Skip to main content
Shanghai Archives of Psychiatry logoLink to Shanghai Archives of Psychiatry
. 2013 Jun;25(3):157–164. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1002-0829.2013.03.005

Binocular rivalry in children with schizophrenia: the conscious and unconscious cognitive processing of interpersonal information

儿童期精神分裂症患者的双眼竞争: 意识和潜意识状态下对人际交往信息的认知加工过程

Meijuan Wang 1, Jinping Zhao 2,3, Jie Qian 4, Yikang Zhu 1, Zhi Yang 3, Yi Jiang 3, Jijun Wang 1, Yasong Du 1, Xuchu Weng 2,5, Chunbo Li 1,*
PMCID: PMC4054552  PMID: 24991151

Abstract

Background

Childhood schizophrenia is a severe mental disorder that is believed to affect both conscious and unconscious cognitive functioning, but there have been few studies that have assessed this.

Objective

Develop a version of the binocular rivalry test that will assess the conscious and unconscious cognitive processing of interpersonal information and use this test to determine whether or not social cognition is impaired in children with schizophrenia.

Methods

Thirty images of three types – with no persons, with 2-3 persons and with 4+ persons – were selected for use in a binocular rivalry test that presented the images both in an interocularly suppressed (unconscious) format and an unsuppressed (conscious) format. Fifteen children under 16 years of age with schizophrenia who had prominent delusional symptoms and 15 healthy children were administered the test. Accuracy rates (in assessing the left or right orientation of a patch presented immediately after the target images) and reaction times were compared between patients and controls.

Results

For all types of pictures, the accuracy of patients was less than that of controls, though the differences were only significant in two of the twelve comparisons assessed. Compared to controls, patients showed a non-significant increase in the attention paid to images with people in them compared to images without people in them, both for conscious and unconscious presentations of the images. We did not find any relationship between the severity of psychotic symptoms in the patients and the degree of impairment in the cognitive processing of images. When asked to assess the attributes of the images, patients reported significantly higher levels of happiness depicted in images with 2-3 people than controls.

Conclusions

The non-significant increase in the attention children with schizophrenia paid to images depicting interpersonal relationships suggests, but does not prove, that the illness is associated with impairments in the cognitive processing of social information. Our use of the binocular rivalry paradigm to identify these differences was only partially successful, largely because of the wide variability in the key index from the test used to assess the amount of attention respondents pay to different types of images.

1. Introduction

Binocular rivalry refers to the phenomenon that when the right and left eye are presented with different stimuli, no stable perception can be formed so there are alternating perceptions.[1] During the binocular rivalry test, the different stimuli presented in front of the retina of each eye do not change, but the fluctuating perception of either one or the other image requires alternating suppression of the conscious perception of one of the eyes.

Childhood schizophrenia is associated with serious disturbances in cognitive processing. Abnormal processing of interpersonal information may be one of the mechanisms that leads to delusional beliefs in these children, particularly delusions of reference.[2] Most studies on cognitive functioning in childhood schizophrenia are potentially confounded by a variety of factors including the severity of the illness, educational status of the respondent, experimental environment, and medication status. One of the advantages of the binocular rivalry test is that it is much less influenced by these factors. It is, therefore, a relatively direct way of assessing the cognitive functioning of children with schizophrenia. The current study uses the binocular rivalry test to compare the processing of social information among children with schizophrenia who have prominent delusions to that of normal children and, thus, to test the hypothesis that their conscious and unconscious processing of social information is impaired.

2. Methods

2.1. Sample

The enrollment process for the study is shown in Figure 1. Enrolled patients were children who met the diagnostic criteria for childhood schizophrenia specified in the Chinese Classification of Mental Disorders, Third Edition (CCMD-3)[3] who were treated in the outpatient or inpatient services of the Shanghai Mental Health Center from January to April 2010. Included patients were under 16 years of age, had delusions as their primary symptoms, were right-handed, did not have other mental or serious physical illnesses, and were able to complete the binocular rivalry test. The severity of schizophrenia at the time of the binocular rivalry test was assessed using the severity of illness subscale of the Clinical Global Impression scale (CGI-SI)[4] which rates illness severity on a 7-point Likert scale (from 1=not at all ill, to 7=extremely ill). The level of paranoia at the time of the test was also assessed by the treating clinician using a similar 7-point Likert scale that we constructed for the purpose of the current experiment.

Figure 1. Enrollment of subject in the study.

Figure 1.

The control group consisted of 15 students under 16 years of age nominated by their teachers from one grade 8 class and one grade 10 class in a public school in Shanghai. All of them were right-handed, did not have mental or serious physical illnesses, did not have a family history of mental illness, and were able to complete the binocular rivalry test.

All participants in this study and one parent for each participant signed informed consent forms. The study was approved by the institutional review board of the Shanghai Mental Health Center.

2.2. Assessment of binocular rivalry

A total of 30 images depicting different levels of interpersonal interactions were selected from a series of candidate images. Photoshop 7.0 software was used to alter the colors, brightness and sizes of these images in order to generate monochrome images that are 150 mm×112 mm in size. These images were then categorized into the following three groups (10 images in each group): images with no people, images with two to three people, and images with more than three people.

During the test each image was presented on a 17-inch monitor (brand: Lenovo Model: L193pC, 1280 *1024) using MATLAB (Matrix Laboratory) software. Participants sat behind the electric stereoscope (66 Vision- Tech Co., Ltd., Model: YT2B-2201857) and placed their chin on the supporting bracket, which was adjusted according to the sitting height of the participant to ensure that their eyes were at the same level as the cross (‘+’) on the monitor.

The version of the binocular rivalry test employed in this study is similar to that described elsewhere.[5] At the beginning of the test, the dominant eye of the subject was determined. During the test two pairs of images are independently presented to each eye. There are two versions of the test that were both administered to each participant. In the interocularly suppressed version of the test, the dominant eye is presented with two colored mosaic patches and the non-dominant eye is simultaneously presented with a monochrome mosaic patch and a monochrome version of one of the 30 target images described above (which alternate randomly between the right and left side of the visual field of the non-dominant eye). In the unsuppressed version of the test, the images presented to the dominant and non-dominant eyes are identical: both eyes are presented with paired images of a monochrome mosaic patch and a monochrome version of one of the 30 images which alternate between the right and left sides of the visual field.

The viewing angle for all images was 4.1°×6.2°. Each set of images was shown for 800 ms followed by a 100 ms presentation of a grating (a Gabor patch) with a viewing angle of 2.5°×2.5° which was 1° skewed to either the left or the right side at random. The task of the participant was to determine the direction of the skew (left or right) and to press the right or left side of the mouse for the computer accordingly. In the suppressed version of the test there were a total of 120 trials (i.e., presentations of the images) divided into two sessions, 60 trials each session with a five-minute break between the two sessions; if the respondent reports seeing a clear image for more than 10 of the trials (i.e., the interocular suppression did not work), the test was considered invalid. In the unsuppressed version of the test there were a total of 90 trials divided into 3 sessions, 30 trials in each session with a two-minute break between sessions. The sequence of image presentation in the two versions of the test is shown in Figure 2.

Figure 2. Diagram of the sequence of presentations of images in each trial for the two versions of the binocular rivalry test used in the study (see methods section for detailed description of the test).

Figure 2.

Several measures are assessed based on the results of the test. (a) the proportion of correct responses (about the orientation of the Gabor patch) when the target image and the Gabor patch are presented on the same side of the visual field (hereinafter referred to as the ‘target image accuracy rate’), (b) the proportion of correct responses when the monochrome mosaic patch and Gabor patch are presented on the same side of the visual field (hereinafter referred to as the ‘mosaic patch accuracy rate’), (c) the difference between the target image and mosaic patch accuracy rates, and (d) the difference in reaction times for responding when the target image is on the same side or the opposite side as the Gabor patch (when this difference score is positive it indicates that the subject pays more attention to the target image than to the monochrome mosaic patch).

After the tests, the participants rated each of the 30 images on three aspects – happiness depicted in the picture, impact on the subject, and relevance to the subject – on three nine-point Likert scales, where ‘1’ represented extremely unhappy, no impact, or completely irrelevant and ‘9’ represented extremely happy, huge impact, or highly relevant.

2.3. Statistical analysis

The statistical software SPSS 17.0 was used to conduct the analysis. Descriptive statistical analyses, independent sample t-test, paired t-test, analysis of variance, non-parametric tests and correlation analysis were used depending on the type of variable. When comparing the results for the three types of images, multiple comparison methods were employed to compare the three paired comparisons (i.e., images with no people v. images with 2-3 people; images with no people v. images with 4+ people; and images with 2-3 people v. images with 4+ people) if the overall assessment found that there was a statistically significant difference between groups. Statistical significance was set at p<0.05.

3. Results

3.1. Demographic data on the subjects

There were 3 males and 12 females in the patient group and 7 males and 8 females in the control group (Fisher's exact test p=0.245). The range of ages was 14-16 years in the patient group and 13-16 years in the control group. The mean (sd) ages in the patient and control groups were 14.2 (2.0) years and 14.0 (1.1) years, respectively. The mean years of education among patients and controls were 7.1 (1.7) years and 7.0 (1.1) years, respectively. There were no statistically significant differences in age or years of education between the two groups. The mean duration of illness of the patients was 17.9 (6.7) months. All of the patients were taking antipsychotic medications.

3.2. Results of the binocular rivalry tests

As shown in Table 1, lower accuracy rates were observed for all three types of images in the patient group compared to the control group in both versions of the test, but these differences were only statistically significant in two of the twelve outcome measures for the test. The accuracy among patients was significantly poorer than that for controls for the interocularly suppressed version of the test when the mosaic patch and Gabor patches were on the same side of the visual field and when the target images were those with no people or with four or more people. There were, moreover, no significant differences between patients and controls in the differences in target image versus mosaic patch accuracy.

Table 1. Comparison of the mean (sd) proportion of correct responses between the 15 patients and 15 control subjectsin 120 trials when target images are only presented to the non-dominant eye (‘suppressed presentation’) and in 90 trials when target images are presented to both eyes (‘unsuppressed presentation’).

suppressed presentation of images to non-dominant eye
unsuppressed presentation of images to both eyes
patients controls t p patients controls t p
Images with no people
Target image accuracya 0.70 (0.14) 0.80 (0.15) -1.75 0.091 0.58(0.15) 0.63 (0.14) -0.88 0.385
Mosaic patchaccuracya 0.69 (0.13) 0.80 (0.14) -2.12 0.043 0.58(0.14) 0.65 (0.12) -1.53 0.137
 Difference 0.01 (0.10) -0.00 (0.09) - 0.667 0.00(0.19) -0.02 (0.14) - 0.675
Images with 2-3 people
Target image accuracya 0.68 (0.13) 0.78 (0.17) -1.78 0.085 0.57 (0.10) 0.63 (0.12) -1.41 0.169
Mosaic patchaccuracya 0.70 (0.13) 0.79 (0.17) -1.52 0.141 0.57 (0.14) 0.63 (0.11) -1.41 0.170
 Difference -0.03 (0.10) -0.01 (0.08) - 0.333 0.00 (0.11) -0.01 (0.09) - 0.950
Images with 4+ people
Target image accuracya 0.69 (0.11) 0.79 (0.16) -2.00 0.058 0.57 (0.09) 0.60 (0.09) -1.06 0.299
Mosaic patchaccuracya 0.67 (0.13) 0.78 (0.17) -2.05 0.050 0.58 (0.13) 0.61 (0.12) -0.82 0.419
 Difference 0.02 (0.09) 0.01 (0.08) - 0.333 -0.01 (0.10) 0.02 (0.06) - 0.219

a See methods section for definition of ‘target image accuracy’ and ‘mosaic patch accuracy’

The difference in mean reaction times (i.e., mean reaction time when target images were presented on the same side of the visual field as the Gabor patch minus the mean reaction time when target images were presented on the opposite side of the visual field of the Gabor patch) are not normally distributed so these results are presented as medians and interquartile ranges and rank tests were used to compare results for different groups of subjects (Table 2). There were no significant differences in the mean ranks of these difference scores between patients and controls for any of the three images in the suppressed or unsuppressed versions of test (using six separate Mann-Whitney tests). There were also no significant differences in difference scores for the suppressed versus unsuppressed presentation of each of the three images either for patients or for controls (using six separate Wilcoxon signed-rank tests).

Table 2. Median (interquartile range) difference inreaction times (in seconds) when the target image is presented on the same side or the opposite side of the visual field as the Gabor patch in the patient and control groups.

suppressed presentation of images to non-dominant eye
unsuppressed presentation of images to both eyes
patient group control group patient group control group
Images with no persons -0.018 (-0.049∼0.013) 0.002 (0.000∼0.022) 0.006 (-0.159∼0.104 ) 0.004 (-0.139∼0.070 )
Images with 2-3 persons -0.006 (-0.099∼0.029 ) -0.001 (-0.014∼0.014) 0.018 (-0.050∼0.573 ) -0.032 (-0.068∼0.031)
Images with 4+ persons 0.000 (-0.029∼0.021) -0.007 (-0.022∼0.005) 0.013 (-0.042∼0.272) -0.001 (-0.098∼0.026)

For patients, the larger (i.e., more positive) median values for the difference scores for the two types of pictures with people compared to pictures without people (shown in Table 2) indicate that patients attended longer to images with persons in them than to images without persons; but these differences were not statistically significant. The opposite was true in controls who attended to pictures without people in them longer than to pictures with people in them. This difference in controls was statistically significant in the suppressed version of the test where the mean ranks of difference scores for reaction times for images with no persons, images with 2-3 persons, and images with 4+ persons were 2.43, 2.03 and 1.53, respectively (Chi-square for Friedman test=6.20, df=2, p=0.045) -- indicating that during unconscious presentation of images, controls paid significantly more attention to the images with no persons than to images with people.

3.3. Attributional rating of images used in the binocular rivalry test

The perceived attributes of the three types of images as assessed by the patients and controls is shown in Table 3. Among patients there were no significant differences between the three types of pictures in the perceived happiness depicted in the pictures, in the perceived impact of the pictures on the individual, or in the perceived relevance of the pictures to the individual. However, among control subjects the perceived happiness depicted in the pictures and the perceived relevance of the pictures to the individuals was greater for images without persons than for images with persons. Compared to controls, patients reported significantly lower levels of happiness depicted in pictures with no persons and higher levels of happiness depicted in pictures with 2-3 persons; patients also reported a significantly lower relevance to themselves of pictures with no persons.

Table 3. Mean (sd) scores of patients and control subjects (on a 1-9 point scale) on three perceived attributes of each type of image used in the binocular rivalry test.

patient group (n=15)
control group (n=15)
patient group v. control group
mean (sd) F p mean (sd) F p t p
Happiness depicted in picture
Images with no persons 5.3(1.1) 0.46 0.634 6.1(0.8) 24.16a <0.001 -3.53 0.003
Images with 2-3 persons 5.0(1.0) 4.3(0.5) 2.27 0.040
Images with 4+ persons 4.9(1.1) 4.7(0.9) 0.51 0.621
Impact on the subject
Images with no persons 4.4(1.5) 0.14 0.868 5.1(1.8) 1.88 0.165 -1.34 0.201
Images with 2-3 persons 4.2(1.5) 4.1(1.3) 0.19 0.855
Images with 4+ persons 4.2(1.5) 4.2(1.5) -0.10 0.924
Relevance to the subject
Images with no persons 3.6(1.8) 0.36 0.702 5.5(1.3) 7.63a 0.001 -3.35 0.005
Images with 2-3 persons 3.4(1.8) 3.5(1.9) -0.03 0.974
Images with 4+ persons 3.1(1.4) 3.5(1.7) -0.68 0.509

a Multiple comparison tests (Tukey)found that the score for the images with no persons was significantly higher than the scores for the images with 2-3 persons and the images with 4+ persons

3.4. Correlation of disease severity with patients' results on the binocular rivalry test and with their attributional ratings of the images used in the test

In the patient group, the mean disease severity score at the time the binocular rivalry test was conducted (on the 1- 7-point CGI-SI scale) was 4.2 (1.4), and the mean paranoia severity score was 3.4 (1.1). There were no statistically significant correlations between disease severity or severity of paranoia with the reaction time and accuracy of the suppressed or unsuppressed versions of the binocular rivalry test. Disease severity was also unrelated to patient's attributional ratings of the 30 images employed in the tests (all p>0.05).

4. Discussion

4.1. Main findings

Previous studies have used the binocular rivalry paradigm to assess conscious and unconscious cognitive functioning in patients with mental disorders including schizophrenia,[6],[7] but to our knowledge this is the first such study in children with schizophrenia. Given the clinical observation that interpersonal relationships are fertile ground for generating delusional beliefs (particularly delusions of reference) in patients with schizophrenia, we posited that these patients would have abnormal cognitive processing of images that involve social communication between individuals and that these cognitive abnormalities would be present during both conscious and unconscious information processing.

Comparison of the results of the binocular rivalry test between patients and controls did not confirm our initial hypothesis but they did provide some support for the hypothesis. For all types of pictures in both the interocularly suppressed (i.e., unconscious) and unsuppressed (i.e., conscious) versions of the test, the accuracy of patients was less than that of controls, though the differences were only significant in two of the twelve comparisons assessed. Compared to controls, patients also show a non-significant increase in the attention paid to images with people in them compared to images without people in them, both for the conscious and unconscious presentation of the images. This suggests that patient with schizophrenia require more attentional input to process interpersonal information. We did not find any relationship between the severity of psychotic symptoms and the degree of impairment in the cognitive processing of images. An interesting ancillary finding that may be relevant to cognitive processing of images is that the reported happiness depicted in the images without people and the reported relevance to the respondent of pictures without people was significantly lower in patients than in controls; conversely the reported happiness depicted in images with 2-3 people in them was significantly higher in patients than in controls.

Several studies have reported that the processing of interpersonal information, which is considered a component of social cognition,[8],[9] is impaired in patients with schizophrenia. A meta-analysis on cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia by Savla and colleagues[10] reported impairments in many aspects of social cognition compared to normal controls. Some studies report that dysfunctional social cognition in schizophrenia is related to the severity of symptoms,[11],[12],[13] but other studies find no direct link between the negative symptoms of schizophrenia and social cognitive impairment.[14],[15] Studies on the relationship between social cognition and the positive symptoms of schizophrenia (such as delusions) are rare,[16] and to our knowledge there has never previously been a study focused on this topic among children with schizophrenia.

4.2. Limitations

This study was an exploratory study to assess the usefulness of the binocular rivalry test as an instrument to assess the cognitive processing of social information in children with schizophrenia. The main problems with this study were that the sample size was small, the reliability and validity of responses to the images selected had not been pre-tested, and the study included both inpatients with relatively acute symptoms and outpatients in remission, all of whom were taking antipsychotic medication. The primary measure used to test the hypothesized impairment in patients' ability to process interpersonal information (i.e., the mean difference in reaction times to target images when presented on the same side or the opposite side of the visual field as the test images) proved to be quite variable both in patients and controls; this made it impossible to provide a definite conclusion about the hypothesis. Studies with larger samples, preferably with narrower entry criteria, that preliminarily assess the ability of different types of images to illicit ‘cognitive processing of social information’ are needed to determine whether or not the binocular rivalry test is an effective method of assessing cognitive processing in patients with schizophrenia.

4.3. Significance

The non-significant increase in the conscious and unconscious attention children with schizophrenia paid to images depicting interpersonal relationships (compared to healthy children of similar ages) suggests, but does not prove, that the illness is associated with impairments in the cognitive processing of social information. Further study with larger samples is needed to determine whether or not impairments in social cognition is independent of the overall cognitive dysfunction seen in patients with schizophrenia and to identify the symptomatic and other factors that are most closely associated with impaired social cognition in schizophrenia. Our use of the binocular rivalry paradigm to identify these differences was only partially successful, largely because of the wide variability in the key index from the test used to assess the amount of attention respondents pay to different types of images. Further refinements in the binocular rivalry test are needed to determine whether or not it can be used to compare cognitive functioning of social information between different types of subjects and within subjects over time.

Biography

graphic file with name sap-25-03-157-g001.gif

Dr. Meijuan Wang graduated with a Bachelor's degree in medicine from the Nanjing Medical University in 2004. She received a Master's degree in Psychiatry and Mental Health from the Tongji University School of Medicine in 2010. Since 2010 she has worked as a resident psychiatrist at the Shanghai Mental Health Center. Her current research interests include childhood schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and anxiety disorders.

Footnotes

Conflict of interest: The authors report no conflict of interest related to this manuscript.

Funding: This study was supported by the Key National Basic Research Development Plan (2007CB512306) and the National Science Foundation (81171267).

References

  • 1.Feng CZ, Jia FQ. Review on binocular rivalry. Advances in Psychological Science. 2008;16(002):213–221. (in Chinese) [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Green MF, Penn DL, Bentall R, Carpenter WT, Gaebel W, Gur RC, et al. Social cognition in schizophrenia: an NIMH workshop on definitions, assessment, and research opportunities. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34(6):1211–20. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Society of Psychiatry, Chinese Medical Association . Chinese Classification of Mental Disorders, Third Edition (CCMD-3) Jinan, Shandong: Shandong People's Press; 2001. (in Chinese) [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Wu WY. Clinical Global Impression (CGI) Shanghai Archives of Psychiatry. 1990;2(suppl):70–71. (in Chinese) [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Jiang Y, Costello P, Fang F, Huang M, He S. A gender- and sexual orientation-dependent spatial attentional effect of invisible images. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2006;103(45):17048–17052. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605678103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Zhu YK, Li CB, Wu WY. Application of the binocular rivalry paradigm in psychiatry. Shanghai Archives of Psychiatry. 2011;23(4):237–240. (in Chinese) [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Yang E, Blake R. Emotional binocular rivalry in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2007;33(2):551–551. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Green MF, Penn DL, Bentall R, Carpenter WT, Gaebel W, Gur RC, et al. Social cognition in schizophrenia: an NIMH workshop on definitions, assessment, and research opportunities. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34(6):1211–1220. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Goldberg JO, Schmidt LA. Shyness, sociability, and social dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2001;48(2–3):343–349. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(00)00143-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Savla GN, Vella L, Armstrong CC, Penn DL, Twamley EW. Deficits in domains of social cognition in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of the empirical evidence. Schizophr Bull. 2012 doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bora E, Gökçen S, Kayahan B, Veznedaroglu B. Deficits of social–cognitive and social–perceptual aspects of theory of mind in remitted patients with schizophrenia: effect of residual symptoms. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2008;196(2):95–99. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e318162a9e1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Pousa E, Duñó R, Brébion G, David AS, Ruiz AI, Obiols JE. Theory of mind deficits in chronic schizophrenia: evidence for state dependence. Psychiatry Res. 2008;158(1):1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2006.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Mancuso F, Horan WP, Kern RS, Green MF. Social cognition in psychosis: multidimensional structure, clinical correlates, and relationship with functional outcome. Schizophr Res. 2010;125(2–3):143–151. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Couture SM, Granholm EL, Fish SC. A path model investigation of neurocognition, theory of mind, social competence, negative symptoms and real-world functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2010;125(2–3):152–160. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.09.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lysaker PH, Erikson M, Macapagal KR, Tunze C, Gilmore E, Ringer JM. Development of personal narratives as a mediator of the impact of deficits in social cognition and social withdrawal on negative symptoms in schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2012;200(4):290–295. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31824cb0f4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Penn DL, Sanna LJ, Roberts DL. Social cognition in schizophrenia: an overview. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34(3):408–411. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Shanghai Archives of Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Shanghai Mental Health Center

RESOURCES