Abstract
The demand for meta-analyses in basic biomedical research has been increasing because the phenotyping of genetically modified mice does not always produce consistent results. Melanin-concentrating hormone (MCH) has been reported to be involved in a variety of behaviors that include feeding, body-weight regulation, anxiety, sleep, and reward behavior. However, the reported behavioral and metabolic characteristics of MCH signaling-deficient mice, such as MCH-deficient mice and MCH receptor 1 (MCHR1)-deficient mice, are not consistent with each other. In the present study, we performed a meta-analysis of the published data related to MCH-deficient and MCHR1-deficient mice to obtain robust conclusions about the role of MCH signaling. Overall, the meta-analysis revealed that the deletion of MCH signaling enhanced wakefulness, locomotor activity, aggression, and male sexual behavior and that MCH signaling deficiency suppressed non-REM sleep, anxiety, responses to novelty, startle responses, and conditioned place preferences. In contrast to the acute orexigenic effect of MCH, MCH signaling deficiency significantly increased food intake. Overall, the meta-analysis also revealed that the deletion of MCH signaling suppressed the body weight, fat mass, and plasma leptin, while MCH signaling deficiency increased the body temperature, oxygen consumption, heart rate, and mean arterial pressure. The lean phenotype of the MCH signaling-deficient mice was also confirmed in separate meta-analyses that were specific to sex and background strain (i.e., C57BL/6 and 129Sv). MCH signaling deficiency caused a weak anxiolytic effect as assessed with the elevated plus maze and the open field test but also caused a weak anxiogenic effect as assessed with the emergence test. MCH signaling-deficient mice also exhibited increased plasma corticosterone under non-stressed conditions, which suggests enhanced activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. To the best of our knowledge, the present work is the first study to systematically compare the effects of MCH signaling on behavioral and metabolic phenotypes.
Introduction
Meta-analysis is a statistical method of combining the results of individual studies to obtain the most reliable conclusions [1]. In clinical medicine, the power of meta-analysis is indispensable for building feasible and practical guidelines for the treatment of diseases. Although the use of meta-analysis in basic biomedical research is rare, the demand has been increasing because many researchers are working on genetically modified mice to examine the roles of genes in animal behavior and metabolism, and the results of the behavioral studies are not always consistent across research laboratories [2]. Studies with positive results tend to be published in scientific journals with many readers, while those with negative results are easily overlooked because they are typically coupled with positive results and published in journals with fewer readers.
Melanin-concentrating hormone (MCH) is a 17-amino-acid cyclic neuropeptide that was originally isolated from teleost fish; in these fish, MCH causes the aggregation of melanosomes in the scales, which results in color changes [3]. MCH expression is restricted to the lateral hypothalamus of the brain, and MCH is not expressed outside of the brain. Analyses of the gene expressions in the hypothalamus of leptin-deficient mice have revealed the orexigenic effect of MCH in mammals [4]. There are one or two G-protein-coupled receptors for MCH: humans express MCHR1 and MCHR2, whereas rodents express only MCHR1 [5], [6]. In contrast to the restricted localization of MCH neurons in the lateral hypothalamic area and the zona incerta, MCH neurons send their projections broadly across the brain, and MCHR1 is also widely expressed in the brain [7]. The broad projections of MCH neurons suggest that MCH signaling may be involved in the regulation of a wide array of behaviors [6].
Consistent with the orexigenic effect of the injection of MCH into the cerebral ventricles [4], [8], [9], MCH-overexpressing mice exhibit increased food intake and develop mild obesity [10]. Conversely, MCH-deficient mice and MCH neuron-ablated mice exhibit decreased food intake and lean phenotypes [11], [12]. In addition to reduced food intake, increased energy expenditure also contributes to the lean phenotypes of MCH-deficient mice and MCH neuron-ablated mice [11]–[13]. Furthermore, MCHR1-deficient mice exhibit a lean phenotype and resistance to high-fat diet-induced obesity [14]–[17]. These results indicate that the MCH-MCHR1 system works to accumulate energy in the body; therefore, MCH-receptor antagonists are currently believed to be attractive therapeutic targets for the treatment of obesity. However, it should be noted that the lean phenotypes of the MCH- and MCHR1-deficient mice have not always been replicated [18], [19], and increased food intake by MCH-deficient mice has also been reported [13].
In addition to metabolic parameters, MCH signaling appears to be involved in a variety of behavioral phenotypes. MCH- and MCHR1-deficient mice exhibit decreased non-REM sleep time [20], [21], impaired olfactory function [22], reduced responses to novelty [19], suppressed startle responses [19], and reduced sucrose preference [23]. Moreover, MCH signaling-deficient mice exhibit increased aggression [22], alcohol preference [23], sexual motivation [22], and sociability [24]. However, the findings from behavioral analyses have not been consistent. For example, locomotor activity is one of the most commonly measured parameters, and the measurement of locomotor activity is highly automated; however, some groups have reported that MCH- or MCHR1-deficient mice exhibit increased locomotion [13], [15], [25], [26], while others have reported no differences [21], [22], [27], [28]. Inconsistent findings regarding the roles of MCH signaling in anxiety behavior have also been reported; one study reported anxiolytic phenotypes among MCHR1-deficient mice [24], whereas other groups have reported normal anxiety behavior in MCHR1-deficient mice [27], [29]. These results prompted us to perform a meta-analysis to examine the contributions of MCH signaling to behavioral and metabolic parameters.
We performed a meta-analysis of the previously reported data regarding MCH signaling-deficient mice, MCH-deficient mice, and MCHR1-deficient mice. Because genetic background and sex affect behavioral and metabolic characteristics, we also performed separate meta-analyses segregated by background strain and sex.
Methods
Search Strategy
We performed an extensive electronic search to identify studies that had examined the effects of the deletion of MCH signaling on the behavioral phenotypes of mice. We searched PubMed (1948-Jan 2014) using the free-text search terms “melanin-concentrating hormone” or “melanin-concentrating hormone receptor” plus “knockout” or “deficient” on Jan 1, 2014. The first report about MCH signaling-deficient mouse was published in 1998 for MCH-deficient mouse [11], followed by the literature published in 2002 which was the first report about MCHR1-deficient mouse [30]. Additional searches were performed using the first, last, and corresponding authors of any of the reports identified in the initial PubMed search. The search was restricted to papers written in English because paper translations were not feasible within the scope of this review. No other restrictions were placed on the search criteria. This search retrieved 164 studies. The reference lists of the included papers and recent review papers in the field were hand-searched along with back issues to identify additional references (See PRISMA flowchart; Figure. 1).
Study Selection
A two-step procedure was used to identify the articles that were eligible for inclusion. First, the articles were assessed by reviewing their titles and abstracts to determine whether the articles met the following inclusion criteria: written in the English language; reported as an empirical article; used MCH-deficient or MCHR1-deficient mice; and included behavioral or metabolic experiments. These inclusion criteria were applied by two independent assessors, and the title and abstract reviews were conducted by same assessors. In cases in which the abstract provided insufficient information to make a final decision, the study was selected for full-text review. The full-text review was also conducted by two assessors. Next, the methodologies of the selected articles that were related to study quality were critically examined in full-text reviews. Studies that did not use wild-type littermates or wild-type animals from the same colony as the control mice were excluded. The parameter we used to index food intake was not normalized by body weight. The confirmation of the methodology was conducted by two assessors. The final decisions regarding inclusion were made by all of the present authors. Ultimately, 405 experiments from 33 studies were retained for the analysis. We did not include energy expenditure data in this study due to the inconsistency of the reported data (Figure 2D and Table 1 [15]).
Table 1. Results of meta-analysis.
MCH Signaling Function | Behavioral/Physiological/Biochemical Parameters | Sample Size | Number of Studies | r | 95% Confidence Interval | P value vs. 0 | |
Lower Limit | Upper Limit | ||||||
Anxiety- and Mood-Related Behavior | |||||||
Elevated Plus Maze | 232 | 11 | 0.26 | 0.12 | 0.38 | <0.05 | |
Elevated Plus Maze (Habituated Environment) | 84 | 4 | 0.26 | 0.03 | 0.46 | <0.05 | |
Emergence Test | 42 | 2 | −0.33 | −0.02 | −0.58 | <0.05 | |
Emergence Test (Habituated Environment) | 42 | 4 | −0.12 | 0.21 | −0.42 | N.S. | |
Exploratory Behavior | 84 | 4 | 0.13 | −0.10 | 0.34 | N.S. | |
Forced Swim test | 68 | 2 | 0.23 | −0.01 | 0.45 | N.S. | |
Open Field Test | 330 | 9 | 0.26 | 0.15 | 0.36 | <0.05 | |
Open Field Test (Habituated Environment) | 168 | 8 | 0.31 | 0.15 | 0.45 | <0.05 | |
Response to Handling | 84 | 4 | 0.12 | −0.11 | 0.34 | N.S. | |
Response to Novelty | 84 | 4 | 0.25 | 0.03 | 0.45 | <0.05 | |
Response to Supine Restraint | 42 | 2 | 0.00 | −0.32 | 0.32 | N.S. | |
Startle Response | 42 | 2 | 0.35 | 0.04 | 0.60 | <0.05 | |
Stress−Induced Hyperthermia | 52 | 2 | 0.68 | 0.49 | 0.81 | <0.05 | |
Body Weight and Energy Metabolism | |||||||
Body Length | 54 | 3 | 0.17 | −0.12 | 0.44 | N.S. | |
Body Temperature | 68 | 3 | −0.64 | −0.46 | −0.76 | <0.05 | |
Body Temperature (Dark Phase) | 32 | 2 | −0.18 | 0.20 | −0.51 | N.S. | |
Body Temperature (Light Phase) | 32 | 2 | −0.52 | −0.19 | −0.75 | <0.05 | |
Body Weight | 192 | 11 | 0.25 | 0.10 | 0.39 | <0.05 | |
Body Weight (High Fat Diet) | 90 | 5 | 0.49 | 0.30 | 0.64 | <0.05 | |
Fat Mass | 143 | 11 | 0.67 | 0.55 | 0.76 | <0.05 | |
Fat Mass (High Fat Diet) | 30 | 3 | 0.73 | 0.47 | 0.88 | <0.05 | |
Food Intake | 129 | 7 | −0.26 | −0.08 | −0.43 | <0.05 | |
Food Intake (High Fat Diet) | 116 | 7 | −0.21 | −0.02 | −0.40 | <0.05 | |
Lean Mass | 82 | 5 | 0.19 | −0.04 | 0.41 | N.S. | |
Lean Mass (High Fat Diet) | 16 | 2 | 0.17 | −0.42 | 0.66 | N.S. | |
Liver Weight | 24 | 1 | 0.70 | 0.42 | 0.86 | <0.05 | |
Oxygen Consumption | 24 | 3 | −0.96 | −0.90 | −0.99 | <0.05 | |
Oxygen Consumption (High Fat Diet) | 15 | 2 | −0.98 | −0.94 | −1.00 | <0.05 | |
Cardiovascular System | |||||||
Heart Rate | 16 | 1 | −0.89 | −0.71 | −0.96 | <0.05 | |
Heart Rate (Dark Phase) | 16 | 1 | −0.86 | −0.64 | −0.95 | <0.05 | |
Heart Rate (Light Phase) | 16 | 1 | −0.68 | −0.28 | −0.88 | <0.05 | |
Mean Arterial Pressure | 16 | 1 | −0.85 | −0.61 | −0.95 | <0.05 | |
Mean Arterial Pressure (Dark Phase) | 16 | 1 | −0.24 | 0.29 | −0.66 | N.S. | |
Mean Arterial Pressure (Light Phase) | 16 | 1 | −0.08 | 0.43 | −0.55 | N.S. | |
Endocrinology and Lab Data | |||||||
Blood Ethanol Kinetics | 72 | 4 | −0.06 | 0.19 | −0.30 | N.S. | |
Corticosterone Level | 52 | 3 | −0.50 | −0.25 | −0.69 | <0.05 | |
CRF Level in CE | 10 | 1 | 0.03 | −0.61 | 0.65 | N.S. | |
CRF Level in PVN | 10 | 1 | 0.55 | −0.13 | 0.88 | N.S. | |
Estrous Cycling | 20 | 1 | −0.18 | 0.28 | −0.58 | N.S. | |
Free Fatty Acid Level | 24 | 1 | 0.05 | −0.36 | 0.44 | N.S. | |
Ghrelin Level | 34 | 2 | 0.13 | −0.23 | 0.46 | N.S. | |
Glucose Level | 124 | 7 | 0.18 | −0.01 | 0.36 | N.S. | |
Glucose Level (High Fat Diet) | 64 | 6 | 0.08 | −0.21 | 0.35 | N.S. | |
Insulin Level | 140 | 8 | 0.13 | −0.05 | 0.30 | N.S. | |
Insulin Level (High Fat Diet) | 30 | 3 | 0.49 | 0.11 | 0.75 | <0.05 | |
Leptin Level | 130 | 8 | 0.57 | 0.42 | 0.68 | <0.05 | |
Leptin Level (High Fat Diet) | 30 | 3 | 0.65 | 0.33 | 0.83 | <0.05 | |
Leptin Level (per gram Fat) | 17 | 2 | −0.17 | 0.39 | −0.65 | N.S. | |
Leptin Level (per gram Fat) (High Fat Diet) | 36 | 4 | 0.51 | 0.16 | 0.74 | <0.05 | |
Liver Triglyceride Level | 24 | 1 | 0.60 | 0.26 | 0.81 | <0.05 | |
Liver Triglyceride Level (High Fat Diet) | 14 | 1 | 0.77 | 0.40 | 0.92 | <0.05 | |
T4 Level | 66 | 3 | 0.54 | 0.33 | 0.70 | <0.05 | |
Total Cholesterol Level | 24 | 1 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | <0.05 | |
Triglyceride Level | 72 | 3 | 0.20 | −0.05 | 0.42 | N.S. | |
Learning and Memory | |||||||
Spatial Learning Function | 42 | 2 | −0.21 | 0.12 | −0.49 | N.S. | |
Locomotion | |||||||
Locomotor Activity | 609 | 10 | −0.18 | −0.10 | −0.26 | <0.05 | |
Locomotor Activity (High Fat Diet) | 15 | 2 | −0.93 | −0.77 | −0.98 | <0.05 | |
Locomotor Activity (Dark Phase) | 56 | 5 | −0.59 | −0.36 | −0.75 | <0.05 | |
Locomotor Activity (Dark Phase) (High Fat Diet) | 15 | 2 | −0.87 | −0.60 | −0.96 | <0.05 | |
Locomotor Activity (Light Phase) | 32 | 2 | −0.30 | 0.07 | −0.60 | N.S. | |
Reward System | |||||||
Alcohol Preference | 30 | 2 | −0.59 | −0.26 | −0.79 | <0.05 | |
Conditioned Place Preference | 14 | 1 | 0.66 | 0.20 | 0.88 | <0.05 | |
Motor Activation by Psychostimulants | 38 | 1 | −0.44 | −0.13 | −0.66 | <0.05 | |
Sensitization to Psychostimulants | 31 | 1 | −0.11 | 0.28 | −0.46 | N.S. | |
Sucrose Preference | 15 | 1 | 0.39 | −0.15 | 0.75 | N.S. | |
Sensorimotor Function | |||||||
Motor Coordination | 210 | 10 | −0.11 | 0.03 | −0.25 | N.S. | |
Olfaction | 13 | 1 | 0.60 | 0.08 | 0.87 | <0.05 | |
Sleep-Wakefulness | |||||||
Non-REM Sleep (24 h) | 68 | 3 | 0.54 | 0.33 | 0.70 | <0.05 | |
Non-REM Sleep (Dark Phase) | 52 | 3 | 0.50 | 0.24 | 0.69 | <0.05 | |
Non-REM Sleep (Light Phase) | 52 | 3 | 0.26 | −0.03 | 0.51 | N.S. | |
REM Sleep (24 h) | 52 | 3 | −0.07 | 0.23 | −0.35 | N.S. | |
REM Sleep (Dark Phase) | 52 | 3 | 0.01 | −0.28 | 0.30 | N.S. | |
REM Sleep (Light phase) | 52 | 3 | −0.18 | 0.11 | −0.45 | N.S. | |
Wakefulness (24 h) | 68 | 4 | −0.51 | −0.30 | −0.68 | <0.05 | |
Wakefulness (Dark Phase) | 52 | 3 | −0.38 | −0.10 | −0.60 | <0.05 | |
Wakefulness (Light Phase) | 52 | 3 | −0.22 | 0.08 | −0.48 | N.S. | |
Social Behavior | |||||||
Resident-Intruder Test | 16 | 1 | −0.70 | −0.32 | −0.89 | <0.05 | |
Male Mating Behavior | 10 | 1 | −0.83 | −0.43 | −0.96 | <0.05 | |
Social Interaction Test | 30 | 1 | −0.36 | 0.00 | −0.64 | <0.05 | |
Aminergic and Peptidergic Signaling | |||||||
Agouti-Related Protein Level in Hypothalamus | 12 | 1 | 0.19 | −0.43 | 0.69 | N.S. | |
DA Level in NAc | 25 | 1 | 0.08 | −0.32 | 0.46 | N.S. | |
DAT Level in CPu | 24 | 2 | −0.21 | 0.24 | −0.59 | N.S. | |
DAT Level in GP | 10 | 1 | 0.00 | −0.63 | 0.63 | N.S. | |
DAT Level in NAc | 34 | 3 | −0.21 | 0.18 | −0.54 | N.S. | |
DAT Level in OT | 10 | 1 | 0.00 | −0.63 | 0.63 | N.S. | |
DAT Level in SN | 10 | 1 | −0.16 | 0.53 | −0.72 | N.S. | |
DAT Level in VTA | 10 | 1 | −0.29 | 0.42 | −0.78 | N.S. | |
D1R Level in CPu | 24 | 2 | −0.03 | 0.41 | −0.45 | N.S. | |
D1R Level in GP | 10 | 1 | 0.00 | −0.63 | 0.63 | N.S. | |
D1R Level in NAc | 34 | 3 | 0.47 | 0.11 | 0.71 | <0.05 | |
D1R Level in OT | 10 | 1 | 0.24 | −0.46 | 0.76 | N.S. | |
D1R Level in SN | 10 | 1 | 0.24 | −0.46 | 0.76 | N.S. | |
D1R Level in VTA | 10 | 1 | 0.63 | 0.00 | 0.90 | <0.05 | |
D2R Level in CPu | 24 | 2 | 0.52 | 0.12 | 0.78 | <0.05 | |
D2R Level in GP | 10 | 1 | 0.00 | −0.63 | 0.63 | N.S. | |
D2R Level in NAc | 34 | 3 | 0.32 | −0.06 | 0.62 | N.S. | |
D2R Level in OT | 10 | 1 | 0.63 | 0.00 | 0.90 | <0.05 | |
D2R Level in SN | 10 | 1 | 0.24 | −0.46 | 0.76 | N.S. | |
D2R Level in VTA | 10 | 1 | 0.24 | −0.46 | 0.76 | N.S. | |
NE Level in NAc | 25 | 1 | 0.08 | −0.32 | 0.46 | N.S. | |
NET Level in CPu | 10 | 1 | −0.14 | 0.53 | −0.71 | N.S. | |
NET Level in GP | 10 | 1 | −0.78 | −0.30 | −0.95 | <0.05 | |
NET Level in NAc | 20 | 2 | −0.64 | −0.24 | −0.86 | <0.05 | |
NET Level in OT | 10 | 1 | −0.30 | 0.41 | −0.78 | N.S. | |
NET Level in SN | 10 | 1 | −0.62 | 0.02 | −0.90 | N.S. | |
NET Level in VTA | 10 | 1 | −0.51 | 0.18 | −0.86 | N.S. | |
Neuropeptide Y Level in Hypothalamus | 12 | 1 | 0.19 | −0.43 | 0.69 | N.S. | |
NR1 Subunit Level in HPC | 36 | 3 | 0.58 | 0.28 | 0.78 | <0.05 | |
NR2A Subunit Level in HPC | 36 | 3 | 0.22 | −0.16 | 0.53 | N.S. | |
NR2B Subunit Level in HPC | 36 | 3 | 0.22 | −0.16 | 0.53 | N.S. | |
Orexin Level in Hypothalamus | 12 | 1 | −0.19 | 0.43 | −0.69 | N.S. | |
Pro-Opiomelanocortin Level in Hypothalamus | 12 | 1 | 0.79 | 0.40 | 0.94 | <0.05 | |
5HT Level in NAc | 25 | 1 | 0.08 | −0.32 | 0.46 | N.S. | |
5HT Level in PFC | 18 | 1 | 0.83 | 0.60 | 0.94 | <0.05 | |
5HTT Level in CPu | 10 | 1 | 0.00 | −0.63 | 0.63 | N.S. | |
5HTT Level in GP | 10 | 1 | 0.32 | −0.39 | 0.79 | N.S. | |
5HTT Level in NAc | 20 | 2 | 0.25 | −0.26 | 0.65 | N.S. | |
5HTT Level in OT | 10 | 1 | 0.60 | −0.05 | 0.89 | N.S. | |
5HTT Level in SN | 10 | 1 | 0.43 | −0.27 | 0.83 | N.S. | |
5HTT Level in VTA | 10 | 1 | 0.00 | −0.63 | 0.63 | N.S. | |
Others | |||||||
Litter Size | 304 | 2 | 0.05 | −0.06 | 0.17 | N.S. | |
Mean Pup Mass | 304 | 2 | 0.09 | −0.02 | 0.20 | N.S. | |
Pup Mortality | 163 | 1 | −0.14 | 0.01 | −0.29 | N.S. |
r = mean effect size, P value was calculated by Z test. CE, central amygdaloid nucleus; CPu, caudate-putamen; CRF, corticotropin-releasing factor; D1R, dopamine D1 receptor; D2R, dopamine D2 receptor; DA, dopamine; DAT, dopamine transporter; GP, globus pallidus; HPC, hippocampus; 5HTT, 5HT transporter; NAc, nucleus accumbens; NE, norepinephrine; NET, norepinephrine transporter; OT, olfactory tubercle; PFC, prefrontal cortex; PVN, paraventricular nucleus; SN, substantia nigra; VTA, ventral tegmental area.
Coding of the Variables
The effects of the deletion of MCH signaling on behavioral and metabolic phenotypes were derived by coding the performances in behavioral tests or the values that resulted from physiological and biochemical assays (Table S1). Regarding the behavioral phenotypes, the types of behavioral tests and the parameters of the behavioral tests were coded. Regarding the metabolic phenotypes, the units of the physiological and biochemical assays were coded. The behavioral and metabolic phenotypes were also coded for the background strain, sex, age, and sample size. Three background strains (C57BL/6, 129, and 129×C57BL/6) were coded, but the substrain, such as C57BL/6J, C57BL/6N, 129SvEv, and 129SvEvBrd, was not considered. In some cases, the sex or age was not described, and these cases were excluded from the separate meta-analyses.
Sensitivity Analysis
To assess potential publication bias, the tendency that significant results are more likely to be published than negative results, funnel plots were first created for the overall meta-analysis and for each separate meta-analysis. Because larger study samples tend to provide better estimates of the true effect sizes, inverted funnel shapes should be observed when the effect sizes are plotted against the sample sizes. Further, the funnel plots were assessed for asymmetry using the Egger test [1], [31]. Asymmetry of funnel plots suggests the publication bias due to the under-representation in the negative tail of the plot. Differences were considered significant at p<0.05.
Calculation of Effect Sizes
We used the correlation coefficient r to index effect sizes. The value of the test statistics resulting from two-tailed t-tests, one-way analyses of variance (two groups), chi-squared tests (2×2), and Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney tests were converted to the correlation coefficient r [32]. In some cases, the value of the test statistic was not reported. In these cases, we calculated the value of the test statistic from the p-value and the sample size n or the df [33]. Furthermore, if only the sample means and standard errors of two groups were reported, we calculated a t-value from these values. The obtained r values indicate how well a particular parameter correlates with the deletion of MCH signaling; an r of ±1.0 indicates a perfect (positive/negative) correlation. Correlations are positive when the parameter is suppressed or decreased in the MCH signaling-deficient mice. To estimate the 95% confidence intervals, we transformed the r values into Fisher’s z values. Fisher’s z is normally distributed, and its variance is 1/(n-3). We then calculated the 95% confidence interval the Fisher’s z metric and converted the Fisher’s z back to r. Statistical analysis was performed using Microsoft Excel.
Analysis of Effect Sizes
The mean effect size of each parameter was calculated. In our analyses, the number of experiments that we used to calculate the mean of each parameter was small. Hence, there was not enough power for a test of heterogeneity. We assume that there is one true effect size which is shared by all the included experiments and conducted the analyses using the fixed-effect model [34]. To calculate the means, we calculated the weighted means of the Fisher’s z-transformed correlations and converted the results back to the r metric. Then, the sample sizes were summed in those parameters. In the overall meta-analysis, significance testing was performed using the 95% confidence intervals. If the confidence interval did not contain zero, we judged the effect of the deletion of MCH signaling on that parameter to be statistically significant. In the separate meta-analyses, we adopted the parameters which were balanced in terms of MCH/MCHR1 KO, background strain, and sex. Further, the mean effect sizes of each group that were judged to be statistically significant were compared with each other using z-tests. Differences were considered significant at p<0.05. In the present analysis, the correlations were considered strong when the lower limit of the 95% confidence interval was >0.3.
Results
Publication Bias
To assess potential publication bias, the tendency that significant results are more likely to be published than negative results, we examined the distributions of the included parameters (Table 1) by funnel plot, showing inverted funnel shapes (Figure. 2). The Egger tests assessing asymmetry of plotted parameters revealed no significant differences (p>0.05).
In the separate meta-analyses of the MCH and MCHR1 strains, the background strain, and each gender, the distributions of the included behavioral and metabolic parameters (Tables 2–4) exhibited inverted funnel shapes (Figures. S1–3). The Egger tests revealed no significant differences (p>0.05), which suggests that the results of the separate meta-analyses (Tables 2–4) were relatively robust and that the effects of publication bias were small.
Table 2. Results of meta-analysis in MCH KO or MCHR1 KO mice.
Behavioral/Physiological/Biochemical Parameters | MCH KO | P value vs. MCHR1 KO | MCHR1 KO | ||||||||
Sample Size | r | 95% Confidence Interval | P value vs. 0 | Sample Size | r | 95% Confidence Interval | P value vs. 0 | ||||
Lower Limit | Upper Limit | Lower Limit | Upper Limit | ||||||||
Body Weight | 68 | 0.21 | −0.06 | 0.46 | N.S. | - | 124 | 0.26 | 0.08 | 0.43 | <0.05 |
Body Weight (High Fat Diet) | 23 | 0.69 | 0.36 | 0.87 | <0.05 | N.S. | 20 | 0.54 | 0.12 | 0.79 | <0.05 |
Fat Mass | 76 | 0.67 | 0.51 | 0.79 | <0.05 | N.S. | 44 | 0.68 | 0.46 | 0.82 | <0.05 |
Food Intake | 31 | −0.17 | 0.21 | −0.51 | N.S. | - | 46 | 0.01 | −0.29 | 0.31 | N.S. |
Food Intake (High Fat Diet) | 45 | 0.16 | −0.16 | 0.45 | N.S. | - | 20 | −0.04 | 0.41 | −0.47 | N.S. |
Glucose Level | 21 | 0.11 | −0.33 | 0.52 | N.S. | - | 24 | 0.21 | −0.21 | 0.56 | N.S. |
Insulin Level | 26 | 0.07 | −0.35 | 0.47 | N.S. | - | 48 | 0.30 | 0.01 | 0.54 | <0.05 |
Lean Mass | 38 | 0.20 | −0.14 | 0.50 | N.S. | - | 16 | 0.44 | −0.07 | 0.77 | N.S. |
Leptin Level | 16 | 0.31 | −0.29 | 0.74 | N.S. | - | 48 | 0.77 | 0.61 | 0.86 | <0.05 |
Locomotor Activity | 39 | −0.21 | 0.12 | −0.51 | N.S. | - | 76 | −0.13 | 0.10 | −0.35 | N.S. |
Locomotor Activity (Dark Phase) | 16 | −0.72 | −0.29 | −0.91 | <0.05 | N.S. | 32 | −0.40 | −0.04 | −0.67 | <0.05 |
Non-REM Sleep (24 h) | 16 | 0.82 | 0.54 | 0.93 | <0.05 | N.S. | 32 | 0.61 | 0.31 | 0.80 | <0.05 |
Non-REM Sleep (Dark Phase) | 16 | 0.79 | 0.48 | 0.92 | <0.05 | - | 16 | 0.49 | −0.01 | 0.79 | N.S. |
Non-REM Sleep (Light Phase) | 16 | 0.43 | −0.09 | 0.76 | N.S. | - | 16 | 0.49 | −0.01 | 0.79 | N.S. |
REM Sleep (24 h) | 16 | 0.45 | −0.06 | 0.77 | N.S. | - | 16 | −0.17 | 0.36 | −0.61 | N.S. |
REM Sleep (Dark Phase) | 16 | 0.34 | −0.19 | 0.71 | N.S. | - | 16 | −0.17 | 0.36 | −0.61 | N.S. |
REM Sleep (Light Phase) | 16 | 0.24 | −0.29 | 0.65 | N.S. | - | 16 | −0.17 | 0.36 | −0.61 | N.S. |
Wakefulness (24 h) | 16 | −0.84 | −0.58 | −0.94 | <0.05 | N.S. | 32 | −0.58 | −0.27 | −0.78 | <0.05 |
Wakefulness (Dark Phase) | 16 | −0.78 | −0.46 | −0.92 | <0.05 | - | 16 | −0.17 | 0.36 | −0.61 | N.S. |
Wakefulness (Light Phase) | 16 | −0.45 | 0.06 | −0.77 | N.S. | - | 16 | −0.49 | 0.01 | −0.79 | N.S. |
r = mean effect size, P value was calculated by Z test.
Table 4. Results of meta-analysis in male or female mice.
Behavioral/Physiological/Biochemical Parameters | Male | P value vs. Female | Female | ||||||||
Sample Size | r | 95% Confidence Interval | P value vs. 0 | Sample Size | r | 95% Confidence Interval | P value vs. 0 | ||||
Lower Limit | Upper Limit | Lower Limit | Upper Limit | ||||||||
Body Weight | 102 | 0.54 | 0.37 | 0.67 | <0.05 | - | 52 | 0.12 | −0.17 | 0.39 | N.S. |
Fat Mass | 87 | 0.72 | 0.58 | 0.81 | <0.05 | N.S. | 33 | 0.54 | 0.23 | 0.76 | <0.05 |
Food Intake | 46 | 0.01 | −0.29 | 0.31 | N.S. | - | 24 | −0.47 | −0.08 | −0.74 | <0.05 |
Food Intake (High Fat Diet) | 22 | −0.57 | −0.19 | −0.80 | <0.05 | N.S. | 22 | −0.51 | −0.11 | −0.77 | <0.05 |
Forced Swim Test | 34 | 0.12 | −0.23 | 0.44 | N.S. | - | 34 | 0.34 | 0.00 | 0.61 | <0.05 |
Glucose Level | 47 | 0.14 | −0.18 | 0.43 | N.S. | - | 69 | 0.21 | −0.04 | 0.43 | N.S. |
Glucose Level (High Fat Diet) | 10 | −0.09 | 0.57 | −0.68 | N.S. | - | 10 | −0.52 | 0.16 | −0.87 | N.S. |
Insulin Level | 67 | −0.03 | 0.23 | −0.29 | N.S. | - | 65 | 0.31 | 0.05 | 0.52 | <0.05 |
Lean Mass | 38 | 0.20 | −0.14 | 0.50 | N.S. | - | 20 | −0.09 | 0.37 | −0.51 | N.S. |
Leptin Level | 67 | 0.55 | 0.33 | 0.70 | <0.05 | N.S. | 55 | 0.60 | 0.39 | 0.76 | <0.05 |
Leptin Level (per gram Fat) (High Fat Diet) | 10 | 0.55 | −0.12 | 0.88 | N.S. | - | 10 | 0.38 | −0.32 | 0.82 | N.S. |
Open Field Test | 80 | 0.45 | 0.26 | 0.61 | <0.05 | - | 34 | 0.12 | −0.23 | 0.44 | N.S. |
T4 Level | 24 | 0.54 | 0.18 | 0.78 | <0.05 | - | 24 | 0.25 | −0.17 | 0.60 | N.S. |
Triglyceride Level | 24 | 0.14 | −0.27 | 0.52 | N.S. | - | 24 | −0.17 | 0.25 | −0.54 | N.S. |
r = mean effect size, P value was calculated by Z test.
Overall Meta-analysis: Comparison of the Effects of MCH Signaling on Phenotypes
The effects of MCH signaling on behavioral and metabolic parameters are summarized in Table 1. Overall, the meta-analysis revealed that the deletion of MCH signaling suppressed non-REM sleep, anxiety, response to novelty, startle response, stress-induced hyperthermia, conditioned place preference, and olfaction (p<0.05) and that MCH signaling deficiency enhanced locomotor activity, wakefulness, alcohol preference, motor activation by psychostimulants, aggression, male sexual behavior, and social interaction (p<0.05).
Regarding the metabolic parameters, the deletion of MCH signaling suppressed body weight, fat mass, leptin level, liver triglyceride level, and total cholesterol level (p<0.05), and MCH signaling deficiency enhanced food intake, body temperature, oxygen consumption, heart rate, mean arterial pressure, corticosterone level, and norepinephrine transporter level in the globus pallidus and nucleus accumbens (p<0.05). Additionally, MCH-signaling deficiency decreased D1 receptor levels in the nucleus accumbens and the ventral tegmental area, D2 receptor levels in the caudate-putamen and olfactory tubercle, NR1 subunit levels in the hippocampal formation, and 5-HT levels in the prefrontal cortex (p<0.05).
Strong correlations (95% confidential interval lower limit>0.3) were observed among the parameters related to body-weight regulation, including oxygen consumption, body temperature, fat mass, heart rate, arterial pressure, plasma leptin, locomotor activity, and serum total cholesterol. Other parameters that exhibited strong correlations (95% confidence interval with a lower limit>0.3) were non-REM sleep time, wake time, stress-induced hyperthermia, aggression, male sexual behavior, 5-HT levels in the prefrontal cortex, and norepinephrine transporter levels in the globus pallidus.
We did not find any inconsistency in the effect size between the earlier literatures before 2002 and recent literatures published in 2005 and later. For example, glucose level of MCH-deficient mouse reported in 1998 [11], is consistent with that reported in 2005 [13].
Separate Meta-analyses for MCH Deficiency or MCHR1 Deficiency
The different effects of MCH deficiency and MCHR1 deficiency on the behavioral and metabolic parameters are summarized in Table 2. Separate meta-analyses for the deficiencies of the ligand and receptor revealed that both MCH deficiency and MCHR1 deficiency suppressed the body weight when the mice were on a high-fat diet, the fat mass, and the non-REM sleep time, whereas both MCH-deficient mice and MCHR1-deficient mice exhibited increased wake times and locomotor activities during the dark phase (p<0.05). MCHR1 deficiency produced significant effects on body weight, plasma insulin, and plasma leptin, and the MCH-deficient mice did not exhibit any significant effects in these parameters.
Separate Meta-analyses by Background Strain
Behavioral studies of MCH and MCHR1 deficiency were performed using mice of the C57BL/6 (B6), 129/Sv (129), and 129-B6 mixed (mixed) backgrounds. Because each strain has different behavioral and metabolic characteristics due to genetic variations, the phenotypes of the gene-modified mice may vary among the different strains. Nevertheless, the phenotypes to which alterations in MCH signaling mainly contribute are thought to be recognizable in MCH signal-deficient mice of different strains. The results of the separate meta-analyses of the effects of the MCH and MCHR1 deficiencies on the behavioral and metabolic parameters are summarized in Table 3. In both the 129 and B6 backgrounds, the MCH and MCHR1 deficiencies decreased fat mass and increased oxygen consumption and locomotor activity (p<0.05). Interestingly, significant increases in food intake and body weight in the MCH-deficient mice were found only in the 129 (p<0.05) and not in the B6 background. The deletion of MCH signaling increased body temperature in the B6 and mixed backgrounds (p<0.05) and suppressed stress-induced hyperthermia in the B6 and mixed backgrounds (p<0.05). Due to the small number of studies that used mixed-background mice, data regarding body weight, fat mass, oxygen consumption, locomotor activity, and food intake were not available.
Table 3. Results of meta-analysis in C57BL/6, 129, or 129×C57BL/6 mice.
Behavioral/Physiological/Biochemical Parameters | C57BL/6 | P value vs. 129 | P value vs. 129×C57BL/6 | 129 | P value vs. 129×C57BL/6 | 129×C57BL/6 | ||||||||||||
Sample Size | r | 95% Confidence Interval | P value vs. 0 | Sample Size | r | 95% Confidence Interval | P value vs. 0 | Sample Size | r | 95% Confidence Interval | P value vs. 0 | |||||||
Lower Limit | Upper Limit | Lower Limit | Upper Limit | Lower Limit | Upper Limit | |||||||||||||
Body Length | 20 | 0.43 | −0.01 | 0.73 | N.S. | - | - | 22 | −0.10 | 0.33 | −0.50 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Body Temperature | 38 | −0.50 | −0.20 | −0.72 | <0.05 | - | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - | 30 | −0.75 | −0.54 | −0.88 | <0.05 |
Body Weight | 102 | 0.54 | 0.37 | 0.67 | <0.05 | N.S. | - | 38 | −0.49 | −0.17 | −0.71 | <0.05 | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Body Weight (High Fat Diet) | 23 | 0.69 | 0.36 | 0.87 | <0.05 | - | - | 7 | 0.54 | −0.36 | 0.92 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Corticosterone Level | 24 | 0.14 | −0.27 | 0.52 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | 28 | −0.84 | −0.66 | −0.93 | <0.05 |
Elevated Plus Maze | 106 | 0.38 | 0.19 | 0.54 | <0.05 | - | N.S. | 44 | 0.08 | −0.24 | 0.38 | N.S. | - | 42 | 0.36 | 0.05 | 0.61 | <0.05 |
Elevated Plus Maze (Habituated Environment) | 40 | 0.57 | 0.30 | 0.75 | <0.05 | - | - | 44 | −0.08 | 0.24 | −0.38 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Emergence Test | 40 | −0.58 | −0.31 | −0.76 | <0.05 | - | - | 44 | −0.14 | 0.17 | −0.43 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Emergence Test (Habituated Environment) | 40 | −0.24 | 0.09 | −0.52 | N.S. | - | - | 44 | −0.10 | 0.21 | −0.40 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Exploratory Behavior | 40 | 0.19 | −0.15 | 0.48 | N.S. | - | - | 44 | 0.07 | −0.24 | 0.37 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Fat Mass | 56 | 0.70 | 0.51 | 0.82 | <0.05 | N.S. | - | 23 | 0.61 | 0.18 | 0.84 | <0.05 | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Fat Mass (High Fat Diet) | 23 | 0.71 | 0.40 | 0.88 | <0.05 | N.S. | - | 7 | 0.79 | 0.10 | 0.97 | <0.05 | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Food Intake | 31 | −0.17 | 0.21 | −0.51 | N.S. | - | - | 8 | −0.86 | −0.39 | −0.97 | <0.05 | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Food Intake (High Fat Diet) | 45 | 0.16 | −0.16 | 0.45 | N.S. | - | - | 7 | −0.66 | 0.18 | −0.94 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Ghrelin Level | 24 | 0.28 | −0.14 | 0.61 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | 10 | −0.33 | 0.38 | −0.79 | N.S. |
Glucose Level | 23 | 0.21 | −0.25 | 0.60 | N.S. | - | - | 8 | 0.22 | −0.58 | 0.80 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Glucose Level (High Fat Diet) | 37 | 0.13 | −0.24 | 0.46 | N.S. | - | - | 7 | 0.83 | 0.20 | 0.97 | <0.05 | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Insulin Level | 9 | −0.30 | 0.45 | −0.80 | N.S. | - | - | 8 | −0.20 | 0.59 | −0.79 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Insulin Level (High Fat Diet) | 23 | 0.53 | 0.11 | 0.79 | <0.05 | - | - | 7 | 0.31 | −0.58 | 0.86 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Lean Mass | 38 | 0.20 | −0.14 | 0.50 | N.S. | - | - | 8 | 0.36 | −0.46 | 0.85 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Lean Mass (High Fat Diet) | 9 | 0.55 | −0.18 | 0.89 | N.S. | - | - | 7 | −0.46 | 0.45 | −0.90 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Leptin Level | 9 | 0.43 | −0.33 | 0.85 | N.S. | - | - | 8 | 0.41 | −0.41 | 0.87 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Leptin Level (High Fat Diet) | 23 | 0.66 | 0.31 | 0.85 | <0.05 | - | - | 7 | 0.60 | −0.28 | 0.93 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Leptin Level (per gram fat) | 9 | −0.31 | 0.44 | −0.81 | N.S. | - | - | 8 | 0.00 | −0.70 | 0.70 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Leptin Level (per gram fat) (High Fat Diet) | 9 | 0.48 | −0.27 | 0.87 | N.S. | - | - | 7 | 0.66 | −0.19 | 0.94 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Locomotor Activity | 39 | −0.21 | 0.12 | −0.51 | N.S. | - | - | 8 | −0.93 | −0.66 | −0.99 | <0.05 | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Locomotor Activity (High Fat Diet) | 8 | −0.94 | −0.70 | −0.99 | <0.05 | N.S. | - | 7 | −0.92 | −0.55 | −0.99 | <0.05 | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Locomotor Activity (Dark Phase) | 16 | −0.72 | −0.29 | −0.91 | <0.05 | N.S. | - | 8 | −0.91 | −0.57 | −0.98 | <0.05 | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Locomotor Activity (Dark Phase) (High Fat Diet) | 8 | −0.89 | −0.50 | −0.98 | <0.05 | N.S. | - | 7 | −0.85 | −0.27 | −0.98 | <0.05 | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Motor Coordination | 100 | −0.08 | 0.13 | −0.28 | N.S. | - | - | 110 | −0.15 | 0.05 | −0.33 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Non-REM Sleep (24 h) | 32 | 0.61 | 0.31 | 0.80 | <0.05 | - | - | 20 | 0.05 | −0.40 | 0.48 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Non-REM Sleep (Dark Phase) | 16 | 0.49 | −0.01 | 0.79 | N.S. | - | - | 20 | 0.14 | −0.32 | 0.55 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Non-REM Sleep (Light Phase) | 16 | 0.49 | −0.01 | 0.79 | N.S. | - | - | 20 | −0.08 | 0.38 | −0.50 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Open Field Test | 160 | 0.24 | 0.09 | 0.39 | <0.05 | - | N.S. | 44 | 0.06 | −0.25 | 0.36 | N.S. | - | 80 | 0.45 | 0.26 | 0.61 | <0.05 |
Open Field Test (Habituated Environment) | 80 | 0.46 | 0.26 | 0.63 | <0.05 | - | - | 88 | 0.16 | −0.07 | 0.36 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Oxygen Consumption | 16 | −0.95 | −0.85 | −0.99 | <0.05 | N.S. | - | 8 | −0.98 | −0.88 | −1.00 | <0.05 | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Oxygen Consumption (High Fat Diet) | 8 | −0.97 | −0.83 | −0.99 | <0.05 | N.S. | - | 7 | −0.99 | −0.95 | −1.00 | <0.05 | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Response to Handling | 40 | -0.08 | 0.25 | −0.39 | N.S. | - | - | 44 | 0.29 | −0.02 | 0.55 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Response to Novelty | 40 | 0.26 | −0.07 | 0.54 | N.S. | - | - | 44 | 0.24 | −0.07 | 0.51 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Response to Supine Restraint | 20 | 0.00 | −0.44 | 0.44 | N.S. | - | - | 22 | 0.00 | −0.42 | 0.42 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Spatial Learning Function | 20 | −0.16 | 0.31 | −0.56 | N.S. | - | - | 22 | −0.25 | 0.19 | −0.61 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Startle Response | 20 | 0.15 | −0.31 | 0.56 | N.S. | - | - | 22 | 0.50 | 0.10 | 0.76 | <0.05 | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Stress Induced Hyperthermia | 22 | 0.46 | 0.05 | 0.74 | <0.05 | - | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - | 30 | 0.78 | 0.59 | 0.89 | <0.05 |
T4 Level | 18 | 0.79 | 0.51 | 0.92 | <0.05 | - | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - | 24 | 0.54 | 0.18 | 0.78 | <0.05 |
Triglyceride Level | 24 | 0.55 | 0.19 | 0.78 | <0.05 | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | 24 | −0.17 | 0.25 | −0.54 | N.S. |
Wakefulness (24 h) | 32 | −0.58 | −0.78 | −0.27 | <0.05 | - | - | 20 | 0.07 | −0.38 | 0.50 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Wakefulness (Dark Phase) | 16 | −0.17 | 0.36 | −0.61 | N.S. | - | - | 20 | −0.09 | 0.37 | −0.51 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Wakefulness (Light Phase) | 16 | −0.49 | 0.01 | −0.79 | N.S. | - | - | 20 | 0.22 | −0.25 | 0.60 | N.S. | - | - | - | - | - | - |
r = mean effect size, P value was calculated by Z test.
Under a high-fat diet, the deficiency in MCH signaling reduced the fat mass and increased oxygen consumption in both the 129 and B6 backgrounds (p<0.05). The lack of MCH signaling exhibited a suppressive effect on glucose levels under a high-fat diet only in the 129 mice (p<0.05), and no significant effect was observed in the B6 mice. Strain-specific effects were also observed in the following parameters: anxiolytic effects, as assessed with an open field test and elevated plus maze test, were observed in the MCH-deficient mice of both the B6 and mixed backgrounds (p<0.05), whereas no significant effects were observed in the 129 background mice; MCH-signaling deficiency in the B6 background produced a significant increase in wake time and a significant decrease in non-REM sleep time, and MCH-signaling deficiency in the 129 background did not produce significant changes in sleep/wakefulness behaviors.
Separate Meta-analyses by Sex
To examine the differential role of MCH signaling in males and females, we performed separate meta-analyses, which are summarized in Table 4. MCH or MCHR1 deficiency suppressed fat mass and plasma leptin and enhanced high-fat diet intake in both sexes (p<0.05). The deletion of MCH signaling in the male mice suppressed body weight, anxiety, and plasma T4 (p<0.05). The deletion of MCH signaling in the female mice suppressed depressive behavior and plasma insulin (p<0.05) and enhanced food intake (p<0.05).
Discussion
The Role of MCH Signaling in Feeding Behavior
Nearly two decades ago and long after the report of the expression of MCH in the mammalian lateral hypothalamus [35], Qu et al. reported the orexigenic effect of MCH [4]. Because the lateral hypothalamus is known to be a “feeding center” [36], the orexigenic effects of MCH received much attention that resulted in numerous reports that used MCH and MCH receptor antagonists [8], [37], [38]. Consistent with these pharmacological studies, the first report of MCH-deficient mice found a mild reduction in food intake [11]. However, later reports failed to reproduce this hypophagia in MCH signaling-deficient mice [13], [15].
Surprisingly, the present meta-analysis revealed that MCH-signaling deficiency caused mild but significant increases in food intake when the mice were given normal or high-fat diets. However, the separate analyses of the MCH-deficient mice and the MCHR1-deficient mice did not show such an effect. The combined meta-analysis of the ligand- and receptor-deficient mice had sufficient statistical power to detect mild hyperphagia in the MCH signaling-deficient mice. The increased food intake of the MCH signaling-deficient mice does not contradict the acute orexigenic effects of MCH that have been reported by independent researchers based on pharmacological experiments [4], [8], [37], [38]; rather, these effects suggest that the acute orexigenic effects of MCH may be cancelled by long-term body-weight homeostasis mechanisms. For example, the administration of orexin, another orexigenic peptide that is expressed in the lateral hypothalamus, results in increased food intake in acute experiments but not in chronic injection experiments [39], [40]. Moreover, the overexpression of orexin renders mice resistant to diet-induced obesity [41], which suggests that short-term feeding behaviors and long-term body-weight homeostasis are regulated by distinct mechanisms.
MCH Signaling Regulates Body Weight Homeostasis
Our meta-analysis confirmed that the body weights were reduced and that oxygen consumption increased in the MCH signaling-deficient mice on both normal chow and high-fat diets. Thus, the lean phenotype of the MCH signaling-deficient mice is likely due to increased energy output and not due to reduced energy input. The increased food intake of the MCH signaling-deficient mice may be caused by feedback regulation that maintains stable body weights. Consistent with this notion, our meta-analysis revealed that the MCH signaling-deficient mice exhibited decreased plasma leptin levels; leptin is a major anorexigenic hormone that is secreted from adipose tissues. Suppressive effects of MCH signaling deficiency on the weight gains of mice on high-fat diets were also found in the separate meta-analyses of the MCH-deficient, MCHR1-deficient, B6, male, and female mice. The lack of a suppressive effect on diet-induced obesity in the 129 background may have resulted from the fact that the wild-type 129 strain is resistant to diet-induced obesity, which, due to a floor effect, would make it difficult to observe a suppressive effect on body-weight gain [13].
The major source of energy output in rodents is the brown adipose tissue; the thermogenic activity of this tissue is regulated by the sympathetic nervous system [42]. Consistently, MCH neurons provide polysynaptic projections to the sympathetic nervous system and the skeletal muscle [43]. The loss of MCH signaling results in enhanced sympathetic tone, which increases energy output. The increased heart rates and higher arterial pressures in the MCH signaling-deficient mice also support the presence of enhanced sympathetic tone. The stress-induced hyperthermia of the MCH signaling-deficient mice may be partly due to the enhanced activity of the sympathetic nervous-brown adipose tissue system [44]. The present meta-analysis also revealed increased locomotion in the MCH signaling-deficient mice on both normal chow and high-fat diets; this increased locomotion would also contribute to energy output via skeletal muscle activity.
MCH Signaling Regulates Sleep/Wakefulness Behavior
MCH neurons are actively firing during non-REM sleep and are more active during REM sleep. Our meta-analysis confirmed that MCH signaling deficiency enhanced wakefulness, suppressed non-REM sleep, and did not alter REM sleep. Because MCH neurons project to the locus coeruleus [7], which is crucial for wakefulness [45], [46], the loss of the inhibitory input of the MCH neurons onto the locus coeruleus may result in enhanced wakefulness.
The lack of a significant effect on REM sleep seems to be inconsistent with the fact that the highest activity levels of the MCH neurons occur during REM sleep [47]. However, the optogenetic activation of MCH neurons in MCHR1-deficient mice enhances REM sleep [48], which suggests that the MCH peptide released from the MCH neurons is not crucial for the generation of REM sleep.
MCH Signaling Regulates Anxiety and Stress Responses
There have been contradictory reports about the anxiety-related behavior of MCH signaling-deficient mice [19], [24], [27], [49], [50]. Our meta-analysis did not produce clear conclusions regarding the effects of MCH signaling deficiency on anxiety; weak anxiolytic effects were found for in the elevated plus maze and open field tests, but weak anxiogenic effects were found in the emergence test. The increased plasma corticosterone levels during non-stressed conditions observed in the MCH signaling-deficient mice suggest that the activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis is enhanced and that these mice should tend to exhibit anxiogenic behavior [51]. Consistent with the enhanced activity of the HPA axis, the MCH signaling-deficient mice exhibited increased stress-induced hyperthermia, startle responses, and responses to novelty.
The Role of MCH Signaling in Reward-seeking Behavior and Addiction
MCHR1 is most strongly expressed in the nucleus accumbens [7], which, along with the midbrain dopamine neurons, constitutes the neural circuitry that regulates reward-seeking behavior and addiction. Consistently, our meta-analysis revealed that the MCH signaling-deficient mice exhibited decreased cocaine-induced conditioned place preferences, which suggests that the MCH signaling-deficient mice are resistant to drug addiction. The increased locomotion of the MCH-deficient mice may have resulted from alterations in the mesolimbic dopamine system. Alterations in the dopaminergic system may also be involved in the hyperphagia exhibited by the MCH signaling-deficient mice. Although MCH signaling-deficient mice exhibit increased alcohol intake and a seemingly contradictory resistance to psychostimulants, the molecular target of alcohol in the brain is not the dopamine transporter but the GABA A receptor [52]. Thus, the neural substrates of alcohol-seeking behavior are thought to be distinct from those related to addictive behavior associated with dopamine-related psychostimulants.
MCH Signaling Regulates Aggression and Sexual Motivation
The present meta-analysis revealed that MCH-signaling deficiency was strongly correlated with enhanced aggression and male sexual behavior (r = −0.704 and r = −0.832, respectively). MCHR1 is expressed in the ventromedial nucleus of the hypothalamus [7], which contains neurons that are involved in attack behavior and male sexual behavior [53], [54]. MCHR1 expression in the medial amygdala also plays an important role in aggression [55]. Defects in the olfactory system of MCH-deficient mice might be involved in the alterations of aggression and male sexual behavior [22].
Therapeutic Implications
Based on an anorexic effect of the MCH peptide and the lean phenotypes of the MCH-deficient and MCHR1-deficient mice, MCHR1 antagonists have been recognized as attractive targets for the treatment of obesity. Continued efforts to develop small compounds that antagonize MCH signaling have extended the list of MCHR1 antagonists; some of these antagonists have been examined in clinical trials [37], [38], [56]–[58]. The current meta-analysis suggests that in addition to the treatment of obesity, possible therapeutic targets of MCHR1 antagonists include anxiety disorder, depressive disorder, hypersomnia, sleepiness, male sexual dysfunction, drug addiction, and hypotension. The possible side effects of the clinical use of MCHR1 antagonists include insomnia, hypersexuality, aggression, and hypertension.
The prominent difference in MCH signaling between humans and rodents is that humans express MCHR2. Although MCHR2 is expressed in the brain in a pattern similar to that of MCHR1, the downstream target of MCHR2 is different from that of MCHR1 because MCHR2 is a Gq-coupled receptor, whereas MCHR1 is a Gi-coupled receptor [59]–[61]. Thus, it is difficult to predict the clinical effects of MCHR1 antagonists in humans without an understanding of the physiological role of MCHR2 signaling. Indeed, a phase I clinical trial of an MCHR1 antagonist was discontinued due to the vivid dreams experienced by the subjects during the first week of treatment (NGD-4715; http://phx.corporate-ir.net/phoenix.zhtml?c=). This result could not be predicted based on the suppression of REM sleep following the injection of MCHR1 antagonists or the normal REM sleep of the MCH signaling-deficient mice.
Limitations and Strengths
Studies that utilized mice in which the MCH neurons were ablated with ataxin-3 or diphtheria toxin were excluded from our meta-analysis because the lack of the GABAergic neurotransmission of the MCH neurons may result in behavioral alterations that are independent of the MCH-MCHR1 signal. Nevertheless, MCH neuron-ablated mice are lean and exhibit increased oxygen consumption, hyperlocomotion, and reduced responses to cocaine; these phenotypes are consistent with the results of the current meta-analysis of MCH signaling-deficient mice [12], [62].
The present meta-analysis is not free of limitations. The sample sizes varied across the parameters: many of the included studies examined body weight and food intake, but few studies examined oxygen consumption, largely because a special experimental apparatus is required to measure and control oxygen and carbon dioxide levels to allow for indirect calorimetry. The numbers of studies that we used to calculate the means of each parameter were small. Hence, there was not enough power for a test of heterogeneity. Furthermore, the number of parameters used for separate meta-analysis was small because we adopted the parameters which were balanced in terms of genotypes, sex, and strains.
To the best of our knowledge, the present work is the first study to systematically compare the effects of MCH signaling on behavioral and metabolic phenotypes. Our analyses revealed the robust contribution of MCH signaling to energy metabolism, reward behavior, and anxiety. Large numbers of researchers and international consortia are working on phenotyping genetically modified mice [63], and in combination with the remarkable progress that has been made in genome-editing techniques, this work has rendered the production of genetically modified mice easier and faster. Thus, the amounts of behavioral and metabolic data related to genetically modified mice will continue to increase. To obtain relevant conclusions from these large datasets, meta-analyses will become increasingly important in basic biomedical research.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Hiroko Arai and Akane Iijima for excellent technical assistance for this study.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number 25126725 to HF, MEXT KAKENHI Grant Number 25460318 to HF, the Research Grant from Nestle Nutrition Council Japan to HF, JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number 25460317 to KT, JSPSKAKENHI Grant Number 24780292 to YT, and the Research Grant from Toho University School of Medicine (No.21-3) to HF. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Egger M, Smith GD, Phillips AN (1997) Metaanalysis Principles and procedures. BMJ 315: 1533–1537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Crabbe JC, Wahlsten D, Dudek BC (1999) Genetics of mouse behavior: interactions with laboratory environment. Science 284: 1670–1672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Kawauchi H, Kawazoe I, Tsubokawa M, Kishida M, Baker BI (1983) Characterization of melanin-concentrating hormone in chum salmon pituitaries. Nature 305: 321–323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Qu D, Ludwig DS, Gammeltoft S, Piper M, Pelleymounter MA, et al. (1996) A role for melanin-concentrating hormone in the central regulation of feeding behaviour. Nature 380: 243–247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Saito Y, Nothacker H, Wang Z, Lin SHS, Leslie F, et al. (1999) Molecular characterization of the melanin-concentrating- hormone receptor. Nature 400: 265–269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Pissios P, Bradley RL, Maratos-Flier E (2006) Expanding the scales: The multiple roles of MCH in regulating energy balance and other biological functions. Endocr Rev 27: 606–620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Saito Y, Cheng M, Leslie FM, Civelli O (2001) Expression of the melanin-concentrating hormone (MCH) receptor mRNA in the rat brain. J Comp Neurol 435: 26–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Rossi M, Choi SJ, O’Shea D, Miyoshi T, Ghatei MA, et al. (1997) Melanin-concentrating hormone acutely stimulates feeding, but chronic administration has no effect on body weight. Endocrinology 138: 351–355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Gomori A, Ishihara A, Ito M, Mashiko S, Matsushita H, et al. (2003) Chronic intracerebroventricular infusion of MCH causes obesity in mice. Melanin-concentrating hormone. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 284: E583–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Ludwig DS, Tritos NA, Mastaitis JW, Kulkarni RN, Kokkotou E, et al. (2001) Melanin-concentrating hormone overexpression in transgenic mice leads to obesity and insulin resistance. J Clin Invest 107: 379–386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Shimada M, Tritos NA, Lowell BB, Flier JS, Maratos-Flier E (1998) Mice lacking melanin-concentrating hormone are hypophagic and lean. Nature 396: 670–674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Alon T, Friedman JM (2006) Late-onset leanness in mice with targeted ablation of melanin concentrating hormone neurons. J Neurosci 26: 389–397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Kokkotou E, Jeon JY, Wang X, Marino FE, Trombly DJ, et al. (2005) Mice with MCH ablation resist diet-induced obesity through strain-specific mechanisms. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 289: R117–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Georgescu D, Sears RM, Hommel JD, Barrot M, Bolan CA, et al. (2005) The Hypothalamic Neuropeptide Melanin-Concentrating Hormone Acts in the Nucleus Accumbens to Modulate Feeding Behavior and Forced-Swim Performance. Nature 25: 2933–2940 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1714-04.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Astrand A, Bohlooly-Y M, Larsdotter S, Mahlapuu M, Andersén H, et al. (2004) Mice lacking melanin-concentrating hormone receptor 1 demonstrate increased heart rate associated with altered autonomic activity. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 287: R749–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Sherwood A, Wosiski-Kuhn M, Nguyen T, Holland PC, Lakaye B, et al. (2012) The role of melanin-concentrating hormone in conditioned reward learning. Eur J Neurosci 36: 3126–3133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Marsh DJ, Weingarth DT, Novi DE, Chen HY, Trumbauer ME, et al. (2002) Melanin-concentrating hormone 1 receptor-deficient mice are lean, hyperactive, and hyperphagic and have altered metabolism. Proc Natl Acad Sci 99: 3240–3245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Glier MB, Pissios P, Babich SL, Macdonald MLE, Hayden MR, et al. (2010) The metabolic phenotype of SCD1-deficient mice is independent of melanin-concentrating hormone. Peptides 31: 123–129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Lalonde R, Qian S (2007) Exploratory activity, motor coordination, and spatial learning in Mchr1 knockout mice. Behav Brain Res 178: 293–304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Willie JT, Sinton CM, Maratos-Flier E, Yanagisawa M (2008) Abnormal response of melanin-concentrating hormone deficient mice to fasting: hyperactivity and rapid eye movement sleep suppression. Neuroscience 156: 819–829. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Ahnaou A, Dautzenberg FM, Huysmans H, Steckler T, Drinkenburg WHIM (2011) Contribution of melanin-concentrating hormone (MCH1) receptor to thermoregulation and sleep stabilization: evidence from MCH1 (−/−) mice. Behav Brain Res 218: 42–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Adams AC, Domouzoglou EM, Chee MJ, Segal-Lieberman G, Pissios P, et al. (2011) Ablation of the hypothalamic neuropeptide melanin concentrating hormone is associated with behavioral abnormalities that reflect impaired olfactory integration. Behav Brain Res 224: 195–200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Duncan EA, Sorrell JE, Adamantidis A, Rider T, Jandacek RJ, et al. (2007) Alcohol drinking in MCH receptor-1-deficient mice. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 31: 1325–1337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Roy M, David NK, Danao J V, Baribault H, Tian H, et al. (2006) Genetic inactivation of melanin-concentrating hormone receptor subtype 1 (MCHR1) in mice exerts anxiolytic-like behavioral effects. Neuropsychopharmacology 31: 112–120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Zhou D, Shen Z, Strack AM, Marsh DJ, Shearman LP (2005) Enhanced running wheel activity of both Mch1r- and Pmch-deficient mice. Regul Pept 124: 53–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Tyhon A, Lakaye B, Adamantidis A, Tirelli E (2008) Amphetamine- and cocaine-induced conditioned place preference and concomitant psychomotor sensitization in mice with genetically inactivated melanin-concentrating hormone MCH(1) receptor. Eur J Pharmacol 599: 72–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Adamantidis A, Thomas E, Foidart A, Tyhon A, Coumans B, et al. (2005) Disrupting the melanin-concentrating hormone receptor 1 in mice leads to cognitive deficits and alterations of NMDA receptor function. Eur J Neurosci 21: 2837–2844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Smith DG, Tzavara ET, Shaw J, Luecke S, Wade M, et al. (2005) Mesolimbic dopamine super-sensitivity in melanin-concentrating hormone-1 receptor-deficient mice. J Neurosci 25: 914–922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Smith DG, Davis RJ, Rorick-Kehn L, Morin M, Witkin JM, et al. (2006) Melanin-concentrating hormone-1 receptor modulates neuroendocrine, behavioral, and corticolimbic neurochemical stress responses in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology 31: 1135–1145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Chen Y, Hu C, Hsu C-K, Zhang Q, Bi C, et al. (2002) Targeted disruption of the melanin-concentrating hormone receptor-1 results in hyperphagia and resistance to diet-induced obesity. Endocrinology 143: 2469–2477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Egger M, Smith GD, Schneider M, Minder C (1997) Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 315: 629–634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Lipsey MW, Wilson D (2000) Practical Meta-Analysis. SAGE Publications, Inc.
- 33. Rosenthal R, DiMatteo MR (2001) Meta-analysis: recent developments in quantitative methods for literature reviews. Annu Rev Psychol 52: 59–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JPT, Rothstein HR (2010) A basic introduction to fixed-effect and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Res Synth Methods 1: 97–111 10.1002/jrsm.12 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Skofitsch G, Jacobowitz DM, Zamir N (1985) Immunohistochemical localization of a melanin concentrating hormone-like peptide in the rat brain. Brain Res Bull 15: 635–649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Anand BK, Brobeck JR (1951) Hypothalamic control of food intake in rats and cats. Yale J Biol Med 24: 123–140. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Takekawa S, Asami A, Ishihara Y, Terauchi J, Kato K, et al. (2002) T-226296: a novel, orally active and selective melanin-concentrating hormone receptor antagonist. Eur J Pharmacol 438: 129–135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Borowsky B, Durkin MM, Ogozalek K, Marzabadi MR, DeLeon J, et al. (2002) Antidepressant, anxiolytic and anorectic effects of a melanin-concentrating hormone-1 receptor antagonist. Nat Med 8: 825–830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Sakurai T, Amemiya A, Ishii M, Matsuzaki I, Chemelli RM, et al. (1998) Orexins and orexin receptors: a family of hypothalamic neuropeptides and G protein-coupled receptors that regulate feeding behavior. Cell 92: 573–585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Yamanaka A, Sakurai T, Katsumoto T, Yanagisawa M, Goto K (1999) Chronic intracerebroventricular administration of orexin-A to rats increases food intake in daytime, but has no effect on body weight. Brain Res 849: 248–252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Funato H, Tsai AL, Willie JT, Kisanuki Y, Williams SC, et al. (2009) Enhanced orexin receptor-2 signaling prevents diet-induced obesity and improves leptin sensitivity. Cell Metab 9: 64–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Cannon B, Nedergaard J (2004) Brown adipose tissue: function and physiological significance. Physiol Rev 84: 277–359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Kerman IA, Bernard R, Rosenthal D, Beals J, Akil H, et al. (2007) Distinct populations of presympathetic-premotor neurons express orexin or melanin-concentrating hormone in the rat lateral hypothalamus. J Comp Neurol 505: 586–601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Nakamura K (2011) Central circuitries for body temperature regulation and fever. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 301: R1207–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Carter ME, Yizhar O, Chikahisa S, Nguyen H, Adamantidis A, et al. (2010) Tuning arousal with optogenetic modulation of locus coeruleus neurons. Nat Neurosci 13: 1526–1533. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Monti JM, Torterolo P, Lagos P (2013) Melanin-concentrating hormone control of sleep-wake behavior. Sleep Med Rev 17: 293–298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Hassani OK, Lee MG, Jones BE (2009) Melanin-concentrating hormone neurons discharge in a reciprocal manner to orexin neurons across the sleep-wake cycle. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106: 2418–2422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Jego S, Glasgow SD, Herrera CG, Ekstrand M, Reed SJ, et al. (2013) Optogenetic identification of a rapid eye movement sleep modulatory circuit in the hypothalamus. Nat Neurosci 16: 1637–1643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Roy M, David N, Cueva M, Giorgetti M (2007) A study of the involvement of melanin-concentrating hormone receptor 1 (MCHR1) in murine models of depression. Biol Psychiatry 61: 174–180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Tyhon A, Adamantidis A, Foidart A, Grisar T, Lakaye B, et al. (2006) Mice lacking the melanin-concentrating hormone receptor-1 exhibit an atypical psychomotor susceptibility to cocaine and no conditioned cocaine response. Behav Brain Res 173: 94–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. De Kloet ER, Joëls M, Holsboer F (2005) Stress and the brain: from adaptation to disease. Nat Rev Neurosci 6: 463–475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Tabakoff B, Hoffman PL (2013) The neurobiology of alcohol consumption and alcoholism: an integrative history. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 113: 20–37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Lin D, Boyle MP, Dollar P, Lee H, Lein ES, et al. (2011) Functional identification of an aggression locus in the mouse hypothalamus. Nature 470: 221–226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Anderson DJ (2012) Optogenetics, sex, and violence in the brain: implications for psychiatry. Biol Psychiatry 71: 1081–1089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Nelson RJ, Trainor BC (2007) Neural mechanisms of aggression. Nat Rev Neurosci 8: 536–546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Shearman L (2003) Chronic MCH-1 receptor modulation alters appetite, body weight and adiposity in rats. Eur J Pharmacol 475: 37–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Gehlert DR, Rasmussen K, Shaw J, Li X, Ardayfio P, et al. (2009) Preclinical Evaluation of Melanin-Concentrating Hormone Receptor 1 Antagonism for the Treatment of Obesity and Depression. 329: 429–438 10.1124/jpet.108.143362. thought [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Moore NA, Sargent BJ, Guzzo PR, Surman MD (2014) From preclinical to clinical development: the example of a novel treatment for obesity. Neurobiol Dis 61: 47–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. An S, Cutler G, Zhao JJ, Huang SG, Tian H, et al. (2001) Identification and characterization of a melanin-concentrating hormone receptor. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98: 7576–7581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Hill J, Duckworth M, Murdock P, Rennie G, Sabido-David C, et al. (2001) Molecular cloning and functional characterization of MCH2, a novel human MCH receptor. J Biol Chem 276: 20125–20129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Sailer AW, Sano H, Zeng Z, McDonald TP, Pan J, et al. (2001) Identification and characterization of a second melanin-concentrating hormone receptor, MCH-2R. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98: 7564–7569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Whiddon BB, Palmiter RD (2013) Ablation of Neurons Expressing Melanin-Concentrating Hormone (MCH) in Adult Mice Improves Glucose Tolerance Independent of MCH Signaling. J Neurosci 33: 2009–2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Brown SDM, Moore MW (2012) The International Mouse Phenotyping Consortium: past and future perspectives on mouse phenotyping. Mamm Genome 23: 632–640. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.