Abstract
From behavioral studies it is known that auditory spatial resolution of azimuthal space declines over age. To date, it is not clear how age affects the respective sensory auditory processing at the pre-attentive level. Here we tested the hypothesis that pre-attentive processing of behaviorally perceptible spatial changes is preserved in older adults. An EEG-study was performed in older adults (65–82 years of age) and a mismatch negativity (MMN) paradigm employed. Sequences of frequent standard stimuli of defined azimuthal positions were presented together with rarely occurring deviants shifted by 10° or 20° to the left or to the right of the standard. Standard positions were at +5° (central condition) from the midsagittal plane and at 65° in both lateral hemifields (±65°; lateral condition). The results suggest an effect of laterality on the pre-attentive change processing of spatial deviations in older adults: While for the central conditions deviants close to MAA threshold (i.e., 10°) yielded discernable MMNs, for lateral positions the respective MMN responses were only elicited by spatial deviations of 20° toward the midline (i.e., ±45°). Furthermore, MMN amplitudes were found to be insensitive to the magnitude of deviation (10°, 20°), which is contrary to recent studies with young adults (Bennemann et al., 2013) and hints to a deteriorated pre-attentive encoding of sound sources in older adults. The discrepancy between behavioral MAA data and present results are discussed with respect to the possibility that under the condition of active stimulus processing older adults might benefit from recruiting additional attentional top-down processes to detect small magnitudes of spatial deviations even within the lateral acoustic field.
Keywords: minimum audible angle, mismatch negativity, age-related, temporal processing, change detection
Introduction
In natural environments we are often confronted with a multitude of sound sources typically producing overlapping sound mixtures. Still, the auditory system is able to single out coherent acoustic objects. Through auditory processing, subjects are able to identify and focus on individual sound sources when confronted with complex acoustic scenarios, a process termed auditory scene analysis (ASA) (Bregman, 1990; Shamma and Micheyl, 2010). While the mechanisms underlying ASA are not fully understood, previous studies suggested pitch, timbre and location of sound stimuli as the most prevalent cues in ASA processing (e.g., Shamma and Micheyl, 2010). Localization of sound sources in the horizontal plane are based on the processing of interaural time and level differences (ITD; ILD) and of monaural spectral cues the latter being the result of signal filtering by the upper body, head, and pinnae (Middlebrooks and Green, 1991; Blauert, 1997). Such calculation of positional information by the central auditory system is necessary, since the respective information is not directly encoded on the cochlear basilar membrane. At the level of cortical auditory areas, the information on horizontal sound source positions is thought to be based on a population rate code (Makous and Middlebrooks, 1990; Stecker and Middlebrooks, 2003; Werner-Reiss and Groh, 2008; Salminen et al., 2012). Binaurally activated neuron populations in both cortical hemispheres are assumed to code for positions in either hemifield by their relative level of activation (hemispheric channel model, e.g., Magezi and Krumbholz, 2010, for review see McAlpine, 2005).
Many studies have shown that older adults are less accurate in localizing sound sources compared to young adults (location identification tasks: Abel and Hay, 1996; Abel et al., 2000; detection tasks: Cranford et al., 1993; pointing tasks: Dobreva et al., 2011; Neher et al., 2011; Freigang et al., 2014, review: Eddins and Hall, 2010), plus, there are a number of reports on a strong age-related decline in spatial discrimination as indicated by elevated Minimum Audible Angle (MAA) thresholds (Häusler et al., 1983; Chandler and Grantham, 1991; Freigang et al., 2014). The MAA is used as the measure of the smallest angular distance between two neighboring sound sources that can be detected correctly (Mills, 1958). It has been proposed that reduced sensitivity to location cues is due to both reduced peripheral hearing (Corso, 1971; Häusler et al., 1983; Abel and Hay, 1996;Cruickshanks et al., 1998) and impaired central auditory processing (CHABA, 1988; Chandler and Grantham, 1991; Humes, 1996; Noble et al., 1997; Dobreva et al., 2011; Neher et al., 2011). Others pointed to a possible contribution of age related changes in cognitive processes. For example, Bertoli et al. (2002) and Alain et al. (2004) reported that focusing attention on a gap detection task may help aged subjects to partly overcome degraded sensory processing. To date, it is not clear to what degree each of these factors contribute to the age-related decline in localization performance, since no data is available specifically focusing on physiological processing of auditory space information.
A sensitive tool to examine pre-attentive sensory processing at the level of the auditory cortex is the evaluation of auditory event-related potential (ERP) Mismatch Negativity (MMN, review: Näätänen et al., 1978). It is hypothesized that the MMN serves as an automatic process to alert the system to deviations in the unattended acoustic environment (Sams et al., 1985; Schröger, 1998; Winkler and Czigler, 1998). MMN has been used in many studies to assess the resolution of acoustic feature processing (e.g., frequency, duration, gap detection) including spatial acuity (Deouell et al., 2006; Pakarinen et al., 2007; Vaitulevich and Shestopalova, 2010; Bennemann et al., 2013). Deouell et al. (2006) reported for young adults MMNs elicitation for spatial signal separation of 10 degrees within the frontal acoustic field. For the mid-lateral (65°) and far-lateral (95°) positions, MMNs were elicited for spatial separation of 5° and 15°, respectively (Bennemann et al., 2013). These MMN data are consistent with behaviorally assessed human localization abilities (Blauert, 1997). Cortical generators for MMN were found bilaterally in the primary and secondary auditory cortices in the superior temporal gyri of the temporal lobes as well as in frontal, parietal, and supratemporal cortical sites (Giard et al., 1995; Kropotov et al., 1995; Picton et al., 2000). The MMNs are thought to have different generators activated in a feature-specific fashion by frequency, duration, or location cues (Paavilainen et al., 1991; Alho, 1995; Picton et al., 2000; Deouell et al., 2006). Relating to the present study, MMN has been reliably elicited in experiments probing location differences by either varying (i) ITDs under headphone conditions (Schröger and Wolff, 1996; Schröger, 1996; Pakarinen et al., 2007), (ii) real spatial disparities under free field conditions (Paavilainen et al., 1989; Nager et al., 2003; Tata and Ward, 2005; Deouell et al., 2006; Richter et al., 2009; Grimm et al., 2012; Bennemann et al., 2013) and (iii) using headphone stimulation but employing head-related transfer functions conditions (Sonnadara et al., 2006). Such MMN components are often followed by the ERP P3a (occurring 200–350 ms post stimulus onset), which is thought to indicate an involuntary switch in attention toward the deviant sound (Picton et al., 2000).
Previous studies showed that the MMN amplitude is reduced in elderly subjects, possibly due to an age-dependent decline in pre-attentive automatic central auditory processing (frequency: Czigler et al., 1992; Schroeder et al., 1995; Alain and Woods, 1999; duration: Woods, 1992; Karayanidis et al., 1995; Pekkonen et al., 1996; Bertoli et al., 2002; Ruzzoli et al., 2012; gap detection: Alain et al., 2004). However, the underlying mechanisms—mostly considered in relation to frequency detection—are still controversially discussed. On the one hand, the findings were interpreted as impairment in maintenance of the sensory memory trace in older adults. On the other hand, the reduced MMNs were considered as an indication for an impairment of the encoding of sensory information (Czigler et al., 1992; Gunter et al., 1996; Pekkonen, 2000; Cooper et al., 2006). Moreover, there are studies (on frequency discrimination) that did not report any age effect at all (Schroeder et al., 1995; Amenedo and Diaz, 1998).
The present study focuses on the question, whether the age-related localization acuity acquired behaviorally from the MAA (Freigang et al., 2014) is already reflected at a pre-attentive level indicated by the MMN, i.e., at an early level of cortical auditory processing and mostly independent of attentional top-down modulations. For this, previously reported (attentive) behavioral MAA thresholds were used as a basis to perform an MMN experiment using a passive (unattended) stimulation condition. Deviants with two spatial disparities were used: 10°, and 20° i.e., on the one hand near to and on the other hand above pericentral MAA thresholds. Furthermore, in addition to the pericentral (5°) also the lateral (65°) acoustic field was explored for deviants shifting both toward the midline and toward the sides. This specific stimulus design enables the evaluation of the acuity in pre-attentive cortical representation of auditory spatial information considering both, stimulus laterality (pericentral vs. lateral) and the direction of spatial change (toward the midline vs. toward the sides; Richter et al., 2009; Bennemann et al., 2013).
If in older adults behaviorally manifested localization acuity (MAA) corresponds to the automatic, pre-attentive cortical encoding (MMN), the latter should yield responses for near- and above-threshold spatial deviations, in particular within the central acoustic field. If, however, the pre-attentive cortical representation of acoustic space is blurred in older adults, then (i) no or only above-threshold deviations are expected to elicit MMN responses and/or (ii) no magnitude effect with respect to the size of the spatial deviations should be found.
Materials and methods
Subjects
Fifteen older adults (65–82 years-of-age [66.8 ± 4.74 years], 7 women) participated in the MMN experiment. Subjects signed an informed consent form and received a compensation for expenses. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Leipzig and is in agreement with the revised Declaration of Helsinki. All subjects performed the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971), were screened for cognitive deficits with the Mini-Mental State Examination [MMSE] (Folstein et al., 1975), and underwent audiometric testing. All subjects scored 27–30 points in the MMSE identifying them as non-conspicuous. The subjects were also screened for hearing loss and only subjects were included, whose pure-tone thresholds at 500 Hz, 1 kHz, and 2 kHz were on average ≤25 dB HL (hearing level). Data from two subjects had to be excluded from the EEG analysis because of multiple movements-induced disruptions of the recordings.
Experimental setup and EEG recordings
Audiometric testing was conducted in an anechoic, sound attenuated test booth (Industrial Acoustics Company, IAC Type 403 A, Niederkrüchten, Germany). Pure-tone thresholds were examined via headphones (Beyerdynamics, DT 770 Pro). Sounds were generated with a sampling rate of 25 kHz by the real-time processor RP2.1 [Tucker Davis Technologies (TDT), System III], and transmitted to headphones via a headphone power amplifier (TDT, HB7). Stimulus generation and hearing threshold acquisition were controlled by MATLAB (version 6.3, The MathWorks Inc., Natick, USA) (Biedermann et al., 2008).
The MMN experiment were conducted in an anechoic, sound attenuated free-field laboratory (45 m2, IAC, Figure 1). Thirty-three broad-band loudspeakers (Visaton, FRS8 4 Ohm, Haan, Germany) mounted in an azimuthal, semicircular array at ear level were used for sound stimulation. A comfortable, fixed chair was positioned in the middle of the semicircle at a distance of 2.35 m from the loudspeakers, such that subjects were aligned straight ahead to the central speaker at 0°. The speaker array covered an azimuthal plane from 85° to the left to 85° to the right (−85°; +85°). The angular distance between two speaker membranes was 4.3° as measured between the centers of the speaker membranes. In the experiments, a minimal distance between two sound sources of 2.1° was achieved by crossfading the signals of two neighboring speakers. That is, two speakers were simultaneously active and the in-between speaker position was generated by varying the relative sound levels of each speaker. Speakers were calibrated individually (for details on the calibration procedure see Schmiedchen et al., 2012). Speakers were hidden behind acoustically transparent gauze, so the participants were unable to make use of visual landmarks during the experiments. The passive MMN experiment was conducted at a low light level. A movie was played from a screen positioned in front at 0° of the subject slightly below the speaker membrane. During the experiment the participants were monitored by an infrared camera. MATLAB (version R2007b) was used to control stimulus presentation and data acquisition. Acoustic stimuli were digitally generated at a sampling rate of 25 kHz using RPvdsEx (Real Time processor visual design studio, TDT) and delivered to two multi-channel signal processors (RX8, TDT System3).
The EEG was recorded with a 64-channel Ag/AgCl active electrode system (actiCAP, Brain Products) according to the international 10-10 system (American Electroencephalographic Society, 1994). Fifty-eight channels were used to record neuronal activity from the scalp. Four electrodes used to record vertical and horizontal electrooculograms (EOG), so subsequently epochs with massive eye movements could be disregarded in the analysis. Two additional electrodes were used to pick up signals at the left and right mastoid sites. The reference electrode was placed on the tip of the nose and the ground electrode at position Fpz. Impedances were kept below 10 KOhm and recorded signals were online sampled with 500 Hz and band-pass filtered between 0.1 and 100 Hz.
Acoustic stimuli
Stimuli were low-pass filtered Gaussian noise bursts (300–1200 Hz), 250 ms in duration with 10 ms cos2 ramps (Richter et al., 2009; Bennemann et al., 2013). The interstimulus interval (ISI) varied randomly between 350 and 450 ms in increments of 10 ms (average ISI = 400 ms). A level roving of ±2.5 dB was applied in randomized 1 dB steps to prevent MMN-effects by loudness cues due to minute differences in the position or orientation of the loudspeakers.
Experimental procedure and data acquisition
Measurement of individual hearing thresholds
Prior to recording EEG, the subjects' individual hearing thresholds were acquired using a yes-no detection criterion combined with a simple staircase paradigm. For this, the low-pass noise signals were presented from the frontal position at 0° (see Figure 1) with an initial intensity of 63 dB SPL. Subjects were instructed to press the left button on a response box to indicate when they detected a sound (yes-response) and the right button when they did not detect the sound (no-response). Intensity was decreased by 2.5 dB for each yes-response and increased by 2.5 dB for each no-response. The stimulus level at the fourth no/yes response switch was taken as the detection threshold. In the EEG experiment the acoustic stimuli were presented at 40 dB SL.
EEG experiment
A passive oddball paradigm was used. Participants were watching a silent, subtitled movie and were instructed to entirely direct their attention on the movie and to ignore the sounds. The recordings were organized in three main and twelve control blocks. Each main block consisted of 1600 standards and 400 deviants with the standards presented from 65° in the left hemifield (−65°), from 5° to the right of the median plane (+5°), and from 65° in the right hemifield (+65°); standard probability was 0.8 (Figure 1). Deviants were presented each with a probability of 0.05. During the recordings a multiple deviant paradigm was applied (Pettigrew et al., 2004; Deouell et al., 2006; Sambeth et al., 2009; Bennemann et al., 2013). Deviants were shifted by 10° or 20° away from the respective standard positions, either toward the midline or toward the side, i.e., for the +5° central condition the deviants were at −15°, −5°, +15°, +25° and for the ±65° standard positions the deviants were at ±85°, ±75°, ±55°, ±45°, respectively. A sparse presentation paradigm was used, with at least three standards between two subsequent deviants. Additionally, twelve control blocks were recorded, where stimuli, previously used as deviants, were presented as standards with a probability of 0.8. Each control block consisted of 100 standards (deviants as standards | main block) and 20 deviants (standard as deviant | main block). The standards from the control blocks and respective deviants from the main blocks were used in the analyses to determine MMN responses caused by changes in spatial position rather than by different representations of physically different stimuli (Kujala et al., 2007).
Data analysis
EEG data were preprocessed offline and analyzed by using the Matlab toolbox EEGLAB (version 10.0.0.0b; Delorme and Makeig, 2004; http://sccn.ucsd.edu/eeglab/). Continuous recordings were FIR band-pass filtered between 1 and 20 Hz. Subsequently, data were segmented for the different deviant and standard conditions by extracting 600 ms epochs which comprise the period 100 ms before stimulus onset (baseline) and 500 ms epochs after stimulus onset. Epochs were baseline-corrected by referencing the channel means to the respective baseline and linear trends were removed from each epoch by applying drift correction. Epochs with amplitudes exceeding ±90 μV were excluded from further analysis and an average of 56 epochs out of 100 was kept per person. Epochs of all main and control blocks were averaged individually for each condition (central, lateral) and respective standard and deviant positions (−15°, −5°, +15°, +25°, and ±85°, ±75°, ±55°, ±45°). Difference waves (DW) were computed by subtracting the ERPs of deviants presented as standards (control stimuli) in control blocks from the ERP of deviants presented in the main block, i.e., DWdeviant = ERPdeviant_main block − ERPdeviant as standard_control block.
Respective grand averages were computed separately from the averages of individual subjects. To increase the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) for MMN, DW were additionally re-referenced to the mastoids (Kujala et al., 2007). To further increase SNR for the lateral blocks, we collapsed the ERP data elicited by deviants presented at ±45°, ±55°, ±75°, and ±85° across hemifields.
To test for statistical significance of MMN and P3a signals, mean MMN and mean P3a amplitudes were measured for each subject within a window of ±10 ms around the peaks of the corresponding grand averaged responses. Mean MMN and mean P3a amplitudes were tested against zero with a one-sample, two-tailed Student's t-test. MMN and P3a signals that failed to reject the null hypothesis were excluded from further analyses. Following this procedure, individual MMN and P3a amplitudes were calculated as the mean within a ±10 ms time window around the individual MMN and P3a latencies measured at the peak amplitude in the respective time windows (100–250 ms for MMN, 200–350 ms for P3a post stimulus onset).
For the analysis of individual MMN amplitudes and latencies for central and lateral stimulus conditions the electrodes Fz was preselected. Furthermore, respective statistical differences were evaluated by a Two-Way rm-ANOVA including the factors “direction of deviation” (toward the midline, toward the side) and factor “magnitude of deviation” (10°, 20°). The analyses of inter-hemispheric differences in MMN amplitude was based on a spatial average of selected left (F2, F4, F6, FC2, FC4, FC6) and right (F1, F3, F5, FC1, FC3, FC5) electrode sites. Left- and right-hemispheric mean amplitude distributions of respective MMN components were tested against zero with a one-sample, two tailed Student's t-test. The effects of sound source laterality (central +5° vs. lateral ±65°) on MMN amplitude and latency was tested by post-hoc paired comparison t-tests. For this, we selected the MMN responses elicited by a spatial deviation of 20° toward the midline (at −15° for the central block and ±45° for the lateral block) evoked at electrode Fz. Voltage topographies of MMN and P3a components were analyzed separately in the respective components' time windows using the open source toolbox sphspline, which is based on spherical interpolation (https://github.com/widmann/sphspline; Perrin et al., 1989). The Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied. All selected comparisons were made by using Bonferroni-corrected paired t-tests.
Results
ERPs were elicited for deviants and the corresponding control stimuli (“deviant as standard”) at each of eight deviant positions (−15°, −5°, +15°, +25°, ±45°, ±55°, ±75°, and ±85°; cf. Figure 2) and respective MMN amplitudes are listed in Table 1. Potential topographies of DWs (“deviant” – “deviant as standard”) within the MMN latency time window of significantly evoked MMNs show broadly distributed negative deflections over frontocentral scalp sites, with the polarity inverting at mastoid sites (Figures 2B,C). The scalp voltage topographies point to putative MMN generators within both cortices, including frontal, supra-temporal, and parietal areas.
Table 1.
Deviant position (direction of deviation) | Mean amplitudesa (±SEM)/μV | MMN latenciesb (±SEM)/ms |
---|---|---|
−15° (toward the midline) | early −1.18 (0.46)* | 178.46 (3.84) |
late −1.13 (0.45)* | 210.00 (4.15) | |
−5° (toward the midline) | −0.89 (0.29)* | 197.08 (6.40) |
+15° (toward the sides) | −1.98 (0.46)* | 192.20 (6.22) |
+25° (toward the sides) | −2.04 (0.46)* | 198.61 (4.16) |
±45° (toward the midline) | −1.15 (0.49)* | 203.80 (6.65) |
±55° (toward the midline) | −0.36 (0.24) | − |
±75° (toward the sides) | −0.18 (0.27) | − |
±85° (toward the sides) | −0.18 (0.37) | − |
Mean amplitudes were calculated within ±10 ms around the peak MMN that occurred 150–250 ms after stimulus onset from grand average re-referenced ERPs. A one-sample two-sided t-test (df = 12) was calculated to test if MMN amplitudes within the time window form a distribution with the mean zero. Significant values are indicated by asterisks, with
p ≤ 0.05.
MMN amplitudes were obtained within the ±10 ms time window around the latency of the MMN peak in the re-referenced grand averages.
Individual MMN latencies measured from individual difference waves at the MMN peak amplitude.
Central stimulus condition (+5° standard position)
Grand averaged ERPs for deviants show a characteristic negative deflection compared to the corresponding ERPs for control stimuli in the MMN time window 140–220 ms post stimulus onset at electrode Fz (Figures 2A,C). Thus, each deviant stimulus reliably elicited an MMN signal within a latency window of 140–220 ms [−15°: t(12) = −6.1387, p < 0.001; −5°: t(12) = −6.6611, p < 0.001; +15°: t(12) = −7.1795, p < 0.001; +25°: t(12) = −7.7759, p < 0.001; Table 1, Figure 2]. Deviants presented at −15° evoked an MMN followed by a P3a at around 300 ms [295 (±11 [SEM]) ms] with an amplitude of 1.24 (±0.5 [SEM]) μV [t(12) = 2.5470, p = 0.0256], while all the other deviant positions (−5°, +15°, +25°, ±45°, ±55°, ±75°, and ±85°) failed to elicit significant P3a-amplitudes (all p > 0.05).
The rm-ANOVA revealed a significant effect of factor “direction of deviation” [F(1, 12) = 7.0792, p = 0.0208] with larger MMN amplitudes evoked by spatial deviations toward the sides than toward the midline [MMNside: −1.96 (±0.33) μV] > MMNmidline: −1.01 (±0.2) μV, t(12) = 3.4387, p = 0.0049. Furthermore, no significant main effect of factor “magnitude of deviation” [F(1, 12) = 0.1943, p = 0.6672] or significant interactions were found [“magnitude of deviation” × “direction of deviation”: F(1, 12) = 0.0618, p = 0.8078].
The MMN latencies were not affected by factors “direction of deviation” [F(1, 12) = 0.7060, p = 0.4172] and “magnitude of deviation” [F(1, 12) = 0.9329, p = 0.3532]. Also no significant interactions were found [“magnitude of deviation” × “direction of deviation”: F(1, 12) = 0.9329, p = 0.3532].
Early and late MMN components at deviant position −15°
The 20° deviation toward the midline elicited two negative peaks referred to as early and late MMN components (indicated by asterisks in Figure 2C). The early MMN component peaked at 178 (±4) ms with a mean amplitude of −1.16(±1.6) μV [t(12) = −16.3762, p < 0.001]. The late MMN component had its maximum at 210 (±4) ms with a mean amplitude of −1.10 (±1.5) μV [t(12) = 19.543, p < 0.001]. When comparing the topographies, the early MMN component yielded a uniform left- and right-hemispheric distribution unlike the late component, which showed a more rightward lateralization (cf. Figure 2C). However, the inter-hemispheric comparisons of left-and right hemispheric MMN amplitudes within the time windows of the early and late MMN components did not reveal a significant difference for either of the two [early MMN: left vs. right hemisphere: t(12) = −0.8104, p = 0.4335; late MMN: left vs. right hemisphere t(12) = 1.2338, p = 0.2409].
Lateral stimulus condition (standard at ±65°)
For the lateral stimulus condition MMN signals elicited by deviants at ±55°, ±75°, and ±85° failed to reach significance levels (all p > 0.05). The MMN amplitudes only yielded significance for spatial deviations toward the midline at ±45° [t(12) = −2.337, p = 0.0376] peaking at 203 (±7) ms.
No effect of sound source laterality on MMN amplitude and latency
To evaluate the putative effect of sound source laterality on the MMN, the responses elicited by 20° spatial deviations toward the midline at central and lateral reference position were evaluated. The comparison yielded no significant effect of sound source laterality [MMN amplitude: central vs. lateral: t(12) = −0.1151, p = 0.9103; MMN latency: central vs. lateral: t(12) = −1.7351, p = 0.1083].
Discussion
The present study examined early sensory auditory processing to changes in sound location in older adults by recording MMN. Previous studies showed that spatial auditory acuity—evaluated by measuring the MAA—is declined in older adults (Freigang et al., 2014). This led to the question, whether this decline in performance is also reflected in early neuronal processes linked to auditory discrimination. To get a better understanding of this issue, we measured the MMN in older adults to 10° and 20° spatial deviations, which corresponds to behaviorally acquired near and above MAA threshold values within the central field. In the present study, reliable MMNs were recorded for 10° and 20° spatial deviations from the +5° central position and for 20° deviations (i.e., at ±45°) from the lateral ±65° standard positions.
Decreased fine-tuning in the pre-attentive processing of pericentral sound sources
For frontal positions, MMNs were found for corresponding near-threshold and above-threshold deviants (Freigang et al., 2014) for both directions of deviation, i.e., toward the midline and toward the sides. Thus, considering that the MMN response is informative about an intact automatic change detection process, which relies on an adequate neuronal integration of the sensory input at subcortical and cortical level, it can be concluded that spatial changes measured behaviorally were also pre-attentively detected in older adults.
However, while in recent studies examining young adults the MMN amplitudes increases with increasing magnitude of spatial deviation (Deouell et al., 2006; Bennemann et al., 2013), here for both magnitudes of deviation (10°, 20°) equal MMN amplitudes were found. This finding implies a loss of at least 10° in gradual coding of auditory space representation at a preattentive level (Deouell et al., 2006; Bennemann et al., 2013). Notably, Deouell and colleagues used 50 ms long spectrally rich tones (fundamental 500 Hz, and three partials [1000, 1500, 2000 Hz]) which enabled the subjects to use ITD cues (based on the 500–1500 Hz-partials) as well as ILD cues (2000 Hz-partials). The presently used low-frequency noise bursts (300–1200 Hz) with a length of 250 ms predominantly provided ITD cues for sound source processing (Blauert, 1997). Given these differences, no definite conclusion can be drawn upon whether the absence of an increase in MMN amplitude with increasing magnitude of deviation can directly be related to differences in age or to the differences in stimulus design. Interestingly, in an MMN study on sound localization by Paavilainen et al. (1989), the same effects of spatial deviations on MMN were reported for both, low- and high-frequency sounds. Furthermore, they found MMN latencies to be shorter with increasing spatial deviation, an effect even more prominent for low- than for high-frequency sounds.
In the study of Deouell et al. (2006) stimuli had a duration of 50 ms, while in the present study the duration of the noise bursts were 250 ms. The longer stimuli were motivated by previous MMN studies suggesting a relative long “temporal window of integration” for auditory events (e.g., Tervaniemi et al., 1994; Winkler et al., 1998; Yabe et al., 1998) requiring stimuli of at least 150–300 ms in duration to achieve full integration of all stimulus-specific acoustic information. Also, a behavioral study by Grantham (1995) on the ability to detect dynamic interaural cues suggested that signal durations of 150–300 ms are necessary to warrant the lowest possible thresholds in binaural discrimination. Considering these findings, we would like to argue that the present mode of acoustic stimulation ensures the full modulating effects of spatial deviation on MMN. Still, further test are needed to clarify the postulated relation between age and pre-attentive encoding of sound sources and these studies will have to include a control group with young adults to enable a direct comparison.
Decreased neural resolution of space within the lateral acoustic field
For the lateral ±65° positions, valid MMN responses were obtained for 20° deviations toward the midline (±45°) but not for the same magnitude of deviation toward the sides. These findings suggest that spatial changes toward the sides of about 16°—verified to be distinguishable by older adults in behavioral experiments (Freigang et al., 2014)—were pre-attentively unrecognized by the MMN system. In young adults, spatial changes of 17° either toward the midline or to the sides within the lateral acoustic field were shown to elicit valid MMN responses (Richter et al., 2009). More specifically, in a study on young adults by Bennemann et al. (2013) the same laterality of 65° was explored and MMN responses were obtained for lateral spatial deviations of 5°, 10°, and 15° toward the sides (i.e., respective deviants at 70°, 75°, and 80°). Furthermore, in the same subjects monotonously increased MMNs were found with larger spatial deviations which led to the ascertainment of at least 5° resolution of neuronal pre-attentive sound source discrimination at such lateral positions. Since—as presently shown—in older adults neither lateral spatial displacements of 10° or 20° toward the sides elicited MMNs, we suspect that the preattentive fine-grained encoding of far-lateral sound sources by the MMN system deteriorates with age. Localization of low-frequency sounds is predominantly based on the processing in ITD (e.g., Middlebrooks and Green, 1991) a finding which might relate to results of earlier studies showing impaired processing of ITD information in older adults (Kirikae et al., 1963; Herman et al., 1977; Strouse et al., 1998; Babkoff et al., 2002). Also, it was shown that in elderly adults the fidelity in encoding temporal information was generally declined (Ross et al., 2007, 2010; Ross, 2008; Ruggles et al., 2011, 2012), which is in agreement with decelerated temporal processing mechanisms (Pichora-Fuller and Schneider, 1991; Frisina and Frisina, 1997; Schneider and Hamstra, 1999; Lister and Roberts, 2005; Freigang et al., 2011). These findings are in line with the notion of a blurred representation of sound sources in older results, particularly for low-frequency sounds.
For the ±65° standard positions, MMNs were elicited by deviants displaced by 20° toward the midline possibly pointing to a specific relation between the position at which a “novelty” occurs and a given reference position. Considering the fact that changes in interaural acoustic cues decrease with increasing lateralities up to 90° (Blauert, 1997; Moore, 1997), the presently tested 20-degree-deviation toward the side (re 65° standard position) resulted in smaller ITD-changes than the same spatial deviation toward the midline. This indicates that the later ITD-changes reached values that were pre-attentively detected by the MMN system.
Still, there is a discrepancy between the MMN data acquired here and previously reported MAA data (Freigang et al., 2014), which might suggest a role of spatial selective attention mechanisms in improving auditory discrimination of adjacent sound sources especially in the lateral acoustic space (for further details see Bennemann et al., 2013).
Previous studies reported that older people benefit from an active attentional focusing possibly compensating for the age-related decline in automatic establishment of memory traces (Bertoli et al., 2002; Alain et al., 2004). Alain et al. (2004) recorded active and passive MMNs in a gap detection task and found that when attention was directed away from the auditory modality, or explicitly focused on a visual task, the physiological response to an near-threshold deviant was absent in older adults. In young adults, in contrast, auditory near-threshold deviants elicited MMNs despite attention being focused on a visual task. The present findings are in agreement with the notion of a general age-effect on the preattentive processing of sound properties (Alain and Woods, 1999; Bertoli et al., 2002; Alain et al., 2004) and refer to its specific importance for auditory space processing (Freigang et al., 2014).
Effect of direction of deviation on MMN within the pericentral space
For the +5° standard position, larger MMN amplitudes were elicited by positional changes toward the sides than toward and across the midline. Noteworthy, the respective deviants at +15° and +25° were within the same hemifield, while the deviants at −5° and −10° were in the opposite hemifield. The larger MMN amplitudes might relate to stronger activation of a defined neuron population and/or activation of a larger neuron population. Interestingly, the respective MNN differences are contradictory to predictions emanating from the “opponent-channel coding” hypothesis (also referred to as hemifield code), the prevailing model for cortical representation of acoustic space (Stecker and Middlebrooks, 2003; Werner-Reiss and Groh, 2008; Magezi and Krumbholz, 2010; Salminen et al., 2012; Briley et al., 2013). The model proposes for each cortical hemisphere neuronal populations tuned to the respective contralateral acoustic hemifield exhibiting activation profiles with maxima for lateral sound source positions and steeply decreasing slopes toward central positions. Still, behavioral studies suggest that both hemifield channels overlap in an area of about 30° in the frontal acoustic field (Boehnke and Phillips, 1999; Phillips, 2008). According to this model, scalp recordings from vertex sites should yield higher cortical activity levels for sounds (i) emanating from lateral compared to central positions (EEG: Magezi and Krumbholz, 2010; Briley et al., 2013) and (ii) originating from opposite hemifields (MEG: Salminen et al., 2012). Previous studies testing the opponent channel coding employed rather large angular distances (30°, 45° to 90°) and did not specifically explored frontal areas of overlapping hemifield channels. Since presently the positional changes fall into the 30° range of overlap of both hemifield channels, it is not possible to anticipate potential effects on the MMN.
Also, there is a possibility that for the central stimulation not only the factor “direction of deviation” might affect the MMN amplitude, but also the factor “crossing the midline.” For our experiments we chose the same standard position at +5° as Deouell and coworkers in an earlier study on young adults (Deouell et al., 2006). In this study the deviants were located at +15° (i.e., more lateral) and at −5° (i.e., to the front and crossing the midline). Unlike in the present study, in young adults both deviants elicited equal MMN responses. Same as in the study by Deouell and coworkers, the MMN had a two-peak structure (termed early and late MMN components) followed by a P3a component for deviants presented at −15° (Deouell et al., 2006). Presently, the occurrence of the deviant across the midline might have established the condition to evoke an involuntary attentional shift, which is what the P3a is thought to stand for (e.g., Horvath et al., 2008).
Conclusion
The present study suggests that the pre-attentive processing of changes in spatial positions can be impaired in older adults despite the fact that the same spatial changes are behaviorally distinguishable. This particularly holds for lateral positions, while preattentive sound source processing is largely preserved for sources within the pericentral and mid-lateral acoustic fields. The constancy of MMN amplitudes for different magnitudes of spatial deviation suggests a decline in spatial resolution. We hypothesize that older adults might benefit from actively engaging top-down attentional processes to detect small magnitudes of spatial changes specifically in the lateral acoustic field.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We wish to thank Ingo Kannetzky, Matthias Freier, Jörg Eckebrecht, and Joachim Dörrscheidt for their help in planning and setting up the free-field setup and implementing the experimental software, and thank Jan Bennemann for his help during data collection. We gratefully acknowledge the funding of this work by the International Max Planck Research School on Neuroscience of Communication: Function, Structure, and Plasticity (IMPRS NeuroCom) and the Erasmus Mundus Student Exchange Network in Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience (ACN).
References
- Abel S. M., Giguère C., Consoli A., Papsin B. C. (2000). The effect of aging on horizontal plane sound localization. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 108, 743 10.1121/1.429607 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abel S. M., Hay V. H. (1996). Sound localization the interaction of aging, hearing loss and hearing protection. Scand. Audiol. 25, 3–12 10.3109/01050399609047549 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alain C., McDonald K. L., Ostroff J. M., Schneider B. (2004). Aging: a switch from automatic to controlled processing of sounds? Psychol. Aging 19, 125–133 10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.125 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alain C., Woods D. L. (1999). Age-related changes in processing auditory stimuli during visual attention: evidence for deficits in inhibitory control and sensory memory. Psychol. Aging 14, 507–519 10.1037/0882-7974.14.3.507 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alho K. (1995). Cerebral generators of mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic counterpart (MMNm) elicited by sound changes. Ear Hear. 16, 38–51 10.1097/00003446-199502000-00004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amenedo E., Diaz F. (1998). Automatic and effortful processes in auditory memory reflected by event-related potentials. Age-related findings. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 108, 361–369 10.1016/S0168-5597(98)00007-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Electroencephalographic Society. (1994). Guideline thirteen: guidelines for standard electrode position nomenclature. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 11, 143–147 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Babkoff H., Muchnik C., Ben-David N., Furst M., Even-Zohar S., Hildesheimer M. (2002). Mapping lateralization of click trains in younger and older populations. Hear. Res. 165, 117–127 10.1016/S0378-5955(02)00292-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennemann J., Freigang C., Schröger E., Rübsamen R., Richter N. (2013). Resolution of lateral acoustic space assessed by electroencephalography and psychoacoustics. Front. Psychol. 4:338 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00338 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bertoli S., Smurzynski J., Probst R. (2002). Temporal resolution in young and elderly subjects as measured by mismatch negativity and a psychoacoustic gap detection task. Clin. Neurophysiol. 113, 396–406 10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00013-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biedermann F., Bungert P., Dörrscheidt G. J., von Cramon D. Y., Rübsamen R. (2008). Central auditory impairment in unilateral diencephalic and telencephalic lesions. Audiol. Neurootol. 13, 123–144 10.1159/000111784 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blauert J. (1997). Spatial Hearing: the Psychophysics of Human Sound Localization. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press [Google Scholar]
- Boehnke S. E., Phillips D. P. (1999). Azimuthal tuning of human perceptual channels for sound location. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 106(4 Pt 1), 1948–1955 10.1121/1.428037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bregman A. S. (1990). Auditory Scene Analysis: the Perceptual Organization of Sound. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press [Google Scholar]
- Briley P. M., Kitterick P. T., Summerfield A. Q. (2013). Evidence for opponent process analysis of sound source location in humans. J. Am. Res. Otolaryngol. 14, 83–101 10.1007/s10162-012-0356-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- CHABA. (1988). Speech understanding and aging. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 83, 859–895 10.1121/1.395965 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chandler D. W., Grantham D. W. (1991). Effects of age on auditory spatial resolution in the horizontal plane. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 89, 1994 10.1121/1.2029814 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper R. J., Todd J., McGill K., Michie P. T. (2006). Auditory sensory memory and the aging brain: a mismatch negativity study. Neurobiol. Aging 27, 752–762 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.03.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corso J. F. (1971). Sensory processes and age effects in normal adults. J. Gerontol. 26, 90–105 10.1093/geronj/26.1.90 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cranford J. L., Andres M. A., Piatz K. K., Reissig K. L. (1993). Influences of age and hearing loss in the precedence effect in sound localization. J. Speech Hear. Res. 36, 437–441 10.1044/jshr.3602.437 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cruickshanks K. J., Wiley T. L., Tweed T. S., Klein B. E., Klein R., Mares-Perlman J. A., et al. (1998). Prevalence of hearing loss in older adults in Beaver Dam, Wisconsin. The epidemiology of hearing loss study. Am. J. Epidemiol. 148, 879–886 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009713 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Czigler I., Csibra G., Csontos A. (1992). Age and inter-stimulus interval effects on event-related potentials to frequent and infrequent auditory stimuli. Biol. Psychol. 33, 195–206 10.1016/0301-0511(92)90031-O [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delorme A., Makeig S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deouell L. Y., Parnes A., Pickard N., Knight R. T. (2006). Spatial location is accurately tracked by human auditory sensory memory: evidence from the mismatch negativity. Eur. J. Neurosci. 24, 1488–1494 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05025.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dobreva M. S., O'Neill W. E., Paige G. D. (2011). Influence of aging on human sound localization. J. Neurophysiol. 105, 2471–2486 10.1152/jn.00951.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eddins D. A., Hall J. W. (2010). Binaural processing and auditory asymmetries, in The Aging Auditory System, eds Gordon-Salant S., Frisina R. D., Popper A. N., Fay R. R. (New York, NY: Springer; ), 135–165 10.1007/978-1-4419-0993-0_6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Folstein M. F., Folstein S. E., McHugh P. R. (1975). Mini-Mental State: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freigang C., Schmidt L., Wagner J., Eckardt R., Steinhagen-Thiessen E., Ernst A., et al. (2011). Evaluation of central auditory discrimination abilities in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 3:6 10.3389/fnagi.2011.00006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freigang C., Schmiedchen K., Nitsche I., Rübsamen R. (2014). Free-field study on auditory localization and discrimination abilities in older adults. Exp. Brain Res. 232, 1–16 10.1007/s00221-014-3825-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frisina D. R., Frisina R. D. (1997). Speech recognition in noise and presbycusis: relations to possible neural mechanisms. Hear. Res. 106, 95–104 10.1016/S0378-5955(97)00006-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giard M., Lavikahen J., Reinikainen K., Perrin F., Bertrand O., Pernier J., et al. (1995). Separate representation of stimulus frequency, intensity, and duration in auditory sensory memory: an event-related potential and dipole-model analysis. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 7, 133–143 10.1162/jocn.1995.7.2.133 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grantham D. W. (1995). Spatial hearing and related phenomena, in Hearing, ed Moore B. C. J. (London: Academic Press; ), 297–346 10.1016/B978-012505626-7/50011-X [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Grimm S., Recasens M., Althen H., Escera C. (2012). Ultrafast tracking of sound location changes as revealed by human auditory evoked potentials. Biol. Psychol. 89, 232–239 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.10.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunter T. C., Jackson J. L., Mulder G. (1996). Focussing on aging: an electrophysiological exploration of spatial and attentional processing during reading. Biol. Psychol. 43, 103–145 10.1016/0301-0511(95)05180-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Häusler R., Colburn S., Marr E. (1983). Sound localization in subjects with impaired hearing: spatial-discrimination and interaural-discrimination tests. Acta Otolaryngol. 96, 1–62 10.3109/00016488309105590 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herman G. E., Warren L. R., Wagener J. W. (1977). Auditory lateralization: age differences in sensitivity to dichotic time and amplitude cues. J. Gerontol. 32, 187–191 10.1093/geronj/32.2.187 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Horvath J., Czigler I., Jacobsen T., Maess B., Schröger E., Winkler I. (2008). MMN or no MMN: no magnitude of deviance effect on the MMN amplitude. Psychophysiology 45, 60–69 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00599.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Humes L. E. (1996). Speech understanding in the elderly. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 7, 161–167 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karayanidis F., Andrews S., Ward P. B., Michie P. T. (1995). ERP indices of auditory selective attention in aging and Parkinson's disease. Psychophysiology 32, 335–350 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.tb01216.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirikae I., Sato T., Shitara T. (1963). A study of hearing in advanced age. Int. J. Audiol. 2, 173–175 10.3109/05384916309101753 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kropotov J. D., Näätänen R., Sevostianov A., Alho K., Reinikainen K., Kropotova O. (1995). Mismatch negativity to auditory stimulus change recorded directly from the human temporal cortex. Psychophysiology 32, 418–422 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.tb01226.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kujala T., Tervaniemi M., Schröger E. (2007). The mismatch negativity in cognitive and clinical neuroscience: theoretical and methodological considerations. Biol. Psychol. 74, 1–19 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lister J. J., Roberts R. A. (2005). Effects of age and hearing loss on gap detection and the precedence effect: narrow-band stimuli. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 48, 482–493 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/033) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magezi D. A., Krumbholz K. (2010). Evidence for opponent-channel coding of interaural time differences in human auditory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 1997–2007 10.1152/jn.00424.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makous J. C., Middlebrooks J. C. (1990). Two-dimensional sound localization by human listeners. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 87, 2188–2200 10.1121/1.399186 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McAlpine D. (2005). Creating a sense of auditory space. J. Physiol. 566, 21–28 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.083113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Middlebrooks J. C., Green D. M. (1991). Sound localization by human listeners. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 42, 135–159 10.1146/annurev.ps.42.020191.001031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mills A. W. (1958). On the minimum audible angle. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 30, 237 10.1121/1.1909553 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moore B. C. J. (1997). An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing, 4th Edn. London, UK: Academic Press [Google Scholar]
- Näätänen R., Gaillard A. W., Mäntysalo S. (1978). Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted. Acta Psychol. 42, 313–329 10.1016/0001-6918(78)90006-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nager W., Kohlmetz C., Joppich G., Möbes J., Münte T. F. (2003). Tracking of multiple sound sources defined by interaural time differences: brain potential evidence in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 344, 181–184 10.1016/S0304-3940(03)00439-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neher T., Laugesen S., Jensen N. S., Kragelund L. (2011). Can basic auditory and cognitive measures predict hearing-impaired listeners' localization and spatial speech recognition abilities? J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 130, 1542 10.1121/1.3608122 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noble W., Byrne D., Ter-Horst K. (1997). Auditory localization, detection of spatial separateness, and speech hearing in noise by hearing impaired listeners. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 102, 2343–2352 10.1121/1.419618 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oldfield R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paavilainen P., Alho K., Reinikainen K., Sams M., Näätänen R. (1991). Right hemisphere dominance of different mismatch negativities. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 78, 466–479 10.1016/0013-4694(91)90064-B [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paavilainen P., Karlsson M.-L., Reinikainen K., Näätänen R. (1989). Mismatch negativity to change in spatial location of an auditory stimulus. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 73, 129–141 10.1016/0013-4694(89)90192-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pakarinen S., Takegata R., Rinne T., Huotilainen M., Näätänen R. (2007). Measurement of extensive auditory discrimination profiles using the mismatch negativity (MMN) of the auditory event-related potential (ERP). Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 177–185 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pekkonen E. (2000). Mismatch negativity in aging and in Alzheimer's and Parkinson's diseases. Audiol. Neurotol. 5, 216–224 10.1159/000013883 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pekkonen E., Rinne T., Reinikainen K., Kujala T., Alho K., Naatanen R. (1996). Aging effects on auditory processing: an event-related potential study. Exp. Aging Res. 22, 171–184 10.1080/03610739608254005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perrin F., Pernier J., Bertrand O., Echallier J. (1989). Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 72, 184–187 10.1016/0013-4694(89)90180-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pettigrew C. M., Murdoch B. E., Ponton C. W., Finnigan S., Alku P., Kei J., et al. (2004). Automatic auditory processing of English words as indexed by the mismatch negativity, using a multiple deviant paradigm. Ear Hear. 25, 284–301 10.1097/01.AUD.0000130800.88987.03 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips D. P. (2008). A perceptual architecture for sound lateralization in man. Hear. Res. 238, 124–132 10.1016/j.heares.2007.09.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pichora-Fuller M. K., Schneider B. A. (1991). Masking-level differences in the elderly: A comparison of antiphasic and time-delay dichotic conditions. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 34, 1410–1422 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Picton T. W., Alain C., Otten L., Ritter W., Achim A. (2000). Mismatch negativity: different water in the same river. Audiol. Neurot. 5, 111–139 10.1159/000013875 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richter N., Schröger E., Rübsamen R. (2009). Hemispheric specialization during discrimination of sound sources reflected by MMN. Neuropsychologia 47, 2652–2659 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.05.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ross B. (2008). A novel type of auditory responses: temporal dynamics of 40-Hz steady-state responses induced by changes in sound localization. J. Neurophysiol. 100, 1265–1277 10.1152/jn.00048.2008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ross B., Fujioka T., Tremblay K. L., Picton T. W. (2007). Aging in binaural hearing begins in mid-life: evidence from cortical auditory-evoked responses to changes in interaural phase. J. Neurosci. 27, 11172–11178 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1813-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ross B., Schneider B., Snyder J. S., Alain C. (2010). Biological markers of auditory gap detection in young, middle-aged, and older adults. PloS ONE 5:e10101 10.1371/journal.pone.0010101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruggles D., Bharadwaj H., Shinn-Cunningham B. G. (2011). Normal hearing is not enough to guarantee robust encoding of suprathreshold features important in everyday communication. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 15516–15521 10.1073/pnas.1108912108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruggles D., Bharadwaj H., Shinn-Cunningham B. G. (2012). Why middle-aged listeners have trouble hearing in everyday settings. Curr. Biol. 22, 1417–1422 10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruzzoli M., Pirulli C., Brignani D., Maioli C., Miniussi C. (2012). Sensory memory during physiological aging indexed by mismatch negativity (MMN). Neurobiol. Aging 33, 625.e621–625.e630 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.03.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salminen N. H., Tiitinen H., May P. J. (2012). Auditory spatial processing in the human cortex. Neuroscientist 18, 602–612 10.1177/1073858411434209 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sambeth A., Pakarinen S., Ruohio K., Fellman V., van Zuijen T. L., Huotilainen M. (2009). Change detection in newborns using a multiple deviant paradigm: a study using magnetoencephalography. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 530–538 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.12.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sams M., Paavilainen P., Alho K., Näätänen R. (1985). Auditory frequency discrimination and event-related potentials. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 62, 437–448 10.1016/0168-5597(85)90054-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmiedchen K., Freigang C., Nitsche I., Rübsamen R. (2012). Effects of trajectory on crossmodal interactions and multisensory integration in the perception of audio-visual motion. Brain Res. 1466, 99–111 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.05.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider B. A., Hamstra S. J. (1999). Gap detection thresholds as a function of tonal duration for younger and older listeners. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 106, 371 10.1121/1.427062 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schroeder M., Ritter W., Vaughan H. (1995). The mismatch negativity to novel stimuli reflects cognitive decline. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 769, 399–401 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb38155.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schröger E. (1996). Interaural time and level differences: integrated or separated processing? Hear. Res. 96, 191–198 10.1016/0378-5955(96)00066-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schröger E. (1998). Measurement and interpretation of the mismatch negativity. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 30, 131–145 10.3758/BF03209423 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Schröger E., Wolff C. (1996). Mismatch response of the human brain to changes in sound location. Neuroreport 7, 3005–3008 10.1097/00001756-199611250-00041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shamma S. A., Micheyl C. (2010). Behind the scenes of auditory perception. Cur. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 361–366 10.1016/j.conb.2010.03.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sonnadara R. R., Alain C., Trainor L. J. (2006). Effects of spatial separation and stimulus probability on the event-related potentials elicited by occasional changes in sound location. Brain Res. 1071, 175–185 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.088 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stecker G. C., Middlebrooks J. C. (2003). Distributed coding of sound locations in the auditory cortex. Biol. Cybern. 89, 341–349 10.1007/s00422-003-0439-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strouse A., Ashmead D. H., Ohde R. N., Grantham D. W. (1998). Temporal processing in the aging auditory system. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 104, 2385–2399 10.1121/1.423748 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tata M. S., Ward L. M. (2005). Early phase of spatial mismatch negativity is localized to a posterior “where” auditory pathway. Exp. Brain Res. 167, 481–486 10.1007/s00221-005-0183-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tervaniemi M., Saarinen J., Paavilainen P., Danilova N., Näätänen R. (1994). Temporal integration of auditory information in sensory memory as reflected by the mismatch negativity. Biol. Psychol. 38, 157–167 10.1016/0301-0511(94)90036-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaitulevich S. F., Shestopalova L. B. (2010). Interhemisphere asymmetry of auditory evoked potentials in humans and mismatch negativity during sound source localization. Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 40, 629–638 10.1007/s11055-010-9305-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werner-Reiss U., Groh J. M. (2008). A rate code for sound azimuth in monkey auditory cortex: implications for human neuroimaging studies. J. Neurosci. 28, 3747–3758 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5044-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winkler I., Czigler I. (1998). Mismatch negativity: deviance detection or the maintenance of the ‘standard’. Neuroreport 9, 3809–3813 10.1097/00001756-199812010-00008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winkler I., Czigler I., Jaramillo M., Paavilainen P. (1998). Temporal constraints of auditory event synthesis: evidence from ERPs. Neuroreport 9, 495–499 10.1097/00001756-199802160-00025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woods D. L. (1992). Auditory selective attention in middle-aged and elderly subjects: an event-related brain potential study. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 84, 456–468 10.1016/0168-5597(92)90033-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yabe H., Tervaniemi M., Sinkkonen J., Huotilainen M., Ilmoniemi R. J., Näätänen R. (1998). Temporal window of integration of auditory information in the human brain. Psychophysiology 35, 615–619 10.1017/S0048577298000183 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]