Abstract
Despite substantial effort and immense need, the treatment options for major neuropsychiatric illnesses like schizophrenia are limited and largely ineffective at improving the most debilitating cognitive symptoms that are central to mental illness. These symptoms include cognitive control deficits, the inability to selectively use information that is currently relevant and ignore what is currently irrelevant. Contemporary attempts to accelerate progress are in part founded on an effort to reconceptualize neuropsychiatric illness as a disorder of neural development. This neuro-developmental framework emphasizes abnormal neural circuits on the one hand, and on the other, it suggests there are therapeutic opportunities to exploit the developmental processes of excitatory neuron pruning, inhibitory neuron proliferation, elaboration of myelination, and other circuit refinements that extend through adolescence and into early adulthood. We have crafted a preclinical research program aimed at cognition failures that may be relevant to mental illness. By working with a variety of neurodevelopmental rodent models, we strive to identify a common pathophysiology that underlies cognitive control failure as well as a common strategy for improving cognition in the face of neural circuit abnormalities. Here we review our work to characterize cognitive control deficits in rats with a neonatal ventral hippocampus lesion and rats that were exposed to Methylazoxymethanol acetate (MAM) in utero. We review our findings as they pertain to early developmental processes, including neurogenesis, as well as the power of cognitive experience to refine neural circuit function within the mature and maturing brain's cognitive circuitry.
Keywords: cognitive control, neurodevelopmental insults, mental illness, hippocampus, neural coordination, schizophrenia models
Introduction
Schizophrenia is a complex mental disorder with varying degrees of severity. One of the most debilitating deficits in schizophrenia patients is impoverished cognitive control, the inability to ignore irrelevant information while using relevant information to complete a task (Nuechterlein et al., 2008). We have chosen to utilize three neurodevelopmental rodent models to examine the hypothesis that poor cognitive control is a result of failed neural coordination, manifest as inappropriate temporal organization of neural electrical activity within and between neural circuits. This neural discoordination hypothesis is agnostic to the etiology of the discoordination. Furthermore, the hypothesis predicts that if neural coordination can be restored, cognitive function will also be restored. We are investigating this hypothesis using different neurodevelopmental models because the disease is increasingly thought to have diverse neurodevelopmental origins. Furthermore, because altering neurodevelopment likely alters a host of nonspecific, or even unidentified factors within the brain, the ability to overcome the functional consequences of abnormal development is quite compelling, especially if compensation for neural discoordination may be achieved without having to target, study, or reverse the original neurodevelopmental insult.
Cognitive control
Cognitive control and neural coordination
Our lab focuses on behaviors that require cognitive control, the process by which relevant information is used, and irrelevant information ignored in the service of a subject's goals. The Stroop test is a canonical test for cognitive control (Stroop, 1935). Subjects can be presented a written word whose meaning is a color. The subject is instructed to name the color in which the word is written and ignore the meaning of the word. In its basic form, the word for a color (e.g., blue) is presented in either the same color (blue) or a different color (e.g., magenta) (Figure 1A). Congruent word-color presentations require minimal cognitive control, whereas the color incongruent presentations require cognitive control to ignore the meaning of the word, which is the prepotent response, and instead report the color in which the word was written. Patients with schizophrenia are impaired at responding to color incongruent presentations and they respond faster to color congruent words than controls (Carter et al., 1992; Perlstein et al., 1998). In a sense, all purposeful experience requires the ability to select for processing the information that is relevant and filter out or ignore what is currently irrelevant, similar to how all such abilities rely on memory. The concept of cognitive control, like memory, as it is studied, however, is more specific. Cognitive control is primarily concerned with the effortful top-down cognitive process (Miller and Cohen, 2001) that biases representations and actions in accord with internal goals. Since cognitive control is not itself an overt action, and like memory, it can only be currently inferred from behavior, it is important to have a clear operational definition of cognitive control along with evaluative tests that can systematically vary the demand for this ability. As we learn more about the neurobiological basis of these psychological concepts it may be possible to define the concepts in terms of their underlying biology (Kelemen and Fenton, 2010).
Our work with rodents is centered on cognitive control because poor cognitive control is a fundamental deficit in schizophrenia and other mental illness, and a good predictor of functional outcome (Carter and Barch, 2007). In addition, cognitive control creates experimental conditions that can help elucidate the form of the neural code that a circuit may be using. Indeed, how neurons code information is a fundamental and open question. The two major hypotheses are dedicated and ensemble coding. Dedicated coding, analogous to the green light in a traffic sign, is based on the concept of cardinal cells (e.g., Barlow, 1972). These are neural units dedicated to signaling high-order stimuli and concepts, like “face” or “grandmother.” Single cells that respond to individuals like “Jennifer Aniston” and specific video clips have been recorded from people (Quiroga et al., 2005; Gelbard-Sagiv et al., 2008), although this may not be sufficiently compelling evidence of a dedicated code (Quiroga et al., 2008).
Our work is founded in the alternative, ensemble coding hypothesis. Ensemble coding uses the activity of a group of cells to encode information (Figure 1B). It is analogous to a stadium jumbotron that uses many lights to encode a message. More messages can be encoded than there are lights, and no single light is essential for a message. Hebb's (1949) cell assembly postulate is an ensemble coding scheme. A cell assembly is a set of linked cells that fire together to represent information. Ensemble coding must avoid the loss of information that would result from conjointly activating multiple representations with overlapping cells, which has been called the superposition catastrophe (Von Der Malsburg and Schneider, 1986). Just as a jumbotron cannot simultaneously display two messages with the same lights, cell assemblies cannot coactivate without mechanisms to appropriately subgroup their conjoint activity. Without effective grouping mechanisms, the coactive assemblies merge into one assembly with catastrophic information loss (“Confusion” in Figure 1B).
One way to avoid the superposition catastrophe when two distinct items need to be represented in a structure is to alternate between activating one representation, then the other (Figure 1B). This mechanism, called temporal or dynamic grouping, may occur on the gamma (40 ms) timescale in neocortex (see Singer, 1999), and the gamma and theta (~140 ms) timescales in hippocampus (Buzsaki, 2010). In fact, during the active place avoidance tasks that we employ, dynamic grouping has been observed in the discharge of rat hippocampal place cells (Kelemen and Fenton, 2010) at multiple time scales from 25 ms to several seconds (Kelemen and Fenton, 2012, 2013).
Straightforward evidence of the hippocampus using an ensemble place code comes from the study of hippocampal place cells. A place cell discharges selectively when a rat is in a portion of the environment called the cell's firing field (O'Keefe and Dostrovsky, 1971; Muller et al., 1987). Place cell firing fields tend to be stable across days (Muller et al., 1987; Thompson and Best, 1990) although they only intermittently participate in the active subset of the hippocampus representation of an environment on the timescales of seconds (Fenton and Muller, 1998; Fenton et al., 2010) and days (Ziv et al., 2013), phenomena consistent with ensemble coding. In addition to this temporal uncertainty, there is also fundamental spatial uncertainty, because place cells in dentate gyrus, CA3 and CA1 have multiple firing fields in sufficiently large environments. This indicates that their place code must be an ensemble code, otherwise the activity of individual place cells have an ambiguous interpretation in terms of spatial location (Fenton et al., 2008; Henriksen et al., 2010; Park et al., 2011).
The hippocampus circuit as an exemplar of neural coordination
Given that the hippocampus expresses an ensemble code, at least for space, then it must utilize neural coordination mechanisms to function and avoid the superposition catastrophe. Electrophysiological recordings from multiple hippocampus neurons and/or sites demonstrate that coordinated activity is accomplished at multiple levels, such as the timing of action potentials, excitation/inhibition balance, and the coordination of oscillatory activity, all rooted in the anatomical connectivity that defines neural circuits (Buzsaki, 2010). Importantly, we observe dynamic grouping of place cell activity during the active place avoidance behavioral paradigm that we use to study cognitive control (Kelemen and Fenton, 2010, 2013) indicating that hippocampal neural coordination is a neural correlate of cognitive control.
For being a relatively simple cortical circuit, the hippocampus is anatomically complex (Figure 1C). The simplistic, traditional version of the circuit is known as the trisynaptic loop, defined as entorhinal cortex ⇒ dentate gyrus ⇒ CA3 ⇒ CA1 ⇒ entorhinal cortex (Andersen et al., 1971). According to the framework first established by Marr (1971), the dentate gyrus is proposed to separate input activity patterns from one another making them more distinctive than they might actually be in the input regions, whereas CA3 is thought to be an autoassociative network that accomplishes the opposite computation, called pattern completion, to attenuate small differences between neural activity patterns. Finally, the CA1 field was conceptualized as a relay to transfer the processed information back to neocortex (Marr, 1971). While each of these information processing steps is important for cognitive function, pattern separation is perhaps the most fundamental for cognitive control and the flexible use of information. It is difficult to understand and act differently in response to two similar experiences if distinctive internal representations are not available to support knowledge and behavior. Consistent with this view, manipulations of the dentate gyrus can improve (Sahay et al., 2011a) or compromise cognitive discrimination and flexibility (Burghardt et al., 2012; Nakashiba et al., 2012).
The contemporary understanding of the hippocampus circuit recognizes that it is substantially more elaborate than the trisynaptic loop suggests (Amaral and Witter, 1989). The entorhinal cortex layer II sends projections to CA3 as well as dentate gyrus, while entorhinal cortex layer III sends direct projections to CA1 (reviewed in Van Strien et al., 2009). Additionally, CA3 and entorhinal cortex send inputs to stratum radiatum and stratum lacunosum of CA1, respectively (Figure 1C). Thus, information coming into CA1 from multiple, anatomically segregated streams must be coordinated to generate coherent and organized CA1 spiking output. Slow gamma (30–60 Hz) oscillations in the CA1 local field potential (LFP) are synchronized with spiking in CA3 and fast gamma (60–100 Hz) oscillations in CA1 are synchronized with medial entorhinal cortex spiking (Colgin et al., 2009). These excitatory pathways are carefully balanced by feedforward and feedback inhibition that is mediated by numerous inhibitory cell types (Freund and Buzsaki, 1996). A number of these interneuron classes are demonstrated to have distinct temporally coordinated relationships to ongoing oscillatory and spiking states of hippocampus activity (Klausberger et al., 2003; Jinno et al., 2007) as well as control of distinct modes of action potential discharge and oscillation-paced spiking (Royer et al., 2012). Thus, the hippocampus circuit is a rich repository of multi-level neural coordination phenomena.
The discoordination hypothesis
Our studies of the rich neural coordination phenomena in hippocampus during tasks with low (Fenton et al., 2008, 2010) and high cognitive control demand (Kelemen and Fenton, 2010, 2012, 2013) naturally lead to the hypothesis that abnormally coordinated neural activity results in poor cognitive control (Fenton, 2008), which is an idea that has been proposed in a number of contexts (Tononi and Edelman, 2000; Phillips and Silverstein, 2003; Lisman et al., 2010). Neural discoordination may occur due to improper functioning in several aspects: excitation/inhibition balance, timing of oscillatory activity, anatomical connectivity or synaptic network function. It is important to recognize that although the activity and functional specialization of any individual brain region may appear to be relatively normal, the dynamics of the interactions within and among brain regions may be disrupted (Tononi and Edelman, 2000). For example, individual place cell properties within the hippocampus may appear to be unchanged while interactions amongst cells within one hippocampus (intrahippocampal coordination) or between the hippocampi (interhippocampal coordination) could be sufficiently altered to cause cognitive impairment (Lee et al., 2012). We emphasize below that the causes of cognition-impairing discoordination can be very diverse and, consequently, the expression of discoordination can also be diverse. The bulk of prior work on mental illness and animal models has focused on disruptions of “unitary” processes like synaptic plasticity, neurotransmission failure or excess, and abnormally localized function, whereas we wish to advocate for the growing awareness that the interactions amongst relatively intact processes may be abnormal in mental illness (Phillips and Silverstein, 2003). As these interactions become better understood we expect that specific forms of neural discoordination will underlie specific features of cognitive dysfunction. Such biomarkers are likely to be important for differentiating patients with diagnoses of heterogeneous illnesses such as schizophrenia, and most other forms of mental illness, including even those with remarkable heterogeneity of symptoms, despite a specific genetic origin like the fragile × syndrome (Mitchell et al., 2013). Unfortunately it is still early days.
The discoordination hypothesis makes three general predictions that our research program is testing. (1) Subjects with neural discoordination will perform poorly in tasks that require cognitive control. (2) Conversely, subjects with poor cognitive control have neural discoordination. (3) Normalizing neural coordination is sufficient to restore cognitive performance in cognitively impaired individuals, regardless of the etiology of the dysfunction. Put another way, how neural discoordination arises is secondary if we can determine a way to restore the system to the appropriately coordinated state.
Cognitive control in rodents
As a rat moves across the substrate of an arena that is housed in a lit room, the rat can know its location using three principle sources of information: (1) local (arena) cues, (2) distal (room) cues, and (3) idiothetic cues from its self-motion senses (reviewed in Bures and Fenton, 2000; Fenton and Bures, 2003). This is analogous to our own ability to know locations in a room relative to the furniture, relative to the enclosing walls, and relative to the distance and direction of our own movements. We have repeatedly demonstrated that any one of these three cue sets is sufficient for place avoidance (Bures et al., 1997, 1998; Fenton et al., 1998; Stuchlik et al., 2001; Kubik and Fenton, 2005; Wesierska et al., 2005). Under normal circumstances, all estimates converge on the same location, making it impossible for a researcher to know which cues the animal is using by observing overt behavior. However, slowly rotating the arena dissociates the information from the spatial frames of the arena and room and this allows resolution of the ambiguity. In an early study, rats were trained to avoid a shock zone on a stationary arena. After they had reached their performance asymptote, the shock was turned off and the arena was rotated for the first time in the rat's life. It was only then that we observed that the animals had learned to avoid both the stationary location of shock in the room and the location of shock on the arena, which was now rotating (Fenton et al., 1998).
We devised the active place avoidance behavioral paradigm on a rotating arena to evaluate forms of cognition, including cognitive control in rodents (Bures et al., 1997; Cimadevilla et al., 2000b, 2001a). The standard task variant is configured to present rodents with the cognitively challenging component of the Stroop test for humans. Rodents are exposed to two competing streams of information, one of which must be ignored to successfully avoid a shock zone (Figures 2A–C). The animals are placed on the slowly rotating arena with a stationary shock zone, the location of which is defined within the room coordinates. Because only the room cues provide relevant information for learning and avoiding the location of shock, the animals must not associate shock with locations on the rotating arena. The rotating cues are irrelevant for avoiding shock because these cues do not provide information to predict shock. Normal laboratory rodents quickly learn this standard task variant, reaching asymptotic performance within three 10-min trials (Figures 2B,D). This so called “Room + Arena-” variant of the task requires the dorsal hippocampus (Cimadevilla et al., 2000a) and is one of the most sensitive rodent tasks to hippocampal dysfunction, as it is impaired by even partial, unilateral inactivation of a hippocampus (Cimadevilla et al., 2001b; Wesierska et al., 2005). We have also directly demonstrated task-relevant neural coordination phenomena in single-unit ensemble and LFP studies during performance of place avoidance tasks (Kelemen and Fenton, 2010, 2012, 2013; Lee et al., 2012).
Rotating the experimental arena, as in the standard place avoidance task variant, allowed us to operationally define cognitive control as the ability to selectively interpret and respond to task-relevant stimuli while suppressing competing irrelevant interpretations and stimuli. If all of the sources of information converge on the same place, cognitive control is unnecessary (stable arena in a lit room). When the arena rotates, the spatial frames are dissociated and cognitive control becomes necessary because during learning, each time the rat is shocked, it must select an interpretation. The rat must learn the stationary room location of the shocks and suppress associating the rotating locations of shock with the irrelevant arena locations. Cognitive control is less necessary for the various one-frame task variants such as the (Room&Arena)+ configuration on a stationary arena or the Room+ variant on a rotating arena with shallow water that hides the local cues. Impaired two-frame avoidance and spared one-frame avoidance is pathognomonic of selectively-impaired cognitive control.
Three schizophrenia-related neurodevelopmental models
Poor cognitive control is one of the most debilitating impairments in schizophrenia and, because it is central to the discoordination hypothesis, it is necessary that animal models valuable for testing the hypothesis have cognitive control deficits. According to the hypothesis, it follows that animal models that do not display impairments in cognitive control will have normal neural coordination. We are testing the discoordination hypothesis using three neurodevelopmental animal models that are considered to be relevant to schizophrenia: (1) a temporary hippocampal inactivation model (Lipska et al., 2002b) in which tetrodotoxin is injected into the neonatal ventral hippocampus (ttxNVHL) on postnatal day 7 (PD7), (2) the permanent neonatal ventral hippocampal lesion (Lipska et al., 1992) caused by injecting the excitotoxin ibotenic acid (iboNVHL) on PD7, and (3) exposure of the developing brain to the toxin methylazoxymethanol acetate (MAM) at gestational day 17 (GD17-MAM) (Lodge and Grace, 2009).
The ttxNVHL and iboNVHL neurodevelopmental models involve direct insults to the developing hippocampus with the intention of altering development of connected brain circuits such as those in the prefrontal cortex, dorsal hippocampus, nucleus accumbens and amygdala (Lipska and Weinberger, 2002). The hippocampus is temporarily inactivated in the ttxNVHL model and permanently damaged in the iboNVHL. TTX inactivation likely lasts about half a day based on studies of the effect of TTX in the adult CNS (Zhuravin and Bures, 1991; Olypher et al., 2006). In both versions of the NVHL model the insult occurs on PD7, a time that is thought to correspond to the third trimester of pregnancy in the course of human brain development (Rakhade and Jensen, 2009; Sengupta, 2011). It is important that the insult occur at an early age to sufficiently impact the development of brain circuits. Performing the lesion in an adult hippocampus does not result in the same schizophrenia-related abnormalities (Lipska et al., 2002a; Tseng et al., 2009) implying there is a developmental component to the consequences of the NVHL procedure. The impact of MAM on the developing brain is nonspecific but a comprehensive review of identified changes is available (Lodge and Grace, 2009). Among brain regions affected by MAM are the prefrontal cortex (Esmaeili and Grace, 2013), nucleus accumbens (Perez and Lodge, 2012), ventral hippocampus (Lodge and Grace, 2007), and the ventral subiculum-nucleus accumbens-prefrontal cortex circuit (Belujon et al., 2014). Histological assessment of hippocampi in GD17-MAM animals shows morphological deficiencies (Matricon et al., 2010) that we have confirmed in unpublished studies. These deficiencies include a thinned pyramidal cell layer with disruptions in the CA1 and CA3 subfields. It may be important that the timing of MAM administration coincides with peak neurogenesis in the CA1 and CA3 subfields during embryogenesis (Schlessinger et al., 1978; Bayer, 1980).
Active place avoidance in the three neurodevelopmental models
We are interested in finding diverse sets of conditions that converge on the canonical target deficit in cognition because the discoordination hypothesis predicts that neural coordination abnormalities accompany cognitive control deficits. We used male Long-Evans rats for the studies discussed here. Adult ttxNVHL animals do not display cognitive deficits in the active place avoidance task (Figure 3A). It is unlikely that the unimpaired performance in the ttxNVHL group is due to a lack of overt lesion since ventral hippocampus is not necessary for spatial learning (Kjelstrup et al., 2002), including active place avoidance. The reason for the lack of ttxNVHL effect on cognitive control is uncertain, though we suspect the insult did not sufficiently alter neural circuit development. While measurements have yet to be done, the discoordination hypothesis predicts there will be normal neural coordination in these animals. In contrast, adult iboNVHL animals display poor cognitive control (Figure 3B) and have abnormal neural coordination while performing the active place avoidance task, consistent with the discoordination hypothesis (Lee et al., 2012, 2014). Similar to iboNVHL animals, adult GD17-MAM animals have deficits in the active place avoidance task (Figure 3C), and according to the discoordination hypothesis, neural discoordination is expected in the GD17-MAM model, especially while the animals are performing cognitively challenging tasks. Accordingly, we are focusing on the iboNVHL and GD17-MAM model for further investigation.
Cognitive flexibility in the active place avoidance task
One common feature of the iboNVHL and GD17-MAM neurodevelopmental models is that animals appear to have impaired active place avoidance learning, but with continued training they can eventually perform the task (Figures 3B,C, 4C,D). After the animals have established asymptotic performance, we can further test cognitive control, specifically cognitive flexibility, by shifting the shock zone 180° from the original location. In these conflict trials, control and ttxNVHL animals quickly acquire the ability to avoid the new shock zone, while iboNVHL and GD17-MAM animals make many more errors (Figure 4). Thus, the initial training was not sufficient to overcome the impairment in these two models and perhaps, conflict learning itself was impaired. Previous work showed that mice learned the active place avoidance task as well as controls after ablation of adult neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus by either X-irradiation or a genetic manipulation (Burghardt et al., 2012). Interestingly, the same mice were impaired on the 180° conflict trials from the initial location. They were impaired in that they continued to avoid the initial shock zone location. This cognitive inflexibility caused them to enter the current shock zone often because this location had been their preferred location for avoiding shock in the initial zone. Further, these mice had no difficulty to extinguish the initially learned avoidance when the shock was turned off. This intact extinction learning indicates the deficit was specific to being able to discriminate between the representations of the initial and new shock zone locations and express flexible behavior in response to the changed shock-location contingency.
An important feature of the dentate gyrus is the existence of neurogenesis into adulthood. These findings with place avoidance contribute to mounting evidence for a specific role of adult neurogenesis and the dentate gyrus in cognition. One hypothesis is that recently matured adult born granule cells modulate the excitability of the dentate gyrus network so that it can more easily express differential patterns of activity in response to similar conditions that differ in subtle but important ways, such as the changed location of transient shock (Sahay et al., 2011b; Ikrar et al., 2013). An alternative proposal is that newly matured granule cells preferentially participate in the encoding of memories and thus as time passes, different neurons will represent different information and events (Aimone et al., 2010; Deng et al., 2010). Both hypotheses recognize a role for adult born neurons in pattern separation-dependent learning. The pattern separation computation is thought to underlie the generation of distinctive neural representations of similar items, which, by averting overgeneralization and catastrophic interference at the level of neural representations, can promote effective cognitive discriminations.
The neurodevelopmental opportunity
Prolonged development of the brain does not permit a simple definition of immature or mature. The formation of neural circuits, in the most basic form, is established by neurogenesis in utero. However, although these foundational neural circuits are present at birth, substantial refinement occurs until adulthood. The complex development of brain circuits and functions has been discussed in comprehensive reviews, such as these examples that consider prolonged changes in both brain organization and behavioral expression (Bachevalier and Beauregard, 1993; Rice and Barone, 2000). Here we will focus on a subset of details that concern the hippocampus in the effort to advance our argument that a developmental perspective is valuable for conceptualizing and investigating mental illness. In the hippocampal formation, prolonged development into the postnatal period is evident in several aspects and the timing of some major developmental processes have been summarized in Table 1.
Table 1.
Shaded areas correspond to changes during development. The darker the color, the larger the change is within that row, but do not correlate to magnitude of changes with respect to other rows. In categories where changes seemed even across development, the middle shade of gray was selected. Data is from rats unless indicated otherwise. CA1, cornu ammonis; reDG, dentate gyrus.
These prolonged developmental processes create a window of vulnerability for negative or positive impact on adult cognitive ability because the functional refinement of neural circuits establishes the foundation upon which future coordinated and temporally sculpted neural activity is based (Buzsaki, 2010). This window of vulnerability is the basis for the two-hit hypothesis of precipitating schizophrenia and other mental illness when environmental factors have a negative impact on cognitive circuits during adolescence. As such, schizophrenia is hypothesized to be a neurodevelopmental disorder (Weinberger, 1987, 1996; Insel, 2010) and certainly the increased odds of the disorder given a neonatal insult suggests some kind of developmental process is involved (Weinberger, 1987). The ttxNVHL and gestational exposure to MAM produce absent or strong forms of cognitive control impairment, respectively, indicating that development must be taken seriously in the realm of cognition. Importantly, the developmental insult doesn't negatively impact everything, as evidenced by the fact that the animals with neurodevelopmental insults can still see, locomote, and learn in many domains, like controls. Adult iboNVHL rats have normal forms of hippocampus-dependent memory (Beninger et al., 2009). Normal learning was also demonstrated in the place avoidance paradigm because adult iboNVHL were indistinguishable from control rats in the task when the arena was covered with shallow water to attenuate the irrelevant arena-frame spatial cues (Lee et al., 2012).
We are evaluating the idea that it is possible to take advantage of the prolonged development in order to correct the cognitive impairments, or at least improve outcomes, by modifying neural circuit refinement. We examined the impact of cognitive training during adolescence on adult active place avoidance (Figure 5). When tested during adolescence, the iboNVHL animals were capable of learning the active place avoidance task as well as control animals (Figure 5B). Remarkably, iboNVHL animals that were trained during adolescence performed as well as control animals during adulthood in the initial learning and conflict learning variants of the active place avoidance task (Figure 5C). These benefits seemed to generalize broadly because the adolescence training also normalized reversal learning in a T-maze (Lee et al., 2012). In contrast, iboNVHL animals still showed cognitive impairments as adults if they were merely exposed to the rotating arena without any explicit cognitive demand during adolescence (Figure 5C). These cognitive behavioral benefits were paralleled by normalization of the synchrony of LFP oscillations between the two hippocampi, which directly measures neural coordination. Thus, cognitive experience in adolescence is demonstrated to improve both cognitive performance and neural coordination. These findings establish a rationale for investigating whether appropriately timed preemptive cognitive training can be therapeutic. We are naturally interested in examining the generality of these findings and are investigating whether preemptive cognitive training in adolescence can also improve cognition and neural coordination in the GD17-MAM model.
Possible targets to therapeutically improve neural coordination and cognitive outcomes
We are investigating potential mechanisms by which preemptive early cognitive training can correct neural coordination and cognition in models of mental dysfunction. As our ongoing investigations focus on the hippocampus, we discuss the hippocampal circuit (Figure 6), but suggest that this perspective on neural circuit computations may be advantageous for considerations of many circuits involved in cognition.
CA1 receives converging, temporally coordinated inputs from CA3 and entorhinal cortex (Colgin et al., 2009), and we have observed discoordination in these signals in genetic and pharmacological animal models with active place avoidance deficits (unpublished observations). If synaptic function is not appropriately weighted between these two regions, we hypothesize that cognitive training can adjust relative synaptic functional strengths within the neural circuit in a use-dependent manner. According to this synaptic plasticity hypothesis, we speculate that the appropriate cognitive training can refine and tune the circuit so that it operates in an improved functional state. This synaptic restructuring promotes neural coordination in CA1 in support of cognitive control and flexibility. In addition to synaptic plasticity mechanisms, cognitive training may alter neural circuit and synaptic network function by changing the expression of dendritic spine structure (Yang et al., 2009) and/or structural proteins (Gray et al., 2006; Clem et al., 2008; Wen et al., 2013). In fact, we have observed that place avoidance training in adolescence decreased parvalbumin expression in mPFC interneurons (Lee et al., 2012).
As another potential therapeutic target, we have also considered neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus, which continues past postnatal day 18 (Bayer, 1980) and can be modified throughout adulthood by environmental enrichment and experience (Kempermann et al., 1997; Van Praag et al., 2005; Mirescu and Gould, 2006; Zainuddin and Thuret, 2012; Gregoire et al., 2014). As discussed above, adult dentate gyrus neurogenesis has a role in cognitive flexibility, measured by the place avoidance paradigm. At 1 month of age, dentate gyrus neurogenesis is higher and apoptosis is lower than at 3 months of age in the rat (Silva et al., 2006). Experiences that alter adult dentate gyrus neurogenesis may especially do so during adolescence. Thus, the adolescent cognitive training that normalized cognition and related neural coordination may have adjusted the balance between neurogenesis and apoptosis, promoting better pattern separation and completion computations within the hippocampal circuit.
Schizophrenia, and other forms of mental illness present some of the most difficult medical problems to investigate because of the diverse and unknown etiology of the disorders. Our effort focuses on cognitive control, a key symptom, and evaluates how multiple animal models impact that ability. The goals are to converge on a common pathophysiology that is important for the cognitive deficit, independent of the experimental manipulation, and to identify ways in which to alter and attenuate this pathophysiology. We are a long way from obtaining those goals but an optimistic possibility that this effort has suggested is that neurodevelopmental vulnerability can be exploited and harnessed by cognitive experience to improve cognitive outcomes later in life.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by R01MH084038 and R01MH099128.
References
- Aimone J. B., Deng W., Gage F. H. (2010). Adult neurogenesis: integrating theories and separating functions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 325–337 10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amaral D. G., Witter M. P. (1989). The three-dimensional organization of the hippocampal formation: a review of anatomical data. Neuroscience 31, 571–591 10.1016/0306-4522(89)90424-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen P., Bliss T. V., Skrede K. K. (1971). Lamellar organization of hippocampal pathways. Exp. Brain Res. 13, 222–238 10.1007/BF00234087 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bachevalier J., Beauregard M. (1993). Maturation of medial temporal lobe memory functions in rodents, monkeys, and humans. Hippocampus 3, 191–201 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barlow H. B. (1972). Single units and sensation: a neuron doctrine for perceptual psychology? Perception 1, 371–394 10.1068/p010371 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bayer S. A. (1980). Development of the hippocampal region in the rat. I. Neurogenesis examined with 3H-thymidine autoradiography. J. Comp. Neurol. 190, 87–114 10.1002/cne.901900107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belujon P., Patton M. H., Grace A. A. (2014). Role of the prefrontal cortex in altered hippocampal-accumbens synaptic plasticity in a developmental animal model of schizophrenia. Cereb. Cortex 24, 968–977 10.1093/cercor/bhs380 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beninger R. J., Tuerke K. J., Forsyth J. K., Giles A., Xue L., Boegman R. J., et al. (2009). Neonatal ventral hippocampal lesions in male and female rats: effects on water maze, locomotor activity, plus-maze and prefrontal cortical GABA and glutamate release in adulthood. Behav. Brain Res. 202, 198–209 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.03.044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bures J., Fenton A. A. (2000). Neurophysiology of spatial cognition. News Physiol. Sci. 15, 233–240 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bures J., Fenton A. A., Kaminsky Y., Rossier J., Sacchetti B., Zinyuk L. (1997). Dissociation of exteroceptive and idiothetic orientation cues: effect on hippocampal place cells and place navigation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 352, 1515–1524 10.1098/rstb.1997.0138 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bures J., Fenton A. A., Kaminsky Y., Wesierska M., Zahalka A. (1998). Rodent navigation after dissociation of the allocentric and idiothetic representations of space. Neuropharmacology 37, 689–699 10.1016/S0028-3908(98)00031-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burghardt N. S., Park E. H., Hen R., Fenton A. A. (2012). Adult-born hippocampal neurons promote cognitive flexibility in mice. Hippocampus 22, 1795–1808 10.1002/hipo.22013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsaki G. (2010). Neural syntax: cell assemblies, synapsembles, and readers. Neuron 68, 362–385 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter C. S., Barch D. M. (2007). Cognitive neuroscience-based approaches to measuring and improving treatment effects on cognition in schizophrenia: the CNTRICS initiative. Schizophr. Bull. 33, 1131–1137 10.1093/schbul/sbm081 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter C. S., Robertson L. C., Nordahl T. E. (1992). Abnormal processing of irrelevant information in chronic schizophrenia: selective enhancement of Stroop facilitation. Psychiatry Res. 41, 137–146 10.1016/0165-1781(92)90105-C [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen L., Perez S. M., Lodge D. J. (2014). An augmented dopamine system function is present prior to puberty in the methylazoxymethanol acetate rodent model of schizophrenia. Dev. Neurobiol. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1002/dneu.22172 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cimadevilla J. M., Fenton A. A., Bures J. (2000a). Functional inactivation of dorsal hippocampus impairs active place avoidance in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 285, 53–56 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)01019-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cimadevilla J. M., Fenton A. A., Bures J. (2001a). New spatial cognition tests for mice: passive place avoidance on stable and active place avoidance on rotating arenas. Brain Res. Bull. 54, 559–563 10.1016/S0361-9230(01)00448-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cimadevilla J. M., Kaminsky Y., Fenton A., Bures J. (2000b). Passive and active place avoidance as a tool of spatial memory research in rats. J. Neurosci. Methods 102, 155–164 10.1016/S0165-0270(00)00288-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cimadevilla J. M., Wesierska M., Fenton A. A., Bures J. (2001b). Inactivating one hippocampus impairs avoidance of a stable room-defined place during dissociation of arena cues from room cues by rotation of the arena. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 3531–3536 10.1073/pnas.051628398 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clem R. L., Celikel T., Barth A. L. (2008). Ongoing in vivo experience triggers synaptic metaplasticity in the neocortex. Science 319, 101–104 10.1126/science.1143808 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colgin L. L., Denninger T., Fyhn M., Hafting T., Bonnevie T., Jensen O., et al. (2009). Frequency of gamma oscillations routes flow of information in the hippocampus. Nature 462, 353–357 10.1038/nature08573 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Danglot L., Triller A., Marty S. (2006). The development of hippocampal interneurons in rodents. Hippocampus 16, 1032–1060 10.1002/hipo.20225 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deng J. B., Yu D. M., Wu P., Li M. S. (2007). The tracing study of developing entorhino-hippocampal pathway. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 25, 251–258 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2007.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deng W., Aimone J. B., Gage F. H. (2010). New neurons and new memories: how does adult hippocampal neurogenesis affect learning and memory? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 339–350 10.1038/nrn2822 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Esmaeili B., Grace A. A. (2013). Afferent drive of medial prefrontal cortex by hippocampus and amygdala is altered in MAM-treated rats: evidence for interneuron dysfunction. Neuropsychopharmacology 38, 1871–1880 10.1038/npp.2013.64 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fenton A. A. (2008). “Neural coordination and psychotic disorganization,” in Information Processing by Neuronal Populations, eds Holscher C., Munk M. H. (London: Cambridge University Press; ), 387–408 [Google Scholar]
- Fenton A. A., Bures J. (2003). “Navigation in the moving world Chapter 13,” in The Neurobiology of Spatial Behaviour, ed Jeffery K. (Oxford: Oxford University Press; ), 240–258 [Google Scholar]
- Fenton A. A., Kao H.-Y., Neymotin S. A., Olypher A. V., Vayntrub Y., Lytton W. W., et al. (2008). Unmasking the CA1 ensemble place code by exposures to small and large environments: more place cells and multiple, irregularly-arranged, and expanded place fields in the larger space. J. Neurosci. 28, 11250–11262 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2862-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fenton A. A., Lytton W. W., Barry J. M., Lenck-Santini P. P., Zinyuk L. E., Kubik S., et al. (2010). Attention-like modulation of hippocampus place cell discharge. J. Neurosci. 30, 4613–4625 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5576-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fenton A. A., Muller R. U. (1998). Place cell discharge is extremely variable during individual passes of the rat through the firing field. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 3182–3187 10.1073/pnas.95.6.3182 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fenton A. A., Wesierska M., Kaminsky Y., Bures J. (1998). Both here and there: simultaneous expression of autonomous spatial memories in rats. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 11493–11498 10.1073/pnas.95.19.11493 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freund T. F., Buzsaki G. (1996). Interneurons of the hippocampus. Hippocampus 6, 347–470 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gelbard-Sagiv H., Mukamel R., Harel M., Malach R., Fried I. (2008). Internally generated reactivation of single neurons in human hippocampus during free recall. Science 322, 96–101 10.1126/science.1164685 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray N. W., Weimer R. M., Bureau I., Svoboda K. (2006). Rapid redistribution of synaptic PSD-95 in the neocortex in vivo. PLoS Biol. 4:e370 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040370 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gregoire C. A., Bonenfant D., Le Nguyen A., Aumont A., Fernandes K. J. (2014). Untangling the influences of voluntary running, environmental complexity, social housing and stress on adult hippocampal neurogenesis. PLoS ONE 9:e86237 10.1371/journal.pone.0086237 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hebb D. O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior. New York, NY: Wiley [Google Scholar]
- Henriksen E. J., Colgin L. L., Barnes C. A., Witter M. P., Moser M. B., Moser E. I. (2010). Spatial representation along the proximodistal axis of CA1. Neuron 68, 127–137 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holter N. I., Zuber N., Bruehl C., Draguhn A. (2007). Functional maturation of developing interneurons in the molecular layer of mouse dentate gyrus. Brain Res. 1186, 56–64 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.09.089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ikrar T., Guo N., He K., Besnard A., Levinson S., Hill A., et al. (2013). Adult neurogenesis modifies excitability of the dentate gyrus. Front. Neural Circuits 7:204 10.3389/fncir.2013.00204 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insel T. R. (2010). Rethinking schizophrenia. Nature 468, 187–193 10.1038/nature09552 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jinno S., Klausberger T., Marton L. F., Dalezios Y., Roberts J. D., Fuentealba P., et al. (2007). Neuronal diversity in GABAergic long-range projections from the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 27, 8790–8804 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1847-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelemen E., Fenton A. A. (2010). Dynamic grouping of hippocampal neural activity during cognitive control of two spatial frames. PLoS Biol. 8:e1000403 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000403 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelemen E., Fenton A. A. (2012). The Organization of Neuronal Discharge on Timescales of Milliseconds and Seconds is Related to the Spatial Response Properties Of Hippocampal Neurons. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag [Google Scholar]
- Kelemen E., Fenton A. A. (2013). Key features of human episodic recollection in the cross-episode retrieval of rat hippocampus representations of space. PLoS Biol. 11:e1001607 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001607 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kempermann G., Kuhn H. G., Gage F. H. (1997). More hippocampal neurons in adult mice living in an enriched environment. Nature 386, 493–495 10.1038/386493a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kjelstrup K. G., Tuvnes F. A., Steffenach H. A., Murison R., Moser E. I., Moser M. B. (2002). Reduced fear expression after lesions of the ventral hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 10825–10830 10.1073/pnas.152112399 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klausberger T., Magill P. J., Marton L. F., Roberts J. D., Cobden P. M., Buzsaki G., et al. (2003). Brain-state- and cell-type-specific firing of hippocampal interneurons in vivo. Nature 421, 844–848 10.1038/nature01374 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kubik S., Fenton A. A. (2005). Behavioral evidence that segregation and representation are dissociable hippocampal functions. J. Neurosci. 25, 9205–9212 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1707-05.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langston R. F., Ainge J. A., Couey J. J., Canto C. B., Bjerknes T. L., Witter M. P., et al. (2010). Development of the spatial representation system in the rat. Science 328, 1576–1580 10.1126/science.1188210 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee H., Dvorak D., Fenton A. A. (2014). Targeting neural synchrony deficits is sufficient to improve cognition in a schizophrenia-related neurodevelopmental model. Front. Psychiatry 5:15 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee H., Dvorak D., Kao H. Y., Duffy A. M., Scharfman H. E., Fenton A. A. (2012). Early cognitive experience prevents adult deficits in a neurodevelopmental schizophrenia model. Neuron 75, 714–724 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipska B. K., Aultman J. M., Verma A., Weinberger D. R., Moghaddam B. (2002a). Neonatal damage of the ventral hippocampus impairs working memory in the rat. Neuropsychopharmacology 27, 47–54 10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00282-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipska B. K., Halim N. D., Segal P. N., Weinberger D. R. (2002b). Effects of reversible inactivation of the neonatal ventral hippocampus on behavior in the adult rat. J. Neurosci. 22, 2835–2842 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipska B. K., Jaskiw G. E., Chrapusta S., Karoum F., Weinberger D. R. (1992). Ibotenic acid lesion of the ventral hippocampus differentially affects dopamine and its metabolites in the nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex in the rat. Brain Res. 585, 1–6 10.1016/0006-8993(92)91184-G [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipska B. K., Weinberger D. R. (2002). A neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia: neonatal disconnection of the hippocampus. Neurotox. Res. 4, 469–475 10.1080/1029842021000022089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lisman J. E., Pi H. J., Zhang Y., Otmakhova N. A. (2010). A thalamo-hippocampal-ventral tegmental area loop may produce the positive feedback that underlies the psychotic break in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 68, 17–24 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.04.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lodge D. J., Grace A. A. (2007). Aberrant hippocampal activity underlies the dopamine dysregulation in an animal model of schizophrenia. J. Neurosci. 27, 11424–11430 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2847-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lodge D. J., Grace A. A. (2009). Gestational methylazoxymethanol acetate administration: a developmental disruption model of schizophrenia. Behav. Brain Res. 204, 306–312 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.01.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marr D. (1971). Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 262, 23–81 10.1098/rstb.1971.0078 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matricon J., Bellon A., Frieling H., Kebir O., Le Pen G., Beuvon F., et al. (2010). Neuropathological and Reelin deficiencies in the hippocampal formation of rats exposed to MAM; differences and similarities with schizophrenia. PLoS ONE 5:e10291 10.1371/journal.pone.0010291 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meier S., Brauer A. U., Heimrich B., Nitsch R., Savaskan N. E. (2004). Myelination in the hippocampus during development and following lesion. Cell. Mol. Life Sci. 61, 1082–1094 10.1007/s00018-004-3469-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller E. K., Cohen J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mirescu C., Gould E. (2006). Stress and adult neurogenesis. Hippocampus 16, 233–238 10.1002/hipo.20155 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell K. J., O'donnell P., Durstewitz D., Fenton A. A., Gingrich J. A., Gordon J. A., et al. (2013). “A framework for the use of models in schizophrenia,” in Schizophrenia: Evolution and Synthesis, eds Silverstein S. M., Moghaddam B., Wykes T. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; ), 212–226 [Google Scholar]
- Muller R. U., Kubie J. L., Ranck J. B., Jr. (1987). Spatial firing patterns of hippocampal complex-spike cells in a fixed environment. J. Neurosci. 7, 1935–1950 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakashiba T., Cushman J. D., Pelkey K. A., Renaudineau S., Buhl D. L., McHugh T. J., et al. (2012). Young dentate granule cells mediate pattern separation, whereas old granule cells facilitate pattern completion. Cell 149, 188–201 10.1016/j.cell.2012.01.046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nuechterlein K. H., Green M. F., Kern R. S., Baade L. E., Barch D. M., Cohen J. D., et al. (2008). The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, part 1: test selection, reliability, and validity. Am. J. Psychiatry 165, 203–213 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07010042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Keefe J., Dostrovsky J. (1971). The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res. 34, 171–175 10.1016/0006-8993(71)90358-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olypher A. V., Klement D., Fenton A. A. (2006). Cognitive disorganization in hippocampus: a physiological model of the disorganization in psychosis. J. Neurosci. 26, 158–168 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2064-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Reilly K. C., Gulden Dahl A., Ulsaker Kruge I., Witter M. P. (2013). Subicular-parahippocampal projections revisited: development of a complex topography in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 521, 4284–4299 10.1002/cne.23417 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park E., Dvorak D., Fenton A. A. (2011). Ensemble place codes in hippocampus: CA1, CA3, and dentate gyrus place cells have multiple place fields in large environments. PLoS ONE 6:e22349 10.1371/journal.pone.0022349 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perez S. M., Lodge D. J. (2012). Aberrant dopamine D2-like receptor function in a rodent model of schizophrenia. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 343, 288–295 10.1124/jpet.112.193201 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perlstein W. M., Carter C. S., Barch D. M., Baird J. W. (1998). The Stroop task and attention deficits in schizophrenia: a critical evaluation of card and single-trial Stroop methodologies. Neuropsychology 12, 414–425 10.1037/0894-4105.12.3.414 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips W. A., Silverstein S. M. (2003). Convergence of biological and psychological perspectives on cognitive coordination in schizophrenia. Behav. Brain Sci. 26, 65–82 discussion: 82–137. 10.1017/S0140525X03000025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pokorny J., Yamamoto T. (1981). Postnatal ontogenesis of hippocampal CA1 area in rats. I. Development of dendritic arborisation in pyramidal neurons. Brain Res. Bull. 7, 113–120 10.1016/0361-9230(81)90075-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quiroga R. Q., Kreiman G., Koch C., Fried I. (2008). Sparse but not ‘grandmother-cell’ coding in the medial temporal lobe. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 87–91 10.1016/j.tics.2007.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quiroga R. Q., Reddy L., Kreiman G., Koch C., Fried I. (2005). Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain. Nature 435, 1102–1107 10.1038/nature03687 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rakhade S. N., Jensen F. E. (2009). Epileptogenesis in the immature brain: emerging mechanisms. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 5, 380–391 10.1038/nrneurol.2009.80 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice D., Barone S., Jr. (2000). Critical periods of vulnerability for the developing nervous system: evidence from humans and animal models. Environ. Health Perspect. 108 Suppl. 3, 511–533 10.1289/ehp.00108s3511 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Royer S., Zemelman B. V., Losonczy A., Kim J., Chance F., Magee J. C., et al. (2012). Control of timing, rate and bursts of hippocampal place cells by dendritic and somatic inhibition. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 769–775 10.1038/nn.3077 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sahay A., Scobie K. N., Hill A. S., O'Carroll C. M., Kheirbek M. A., Burghardt N. S., et al. (2011a). Increasing adult hippocampal neurogenesis is sufficient to improve pattern separation. Nature 472, 466–470 10.1038/nature09817 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sahay A., Wilson D. A., Hen R. (2011b). Pattern separation: a common function for new neurons in hippocampus and olfactory bulb. Neuron 70, 582–588 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schlessinger A. R., Cowan W. M., Swanson L. W. (1978). The time of origin of neurons in Ammon's horn and the associated retrohippocampal fields. Anat. Embryol. 154, 153–173 10.1007/BF00304660 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sengupta P. (2011). A scientific review of age determination for a laboratory rat: how old is it in comparison with human age? Biomed. Int. 2, 81–89 [Google Scholar]
- Silva R., Lu J., Wu Y., Martins L., Almeida O. F., Sousa N. (2006). Mapping cellular gains and losses in the postnatal dentate gyrus: implications for psychiatric disorders. Exp. Neurol. 200, 321–331 10.1016/j.expneurol.2006.02.119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singer W. (1999). Time as coding space? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 9, 189–194 10.1016/S0959-4388(99)80026-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stroop J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 643–662 10.1037/h0054651 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stuchlik A., Fenton A. A., Bures J. (2001). Substratal idiothetic navigation of rats is impaired by removal or devaluation of extramaze and intramaze cues. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 3537–3542 10.1073/pnas.051630498 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson L. T., Best P. J. (1990). Long-term stability of the place-field activity of single units recorded from the dorsal hippocampus of freely behaving rats. Brain Res. 509, 299–308 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90555-P [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tononi G., Edelman G. M. (2000). Schizophrenia and the mechanisms of conscious integration. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 31, 391–400 10.1016/S0165-0173(99)00056-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tseng K. Y., Chambers R. A., Lipska B. K. (2009). The neonatal ventral hippocampal lesion as a heuristic neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia. Behav. Brain Res. 204, 295–305 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.11.039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Praag H., Shubert T., Zhao C., Gage F. H. (2005). Exercise enhances learning and hippocampal neurogenesis in aged mice. J. Neurosci. 25, 8680–8685 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1731-05.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Strien N. M., Cappaert N. L., Witter M. P. (2009). The anatomy of memory: an interactive overview of the parahippocampal-hippocampal network. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 272–282 10.1038/nrn2614 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Von Der Malsburg C., Schneider W. (1986). A neural cocktail-party processor. Biol. Cybern. 54, 29–40 10.1007/BF00337113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinberger D. R. (1987). Implications of normal brain development for the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 44, 660–669 10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800190080012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinberger D. R. (1996). On the plausibility of “the neurodevelopmental hypothesis” of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 14, 1S–11S 10.1016/0893-133X(95)00199-N [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wen J. A., Deblois M. C., Barth A. L. (2013). Initiation, labile, and stabilization phases of experience-dependent plasticity at neocortical synapses. J. Neurosci. 33, 8483–8493 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3575-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wesierska M., Dockery C., Fenton A. A. (2005). Beyond memory, navigation, and inhibition: behavioral evidence for hippocampus-dependent cognitive coordination in the rat. J. Neurosci. 25, 2413–2419 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3962-04.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wills T. J., Cacucci F., Burgess N., O'Keefe J. (2010). Development of the hippocampal cognitive map in preweanling rats. Science 328, 1573–1576 10.1126/science.1188224 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang G., Pan F., Gan W. B. (2009). Stably maintained dendritic spines are associated with lifelong memories. Nature 462, 920–924 10.1038/nature08577 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zainuddin M. S., Thuret S. (2012). Nutrition, adult hippocampal neurogenesis and mental health. Br. Med. Bull. 103, 89–114 10.1093/bmb/lds021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhuravin I. A., Bures J. (1991). Extent of the tetrodotoxin induced blockade examined by pupillary paralysis elicited by intracerebral injection of the drug. Exp. Brain Res. 83, 687–690 10.1007/BF00229849 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ziv Y., Burns L. D., Cocker E. D., Hamel E. O., Ghosh K. K., Kitch L. J., et al. (2013). Long-term dynamics of CA1 hippocampal place codes. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 264–266 10.1038/nn.3329 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]