Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2014 Apr 9;38(6):1737–1744. doi: 10.1111/acer.12408

Receptivity to and Recall of Alcohol Brand Appearances in U.S. Popular Music and Alcohol-Related Behaviors

Brian A Primack 1, Auden McClure 2, Zhigang Li 3, James D Sargent 4
PMCID: PMC4047148  NIHMSID: NIHMS571896  PMID: 24716496

Abstract

Background

The average U.S. adolescent is exposed to about 2.5 hours of popular music per day and 8 mentions of alcohol brands every day. Alcohol brand mentions may function as advertising whether or not they are sanctioned by the alcohol industry. Our study aimed to determine associations between adolescents' involvement with music containing alcohol brand mentions and alcohol-related behaviors.

Methods

In 2010–2011 we conducted a random-digit-dial survey using national U.S. land line and cell phone frames. Through screening interviews, we identified 6,466 eligible households with subjects between 15 to 23 years of age, of whom 3422 (52%) completed the telephone survey. Of these, 2541 opted to participate in a subsequent Web-based component. Independent variables included a composite score indicating owning and liking popular songs with alcohol brand mentions and correct recall of alcohol brands in songs. Outcome measures included ever having consumed a complete drink, ever bingeing, bingeing at least monthly, and having experienced problems from alcohol use.

Results

Among the 2541 participants, compared with those in the lowest tertile on the receptivity scale, those in the highest tertile had higher odds of having had a complete drink (OR=3.4; 95% CI=2.2, 5.2) after adjusting for age, sex, race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, sensation seeking, friend alcohol use, and parent alcohol use. Compared with those who did not identify at least one alcohol brand correctly, those who did had over twice the odds of having had a complete drink (OR=2.1; 95% CI=1.2, 3.8) after adjusting for all covariates. Results were also significant for the outcome of ever bingeing but not for bingeing at least monthly or having had problems due to drinking.

Conclusions

In a national sample of U.S. adolescents and young adults, there were independent associations between involvement with popular music containing alcohol brand mentions and both having ever had a complete drink and having ever binged on alcohol.

Keywords: Alcohol, music, product placement, marketing, adolescent

INTRODUCTION

Alcohol consumption is considered the leading root cause of mortality in adolescence and young adulthood (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2007; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2012). Alcohol is also considered the leading cause of morbidity in this population, due to its established association with nonfatal injuries (Hingson, Heeren, Winter, & Wechsler, 2005; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2007), other substance use (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2007; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2012), risky sexual behavior (Sales et al., 2012), academic failure (Bradley and Greene, 2013), physical and sexual assault (Rothman et al., 2012), and alcohol dependence (Dawson et al., 2007).

Despite our understanding of the impact of alcohol use on adolescents and young adults, consumption remains epidemic. For example, 39% of U.S. adolescents are current drinkers, defined as having a complete alcoholic drink during the past 30 days (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2012), and over one-fifth (22%) of adolescents are current binge drinkers (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2012). These early exposures are particularly concerning in light of the fact that the odds of future alcohol abuse or dependence increases for each year of age below 21 that alcohol consumption begins (Dawson et al., 2007). Alcohol use is associated with multiple socio-demographic (Nash et al., 2005; Schulte et al., 2009), environmental (Nash et al., 2005; Rothman et al., 2012), and personal factors (Nash et al., 2005; Sales et al., 2012). However, a growing body of literature suggests that exposure to certain mass media representations of alcohol—both narrative (e.g., movies) and persuasive (e.g., advertisements)—may be among the strongest risk factors for adolescent alcohol use (Sargent et al., 2006; Anderson et al., 2009; Dal Cin et al., 2009; Chung et al., 2010; McClure et al., 2009; Smith and Foxcroft, 2009; Wills et al., 2009; Hanewinkel et al., 2012). These media exposures are common; 83% of contemporary films (including 57% of G/PG films) depict alcohol use, exposing the average U.S. youth 10–14 years of age to 5.6 hours of movie alcohol use and 244 alcohol brand appearances annually (Dal Cin et al., 2008).

Little research, however, has focused on alcohol representations in popular music, which has emerged as an important source of alcohol-related media exposure (Primack et al., 2008; Siegel et al., 2013). Music is currently the fastest-growing media exposure among adolescents at 2.5 hours per day (Rideout et al., 2010). About a quarter of songs contain references to drinking and alcohol brand mentions, exposing the average adolescent to about 14 references to drinking per song-hour (Primack et al., 2008), and approximately 8 mentions of alcohol brands every day (Primack et al., 2012). These myriad references are commonly associated with consequences that adolescents find particularly compelling, such as sex, popularity, and partying (Primack et al., 2008; Primack et al., 2012). Brand mentions are important to assess because they may function as advertising, whether or not they are paid for or sanctioned by the alcohol industry. Developing brand recognition is a crucial step in the marketing of any product, and, for both cigarettes and alcohol, brand exposure and receptivity are considered important risk factors for the initiation and maintenance of the use of these substances among adolescents (Henriksen et al., 2008; McClure et al., 2013).

Prior research has not, however, examined independent associations between youth involvement in alcohol-containing music and alcohol-related behaviors. This is an important gap in the literature, especially in light of accelerating formal associations between the music and alcohol industries (Primack et al., 2012). Therefore, we aimed to determine independent associations between adolescents' receptivity to such music and alcohol-related behaviors among late adolescents and young adults.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants and Procedures

Context

From Fall 2010 to Spring 2011, we assessed a nationally representative sample of 15–23 year olds using random digit dial survey methods. The survey was approved by the Human Subjects Protection Committee at Dartmouth Medical School and Westat (Rockville, MD), a national research organization with survey sites across the U.S.

Survey

We first conducted a random-digit-dial telephone survey, using land line and cell phone frames to recruit households with age-eligible youths. Telephone surveys were conducted by trained interviewers who used a computer-assisted telephone interview (CATI) system and administered the survey in English or Spanish. Verbal parental permission and adolescent assent were obtained prior to interviewing each respondent under age 18, and verbal consent was obtained from those 18 years and over. Participants under the age of 18 were given the option of answering sensitive questions using the telephone touch pad to provide confidentiality in the event that others were present or listening. Participants were then directed to a Web-based survey in which song titles were used to assess receptivity to popular songs with alcohol mentions. Participants without internet access were allowed to respond to the survey via regular mail.

Sample selection, response rate, and representativeness

Through screening interviews, we identified 6,466 eligible households with subjects between 15 to 23 years of age, of whom 3422 (52%) completed the telephone CATI. Of these, 2541 completed both the telephone and web-based portion of the survey. For households with more than one age-eligible subject, we randomly selected one for enrollment. Additional detail on recruitment and response rates is available on request from the senior author. Youths were drawn from every state in the continental United States, with the distribution approximating the U.S. population. The survey sample was somewhat more likely to be from the Midwest census region of the US. Respondents also tended to be younger and were more likely to be White, but there was an even distribution with respect to household income.

Measures

Primary Independent Variables: Involvement with Music Containing Alcohol Brand Appearances

Our two measures included a composite score assessing receptivity (liking and ownership) to songs with alcohol brands, and correct recall of at least one alcohol brand contained in the song lyrics. For this assessment, ten songs were selected randomly from all 41 top songs from 2005–2007 containing alcohol brand mentions (Primack et al., 2012). For each song listed, participants were instructed to “please tell us if you like the song [yes/no], own the song [yes/no], and what brand of alcohol is mentioned in the song, if any [free text response].” For liking and owning a song, we generated a raw composite score from 0 to 20 for each participant based on the number of “yes” responses, and we collapsed the scale into tertiles, representing the participant's receptivity to music with alcohol branding as low, medium, or high. The alcohol brand recall item was coded as “yes” if the participant correctly identified at least one brand from the list of 10 songs and “no” if he or she did not. The variable was dichotomized because only 8% of respondents correctly identified one or more alcohol brand mentions from any of the 10 songs.

Primary Dependent Variables: Alcohol-Related Outcomes

The outcomes assessment was based on items from the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (Saunders et al., 1993). Our four outcome measures derived from these items were ever having had a complete drink of alcohol, ever having binged on alcohol, bingeing at least monthly, and a derived measure assessing problems due to alcohol use, such as injuries or memory loss. Before our item assessing ever having had a complete drink of alcohol, we used pictograms to illustrate the definition of a complete drink for each type of alcohol (e.g., 12 ounces for beer, 5 ounces for wine, and 1.5 ounces for hard liquor). We assessed binge drinking by asking how often the participant had 6 or more drinks on the same occasion (Saunders et al., 1993), with response choices of (a) never, (b) less than monthly, (c) monthly, (d) weekly, and (e) daily or almost daily. For outcome 2 (ever bingeing) we dichotomized between responses (a) and (b), and for outcome 3 (bingeing at least monthly), we dichotomized between responses (b) and (c). Finally, we asked participants if they had ever had a problem directly due to alcohol, including 7 separate items (α =0.73) listed in the notes of Table 3. If an individual endorsed any one of those items, he or she was defined as having had a problem due to alcohol.

Table 3.

Weighted Multivariable Associations between Correctly Identified at Least One Alcohol Brand and Alcohol Outcomes in a National U.S. Sample.

Has Had Complete Drink* Has Ever Had Binge Binges at Least Monthly Had Problems from Drinking
% N = 2541 n = 1488 n = 1488 n = 1488
Adjusted OR (95% CI) § Adjusted OR (95% CI) § Adjusted OR (95% CI) § Adjusted OR (95% CI) §
Correctly Identified at Least One Alcohol Brand
No 91 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference]
Yes 9 2.1 (1.2,3.8) 2.0 (1.2,3.3) 2.2 (0.9, 5.3) 1.5 (0.9, 2.6)
Age
15–16 23 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference]
17–18 22 1.9 (1.3,2.6) 1.6 (0.9, 2.8) 2.0 (1.0,4.1) 1.4 (0.9, 2.4)
19–20 21 3.1 (2.0,4.9) 2.3 (1.3,4.0) 2.5 (1.2,5.2) 1.4 (0.8,2.7)
21–23 34 12.9 (7.6, 21.7) 2.8 (1.7,4.7) 3.2 (1.6,6.2) 1.4 (0.9,2.3)
Sex
Male 51 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference]
Female 49 0.8 (0.6,1.1) 0.4 (0.3,0.5) 0.2 (0.1, 0.4) 0.8 (0.5,1.1)
Race/Ethnicity
White 58 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference]
Black 15 0.7 (0.4, 1.3) 0.4 (0.2,1.1) 0.1 (0.0,0.6) 1.0 (0.4, 2.3)
Hispanic 19 1.5 (0.9, 2.5) 0.7 (0.4, 1.2) 1.0 (0.4, 2.2) 1.6 (0.9,2.7)
Other 8 0.8 (0.5,1.3) 0.7 (0.4, 1.2) 0.4 (0.2, 0.9) 0.8 (0.4, 1.5)
Socioeconomic Status
Low 33 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference]
Medium 34 1.4 (0.9,2.2) 1.2 (0.8, 1.9) 0.8 (0.5, 1.4) 0.8 (0.5,1.2)
High 34 1.1 (0.7,1.6) 1.0 (0.6, 1.6) 0.7 (0.4, 1.3) 1.0 (0.6,1.5)
Sensation Seeking
Low 35 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference]
Medium 34 2.0 (1.4,2.9) 1.8 (1.1,3.0) 2.7 (1.3,5.7) 2.6 (1.6,4.2)
High 31 3.3 (2.2,4.9) 2.7 (1.8,4.2) 4.6 (2.5, 8.8) 3.3 (2.0, 5.4)
Friend Alcohol Use
None or a few 45 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference]
More than a few 14 2.9 (2.0,4.1) 2.0 (1.2,3.4) 2.3 (0.9, 6.0) 1.6 (0.8, 2.9)
Most 40 6.2 (4.1,9.3) 4.7 (3.1,7.0) 8.6 (4.1, 17.9) 3.7 (2.3,6.2)
Parental Alcohol Use
None or occasionally 72 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference]
Weekly 16 2.3 (1.7,3.2) 1.4 (1.0,2.1) 1.8 (1.0,3.0) 1.3 (0.8, 2.0)
Daily 12 2.7 (1.7,4.2) 1.2 (0.7, 2.0) 1.9 (1.1,3.5) 1.7 (1.0,2.9)
*

A drink was defined as 12 ounces of beer, 5 ounces of wine, or 2 ounces of hard liquor.

A binge was defined as 6 or more drinks in a single sitting (AUDIT criteria).

Specific problems asked about included (1) inability to stop drinking; (2) failure to do something important that they had to do; (3) needing a drink of alcohol the next morning; (4) feeling guilty after drinking; (5) forgetting what happened while they were drinking; (6) injuring themselves or someone else because of drinking; or (7) being told by someone that they had a drinking problem.

§

Adjusted for age, sex, race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, sensation seeking, friend alcohol use, and parent alcohol use.

Covariates

We collected data on a number of socio-demographic characteristics that have been related to alcohol use and/or alcohol brand receptivity in prior studies. These included age, sex, race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, sensation seeking, friend alcohol use, and parent alcohol use. Socioeconomic status was assessed using a composite measure of household income and parental education (α=0.61) and collapsed into tertiles. Sensation seeking was measured with a 4-item scale (α=0.71) validated in a population similar to ours (Stephenson et al., 2003), also collapsed into tertiles. Based on distributions of the data, friend alcohol use was classified as “none or a few”; “more than a few”; or “most” and parental alcohol use was categorized as “none or occasionally”; “weekly”; or “daily.”

Analysis

Descriptive analyses were used to summarize sample characteristics across alcohol use outcomes, and chi-square tests to determine whether differences were significant. We then used multivariable analyses to assess associations between our independent variables and alcohol outcomes. Because all outcome variables were dichotomous, we used logistic regression, and primary models controlled for all covariates. For the outcome of ever drinking, analyses involved all participants. However, only drinkers were asked about ever bingeing, bingeing at least monthly, and problems due to alcohol. For all analyses, we used a two-tailed α of 0.05 to define statistical significance.

All primary descriptive statistics and logistic regression models were carried out using sampling weights. The jackknife method was used to calculate the standard errors of the parameter estimators with the replicated weights that were developed by Westat. Potential confounders were also adjusted in the models. All the weighted analyses were conducted in SAS 9.3. To promote scientific transparency, we also present unweighted analyses, which apply to this specific sample of youths which, while national in scope, underrepresents minority youths.

RESULTS

Sample

While the majority of the 2541 individuals completed the survey using a Web-based interface (n=2221, 87%), some completed this component by regular mail (n=320, 13%). The final weighted sample was 52% female, 8% African-American, and 12% Hispanic. The average age was 18.3 years (standard deviation [SD] = 2.5).

Alcohol Outcomes

Of the 2541 participants, 1488 (59%) reported having had a complete alcoholic drink. Of the 1488 participants who had had a complete drink, 656 (44%) reported having ever binged, 261 (18%) reported bingeing at least monthly, and 547 (37%) reported problems such as injuries due to alcohol.

Alcohol Brand Exposure and Recall

The raw score on our initial measure of alcohol song receptivity ranged from 0 to 20, because 2 points were possible (liking and owning) for each of the 10 songs. Mean score was 3.7 with a standard deviation of 4.2. When scores were collapsed into tertiles, low was defined as raw score 0 (31%), medium as 1–4 (37%), and high as 5 or more (32%). One hundred eighty-nine (7.4%) participants correctly identified (recalled) at least one alcohol brand mention from the list of 10 songs.

Bivariable Analyses

Associations between covariates and outcomes are shown in Table 1. In weighted analyses, sex, sensation seeking, friend alcohol use, and parental alcohol use were significantly associated with all outcomes. However, age, race/ethnicity, and socioeconomic status were associated with some alcohol outcomes but not others.

Table 1.

Weighted Sample Characteristics by Alcohol Use Outcomes for a National U.S. Sample.

Whole Sample Has Had Complete Drink* Has Ever Had Binge Binges at Least Monthly Had Problems from Drinking
N = 2541 n = 1488 n = 1488 n = 1488
No Yes P No Yes P No Yes P No Yes P
N = 2541 n = 1053 n = 1488 n = 832 n = 656 n = 1227 n = 261 n = 941 n = 547
% % % % % % % % %
Age
15–16 23 43 13 <.0001 17 8 <.0001 15 4 .0002 15 10 .19
17–18 22 28 20 22 17 21 15 19 20
19–20 21 19 22 20 25 22 25 21 24
21–23 34 11 45 40 51 43 55 45 46
Sex
Male 51 47 53 0.02 43 65 <.0001 48 75 <.0001 50 59 .01
Female 49 53 47 57 35 52 25 50 41
Race/Ethnicity
White 58 55 60 <.0001 53 68 0.001 57 73 .003 60 60 .80
Black 15 20 11 15 8 14 3 12 10
Hispanic 19 15 21 24 17 21 19 20 22
Other 8 10 8 8 7 8 5 8 7
Socioeconomic Status
Low 33 31 34 .001 34 33 .96 33 37 .58 33 35 .42
Medium 34 29 36 36 37 36 36 38 33
High 34 40 30 31 30 31 27 29 32
Sensation Seeking
Low 35 49 27 <.0001 34 20 <.0001 31 11 <.0001 35 16 <.0001
Medium 34 34 34 36 32 36 28 34 35
High 31 17 38 29 48 32 61 31 49
Friend Alcohol Use
None or a few 45 77 29 <.0001 42 14 <.0001 35 6 <.0001 39 15 <.0001
More than a few 14 12 16 17 14 17 9 17 13
Most 40 12 55 40 72 48 85 44 72
Parental Alcohol Use
None or occasionally 72 83 65 <.0001 70 61 .03 69 52 .001 71 58 .001
Weekly 16 10 19 16 23 17 27 17 23
Daily 12 7 15 14 16 14 21 12 20
*

A drink was defined as 12 ounces of beer, 5 ounces of wine, or 2 ounces of hard liquor.

A binge was defined as 6 or more drinks in a single sitting (AUDIT criteria).

Specific problems asked about included (1) inability to stop drinking; (2) failure to do something important that they had to do; (3) needing a drink of alcohol the next morning; (4) feeling guilty after drinking; (5) forgetting what happened while they were drinking; (6) injuring themselves or someone else because of drinking; or (7) being told by someone that they had a drinking problem.

Multivariable Analyses

Weighted multivariable analyses demonstrating associations between independent and dependent variables are shown in Table 2 (for liking and owning music with alcohol branding) and Table 3 (for correctly identifying alcohol brands).

Table 2.

Weighted Multivariable Associations between Liking and Ownership of Music with Alcohol Branding and Alcohol Outcomes in a National U.S. Sample.

Has Had Complete Drink* Has Ever Had Binge Binges at Least Monthly Had Problems from Drinking
% N = 2541 n = 1488 n = 1488 n = 1488
Adjusted OR (95% CI) § Adjusted OR (95% CI) § Adjusted OR (95% CI) § Adjusted OR (95% CI) §
Liking and Ownership of Music with Alcohol Branding
Low 29 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference]
Medium 35 1.8 (1.3,2.6) 1.7 (1.0,2.7) 1.4 (0.8,2.5) 1.3 (0.7, 2.3)
High 36 3.4 (2.2,5.2) 1.9 (1.2,3.2) 1.7 (0.9, 3.0) 1.5 (0.8, 2.7)
Age
15–16 23 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference]
17–18 22 1.9 (1.3,2.6) 1.6 (0.9, 2.8) 1.9 (1.0,3.9) 1.4 (0.8, 2.4)
19–20 21 3.2 (2.0, 5.0) 2.4 (1.4,4.2) 2.4 (1.2,4.9) 1.4 (0.8,2.5)
21–23 34 12.8 (7.9, 20.7) 2.9 (1.7,4.9) 3.5 (1.8,6.7) 1.3 (0.8, 2.2)
Sex
Male 51 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference]
Female 49 0.7 (0.5, 1.0) 0.3 (0.2,0.5) 0.2 (0.1, 0.4) 0.7 (0.5,1.0)
Race/Ethnicity
White 58 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference]
Black 15 0.4 (0.2, 0.8) 0.4 (0.2,0.9) 0.1 (0.0,0.6) 0.9 (0.4, 2.1)
Hispanic 19 1.3 (0.8, 2.2) 0.7 (0.4, 1.1) 0.9 (0.4, 1.9) 1.5 (0.9, 2.5)
Other 8 0.7 (0.4, 1.2) 0.6 (0.3,1.2) 0.4 (0.2, 0.9) 0.8 (0.4, 1.4)
Socioeconomic Status
Low 33 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference]
Medium 34 1.3 (0.9, 2.0) 1.2 (0.8, 1.9) 0.8 (0.5, 1.4) 0.8 (0.5,1.2)
High 34 1.0 (0.7,1.5) 1.0 (0.7,1.7) 0.7 (0.4, 1.3) 0.9 (0.6, 1.5)
Sensation Seeking
Low 35 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference]
Medium 34 1.9 (1.4, 2.7) 1.8 (1.1,2.9) 2.5 (1.2,5.2) 2.5 (1.5,4.2)
High 31 3.1 (2.1,4.6) 2.4 (1.6,3.7) 4.3 (2.3, 8.3) 3.0 (1.8,5.0)
Friend Alcohol Use
None or a few 45 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference]
More than a few 14 2.7 (1.9,4.0) 2.2 (1.3,3.7) 2.5 (1.0, 6.3) 1.6 (0.9, 2.9)
Most 40 6.0 (4.0, 9.0) 4.9 (3.3, 7.4) 9.1 (4.5,18.5) 3.8 (2.3,6.2)
Parental Alcohol Use
None or occasionally 72 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference] 1.0 [Reference]
Weekly 16 2.2 (1.6, 3.1) 1.4 (0.9, 2.0) 1.8 (1.0, 3.1) 1.3 (0.8, 2.0)
Daily 12 2.6 (1.7, 4.0) 1.2 (0.7, 1.9) 1.9 (1.1, 3.3) 1.7 (1.0, 2.9)
*

A drink was defined as 12 ounces of beer, 5 ounces of wine, or 2 ounces of hard liquor.

A binge was defined as 6 or more drinks in a single sitting (AUDIT criteria).

Specific problems asked about included (1) inability to stop drinking; (2) failure to do something important that they had to do; (3) needing a drink of alcohol the next morning; (4) feeling guilty after drinking; (5) forgetting what happened while they were drinking; (6) injuring themselves or someone else because of drinking; or (7) being told by someone that they had a drinking problem.

§

Adjusted for age, sex, race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, sensation seeking, friend alcohol use, and parent alcohol use.

Compared with those in the lowest tertile on the alcohol song receptivity scale, those in the highest tertile had three times the odds of having had a complete drink (OR=3.4; 95% CI=2.2, 5.2) after adjusting for age, sex, race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, sensation seeking, friend alcohol use, and parent alcohol use. As is indicated in Table 2, the only variables with stronger associations with having had a complete drink were age=21–23 (vs. age=15–16) and “most” friends being alcohol users (vs. “none or a few” friends). Compared with those who did not identify at least one alcohol brand correctly, those who did had nearly twice the odds of having had a complete drink (OR=2.1; 95% CI=1.2, 3.8) after adjusting for all covariates.

Compared with those in the lowest tertile on the alcohol song receptivity scale, those in the highest tertile had nearly twice the odds of ever bingeing (OR=1.9; 95% CI=1.2, 3.2) after adjusting for all covariates. Compared with those who did not identify at least one alcohol brand correctly, those who did also had twice the odds of ever binge drinking (OR=2.0; 95% CI=1.2, 3.3) after adjusting for all covariates. Results for the outcomes of bingeing at least monthly and having had problems from alcohol had odds ratio point estimates greater than 1 (between 1.3 and 2.2). However, for both independent variables, none of these associations reached statistical significance (Table 3).

Unweighted Analyses

In unweighted analyses, results were parallel but generally had stronger levels of significance (Appendix 1). In some cases, nonsignificant results in weighted analyses were significant in unweighted analyses. For example, compared with those in the lowest tertile on the alcohol song receptivity scale, those in the highest tertile had nearly twice the odds of having had problems from drinking (OR=1.9; 95% CI=1.4, 2.7) after adjusting for all covariates. Also, compared with those in the lowest tertile on the alcohol song receptivity scale, those in the highest tertile had more than twice the odds of bingeing at least monthly (OR=2.2; 95% CI=1.4, 3.3) after adjusting for all covariates (Appendix 1).

DISCUSSION

The study finds an association between liking and owning music containing drinking references and two outcomes, having had a complete alcoholic drink and binge drinking. Furthermore, the few adolescents who correctly recalled the alcohol brand mentioned in these songs had increased odds for each of these outcomes. These findings suggest that music be considered along with other entertainment venues that shape how adolescents think about alcohol (Stoolmiller et al., 2012). Longitudinal studies demonstrate that exposure to movie alcohol depictions predicts alcohol onset and subsequent binge drinking (McClure et al., 2009). Taken together, the evidence supports the notion that having movie and music industry stars endorse drinking on screen and in their songs promotes it among adolescents familiar with such entertainment.

It should be noted, however, that the cross-sectional design of this study limits our ability to make causal inferences regarding these associations. Therefore, while it may be that alcohol mentions in songs prompt adolescents to form more favorable attitudes about alcohol, it is also highly plausible that music-oriented adolescents who develop favorable attitudes about drinking for other reasons could be drawn to genres that promote drinking and often mention brands (McClure et al., 2009). In particular, longitudinal studies may enable us to disentangle directionality regarding this relationship.

Classic theories of communication suggest that receptivity to a message involves not only exposure to a message, but also an understanding of and agreement with the message (Petty & Cacciopo, 1981). It should be noted that it is a challenge to accurately assess constructs such as these as they relate to music and/or alcohol brands. We attempted to capture these constructs as fully as possible by assessing them in various ways. For example, we asked not only about ownership of music but also about “liking” of music, which may indicate a level of affinity for and/or agreement with the music beyond simple familiarity. Despite this, our primary composite independent variable may simply be a surrogate for overall media exposure, which is why we also used the dichotomous recognition measure; being able to recall the correct brand would be a higher level of receptivity, beyond owning or liking the song. Thus, it is noteworthy that the results were consistent regardless of the independent variable used. However, it remains an important task of future work to explore improved ways of assessing complex constructs such as exposure to, recall of, and receptivity to media messages.

Although odds ratio point estimates were elevated for both independent variables and each of the other dependent variables—bingeing at least monthly and having had problems from bingeing—these associations did not achieve statistical significance. One possible explanation is that exposure to alcohol brand appearances in music is more strongly related to the earlier alcohol-related transitions (e.g., experimentation) than the later transitions (e.g., consolidation), for which factors such as age, sensation seeking, and friend alcohol use are more potent. However, because the odds ratios were elevated and close to significance in many cases, it is also possible that we did not have sufficient power to determine these differences. Considering the widespread popularity of music among adolescents and young adults, the findings of this study raise concerns about the practice of alcohol companies that sign promotional deals with singer/songwriters. Most of the brands mentioned in these songs involved premium distilled spirits, such as vodka, tequila, rum, and cognac. While advertising for these brands is regulated by guidelines promulgated by the Distilled Industries Council of the United States (DISCUS, 2011), the DISCUS rule that “Beverage alcohol advertising and marketing materials should portray beverage alcohol products and drinkers in a responsible manner” may be in violation in some of these cases. Additional study involving the lyrics of these songs, other related marketing materials, and the DISCUS legal text may help determine whether there are violations.

If it is ultimately determined that media messages such as these are prospectively associated with problematic alcohol-related outcomes, it ultimately may be valuable to develop intervention strategies which aim to reduce the susceptibility of individuals to the alcohol-related messages to which they will inevitably be exposed. For example, “Media literacy,” an innovative approach that teaches young people to understand, analyze, and criticize media messages, may be a promising complement to exposure reduction (Bergsma and Carney, 2008; Chen, 2013; Primack et al., 2014). Yet while media literacy has been commonly utilized to address tobacco use (Pinkleton et al., 2007; Bergsma and Carney, 2008; Primack et al., 2009a; Primack, 2014), it has been less frequently applied to alcohol (Kupersmidt et al., 2010; Chen, 2013). This may be an important gap, because media literacy aims to leverage developmentally normative adolescent tendencies such as rebelliousness and sensation seeking against industry messaging (Primack et al., 2006; Bergsma & Carney, 2008; Primack et al., 2009b).

Limitations

It is a limitation that our response rate was only about 39% (2541/6466), and Westat was not able to collect demographic data on non-responders. However, it should also be noted that we present here results of analyses which were weighted to approximate the US population, which may in part improve generalizability of findings. It should also be noted that we assessed alcohol outcomes using self-report rather than using any type of biochemical validation. These types of measures, however, tend to be highly accurate, especially when they are anonymous (Brodey et al., 2007). Finally, we defined an alcohol “binge” as 6 or more drinks, because this is the AUDIT standard, and because we involved youth up to age 23 in the sample. However, this outcome may not have been highly sensitive for younger participants, for whom a 3–5 drink binge may still be clinically significant. However, it was for this reason that we utilized many different outcome variables, and it is notable that the results were quite consistent regardless of the specific outcome variable.

Conclusion

Despite these limitations, the results of this study show that there are strong, independent associations between liking, owning and correctly identifying music containing alcohol brand appearances and two early problematic outcomes related to alcohol: having had a complete alcoholic drink and having ever binged on alcohol. These results, especially in light of accelerating formal associations between the music and alcohol industries (Primack et al., 2012), suggest that it would be valuable to continue surveillance of alcohol brand references in popular music and to consider policy and/or educational interventions designed to reduce the impact of these exposures on alcohol outcomes.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This work was supported by the National Cancer Institute (K07-CA114315 to B.P., a SYNERGY Scholars Award from the Dartmouth Center for Clinical and Translational Research to A.M., and R01-CA077026 to J.S.). Although they provided financial support, these funding sources were not involved in the design and conduct of the study; the collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; the preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; or the decision to submit the manuscript for publication. Dr. Primack had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

DECLARATION OF INTERESTS This work was supported by the National Cancer Institute (K07-CA114315 to B.P., a SYNERGY Scholars Award from the Dartmouth Center for Clinical and Translational Research to A.M., and R01-CA077026 to J.S.). Although they provided financial support, these funding sources were not involved in the design and conduct of the study; the collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; the preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; or the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

REFERENCES

  1. Anderson P, de Bruijn A, Angus K, Gordon R, Hastings G. Impact of alcohol advertising and media exposure on adolescent alcohol use: a systematic review of longitudinal studies. Alcohol Alcohol. 2009;44:229–243. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agn115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bergsma LJ, Carney ME. Effectiveness of health-promoting media literacy education: a systematic review. Health Educ Res. 2008;23:522–542. doi: 10.1093/her/cym084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bradley BJ, Greene AC. Do health and education agencies in the United States share responsibility for academic achievement and health? A review of 25 years of evidence about the relationship of adolescents' academic achievement and health behaviors. J Adolesc Health. 2013;52:523–532. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Brodey B, McMullin D, Winters K, Rosen C, Downing D, Koble J. Adolescent substance use assessment in a primary care setting. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2007;33:447–454. doi: 10.1080/00952990701315079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance—United States, 2011. MMWR. 2012;61(SS-4):1–168. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Chen YC. The effectiveness of different approaches to media literacy in modifying adolescents' responses to alcohol. J Health Commun. 2013;18:723–39. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2012.757387. 2013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chung PJ, Garfield CF, Elliott MN, Ostroff J, Ross C, Jernigan DH, Vestal KD, Schuster MA. Association between Adolescent Viewership and Alcohol Advertising on Cable Television. Am J Public Health. 2010;100:555–562. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2008.146423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Dal Cin S, Worth KA, Dalton MA, Sargent JD. Youth exposure to alcohol use and brand appearances in popular contemporary movies. Addiction. 2008;103:1925–1932. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02304.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Dal Cin S, Worth KA, Gerrard M, Stoolmiller M, Sargent JD, Wills TA, Gibbons FX. Watching and drinking: expectancies, prototypes, and friends' alcohol use mediate the effect of exposure to alcohol use in movies on adolescent drinking. Health Psychol. 2009;28:473–483. doi: 10.1037/a0014777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Dawson DA, Goldstein RB, Grant BF. Rates and correlates of relapse among individuals in remission from DSM-IV alcohol dependence: A 3-year follow-up. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31:2036–45. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00536.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Distilled Spirits Council of the United States [DISCUS Web site] [Accessed December 20, 2013];Code of responsible practices for beverage alcohol advertising and marketing. 2011 May 26; Available at: http://www.discus.org/responsibility/code/.
  12. Hanewinkel R, Sargent JD, Poelen EAP, Scholte R, Florek E, Sweeting H, Hunt K, Karlsdottir S, Jonsson SH, Mathis F, Faggiano F, Morgenstern M. Alcohol consumption in movies and adolescent binge drinking in 6 European countries. Pediatrics. 2012;129:1–12. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2809. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Henriksen L, Feighery EC, Schleicher NC, Fortmann SP. Receptivity to alcohol marketing predicts initiation of alcohol use. J Adolesc Health. 2008;42:28–35. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hingson R, Heeren T, Winter MV, Wechsler H. Magnitude of alcohol-related mortality and morbidity among U.S. college students age 18–24: changes from 1998 to 2001. Annu Rev Public Health. 2005;26:259–279. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Kupersmidt JB, Scull TM, Austin EW. Media literacy education for elementary school substance use prevention: Study of media detective. Pediatrics. 2010;126:525–531. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-0068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. McClure AC, Stoolmiller M, Tanski SE, Worth KA, Sargent JD. Alcohol-branded merchandise and its association with drinking attitudes and outcomes in US adolescents. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163:211–217. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2008.554. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. McClure AC, Stoolmiller M, Tanski SE, Engels RCME, Sargent JD. Alcohol marketing receptivity, marketing-specific cognitions, and underage binge drinking. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2013;37:E404–E413. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2012.01932.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Nash SG, McQueen A, Bray JH. Pathways to adolescent alcohol use: family environment, peer influence, and parental expectations. J Adolesc Health. 2005;37:19–28. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Petty RE, Cacciopo JT. Attitudes and Persuasion: Classic and Contemporary Approaches. William C Brown and Company; Dubuque, IA: 1981. [Google Scholar]
  20. Pinkleton BE, Weintraub Austin E, Cohen M, Miller A, Fitzgerald E. A statewide evaluation of the effectiveness of media literacy training to prevent tobacco use among adolescents. Health Commun. 2007;21:23–34. doi: 10.1080/10410230701283306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Primack BA, Gold MA, Land SR, Fine MJ. Association of cigarette smoking and media literacy about smoking among adolescents. J Adolesc Health. 2006;39:465–472. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2006.05.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Primack BA, Dalton MA, Carroll MV, Agarwal AA, Fine MJ. Content analysis of tobacco, alcohol, and other drugs in popular music. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2008;162:169–175. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2007.27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Primack BA, Fine D, Yang CK, Wickett D, Zickmund S. Adolescents' impressions of antismoking media literacy education: qualitative results from a randomized controlled trial. Health Educ Res. 2009a;24:608–621. doi: 10.1093/her/cyn062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Primack BA, Sidani J, Carroll MV, Fine MJ. Associations between smoking and media literacy in college students. J Health Commun. 2009b;14:541–555. doi: 10.1080/10810730903089598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Primack BA, Nuzzo E, Rice KR, Sargent JD. Alcohol brand appearances in US popular music. Addiction. 2012;107:557–566. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03649.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Primack BA, Douglas EL, Land SR, Miller E, Fine MJ. Comparison of media literacy and usual education to prevent tobacco use: A cluster-randomized trial. Journal of School Health. 2014 doi: 10.1111/josh.12130. In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Rideout V, Foehr U, Roberts DF. Generation M2: media in the lives of 8–18 year-olds. Kaiser Family Foundation; Menlo Park, CA: 2010. [Accessed December 20, 2013]. Available at: http://kff.org/other/event/generation-m2-media-in-the-lives-of/. [Google Scholar]
  28. Rothman EF, McNaughton Reyes L, Johnson RM, LaValley M. Does the alcohol make them do it? Dating violence perpetration and drinking among youth. Epidemiol Rev. 2012;34:103–19. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxr027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Sales JM, Brown JL, Vissman AT, DiClemente RJ. The association between alcohol use and sexual risk behaviors among African American women across three developmental periods: a review. Curr Drug Abuse Rev. 2012;5:117–28. doi: 10.2174/1874473711205020117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Sargent JD, Wills TA, Stoolmiller M, Gibson J, Gibbons FX. Alcohol use in motion pictures and its relation with early-onset teen drinking. J Stud Alcohol. 2006;67:54–65. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, de la Fuente JR, Grant M. Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption. Addiction. 1993;88:791–804. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Siegel M, Johnson RM, Tyagi K, Power K, Lohsen MC, Ayers AJ, Jernigan DH. Alcohol Brand References in U.S. Popular Music, 2009–2011. Subst Use Misuse. 2013;48:1475–1484. doi: 10.3109/10826084.2013.793716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Smith LA, Foxcroft DR. The effect of alcohol advertising, marketing and portrayal on drinking behaviour in young people: systematic review of prospective cohort studies. BMC Public Health. 2009;9:51. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-9-51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Stephenson MT, Hoyle RH, Palmgreen P, Slater MD. Brief measures of sensation seeking for screening and large-scale surveys. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2003;72:279–286. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2003.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Stoolmiller M, Wills TA, McClure AC, Tanski SE, Worth KA, Gerrard M, Sargent JD. Comparing media and family predictors of alcohol use: a cohort study of US adolescents. BMJ Open. 2012;2:e000543. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2011-000543. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services . The Surgeon General's call to action to prevent and reduce underage drinking. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Office of the Surgeon General; Rockville, MD: 2007. [Accessed December 20, 2013]. Available at: http://www.surgeongeneral.gov/library/calls/underagedrinking/. [Google Scholar]
  37. Wills TA, Sargent JD, Gibbons FX, Gerrard M, Stoolmiller M. Movie exposure to alcohol cues and adolescent alcohol problems: a longitudinal analysis in a national sample. Psychol Addict Behav. 2009;23:23–35. doi: 10.1037/a0014137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES