Abstract
We recently demonstrated that vasopressin (AVP) in the lateral septum modulates social play behavior differently in male and female juvenile rats. However, the extent to which different social contexts (i.e., exposure to an unfamiliar play partner in different environments) affect the regulation of social play remains largely unknown. Given that AVP and the closely related neuropeptide oxytocin (OXT) modulate social behavior as well as anxiety-like behavior, we hypothesized that these neuropeptides may regulate social play behavior differently in novel (novel cage) as opposed to familiar (home cage) social environments. Administration of the specific AVP V1a receptor (V1aR) antagonist (CH2)5Tyr(Me2)AVP into the lateral septum enhanced home cage social play behavior in males but reduced it in females, confirming our previous findings. These effects were context-specific because V1aR blockade did not alter novel cage social play behavior in either sex. Furthermore, social play in females was reduced by AVP in the novel cage and by OXT in the home cage. Additionally, females administered the specific OXT receptor antagonist desGly-NH2,d(CH2)5−[Tyr(Me)2,Thr4]OVT showed less social play in the novel as compared to the home cage. AVP enhanced anxiety-related behavior in males (tested on the elevated plus-maze), but failed to do so in females, suggesting that exogenous AVP alters social play and anxiety-related behavior via distinct and sex-specific mechanisms. Moreover, none of the other drug treatments that altered social play had an effect on anxiety, suggesting that these drug-induced behavioral alterations are relatively specific to social behavior. Overall, we showed that AVP and OXT systems in the lateral septum modulate social play in juvenile rats in neuropeptide-, sex- and social context-specific ways. These findings underscore the importance of considering not only sex, but also social context, in how AVP and OXT modulate social behavior.
Keywords: female, juvenile, lateral septum, male, oxytocin receptor, play-fighting, sex difference, V1a receptor
Introduction
Social play (also referred to as play-fighting or rough-and-tumble play) is predominantly displayed among juveniles of both sexes across many mammalian species (Bekoff and Byers, 1998; Pellis and Iwaniuk, 2000; Burghardt, 2005). These social play activities seem to contribute to the development of social and emotional skills needed throughout life, as shown in humans, non-human primates, and rodents (Sigman and Ruskin, 1999; Guralnick et al., 2006; Cordoni and Palagi, 2011). Social play deficits are observed in neurodevelopmental disorders such as autism spectrum disorders (ASD), early-onset schizophrenia, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Alessandri, 1992; Moller and Husby, 2000; Jordan, 2003). Likewise, social play activities were found to be altered in juvenile male rats that were exposed to either maternal separation (Veenema and Neumann, 2009) or prenatal immune stress (Taylor et al., 2012). Therefore, identifying the neurochemical substrates involved in the regulation of juvenile social play behavior will increase our understanding of normal as well as impaired social development.
Among other neurochemical substrates, arginine vasopressin (AVP) and oxytocin (OXT) are two likely candidates to regulate juvenile social play. AVP and OXT have robust neuromodulatory effects on adult social behavior, including aggression, social recognition, and pair-bonding (Donaldson and Young, 2008; Veenema and Neumann, 2008; Goodson and Thompson, 2010), but less is known about their role in juvenile social behaviors, such as social play. Furthermore, AVP and OXT systems in the brain are sexually dimorphic (De Vries and Panzica, 2006; Dumais et al., 2013a). For example, males have more AVP-expressing cells in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and medial amygdala and denser AVP-axonal projections to limbic brain regions, especially to the lateral septum (De Vries et al., 1981; Van Leeuwen et al., 1985; Szot and Dorsa, 1993). This sexually dimorphic AVP system is already present in juveniles (De Vries et al., 1981) and is found in many mammalian species (De Vries and Panzica, 2006). Furthermore, adult male rats have higher OXT receptor (OTR) binding densities in various forebrain regions, including the lateral septum (Dumais et al., 2013a). These findings suggest that AVP and OXT, acting on, e.g., the lateral septum, may modulate social behavior, and possibly juvenile social play, in sexually dimorphic ways.
Indeed, we recently showed that pharmacological blockade of AVP V1a receptors in the lateral septum increased social play in juvenile males while it reduced social play in juvenile females (Veenema et al., 2013). In line with this, a negative correlation was found between AVP mRNA expression in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and social play in juvenile males (Paul et al., 2014). It is unknown, however, whether OXT via OTR in the lateral septum modulates social play behavior, and if so, whether it acts in sex-specific ways. Therefore, our first aim was to determine whether acute pharmacological manipulations of the OXT system in the lateral septum alter social play behaviors in juvenile male and female rats. Given its role in facilitating pro-social behaviors, such as mother-infant bonding, filial attachment, and pair-bonding (Williams et al., 1992; Feldman et al., 2007; Kojima and Alberts, 2011; Feldman, 2012), we predicted that OXT would enhance social play behavior and that it may do so in sex-specific ways.
In addition, little is known about the impact of contextual factors on how AVP and OXT influence social behaviors. Indeed, social play behavior in rodents typically has been tested in only one social context, i.e., exposure to a play partner in either a home cage or a novel cage setting. Understanding how different social contexts modify the social effects of AVP and OXT may inform appropriate use of AVP- and OXT-based drugs to restore social function (Bartz et al., 2011) since both neuropeptides have been implicated in ASD and other social disorders (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2011; Modi and Young, 2012) and are currently being tested in clinical trials (clinicaltrials.gov). Therefore, our second aim was to determine whether AVP and OXT regulate social play differently in novel as opposed to familiar environments. We hypothesized that exposure to an unfamiliar rat in the novel environment (novel cage) vs. a familiar environment (home cage) would alter the salience of the stimulus rat, and thus, result in a differential regulation of social play by AVP and OXT. Because AVP typically enhances (Landgraf et al., 1995; Beiderbeck et al., 2007) while OXT reduces (Windle et al., 1997; Ring et al., 2006; Blume et al., 2008) anxiety-like behaviors of rats exposed to novel environments, we predicted that AVP would reduce social play while OXT would increase social play in the novel cage. To determine whether drug-induced alterations in social play were associated with alterations in anxiety-like behavior, we exposed rats to the elevated plus-maze to measure non-social anxiety-like behavior.
To study social context-specific roles of AVP and OXT in the modulation of juvenile social play behavior, the lateral septum was chosen as the region of interest because (1) this area serves as a hub in the brain by integrating social, emotional, and contextual information from different brain systems and contributing to the coordination of adaptive context-specific behavioral responses (Sheehan et al., 2004; Luo et al., 2011), (2) V1aR and the OTR are abundantly expressed in this area in juvenile rats (Lukas et al., 2010; Veenema et al., 2012), and (3) AVP via V1aR in the lateral septum modulates social play behavior in the home cage (Veenema et al., 2013).
Materials and methods
Animals
Male and female Wistar rats (23-day-old) were obtained from Charles River (Raleigh, NC) and maintained under standard laboratory conditions (12 h light/dark cycle, lights off at 14:00 h, 22°C, 50% humidity, food and water ad libitum). Rats were housed in same-sex groups of four in standard rat cages (48 × 27 × 20 cm) unless otherwise mentioned. The experiments were conducted in accordance with the guidelines of the NIH and approved by Boston College Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Cannulation
After 1 week of daily handling to familiarize rats with the injection procedure, juvenile (30-31 days of age) males and females were anesthetized with isoflurane (Butler Schein Animal Health, Dublin, OH) and mounted on a stereotaxic frame with the tooth bar set at −4.5 mm. Single guide cannula (22 gauge; Plastics One, Roanoke, VA) were implanted stereotaxically 2 mm dorsal to the medial part of the lateral septum at coordinates 0.4 mm caudal to bregma (corresponding with Figures 16–19 of the adult rat brain atlas of Paxinos and Watson, 1998; see also Figure 1), −1.0 mm lateral to the midline and 3.6 mm ventral to the surface of the skull under an angle of 10° from the midsagittal plane to avoid damage to the sagittal sinus. This injection site was chosen because it is positioned centrally in the region where males and females express high densities of V1aR and OTR binding in the lateral septum (Lukas et al., 2010; Veenema et al., 2012; Figure 1) and because the injection site corresponds with previous studies showing that pharmacological manipulations of AVP and OXT systems in the rat lateral septum altered social behavior (Veenema et al., 2010, 2012, 2013; Lukas et al., 2013), indicating that the drugs are reaching their target receptors. Cannulae were fixed to the skull with two stainless steel screws and dental cement and closed with a dummy cannula (Plastics One, Roanoke, VA).
Familiar and novel environments
After surgery, rats were individually housed in standard rat cages (48 × 27 × 20 cm). Bedding in the home cage was not changed over the course of the experiment (i.e., 5 days). The first set of rats was tested for social play 3 and 5 days after surgery, once in the home cage and once in a novel cage in counterbalanced order, and given the same drug. The home and novel cage were chosen as familiar and novel environments because rats are exposed to both environments when being reared in animal facilities making them realistic environments. The second set of rats was tested for non-social anxiety on the elevated plus-maze, 3 days after surgery. The 3-day recovery period matches that used for other procedures applied routinely in our lab (e.g., intracerebral microdialysis) and was shown not to affect the display of social behavior (Beiderbeck et al., 2007; Veenema et al., 2010, 2012; Lukas et al., 2011, 2013).
Social play test
During the beginning of the dark phase (between 14:00 and 15:00 h) and under red light conditions, experimental rats were exposed to an age- and sex-matched unfamiliar rat for a period of 10 min. All tests were videotaped for subsequent analysis of behavior by a researcher blind to the treatment condition using JWatcher (http://www.jwatcher.ucla.edu/). The following behaviors were scored for the experimental rat according to Veenema et al. (2013): nape attack (the experimental rat displays nose attacks or nose contacts toward the nape of the neck of the stimulus rat), pinning (the experimental rat holds the stimulus rat on its back), supine (the experimental rat lays on its back and is pinned by the stimulus rat), social play (the total amount of time spent in playful social interactions including nape attacks, pinning, and supine poses), social investigation (the experimental rat is sniffing the stimulus rat), non-social exploration, and allo-grooming (the experimental rat is grooming the stimulus rat).
Elevated plus-maze test
The elevated plus-maze is based on the rat's natural conflict between its exploratory drive and its innate fear of elevated, open and novel spaces (Pellow et al., 1985). It consists of two opposing open (50 × 10 cm; 60 lux) and two opposing closed (50 × 10 × 40 cm; 20 lux) arms, which are connected by a common central area (10 × 10 cm). A raised edge (0.5 cm) on the open arms provided additional grip for the rats. The apparatus is made of dark gray plastics and was elevated to a height of 90 cm above the floor. Tests took place in the late light phase (between 10:00 and 12:00 h). Before each trial, the maze was cleaned with water containing a low concentration of detergent. Rats were placed individually in the center square facing a closed arm and were allowed to explore the maze for 5 min. Behavior was measured by means of a video camera mounted above the maze and scored in an adjacent room by a researcher blind to the treatment condition using JWatcher. An open/closed arm entry was defined as both forepaws and shoulders of the rat being on the respective arm of the elevated plus-maze. The percentage of time spent on the open arms [100 × time on open arms/(time on open arms + time in closed arms)] and the percentage of open arm entries [100 × open arm entries/(open + closed arm entries)] were used as parameters of anxiety-like behavior. The number of total (closed and open arm) entries was used as a measure of locomotor activity.
Microinjections
Selective V1aR and OTR antagonists were used to determine the role of the AVP and OXT receptor systems, respectively, in social play behavior. The effects of exogenous AVP and OXT on social play behavior were also determined. In detail, rats received an injection 20 min prior to either the social play test or the elevated plus-maze test. The injection was given over a period of 1 min into the middle part of the lateral septum via an injector cannula that extended 2 mm beyond the guide cannula and was connected via polyethylene tubing to a Hamilton syringe. The injector cannula was kept in place for 30 s following injection to allow for tissue uptake. The injector cannula was then replaced by a dummy cannula. Rats received either 0.5 μl of Ringer's solution (vehicle), the specific V1aR antagonist d(CH2)5[Tyr(Me)2]AVP (10 ng/0.5 μl; V1aR-A; Manning et al., 2008), synthetic AVP (200 pg/0.5 μl; Sigma-Aldrich), the specific OTR antagonist desGly-NH2,d(CH2)5−[Tyr(Me)2,Thr4]OVT (10 ng/0.5 μl; OTR-A; Manning et al., 2008), or synthetic OXT (200 pg/0.5 μl; Sigma-Aldrich). These drug concentrations were shown to be effective in altering social play (Veenema et al., 2013) or other social behaviors (Veenema et al., 2012; Dumais et al., 2013b; Lukas et al., 2013) in rats. At the end of the experiments, rats were killed with CO2, and charcoal was injected as a marker to check proper placement of the cannulae into the lateral septum histologically on Nissl-stained coronal brain sections. The final number of rats for each group is indicated in the first row of graphs in Figures 2–4.
Statistics
Social play behaviors were analyzed using a Three-Way ANOVA (sex × treatment × context). Bonferroni post-hoc tests, Two-Way ANOVAs, or One-Way ANOVAs were used to test for differences among groups. Elevated plus-maze behaviors were analyzed using a Two-Way ANOVA (sex × treatment), followed by Bonferroni post-hoc tests to test for differences among groups. Data are presented as mean ± s.e.m. Significance was set at p < 0.05. In all figures, a significant treatment effect is indicated by the symbol *, a significant sex effect is indicated by the symbol #, and a significant context effect is indicated by the symbol $.
Results
Modulation of social play behavior by AVP
V1aR-A treatment enhanced social play behaviors in males, but reduced these behaviors in females. These effects were context-specific as they appeared in the home cage but not in the novel cage. AVP treatment reduced social play in females, but only in the novel cage. Details on main effects and details on post-hoc and One-Way ANOVA tests are given in Table 1.
Table 1.
Treatment × Sex × Context | Social play | F(2, 104) = 3.96, p < 0.05 |
Allo-grooming | F(2, 104) = 3.46, p < 0.05 | |
Exploration | F(2, 104) = 6.86, p < 0.005 | |
Treatment × Sex | Pins | F(2, 104) = 14.44, p < 0.001* |
Sex | Supine | F(2, 104) = 8.96, p < 0.005 |
Context | Nape attacks | F(1, 104) = 4.18, p < 0.05 |
Subsequent Two-Way ANOVA's were run to examine the effects of treatment and sex independently for the two contexts. This yielded a significant treatment × sex effect in the home cage only [F(2, 53) = 15.3, p < 0.001].
Treatment effects
V1aR-A treatment in the home cage enhanced social play in males (p < 0.001 vs. vehicle-treated and vs. AVP-treated males, Figure 2A) while it reduced social play in females (p < 0.01 vs. vehicle-treated females, Figure 2A). V1aR-A treatment in the home cage enhanced the number of pins in males (p < 0.05 vs. vehicle-treated and p < 0.001 vs. AVP-treated males, Figure 2C) while it reduced the number of pins in females (p < 0.05 vs. vehicle-treated females, Figure 2C). AVP treatment in the novel cage reduced social play in females (p < 0.05 vs. vehicle-treated females, Figure 2A) while in males this was only significant vs. V1aR-A-treated males (p < 0.05, Figure 2A). Treatment effects were also seen for non-social exploration (Table S1), which were likely secondary to the V1aR-A and AVP treatment effects on social play.
Sex effects
Vehicle-treated male and female rats did not show sex differences in social play behaviors (Figure 2, Table S1). V1aR-A-treated females showed less social play (p < 0.001 home cage, p < 0.01 novel cage, Figure 2A) and fewer pins (p < 0.001 home cage, Figure 2C) than V1aR-A-treated males. In contrast, AVP-treated females in the home cage showed more pins (p < 0.05, Figure 2C) than AVP-treated males. Additional sex effects were found for supine (Table 1, allo-grooming (Table S1), and non-social exploration (Table S1).
Context effects
Social play behaviors in vehicle-treated rats did not differ between home and novel cage. However, V1aR-A-treated males showed less social play (p < 0.01, Figure 2A) in novel vs. home cage. Moreover, AVP-treated females showed less social play (p < 0.005, Figure 2A) in novel vs. home cage. Additional context effects were found for nape attacks (Table 1), allo-grooming (Table S1), and non-social exploration (Table S1).
Modulation of social play behavior by OXT
OTR-A treatment did not alter social play behaviors. However, OXT reduced social play, but only in females and only in the home cage. Details on main effects and details on post-hoc and One-Way ANOVA tests are given in Table 2.
Table 2.
Treatment × Context | Social play | F(2, 113) = 4.12, p < 0.05* |
Nape attacks | F(2, 113) = 3.64, p < 0.05* | |
Sex | Social play | F(1, 113) = 25.2, p < 0.001# |
Nape attacks | F(1, 113) = 55.3, p < 0.001# | |
Pins | F(1, 113) = 3.93, p = 0.050# |
Subsequent Two-Way ANOVA's were run to examine the effects of treatment and context separately by sex. This yielded a significant treatment × context effect in females only [social play: F(2, 62) = 5.01, p < 0.05; nape attacks: F(2, 62) = 3.15, p = 0.050].
Subsequent One-Way ANOVA's were run to examine the sex effect separately for treatment and context. This yielded several sex effects (see text for details).
Treatment effects
OTR-A-treated rats did not differ from vehicle-treated rats in any behavior in the social play test (Figure 3, Table S2). In contrast, OXT-treated females in the home cage showed less social play than vehicle-treated females (p < 0.005) and OTR antagonist-treated females (p < 0.01, Figure 3A). No treatment effects were found for males or for other behaviors (Table S2).
Sex effects
Vehicle-treated rats did not show a sex difference in social play behaviors in home or novel cage (Figure 3, Table S2). OTR-A-treated females displayed less social play (p < 0.05 novel cage, Figure 3A) and fewer nape attacks (p < 0.001 home cage, p < 0.001 novel cage, Figure 3B) than OTR-A-treated males. OXT-treated females displayed less social play (p < 0.001 home cage, p < 0.05 novel cage, Figure 3A), fewer nape attacks (p < 0.001 home cage, p < 0.005 novel cage, Figure 3B) and fewer pins (p < 0.05, home cage, Figure 3C) than OXT-treated males. No sex effects were found for other behaviors (Table S2).
Context effects
Social play behaviors in vehicle-treated rats did not differ between home and novel cage. However, OTR-A-treated females showed less social play (p < 0.05) in novel vs. home cage (Figure 3A) while OXT-treated females showed more social play (p < 0.05, Figure 3A) in novel vs. home cage. No context effects were found for males or for other behaviors (Table S2).
Modulation of non-social anxiety-like behavior by AVP
V1aR-A treatment did not alter anxiety-like behaviors on the elevated plus-maze. However, AVP treatment altered anxiety in sex-specific ways as reflected by treatment x sex effects for percentage time on open arms [F(2, 51) = 3.41, p < 0.05] and percentage open arm entries [F(2, 51) = 7.39, p < 0.05]. Post-hoc testing indicates that AVP-treated males spent less time on the open arms (Figure 4A) and had less open arm entries (Figure 4B) than vehicle-treated males (time: p < 0.01; entries: p < 0.01) and V1aR-A-treated males (entries: p < 0.005). Furthermore, AVP-treated males spent less time on the open arms (p < 0.05, Figure 4A) and had less open arm entries (p < 0.005, Figure 4B) than AVP-treated females. No main effects were found for locomotion (total arm entries; Figure 4C).
Modulation of non-social anxiety-like behavior by OXT
No main effects were found for any of the parameters of anxiety-like behaviors or locomotion, indicating that OTR-A and OXT treatments did not alter non-social anxiety-like behavior or locomotion on the elevated plus-maze (Figures 4D–F).
Discussion
We have shown that AVP and OXT modulate social play behavior in juvenile rats in neuropeptide-, sex-, and social context-specific ways. Specifically, V1aR blockade enhanced social play behaviors in males, but reduced these behaviors in females. These effects were found when rats were tested in their home cage, while no such effects were found in a novel cage. Furthermore, social play behaviors in females were reduced by AVP treatment in the novel cage and by OXT treatment in the home cage. These findings indicate that sex and social context are important factors to consider in how AVP and OXT modulate social play behavior and, most likely, other social behaviors as well.
Regulation of social play by AVP and OXT systems in the lateral septum: sex-specific effects
No sex differences were found for home cage social play behaviors in vehicle-treated rats, confirming our previous findings (Veenema et al., 2013) and those of others using a resident-intruder setting (Panksepp and Beatty, 1980; Panksepp, 1981; Thor and Holloway, 1986). In contrast, robust sex differences were found in home cage social play behaviors in response to acute pharmacological manipulations of the AVP and OXT systems in the lateral septum. Males showed more social play behaviors (duration of social play and number of pins) than females after V1aR-A treatment, confirming our previous findings (Veenema et al., 2013). Moreover, females showed fewer nape attacks than males after OTR-A treatment, supporting our previous findings when given OTR-A intracerebroventricularly (Veenema et al., 2013). Finally, females showed less social play than males after OXT treatment. Together, these findings strongly suggest that the neural mechanisms by which AVP and OXT modulate social play differ between males and females. This could be due to sex differences in either AVP/OXT release in the lateral septum, in the types of lateral septum neurons expressing the V1aR and OTR, in neuropeptide-mediated modulation of lateral septum neuronal activity, or in a combination of these. Since male rats have more AVP fibers (De Vries et al., 1981), lower V1aR binding density (Veenema et al., 2012) and higher OTR binding density (Dumais et al., 2013a) in the lateral septum than female rats, these suggestions would be worth testing. Moreover, the lateral septum projects to various brain areas that modulate motivational and behavioral responses including the lateral hypothalamus, nucleus accumbens, and ventral tegmental area (Sheehan et al., 2004). This may indicate that, by acting on the lateral septum, AVP and OXT drive sex-specific neural circuits that modulate social play. While there is a growing interest in identifying the neural circuits underlying social play behavior (Vanderschuren et al., 1997; Trezza et al., 2010; Siviy and Panksepp, 2011), our findings clearly indicate that this requires the inclusion of both sexes in future studies.
Sex-specific responses to AVP and OXT manipulations in rodents have been reported for behaviors other than juvenile social play, including aggression (Ferris and Potegal, 1988; Gutzler et al., 2010), partner preference (Insel and Hulihan, 1995; Cho et al., 1999; Bales et al., 2013), alloparental behavior (Bales et al., 2004), and social recognition (Bluthé and Dantzer, 1990; Engelmann et al., 1998; Veenema et al., 2012). Likewise, sex differences in the effects of AVP and OXT manipulations on behavioral and brain responses have been reported in adult humans (Thompson et al., 2006; Domes et al., 2007, 2010; Lischke et al., 2012; Rilling et al., 2014). These studies were performed on healthy animals and humans, but their outcomes suggest that males and females may show sex-specific susceptibility to alterations in AVP and/or OXT systems. This may be relevant for understanding sex-biases in prevalence, symptom severity, and treatment responses in neuropsychiatric disorders characterized by social dysfunction and in which AVP and OXT systems may play a role (Rutter et al., 2003; Carter, 2007). Additionally, these sex-specific behavioral responses to drugs targeting the AVP and OXT systems signify the importance of taking into consideration the sex of the individual in all studies, but especially in clinical settings when applying drugs targeting the AVP and/or OXT systems to improve social functioning (MacDonald, 2013).
Regulation of social play by AVP and OXT systems in the lateral septum: context-specific effects
Social context by itself did not affect social play behaviors in vehicle-treated males and females. However, social context modulated the effects of AVP and OXT manipulations on social play behavior. First, the effects of V1aR blockade on social play behavior in the home cage were absent in a novel cage. Second, AVP decreased social play behavior in females in the novel but not in the home cage. Third, social play behavior in females was lower in response to OTR blockade in the novel vs. home cage. Last, OXT decreased social play behavior in females in the home but not in the novel cage.
The context-specific behavioral effects of AVP and OXT systems in the lateral septum correspond with other studies reporting that the social context modulates the behavioral responses of neuropeptide systems. For example, central blockade of the V1aR reduces aggression in dominant male violet-eared waxbills toward an unfamiliar male during mate competition aggression but not during resident-intruder aggression (Goodson et al., 2009). OXT induced flank marking when dominant female Syrian hamsters were tested with a familiar, but not with a novel, subordinate partner (Harmon et al., 2002). Moreover, blockade of the vasoactive intestinal polypeptide receptor in the brain of zebra finches alters social contact behavior in a novel, but not familiar, environment (Kingsbury et al., 2013). There are also human studies showing that the behavioral effects of AVP and OXT depend on social contextual factors (Declerck et al., 2010; Bartz et al., 2011; Rilling et al., 2012; Olff et al., 2013). These findings suggest that context-dependent actions of AVP and OXT are a common feature across different species, underscoring the importance of studying their underlying neural mechanisms.
Because of its anxiogenic effects, we predicted that AVP would reduce social play in the novel cage setting. Indeed, AVP-treated females showed less novel cage social play behavior compared to vehicle-treated females. However, AVP had no effect on anxiety-like behavior on the elevated plus-maze in juvenile females. Conversely, AVP failed to alter novel cage social play behavior in juvenile males, yet it had a strong anxiogenic effect on elevated plus-maze behavior, confirming previous findings in adult male rats (Landgraf et al., 1995; Beiderbeck et al., 2007; Veenema et al., 2010). This indicates that AVP alters novel cage social play behavior via mechanisms different from those that alter elevated plus-maze behavior.
Given the well-known role of OXT in facilitating pro-social behaviors (Williams et al., 1992; Feldman et al., 2007; Kojima and Alberts, 2011; Feldman, 2012) and reducing anxiety-like behaviors (Windle et al., 1997; Ring et al., 2006; Blume et al., 2008), we expected to find a facilitating effect of OXT on social play, especially in the novel cage. In contrast, OXT treatment did not alter social play behavior in the novel cage, but decreased female social play behaviors in the home cage. Interestingly, some rodent studies provide evidence suggesting that OXT in the lateral septum may reduce pro-social behaviors and enhance anxiety. For example, OTR binding density in the lateral septum correlated negatively with huddling behavior in female meadow voles (Beery and Zucker, 2010) and with alloparental behavior in female prairie voles (Olazábal and Young, 2006), and was lower in group-living as opposed to solitary tuco-tucos (Beery et al., 2008). This may indicate that low rather than high OTR binding in the lateral septum is important for social affiliation.
Moreover, OTR activation in the lateral septum enhanced conditioned fear responses in mice exposed to acute social defeat (Guzman et al., 2013). Likewise, exogenous OXT can have anxiogenic effects in humans exposed to social stimuli (Bartz et al., 2010; Striepens et al., 2012; Grillon et al., 2013; MacDonald et al., 2013). These findings suggests that OXT has a modulatory role in responding to the salience and/or valence of social stimuli, likely facilitating affiliative responses to positive social stimuli and anxiogenic responses to negative social stimuli. Indeed, several of the above-mentioned studies have in common that OXT potentiated fear in response to negative social stimuli. No such negative stimuli were present in our study when female juvenile rats were exposed in their home cage to an unfamiliar juvenile rat. However, OTR blockade in the lateral septum (this study) and intracerebroventricularly (Veenema et al., 2013) failed to alter social play behavior in either sex, suggesting that there is no increase in endogenous OXT release in the brain during social play behavior. Hence, exogenous OXT may have induced an artificial situation resulting in a change in the valence of the social play partner, i.e., from a positive one to a more negative one. Future studies will need to test this hypothesis and explore its underlying neural circuitry. Clearly, our results add to an emerging literature revealing the complexity of OXT effects on social behavior and signifying that OXT, at least acting on the lateral septum, can reduce pro-social behaviors, such as social play.
Social play is a highly motivational behavior (Trezza et al., 2010) and the lateral septum plays a key role in modulating context-specific motivational states (Sheehan et al., 2004; Luo et al., 2011; Sartor and Aston-Jones, 2012). Hence, the context-specific effects of V1aR antagonist, AVP, OTR antagonist, and OXT treatments on social play could have been mediated by changes in the activation of motivational systems in the brain. A possible circuit could comprise the hippocampus transmitting contextual information onto the lateral septum which, in turn, relays this information onto areas such as the lateral hypothalamus and the ventral tegmental area to modulate behavioral responses appropriate to particular environmental stimuli (Sheehan et al., 2004). In this scenario, social context-dependent input from the hippocampus to the lateral septum may have modulated the ability of AVP and OXT systems to alter lateral septum activity resulting in context-specific effects of V1aR antagonist, AVP, OTR antagonist, and OXT treatments on social play behavior. This should be tested in future studies.
Despite the context-specific effects, neither administration of AVP nor administration of OTR-A altered social play behavior in males and females tested in the home cage. We cannot exclude that higher drug doses would be effective in altering social play behavior in the home cage. However, the same dose of AVP altered novel cage social play behavior in females, anxiety-related behavior in males, and social recognition in both male and female juvenile rats (Veenema et al., 2012). Therefore, it may be that endogenous AVP already influences social play behavior at maximum levels. Likewise, the same dose of OTR-A impaired social recognition when injected into the lateral septum (Lukas et al., 2013) or in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (Dumais et al., 2013b), an area that shows denser OTR binding than the lateral septum (Dumais et al., 2013a). Determining the release pattern of OXT and AVP during social play behavior may further help define the involvement of these neuropeptides in the sex- and context-specific modulation of social play behavior. Additionally, the neuropeptide- and receptor antagonist-specific effects on social play suggests that it is less likely that AVP and OXT may have acted via each other's receptors (Chini and Manning, 2007; Schorscher-Petcu et al., 2010). Nevertheless, future studies administering receptor antagonists prior to the neuropeptide would be required to further confirm that the effects of AVP and OXT on social play are mediated via their “own” receptor.
In conclusion, we have shown that AVP and OXT systems in the lateral septum regulate social play behavior in juvenile rats. This regulation was found to be (1) neuropeptide-specific, indicating that AVP and OXT in the lateral septum uniquely modulate neural circuits involved in social play, (2) sex-specific, suggesting that such neural circuits differ between males and females, and (3) social context-dependent, demonstrating that generalizations cannot be made about the roles of AVP and OXT across social contexts. These findings may be informative to humans, such that AVP- and/or OXT-based treatments of social deficits may have sex- and social context-dependent effects.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We thank Dr. Maurice Manning (Toledo, OH) for providing the V1aR and OTR antagonists, Danielle Scaramella and Christine Wu for technical assistance, the animal caretakers at Boston College for excellent animal care, and Veenema lab members for critically reading the manuscript. This research was supported by the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation Grant 17382 and NSF Grant IOS 1253386 to Alexa H. Veenema.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://www.frontiersin.org/journal/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00216/abstract
References
- Alessandri S. M. (1992). Attention, play, and social behavior in ADHD preschoolers. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 20, 289–302 10.1007/BF00916693 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bales K. L., Perkeybile A. M., Conley O. G., Lee M. H., Guoynes C. D., Downing G. M., et al. (2013). Chronic intranasal oxytocin causes long-term impairments in partner preference formation in male prairie voles. Biol. Psychiatry 74, 180–188 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.08.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bales K. L., Pfeifer L. A., Carter C. S. (2004). Sex differences and developmental effects of manipulations of oxytocin on alloparenting and anxiety in prairie voles. Dev. Psychobiol. 44, 123–131 10.1002/dev.10165 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartz J. A., Zaki J., Bolger N., Ochsner K. N. (2011). Social effects of oxytocin in humans: context and person matter. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 301–309 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartz J. A., Zaki J., Ochsner K. N., Bolger N., Kolevzon A., Ludwig N., et al. (2010). Effects of oxytocin on recollections of maternal care and closeness. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 21371–21375 10.1073/pnas.1012669107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beery A. K., Lacey E. A., Francis D. D. (2008). Oxytocin and vasopressin receptor distributions in a solitary and a social species of tuco-tuco (Ctenomys haigi and Ctenomys sociabilis). J. Comp. Neurol. 507, 1847–1859 10.1002/cne.21638 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beery A. K., Zucker I. (2010). Oxytocin and same-sex social behavior in female meadow voles. Neuroscience 169, 665–673 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.05.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beiderbeck D. I., Neumann I. D., Veenema A. H. (2007). Differences in intermale aggression are accompanied by opposite vasopressin release patterns within the septum in rats bred for low and high anxiety. Eur. J. Neurosci. 26, 3597–3605 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05974.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bekoff M., Byers J. A. (1998). Animal Play: Evolutionary, Comparative, and Ecological Perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press [Google Scholar]
- Blume A., Bosch O. J., Miklos S., Torner L., Wales L., Waldherr M., et al. (2008). Oxytocin reduces anxiety via ERK1/2 activation: local effect within the rat hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus. Eur. J. Neurosci. 27, 1947–1956 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06184.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bluthé R. M., Dantzer R. (1990). Social recognition does not involve vasopressinergic neurotransmission in female rats. Brain Res. 535, 301–304 10.1016/0006-8993(90)91613-L [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burghardt G. M. (2005). The Genesis of Animal Play: Testing the Limits. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press [Google Scholar]
- Carter C. S. (2007). Sex differences in oxytocin and vasopressin: implications for autism spectrum disorders? Behav. Brain Res. 176, 170–186 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.08.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chini B., Manning M. (2007). Agonist selectivity in the oxytocin/vasopressin receptor family: new insights and challenges. Biochem. Soc. Trans. 35, 737–741 10.1042/BST0350737 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cho M. M., DeVries A. C., Williams J. R., Carter C. S. (1999). The effects of oxytocin and vasopressin on partner preferences in male and female prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Behav. Neurosci. 113, 1071–1079 10.1037/0735-7044.113.5.1071 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cordoni G., Palagi E. (2011). Ontogenetic trajectories of chimpanzee social play: similarities with humans. PLoS ONE 6:e27344 10.1371/journal.pone.0027344 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Declerck C. H., Boone C., Kiyonari T. (2010). Oxytocin and cooperation under conditions of uncertainty: the modulating role of incentives and social information. Horm. Behav. 57, 368–374 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.01.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Vries G. J., Buijs R. M., Swaab D. F. (1981). Ontogeny of the vasopressinergic neurons of the suprachiasmatic nucleus and their extrahypothalamic projections in the rat brain—presence of a sex difference in the lateral septum. Brain Res. 218, 67–78 10.1016/0006-8993(81)90989-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Vries G. J., Panzica G. C. (2006). Sexual differentiation of central vasopressin and vasotocin systems in vertebrates: different mechanisms, similar endpoints. Neuroscience 138, 947–955 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.07.050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Domes G., Heinrichs M., Glascher J., Buchel C., Braus D. F., Herpertz S. C. (2007). Oxytocin attenuates amygdala responses to emotional faces regardless of valence. Biol. Psychiatry 62, 1187–1190 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.03.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Domes G., Lischke A., Berger C., Grossmann A., Hauenstein K., Heinrichs M., et al. (2010). Effects of intranasal oxytocin on emotional face processing in women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 83–93 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.06.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donaldson Z. R., Young L. J. (2008). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of sociality. Science 322, 900–904 10.1126/science.1158668 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dumais K. M., Alonso A. G., Mayer T. E., Bredewold R., Veenema A. H. (2013b). Sex differences in oxytocin receptor binding in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and amygdala: Implications for differential expression and regulation of social behavior. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 378.02/III42. [Google Scholar]
- Dumais K. M., Bredewold R., Mayer T. E., Veenema A. H. (2013a). Sex differences in oxytocin receptor binding in forebrain regions: correlations with social interest in brain region- and sex-specific ways. Horm. Behav. 64, 693–701 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.08.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engelmann M., Ebner K., Wotjak C. T., Landgraf R. (1998). Endogenous oxytocin is involved in short-term olfactory memory in female rats. Behav. Brain Res. 90, 89–94 10.1016/S0166-4328(97)00084-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman R. (2012). Oxytocin and social affiliation in humans. Horm. Behav. 61, 380–391 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman R., Weller A., Zagoory-Sharon O., Levine A. (2007). Evidence for a neuroendocrinological foundation of human affiliation: plasma oxytocin levels across pregnancy and the postpartum period predict mother-infant bonding. Psychol. Sci. 18, 965–970 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02010.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferris C. F., Potegal M. (1988). Vasopressin receptor blockade in the anterior hypothalamus suppresses aggression in hamsters. Physiol. Behav. 44, 235–239 10.1016/0031-9384(88)90144-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodson J. L., Kabelik D., Schrock S. E. (2009). Dynamic neuromodulation of aggression by vasotocin: influence of social context and social phenotype in territorial songbirds. Biol. Lett. 5, 554–556 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0316 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodson J. L., Thompson R. R. (2010). Nonapeptide mechanisms of social cognition, behavior and species-specific social systems. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 784–794 10.1016/j.conb.2010.08.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grillon C., Krimsky M., Charney D. R., Vytal K., Ernst M., Cornwell B. (2013). Oxytocin increases anxiety to unpredictable threat. Mol. Psychiatry, 18, 958–960 10.1038/mp.2012.156 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guralnick M. J., Connor R. T., Neville B., Hammond M. A. (2006). Promoting the peer-related social development of young children with mild developmental delays: effectiveness of a comprehensive intervention. Am. J. Ment. Retard. 111, 336–356 10.1352/0895-8017(2006)111[336:PTPSDO]2.0.CO;2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gutzler S. J., Karom M., Erwin W. D., Albers H. E. (2010). Arginine-vasopressin and the regulation of aggression in female Syrian hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus). Eur. J. Neurosci. 31, 1655–1663 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07190.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guzman Y. F., Tronson N. C., Jovasevic V., Sato K., Guedea A. L., Mizukami H., et al. (2013). Fear-enhancing effects of septal oxytocin receptors. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1185–1187 10.1038/nn.3465 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harmon A. C., Moore T. O., Huhman K. L., Albers H. E. (2002). Social experience and social context alter the behavioral response to centrally administered oxytocin in female Syrian hamsters. Neuroscience 109, 767–772 10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00523-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insel T. R., Hulihan T. J. (1995). A gender-specific mechanism for pair bonding: oxytocin and partner preference formation in monogamous voles. Behav. Neurosci. 109, 782–789 10.1037/0735-7044.109.4.782 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jordan R. (2003). Social play and autistic spectrum disorders: a perspective on theory, implications and educational approaches. Autism 7, 347–360 10.1177/1362361303007004002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kingsbury M. A., Miller K. M., Goodson J. L. (2013). VPAC receptor signaling modulates grouping behavior and social responses to contextual novelty in a gregarious finch: a role for a putative prefrontal cortex homologue. Horm. Behav. 64, 511–518 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.07.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kojima S., Alberts J. R. (2011). Oxytocin mediates the acquisition of filial, odor-guided huddling for maternally-associated odor in preweanling rats. Horm. Behav. 60, 549–558 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.08.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landgraf R., Gerstberger R., Montkowski A., Probst J. C., Wotjak C. T., Holsboer F., et al. (1995). V1 vasopressin receptor antisense oligodeoxynucleotide into septum reduces vasopressin binding, social discrimination abilities, and anxiety-related behavior in rats. J. Neurosci. 15, 4250–4258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lischke A., Gamer M., Berger C., Grossmann A., Hauenstein K., Heinrichs M., et al. (2012). Oxytocin increases amygdala reactivity to threatening scenes in females. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 1431–1438 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.01.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukas M., Bredewold R., Landgraf R., Neumann I. D., Veenema A. H. (2011). Early life stress impairs social recognition due to a blunted response of vasopressin release within the septum of adult male rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 843–853 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.11.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukas M., Bredewold R., Neumann I. D., Veenema A. H. (2010). Maternal separation interferes with developmental changes in brain vasopressin and oxytocin receptor binding in male rats. Neuropharmacology 58, 78–87 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2009.06.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukas M., Toth I., Veenema A. H., Neumann I. D. (2013). Oxytocin mediates rodent social memory within the lateral septum and the medial amygdala depending on the relevance of the social stimulus: male juvenile versus female adult conspecifics. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 916–926 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.09.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luo A. H., Tahsili-Fahadan P., Wise R. A., Lupica C. R., Aston-Jones G. (2011). Linking context with reward: a functional circuit from hippocampal CA3 to ventral tegmental area. Science 333, 353–357 10.1126/science.1204622 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacDonald K. S. (2013). Sex, receptors, and attachment: a review of individual factors influencing response to oxytocin. Front. Neurosci. 6:194 10.3389/fnins.2012.00194 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacDonald K., MacDonald T. M., Brüne M., Lamb K., Wilson M. P., Golshan S., et al. (2013). Oxytocin and psychotherapy: a pilot study of its physiological, behavioral and subjective effects in males with depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 2831–2843 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.05.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manning M., Stoev S., Chini B., Durroux T., Mouillac B., Guillon G. (2008). Peptide and non-peptide agonists and antagonists for the vasopressin and oxytocin V1a, V1b, V2 and OT receptors: research tools and potential therapeutic agents. Prog. Brain Res. 170, 473–512 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)00437-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer-Lindenberg A., Domes G., Kirsch P., Heinrichs M. (2011). Oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain: social neuropeptides for translational medicine. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 524–538 10.1038/nrn3044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Modi M. E., Young L. J. (2012). The oxytocin system in drug discovery for autism: animal models and novel therapeutic strategies. Horm. Behav. 61, 340–350 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moller P., Husby R. (2000). The initial prodrome in schizophrenia: searching for naturalistic core dimensions of experience and behavior. Schizophr. Bull. 26, 217–232 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033442 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olazábal D. E., Young L. J. (2006). Species and individual differences in juvenile female alloparental care are associated with oxytocin receptor density in the striatum and the lateral septum. Horm. Behav. 49, 681–687 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.12.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olff M., Frijling J. L., Kubzansky L. D., Bradley B., Ellenbogen M. A., Cardoso C., et al. (2013). The role of oxytocin in social bonding, stress regulation and mental health: an update on the moderating effects of context and interindividual differences. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 1883–1894 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.06.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panksepp J. (1981). The ontogeny of play in rats. Dev. Psychobiol. 14, 327–332 10.1002/dev.420140405 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panksepp J., Beatty W. W. (1980). Social deprivation and play in rats. Behav. Neural Biol. 30, 197–206 10.1016/S0163-1047(80)91077-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paul M. J., Terranova J. I., Probst C. K., Murray E. K., Ismail N. I., de Vries G. J. (2014). Sexually dimorphic role for vasopressin in the development of social play. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:58 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paxinos G., Watson C. (1998). The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. San Diego, CA: Academic Press [Google Scholar]
- Pellis S. M., Iwaniuk A. N. (2000). Comparative analyses of the role of postnatal development on the expression of play fighting. Dev. Psychobiol. 36, 136–147 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(200003)36:2%3C136::AID-DEV5%3E3.3.CO;2-M [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pellow S., Chopin P., File S. E., Briley M. (1985). Validation of open:closed arm entries in an elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat. J. Neurosci. Methods 14, 149–167 10.1016/0165-0270(85)90031-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rilling J. K., Demarco A. C., Hackett P. D., Chen X., Gautam P., Stair S., et al. (2014). Sex differences in the neural and behavioral response to intranasal oxytocin and vasopressin during human social interaction. Psychoneuroendocrinology 39, 237–248 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.09.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rilling J. K., DeMarco A. C., Hackett P. D., Thompson R., Ditzen B., Patel R., et al. (2012). Effects of intranasal oxytocin and vasopressin on cooperative behavior and associated brain activity in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 447–461 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.07.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ring R. H., Malberg J. E., Potestio L., Ping J., Boikess S., Luo B., et al. (2006). Anxiolytic-like activity of oxytocin in male mice: behavioral and autonomic evidence, therapeutic implications. Psychopharmacology 185, 218–225 10.1007/s00213-005-0293-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M., Caspi A., Moffitt T. E. (2003). Using sex differences in psychopathology to study causal mechanisms: unifying issues and research strategies. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 44, 1092–1115 10.1111/1469-7610.00194 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sartor G. C., Aston-Jones G. S. (2012). A septal-hypothalamic pathway drives orexin neurons, which is necessary for conditioned cocaine preference. J. Neurosci. 32, 4623–4631 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4561-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schorscher-Petcu A., Sotocinal S., Ciura S., Dupré A., Ritchie J., Sorge R. E., et al. (2010). Oxytocin-induced analgesia and scratching are mediated by the vasopressin-1A receptor in the mouse. J. Neurosci. 30, 8274–8284 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1594-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheehan T. P., Chambers R. A., Russell D. S. (2004). Regulation of affect by the lateral septum: implications for neuropsychiatry. Brain Res. Rev. 46, 71–117 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2004.04.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sigman M., Ruskin E. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down syndrome, and developmental delays. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child. Dev. 64, 1–114 10.1111/1540-5834.00002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siviy S. M., Panksepp J. (2011). In search of the neurobiological substrates for social playfulness in mammalian brains. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1821–1830 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.03.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Striepens N., Scheele D., Kendrick K. M., Becker B., Schäfer L., Schwalba K., et al. (2012). Oxytocin facilitates protective responses to aversive social stimuli in males. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 18144–18149 10.1073/pnas.1208852109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szot P., Dorsa D. M. (1993). Differential timing and sexual dimorphism in the expression of the vasopressin gene in the developing rat brain. Dev. Brain Res. 73, 177–183 10.1016/0165-3806(93)90136-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor P. V., Veenema A. H., Paul M. J., Bredewold R., Isaacs S., de Vries G. J. (2012). Sexually dimorphic effects of a prenatal immune challenge on social play and vasopressin expression in juvenile rats. Biol. Sex Differ. 3:15 10.1186/2042-6410-3-15 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson R. R., George K., Walton J. C., Orr S. P., Benson J. (2006). Sex-specific influences of vasopressin on human social communication. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 7889–7894 10.1073/pnas.0600406103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thor D. H., Holloway W. R., Jr. (1986). Social play soliciting by male and female juvenile rats: effects of neonatal androgenization and sex of cagemates. Behav. Neurosci. 100, 275–279 10.1037/0735-7044.100.2.275 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trezza V., Baarendse P. J., Vanderschuren L. J. (2010). The pleasures of play: pharmacological insights into social reward mechanisms. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 31, 463–469 10.1016/j.tips.2010.06.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vanderschuren L. J., Niesink R. J., Van Ree J. M. (1997). The neurobiology of social play behavior in rats. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 21, 309–326 10.1016/S0149-7634(96)00020-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Leeuwen F. W., Caffe A. R., De Vries G. J. (1985). Vasopressin cells in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis of the rat: sex differences and the influence of androgens. Brain Res. 325, 391–394 10.1016/0006-8993(85)90348-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veenema A. H., Beiderbeck D. I., Lukas M., Neumann I. D. (2010). Distinct correlations of vasopressin release within the lateral septum and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis with the display of intermale aggression. Horm. Behav. 58, 273–281 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.03.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veenema A. H., Bredewold R., De Vries G. J. (2012). Vasopressin regulates social recognition in juvenile and adult rats of both sexes, but in sex- and age-specific ways. Horm. Behav. 61, 50–56 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.10.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veenema A. H., Bredewold R., De Vries G. J. (2013). Sex-specific modulation of juvenile social play by vasopressin. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 2554–2561 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.06.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veenema A. H., Neumann I. D. (2008). Central vasopressin and oxytocin release: regulation of complex social behaviours. Prog. Brain Res. 170, 261–276 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)00422-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veenema A. H., Neumann I. D. (2009). Maternal separation enhances offensive play-fighting, basal corticosterone and hypothalamic vasopressin mRNA expression in juvenile male rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 463–467 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams J. R., Carter C. S., Insel T. (1992). Partner preference development in female prairie voles is facilitated by mating or the central infusion of oxytocin. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 652, 487–489 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb34393.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Windle R. J., Shanks N., Lightman S. L., Ingram C. D. (1997). Central oxytocin administration reduces stress-induced corticosterone release and anxiety behavior in rats. Endocrinology 138, 2829–2834 10.1210/en.138.7.2829 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.