset and night awakening, as well as shorter duration of sleep. In studies of children with mild to severe intellectual disabilities of mixed etiologies, more severe levels of intellectual disability were associated with more parent-reported sleep problems, both in terms of sleep onset latency and night waking duration (Richdale et al., 2000; Didden et al., 2002). However, in the ASD group alone, cognitive level and adaptive function did not predict the severity of either type of sleep problem or the duration of sleep. Similar results have been reported previously in a smaller sample (Richdale & Prior, 1995).
The impact of parent-reported sleep problems also differed across the diagnostic groups. Very rarely did parents of children with typical development report that their children’s sleep problems affected child or family function, whereas a high percentage of parents from the other groups so reported. These results are consistent with a previous report (Richdale et al., 2000) of the impact of a child’s sleep problem on the family. We found one in five families of children with ASD and with developmental delay reported sleep problems affecting both child and family function.
One limitation of our study is the reliance on parent report. In studies that have compared both parent report of sleep problems and objective actigraphy of sleep patterns in school age children with ASD, parents were accurate reporters of sleep patterns if their child made them aware of their difficulty (e.g. child disturbs the parent when they are awake at night) (Wiggs & Stores, 2004). The results of the current study are similar to other studies of children with ASD that utilized parent report only using the CSHQ (Couturier et al., 2005; Honomichl et al., 2002), although the CHARGE study is the first to examine sleep in a large, well-defined, population-based sample with confirmed ASD cases and population-based controls; thus, we are confident that these findings are less confounded by recruitment bias or diagnostic uncertainty than previously published reports.
Another potential limitation of this study includes the short response format used to describe the type of sleep problems, a 5-point Likert scale. Other questionnaires, such as the CSHQ (Owens et al., 2000), include a number of statements that parents respond to for clarification of the sleep problems noted. However, the rapidity in which the CHARGE Sleep History Questionnaire can be completed (5 minutes) and the similarity to outcomes using longer questionnaires such as the CSHQ suggest that accurately probing about sleep problems is not time-consuming and the use of a briefer questionnaire is valid and useful.
In summary, this study supports previous research indicating that children with ASD have greater difficulty with sleep onset and night waking than children with developmental delay and typical development. This confirmation is notable, given that the study sample was well-defined, population-based, and that we controlled for medical and other factors affecting sleep. Sleep onset and night awakenings are the predominant problem areas, and these problems affect daily functioning of both the child and the families of one in five children with either ASD or developmental delay. Further research related to both etiology of these sleep disturbances as well as effective interventions is warranted.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (1 P01 ES11269), the U.S. Environmental Protection Agency through the Science to Achieve Results (STAR) program (R829388) and by the M.I.N.D. Institute, University of California, Davis, CA.
REFERENCES
- Ali NJ, Pitson DJ, Stradling JR. Snoring, sleep disturbance, and behaviour in 4–5 year olds. Arch. Dis. Child. 1993;68:360–366. doi: 10.1136/adc.68.3.360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allik H, Larsson JO, Smedje H. Insomnia in school-age children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. BMC Psychiatry. 2006(a);6:18. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-6-18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allik H, Larsson JO, Smedje H. Sleep patterns of school-age children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2006(b);36:585–595. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0099-9. (2006b) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Academy of Sleep Medicine. The International Classification of Sleep Disorders: Diagnostic and Coding Manual (ICSD: DCM2) second edition Chicago: American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. fourth edition. Washington, DC: American Diagnostic Association; 1994. (1994) [Google Scholar]
- Anders T, Goodlin-Jones B, Sadeh A. Sleep disorders. In: Zeanah CH Jr, editor. Handbook of Infant Mental Health. second edition. New York: Guilford Publications, Inc.; 2000. pp. 326–338. (2000) [Google Scholar]
- Armstrong KL, Quinn RA, Dadds MR. The sleep patterns of normal children. Med. J. Aust. 1994;161:202–206. doi: 10.5694/j.1326-5377.1994.tb127383.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cerny BA, Kaiser HF. A study of a measure of sampling adequacy for factor-analytic correlation matrices. Multivar. Behav. Res. 1977;12:43–47. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr1201_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cotton S, Richdale A. Brief report: parental descriptions of sleep problems in children with autism, Down syndrome, and Prader-Willi syndrome. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2006;27:151–161. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2004.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Couturier JL, Speechley KN, Steele M, Norman R, Stringer B, Nicolson R. Parental perception of sleep problems in children of normal intelligence with pervasive developmental disorders: prevalence, severity, and pattern. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2005;44:815–822. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000166377.22651.87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Didden R, Korzilius H, van Aperlo B, van Overloop C, de Vries M. Sleep problems and daytime problem behaviours in children with intellectual disability. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2002;46:537–547. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.2002.00404.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doo S, Wing YK. Sleep problems of children with pervasive developmental disorders: correlation with parental stress. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2006;48:650–655. doi: 10.1017/S001216220600137X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- El-Sheikh M, Buckhalt JA, Mize J, Acebo C. Marital conflict and disruption of children's sleep. Child Dev. 2006;77:31–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00854.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fallone G, Acebo C, Seifer R, Carskadon MA. Experimental restriction of sleep opportunity in children: effects on teacher ratings. Sleep. 2005;28:1561–1567. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.12.1561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gail Williams P, Sears LL, Allard A. Sleep problems in children with autism. J. Sleep Res. 2004;13:265–268. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2004.00405.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gislason T, Benediktsdottir B. Snoring, apneic episodes, and nocturnal hypoxemia among children 6 months to 6 years old: an epidemiologic study of lower limit of prevalence. Chest. 1995;107:963–966. doi: 10.1378/chest.107.4.963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gozal D. Sleep-disordered breathing and school performance in children. Pediatrics. 1998;102:616–620. doi: 10.1542/peds.102.3.616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hering E, Epstein R, Elroy S, Iancu DR, Zelnik N. Sleep patterns in autistic children. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1999;29:143–147. doi: 10.1023/a:1023092627223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hertz-Picciotto I, Croen LA, Hansen R, Jones CR, van de Water J, Pessah IN. The CHARGE study: an epidemiologic investigation of genetic and environmental factors contributing to autism. Environ. Health Perspect. 2006;114:1119–1125. doi: 10.1289/ehp.8483. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Honomichl RD, Goodlin-Jones BL, Burnham M, Gaylor E, Anders TF. Sleep patterns of children with pervasive developmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2002;32:553–561. doi: 10.1023/a:1021254914276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins S, Owen C, Bax M, Hart H. Continuities of common behaviour problems in preschool children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 1984;25:75–89. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1984.tb01720.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson CM. Infant and toddler sleep: a telephone survey of parents in one community. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 1991;12:108–114. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaiser HF, Norman WT. Coefficients alpha for components. Psychol. Rep. 1991;69:111–114. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1991.69.1.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le Couteur A, Lord C, Rutter M. Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised (ADI-R) Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Liu X, Hubbard JA, Fabes RA, Adam JB. Sleep disturbances and correlates of children with autism spectrum disorders. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2006;37:179–191. doi: 10.1007/s10578-006-0028-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH, Jr, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, Pickles A, Rutter M. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2000;30:205–223. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malow BA. Sleep disorders, epilepsy, and autism. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2004;10:122–125. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin J, Hiscock H, Hardy P, Davey B, Wake M. Adverse associations of infant and child sleep problems and parent health: an Australian population study. Pediatrics. 2007;119:947–955. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-2569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minde K, Popiel K, Leos N, Falkner S, Parker K, Handley-Derry M. The evaluation and treatment of sleep disturbances in young children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 1993;34:521–533. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1993.tb01033.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mindell J, Owens J. A clinical guide to pediatric sleep. Philadelphia: LWW; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Mindell JA, Durand VM. Treatment of childhood sleep disorders: generalization across disorders and effects on family members. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 1993;18:731–750. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/18.6.731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mullen EM. Mullen Scales of Early Learning. Circle Pines, Minnesota: American Guidance Services, Inc.; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Newman CJ, O'Regan M, Hensey O. Sleep disorders in children with cerebral palsy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2006;48:564–568. doi: 10.1017/S0012162206001198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owens J, Opipari L, Nobile C, Spirito A. Sleep and daytime behavior in children with obstructive sleep apnea and behavioral sleep disorders. Pediatrics. 1998;102:1178–1184. doi: 10.1542/peds.102.5.1178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owens JA, Fernando S, McGuinn M. Sleep disturbance and injury risk in young children. Behav. Sleep Med. 2005;3:18–31. doi: 10.1207/s15402010bsm0301_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owens JA, Spirito A, McGuinn M. The Children's Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ): psychometric properties of a survey instrument for school-aged children. Sleep. 2000;23:1043–1051. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patzold LM, Richdale AL, Tonge BJ. An investigation into sleep characteristics of children with autism and Asperger's Disorder. J. Paediatr. Child Health. 1998;34:528–533. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1754.1998.00291.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polimeni MA, Richdale AL, Francis AJ. A survey of sleep problems in autism, Asperger's disorder and typically developing children. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2005;49:260–268. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2005.00642.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quine L. Sleep problems in children with mental handicap. J. Ment. Defic. Res. 1991;35:269–290. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.1991.tb00402.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Randazzo AC, Muehlbach MJ, Schweitzer PK, Walsh JK. Cognitive function following acute sleep restriction in children ages 10–14. Sleep. 1998;21:861–868. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richdale AL, Gavidia-Payne S, Francis A, Cotton S. Stress, behaviour and sleep problems in children with an intellectual disability. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2000;25:147–161. [Google Scholar]
- Richdale AL, Prior MR. The sleep/wake rhythm in children with autism. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 1995;4:175–186. doi: 10.1007/BF01980456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richdale AL. Sleep in children with autism and Asperger syndrome. In: Stores G, Wiggs L, editors. Sleep Disturbance in Children and Adolescents with Disorders of Development: its Significance and Management. Oxford, England: Cambridge University Press; 2001. pp. 181–191. [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M, Bailey A, Berument SK, Lord C, Pickles A. Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Sadeh A, Gruber R, Raviv A. Sleep, neurobehavioral functioning, and behavior problems in school-age children. Child Dev. 2002;73:405–417. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sadeh A, Lavie P, Scher A, Tirosh E, Epstein R. Actigraphic home-monitoring sleep-disturbed and control infants and young children: a new method for pediatric assessment of sleep-wake patterns. Pediatrics. 1991;87:494–499. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sadeh A. Evaluating night wakings in sleep-disturbed infants: a methodological study of parental reports and actigraphy. Sleep. 1996;19:757–762. doi: 10.1093/sleep/19.10.757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scher A, Tirosh E, Jaffe M, Rubin L, Sadeh A, Lavie P. Sleep patterns of infants and young children in Israel. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 1995;18:701–711. [Google Scholar]
- Schreck KA, Mulick JA. Parental report of sleep problems in children with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2000;30:127–135. doi: 10.1023/a:1005407622050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sparrow SS, Balla DA, Cicchetti DV. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales Interview Edition Expanded Form Manual. Circle Pines, Minnesota: American Guidance Services, Inc.; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Wiggs L, Stores G. Behavioural treatment for sleep problems in children with severe learning disabilities and challenging daytime behaviour: effect on sleep patterns of mother and child. J. Sleep Res. 1998;7:119–126. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.1998.00107.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiggs L, Stores G. General aspects of sleep and intellectual impairment. In: Stores G, Wiggs L, editors. Sleep Disturbance in Children and Adolescents with Disorders of Development: its Significance and Management. Oxford, England: Cambridge University Press; 2001. pp. 47–52. [Google Scholar]
- Wiggs L, Stores G. Sleep patterns and sleep disorders in children with autistic spectrum disorders: insights using parent report and actigraphy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2004;46:372–380. doi: 10.1017/s0012162204000611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfson AR, Carskadon MA. Sleep schedules and daytime functioning in adolescents. Child Dev. 1998;69:875–887. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]