Abstract
Circadian clocks in the peripheral tissues of mice are known to be entrained by pulse stimuli such as restricted feeding, novel wheel running, and several other agents. However, there are no reports on high temperature pulse-mediated entrainment on the phase-shift of peripheral clocks in vivo. Here we show that temperature treatment of mice for two days at 41°C, instead of 37°C, for 1–2 h during the inactive period, using a temperature controlled water bath stimulated phase-advance of peripheral clocks in the kidney, liver, and submandibular gland of PER2::LUCIFERASE mice. On the other hand, treatment for 2 days at 35°C ambient room temperature for 2 h did not cause a phase-advance. Maintenance of mice at 41°C in a water bath, sustained the core body temperature at 40–41°C. However, the use of 37°C water bath or the 35°C ambient room temperature elevated the core body temperature to 38.5°C, suggesting that at least a core body temperature of 40–41°C is necessary to cause phase-advance under light-dark cycle conditions. The temperature pulse stimulation at 41°C, instead of 37°C water bath for 2 h led to the elevated expression of Per1 and Hsp70 in the peripheral tissue of mice. In summary, the present study demonstrates that transient high temperature pulse using water bath during daytime causes phase-advance in mouse peripheral clocks in vivo. The present results suggest that hot water bath may affect the phase of peripheral clocks.
Introduction
The circadian clock enables an organism to adapt its physiology and behavior to routine environmental changes. Circadian clock system is located in the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) in the brain, which is the main oscillator, as well as in the peripheral organs, the peripheral oscillator. Genetic and molecular approaches have identified a core clock mechanism of the circadian oscillator that is governed by interconnected transcriptional and translational delayed feedback loops [1], [2]. The CLOCK/BMAL1 heterodimer initiates the transcription process by binding to specific DNA elements, E- and E'-boxes in the promoters of target genes [3], [4]. This set of activated genes includes members of the negative limb of the feedback loop including the Per (Per1 and Per2) and Cry (Cry1 and Cry2) genes [3], [5], [6]. The resulting PER and CRY proteins dimerize and inhibit further CLOCK/BMAL1 transcriptional activity, allowing the cycle to repeat from the level of low transcriptional activity [7]–[9]. The circadian clock genes are necessary for daily rhythm of core body temperature and also for cold temperature-induced heat production behavior. Mutant mice with Rev-erbα gene exhibit resistance to body temperature decrease during the inactive period compared to the wild type mice [10]. On the other hand, mutant mice with Per2 gene exhibit augmented body temperature and are not viable in decreased ambient temperature [11].
In in vitro cell line experiments, NIH3T3 cells and RAT1 fibroblasts showed an entrained circadian rhythm at temperatures within the physiologic range (36°C to 38.5°C) or in routine temperature differences of only 3°C [12], [13]. In in vitro SCN and peripheral tissues experiments, circadian changes in temperature comparable to those seen with core body temperature rhythms are capable of entraining and enhancing the amplitude of the circadian rhythms of peripheral tissues, while the adult SCN remains resistant [14]. The temperature pulse-mediated resetting of Per2 expression in tissue explants has been shown to be blunted in the presence of the heat shock transcription factor (HSF) inhibitor [14] or in HSF1-deficient fibroblasts [15]. Thus, HSF may be involved in the temperature-induced phase entrainment of circadian clocks [12], [14]. Recently, the cold-inducible RNA-binding protein (Cirp) mRNA and protein levels oscillated throughout the day in cells has been reported to be synchronized with body temperature rhythms (34–38°C), but not in cells maintained at 36.5°C, after synchronization of core clock machinery by serum stimulation. The reduction of CIRP protein levels in fibroblasts by RNA interference showed that CIRP is required for high-amplitude circadian gene expression [16].
Very few in vivo studies have investigated temperature cycle as an entrainment factor in endothermic animals such as birds and mammals. Occasional studies on a few species of rodents [17]–[21] have indicated that temperature cycles have the potential to entrain circadian rhythms, but are rather weak entraining factors. Almost all experiments thus far evaluated the locomotor activity as a function of the circadian rhythm, which is controlled by SCN circadian clocks. Daily cycles of light and temperature are perhaps the two most reliable environmental time cues for living systems on earth. Although it is well established that light is the strongest time cue for the mammalian SCN circadian rhythm, mechanisms underlying the entrainment of peripheral circadian clocks in response to high temperature pulses has not been well established in vivo.
Recently, we established a simple experimental protocol to evaluate the circadian clock rhythm of peripheral organs with an in vivo imaging system (IVIS) using PER2::LUCIFERASE (PER2::LUC) knock-in mice [22]. In the present experiment, we examined whether circadian rhythm of peripheral clocks can be entrained in response to warm temperature pulse using the IVIS method. Due to the compensatory response in mice, ambient room temperature fluctuations (24–35°C) may be insufficient to continuously increase the core body temperature in mice. Hence, in the present experiment, mice were exposed to warm water bath to ensure the continuous maintenance of elevated core body temperature.
Methods
Ethics Statement
All experimental protocols were approved by Committee for Animal Experimentation of the School of Science and Engineering at Waseda University (permission protocol #; 2013-A058, 2013-A061) and in accordance with the law (No. 105) passed and notification (No. 6) of the Japanese Government.
Animals
PER2::LUC C57BL/6J knock-in mice (courtesy of Dr. Joseph Takahashi, Southwestern Medical Center, Texas University, TX, USA) [23], were backcrossed more than 5 times against ICR mice (Tokyo Laboratory Animals, Tokyo, Japan). Heterozygous PER2::LUC knock-in mice were kept in a room maintained on a 12∶12 h light-dark cycle with lights on at 08∶00, which we defined as a zeitgeber time 0 (ZT 0), at a temperature of 23±1°C, 60±5% humidity, and a light intensity of 100–150 lux at cage level, and were provided with a standard monounsaturated fatty diet (Oriental Yeast Co. Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) and water ad libitum. Four female mice were housed as a single group for the current experiments. In addition, the experiment was conducted on a separate set of male mice to confirm that the phase change was due to the high temperature of the water bath and not the estrous cycle seen in female mice.
Exposing mice to temperature-controlled water bath
Water incubator and mixer (As one Inc., Osaka, Japan) were set at the edge of case and a cylindrical sieve made of 6.7 mm square stainless mesh was placed inside. Water was poured into the case to attain a height of 26–28 mm from the surface of the water to the stainless net. After the incubator was set to maintain the required temperature, a plastic pipe whose diameter was 131 mm, height was 300 mm, was set on the cylindrical sieve. Warm water was poured for experimental mice, but no water for control mice, and intact mice were kept under home cage. Two mice were placed on the cylindrical sieve during treatment (0.5–2.0 h) (Fig. S1A) once a day or once a day for 2–3 continuous days. In some mice, warm water treatment was applied for 2 continuous days, and then bioluminescence recording was started 24 h after heating. Non-photic entrainment stimuli such as food and novel wheel running were provided mid-day to stimulate the phase-advance of peripheral clocks [24]–[26]. Therefore, in the present experiments, mice were subjected to warm water stimulation at mid-day.
Measurement of the core body temperature
We prepared PER2::LUC heterozygous mice for recording their core body temperature. A small temperature sensor (Thermochron SL; KN Laboratories Inc., Osaka, Japan) was shaped similar to a button battery. One side of the sensor was covered with a plastic shield and the other side served as the sensing surface. The mice were anesthetized with midazolam/xylazine. The sensor was then implanted, with the sensing surface near the liver, and the covered surface just under the skin. This specific positioning prevented the sensor from detecting changes in the environmental temperature. After a recovery period of one week, the temperature was automatically recorded at 10 min intervals for one month. The recordings were analyzed using RhManeger (Version 2.10; KN Laboratories Inc., Osaka, Japan).
In vivo monitoring of PER2::LUC bioluminescence
In vivo monitoring of PER2::LUC bioluminescence was performed as previously described [22] using an IVIS kinetics system (Caliper Life Sciences, Hopkinton, MA, USA, and Summit Pharmaceuticals International Corporation, Tokyo, Japan). Mice were anaesthetized with isoflurane (Mylan Inc., Tokyo, Japan) and concentrated oxygen (SO-005B; Sanyo Electronic Industries Co. Ltd, Okayama, Japan) in a black box using the gas anesthesia system (XGI-8; Caliper Life Sciences). Anesthetized mice were injected with d-luciferin potassium salt (Promega, Madison, WI, USA) subcutaneously (15 mg/kg). Images were acquired with an exposure time 1, 6, and 8 min after luciferin injection in the dorsal-up position for the kidney, and at 10 and 12 min after injection in the ventral-up position for the liver and the submandibular gland. Each bioluminescence image was merged with the corresponding grey-scale image.
Images were obtained 6 times a day (ZT 7, 11, 15, 19, 23, and 3) (Fig. S1B). Mice were returned to their home cages after each imaging session, where they recovered quickly from anesthesia. The total time under anesthesia was approximately 20 min per session. A previous study has shown that luciferase activity in the peripheral tissues and behavior were unaffected by four 1-h-long sessions of anesthesia and bioluminescence analysis per day [22].
Analysis of in vivo monitoring data
In vivo monitoring data were analyzed as described previously [22]. The bioluminescence emitted from each organ (kidney, liver, and submandibular gland) was calculated automatically using Living Image 3.2 software (Caliper Life Sciences). For each organ, the region of interest was set to the same shape and size in all images. In case of the kidney, data from the right and left kidneys were combined for the analysis. The average photon/sec value from the set of 6 time points for each day was taken as 100%, and the bioluminescence rhythm for the entire day was expressed as a percentage of each set for each organ. The peak phase and amplitude of the normalized data were determined using the single cosinor procedure (Acro.exe, version 3.5; [27]). Accuracy of rhythmicity was evaluated by cosinor amplitude and goodness of fit, and peripheral clock oscillation with <40% in amplitude and/or 0.1> in goodness of fit was judged as arrhythmicity. As we did not find any cases in intact mice and control mice (Fig. 1B), we followed the guidelines from our previous paper [22].
Isolation of total RNA and Real-time RT-PCR
The messenger RNA from tissues was measured by real-time reverse transcription polymerase chain reaction (RT-PCR), as described previously [28], [29]. A total RNA concentration of 15 ng was reverse transcribed and amplified with One-Step SYBR RT-PCR kit (TaKaRa Bio Inc. Shiga, Japan) in Piko Reall (Thermo Fisher Scientific Inc. Kanagawa, Japan). Specific primer pairs were designed based on published data for Gapdh, Per1, Per2, Bmal1, and HSP70 genes. The primer sequences were as follows: mouse Gapdh 5′-tggtgaaggtcggtgtgaac-3′, 5′-aatgaaggggtcgttgatgg-3′; mouse Per1: 5′-caagtggcaatgagtccaacg-3′, 5′-cgaagtttgagctcccgaagtg-3′; mouse Per2: 5′-tgtgtgcttacacgggtgtccta-3′, 5′-acgtttggtttgcgcatgaa-3′; mouse Bmal1: 5′-ccacctcagagccattgataca-3′, 5′-gagcaggtttagttccactttgtct-3′; mouse Hsp70: 5′- aacgtgctgcggatcatcaa-3′, 5′-gaagrccrgcagcagcttct-3′. The relative levels of the target gene PCR products were normalized to those of Gapdh. Data were analyzed using the delta-delta Ct method. A melt curve analysis was performed to check for non-specific products. The results indicated the absence of nonspecific products.
Statistical analysis
All values are expressed as mean + or ± SEM. Statistical analyses was performed using GraphPad Prism version 6.03 (GraphPad software, San Diego, CA, USA). We checked if the data were distributed normally by using the D'Agostino-Pearson/Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. In addition, we checked for equal or biased variation using the F-value test. Parametric analysis for multigroups was conducted using a one-way ANOVA with a Tukey test for post hoc analysis, and non-parametrical analysis for multigroups was done using the Kruskal-Wallis test with a Dunns test for post hoc analysis. The Mann-Whitney test was used for non-parametrical analysis of 2 groups.
Results
Temperature pulse of 41°C for 2 h causes a phase-shift in the peripheral circadian clocks
We used a temperature controlled water bath and placed the mice to change the core body temperature of the mice efficiently. Before examining the effect of temperature pulse using water baths, we checked the difference between the intact group and the control group (Fig. 1). The control group represented a group of mice placed in a cylindrical sieve at room temperature without water. Waveforms of bioluminescence rhythm of kidney, liver, and submandibular gland (sub gla) recorded in the intact or the control mice are shown in Figure 1A. We judged whether luciferase activity was rhythmic by checking whether they satisfied followed criteria: cosinor amplitude >40% and goodness of fit <0.1 (Fig. 1B) according to the previously described method [22]. The intact and the control group mice exhibited rhythmic luciferase activity. The PER2::LUC expression phases of peripheral clock were slightly advanced in the control group than in the intact group (Figs. 1A, C). There were no differences in cosinor amplitude between two groups (Fig. 1D).
Next, we examined the effect of 41°C heat shock using the 41°C water bath on the peripheral clock phase-shift and if it was dependent on the duration of the pulse stimulation (Fig. 2). We have presented each set of data for judgment of oscillation accuracy (Fig. 2A). Of the 25 water bath samples, four, seven, and a single sample from the kidney, liver, and the sub gla were omitted from data analysis (Fig. 2B). The phase-shift of the peripheral clock was dependent and proportional to the duration of the stimulus (Fig. 2B). The waveform of the liver clock in mice that were kept in the 41°C water bath for 1.5 h or 2 h peaked at ZT11 and at ZT19 as two peaks. However, that of the kidney and sub gla peaked at ZT11 as single peak (Fig. S2). Since two desynchronized waveforms with a single peak could have been observed from any subdivision of the liver, we analyzed the bioluminescence rhythm in 12 subdivisions from one liver sample (Figures S3A and B). However, waveforms with two peaks for the total liver still showed bimodal peaks in each subdivision (Figure S3B), but those with one peak for the total liver showed a single peak in each subdivision (Figure S3A). There were no differences detected in cosinor amplitude in any of the organs among the five groups (Fig. 2C). To record the accurate core body temperature that acts as a stimulus for phase-shift in the peripheral organs, we installed a small sensor in the intraperitoneal cavity of the experimental mice. Core body temperatures in the group treated using the 41°C water bath were maintained at 40–41°C during ZT 4–6 (Fig. 2 D).
The body temperature of female mice fluctuates with the estrous cycle, and these changes possibly influence the warm water–induced phase reset seen in female mice. To confirm that warm water causes the circadian phase reset, the experiment was conducted separately on male mice. These mice that were kept in 41°C water bath for 2 h over 2 days exhibited clear and significant phase advances of all peripheral clocks (Fig. S4).
In the next experiment, we determined whether placing mice in a 41°C water bath resulted in advanced phase-shift in each organ and was dependent on the duration of treatment (1 day, 2 days, and 3 days) (Fig. 3, Fig. S5). Of the 17 samples, four kidney and four liver samples were omitted from data analysis and no samples were excluded for sub gla (Fig. 3B). A two-day treatment schedule consisting of a single 2 h session per day was sufficient to stimulate a phase-advance of the peripheral clocks (Fig. 3B). The 3-day treatment schedule caused a smaller phase-advance of the kidney and liver clocks than the 2-day treatment, but the phase advance of the sub gla clock in both treatment schedules were similar (Fig. 3B). The waveform of the liver clock in the 3-day treatment schedule peaked once (Fig. S5), while that in the 2-day treatment peaked twice. We then confirmed whether the phase-advance caused by the water bath treatment of 2 h at 41°C and over 2 days was a transient or permanent phase shift. In this experiment, we examined whether the phase-advance caused by the water bath treatment at 41°C was maintained or pull-backed to the control phase. This was studied via an IVIS recording that was done 24 h after the final heating. The phases of the peripheral clocks, especially the kidney clock, returned to the control level 24 h after heating (Fig. 3B). There were no differences in cosinor amplitude among the five groups in any of the organs tested (Fig. 3C).
We examined the effect of different temperature pulse in stimulating the phase-shift (Fig. 4, Fig. S6). Of the 16 samples, three kidney and three liver samples were omitted from data analysis, and none was excluded for the sub gla (Fig. 4B). Maintenance of mice in the water bath set at 41°C led to a significant phase-advance of the peripheral clock in the kidney, liver, and sub gla compared with the corresponding organs of mice maintained at 37°C and those of the control group (Fig. 4B). There were no differences in cosinor amplitude among the three groups (Fig. 4C). The core body temperature of mice treated at 37°C was found to be elevated to 38–39°C, although this change was very similar to that observed in control mice (Fig. 2D).
Our data indicated that changes in core body temperature influenced the phase-shift and circadian rhythm; we sought to examine the effect of 35°C ambient room temperature (Fig. 5). All four mice in this group retained the rhythmic PER2::LUC expression and there were no significant differences in phase-shift between the intact (24°C) and 35°C ambient room temperature groups (Fig. 5B). Cosinor amplitude of the kidney significantly decreased after subjecting mice to 35°C or room temperature (Fig. 5C). The core body temperature of these mice at 35°C ambient room temperature elevated rapidly, but also rapidly declined during the treatment (Fig. 5D).
Temporary temperature pulse at 41°C for 2 h up-regulates Per gene expression
In an effort to understand the factors that advance the phase of PER2::LUC expression and circadian rhythms, we investigated the clock gene expression after temporary temperature pulse in a 41°C hot water bath for 2 h. As a result, Per1 expression was found to be significantly elevated in the kidney and the liver (Fig. 6).
Elevated Hsp70 levels were observed after subjecting mice to 41°C water bath treatment, indicating that the temperature bath simulated a heat shock effect.
Glucocorticoid stimulation does not stimulate phase-shift in the peripheral clocks
Considering that mice received both water and hot stimulation, we were interested to test the effect of stress responses through corticosterone release on the phase-shift of the peripheral clocks. To this end, we treated mice with dexamethasone to understand the exclusive effect of corticosterone on the phase-shift of peripheral clock. Six mice were injected with saline as a control group, and three mice with 10 mg/kg dexamethasone at ZT 5 for two continuous days. Compared to the control group, the dexamethasone group showed no significant phase shifting in any of the organs tested (Fig. 7A). Cosinor amplitude in the organs tested was almost same as the control group (Fig. 7B).
Discussion
In the present set of experiments, mice exposed to 41°C in a temperature controlled water bath for 2 days during the inactive period resulted in advanced phase of the peripheral clocks in an exposure time (0.5–2 h)-dependent manner. On the other hand, mice exposed to 37°C water bath or control mice placed in a small cylindrical pipe on the sieve at room temperature exhibited a slight phase-advance. The core body temperature of mice maintained at 41°C in the water bath was maintained between 40°C and 41°C during the treatment period, while exposure to 37°C or a small cylindrical pipe were maintained between 38°C and 39°C. Therefore, our data suggest that in the present experimental conditions, a core body temperature change of 40–41°C for at least 2 h is necessary for entraining signals. This hot water-induced phase advance was observed in both female and male mice, suggesting no sexual differences. A change in room temperature from 24°C to 35°C for 2 h increased the core body temperature to 40°C for the first 30 min, but then the core body temperature rapidly declined to 38.5°C, effected by the innate compensation response to ambient high temperature in mice. Thus, the lack of constantly maintained core body temperature increase may be insufficient to cause phase-shift, and the core body temperature fluctuations from the ambient temperature experiments, support this result.
In this experiment, the effect of stress might contribute to the warm water-induced phase-shift. Control mice kept in cylindrical sieve with stainless net made from rough mesh exhibited a small shift of phase-advance in comparison to intact mice. On the other hand, dexamethasone injection at ZT 5 for 2 days did not produce a phase-advance. Taken together, it is suggested that glucocorticoid-release by stress factors such as warm water, cylindrical sieve, and ambient high temperature may not be largely involved in warm water-induced phase-advance. Previously Balsalobre [30] examined the phase-shifts based on timing of dexamethasone injection, and they found a large phase delay after injection around at ZT 11-ZT 18, but no shifts after injection at ZT 5, supporting our current observations.
In in vitro cell line experiments, NIH3T3 cells and RAT1 fibroblasts have been reported to show entrained circadian rhythm in temperatures within the physiologic range (36–38.5°C) or routine temperature differences of only 3°C [12], [13]. Furthermore, as observed in our in vivo peripheral tissue experiments, temperature-dependent circadian changes comparable to those seen with core body temperature rhythms (36–38°C) are capable of entraining and enhancing the amplitude of the circadian rhythms of peripheral tissues in vitro [14]. In vivo, in activity rhythm experiments, 60% to 80% of the mice were entrained by ambient temperature cycle (24–32°C) [31]. Although circadian fluctuation of body temperature here was 36.5–38.5°C, and these changes were comparable to previous reports, a core body temperature increase to 38–39°C using a 37°C water bath, or 38.5–40°C at 35°C ambient room temperature, failed to cause any phase-shifts. These results suggest that transient temperature stimulation is not enough as an entraining signal for phase-shift of peripheral clocks under light-dark cycle conditions. There are two types of entraining signals affecting circadian rhythm, parametric and non-parametric stimulation [32]. Parametric temperature stimulation consisting of 12 h high and 12 h low temperature cycle may be effective for entraining signals compared with a non-parametric temperature stimulation, which comprises 1–2 h high or low temperature in a day. In the present study, we adopted a non-parametric stimulation approach because the parametric stimulation does not clarify whether entrainment mediated by temperature cycles is caused directly by temperature or indirectly through the effect of temperature on locomotor and/or feeding activity [31]. Actually motor activity and feeding are well known entraining factors of circadian rhythm [24]–[26]. Thus, this is to our knowledge, the very first study demonstrating the direct entraining effect of temperature on peripheral circadian clocks. Although it is not clear as to why a high core body temperature (40–41°C) is necessary to cause phase shift in the present experiment, one plausible reason is that the current experiment was examined under light-dark cycle conditions, in which the SCN dependent circadian rhythm is active. A peak of phase of the peripheral clock, which is induced by temperature stimulation, which had moved to daytime, may be influenced by SCN and/or nocturnal feeding and therefore may be pulled back to nighttime. Phase-advance by 2 day warm water stimulation was attenuated by 24 h after heating, suggesting this possibility. Thus, it is possible that the warm water (41°C) treatment is too weak to entrain the signal, which is easily influenced by other signals. In the current experiments, it is difficult to rule out the possibility that there is the direct change of luminescence by heat and this direct effect modified estimated clock phase value of the first cycle. In the present experiment, we found that, after the warm water (41°C) stimulation for 2 days, the liver clock rhythm peaked twice. One peak possibly corresponds to the new rhythm and the other to the original rhythm (Fig. S2). Another possible explanation is that each peak was recorded from each subdivision of the liver. However, this explanation can be discarded because we could not find any difference between the subdivisions in the liver (Fig. S3). With regard to the role of the SCN on the warm-water-induced phase advance of the peripheral clock, the possibility that the SCN was entrained by a transient increase in the core body temperature and the possibility of the phase of the peripheral clock thus being indirectly entrained are excluded because the SCN was resistant to temperature entrainment compared to the lung and the pituitary [14]. On the other hand, in the in vitro experiment, such a compensatory signal is absent, and circadian rhythm system may exhibit more sensitivity to changing temperature. If compared 3-day warm water stimulation with 2-day warm stimulation, former stimulation became weaker than later one, suggesting that 3-day heating stimulation may be acquired the tolerance response. Among peripheral tissues, the kidney and the liver clock were sensitively impaired by high temperature stimulation compared to the sub gla clocks, because the accuracy of rhythmicity was impaired more in the kidney and the liver after warm water stimulation. Differences in sensitivity of each peripheral clock to entraining signals, such as glucocortisone, and restricted feeding have been previously reported [33].
The resetting of Per2 expression by temperature pulses in tissue explants has been shown to be blunted in the presence of the heat shock transcription factor (HSF) inhibitor [14] or in HSF1-deficient fibroblasts [15]. Thus, HSF may be involved in the temperature-dependent phase entrainment of circadian clocks [12], [14]. Here in this study, high temperature pulses (41°C for 2 h) transiently increased Per1, Per2, and Hsp70 mRNA expression, and decreased Bmal1 mRNA expression in the peripheral tissues, whereas medium temperature pulses (37°C for 2 h), which resulted in slightly phase-shift did not change the expression levels of these genes. Therefore, the transient change of clock gene expression may trigger the phase-shift.
In summary, we have demonstrated that transient high temperature pulses using water bath during daytime caused phase-advances in mouse peripheral clocks in vivo. The present results suggest that hot water bath may affect the phase of peripheral clocks.
Supporting Information
Funding Statement
This study was supported by a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (B) (23300278) from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology of Japan (S.S.). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Lowrey PL, Takahashi JS (2011) Genetics of Circadian Rhythms in Mammalian Model Organisms. In: Stuart Brody, editors. The Genetics of Circadian Rhythms: Academic Press. 175–230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Buhr ED, Takahashi JS (2013) Molecular components of the mammalian circadian clock. In: Kramer A, Merrow M, editors. Circadian Clocks. Springer Berlin Heidelberg. 3–27. [Google Scholar]
- 3. Gekakis N, Staknis D, Nguyen HB, Davis FC, Wilsbacher LD, et al. (1998) Role of the CLOCK protein in the mammalian circadian mechanism. Science 280: 1564–1569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Yoo SH, Ko CH, Lowrey PL, Buhr ED, Song EJ, et al. (2005) A noncanonical E-box enhancer drives mouse Period2 circadian oscillations in vivo. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102: 2608–2613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Hogenesch JB, Gu YZ, Jain S, Bradfield CA (1998) The basic-helix–loop–helix-PAS orphan MOP3 forms transcriptionally active complexes with circadian and hypoxia factors. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95: 5474–5479. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Kume K, Zylka MJ, Sriram S, Shearman LP, Weaver DR, et al. (1999) mCRY1 and mCRY2 are essential components of the negative limb of the circadian clock feedback loop. Cell 98: 193–205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Griffin EA, Staknis D, Weitz CJ (1999) Light-independent role of CRY1 and CRY2 in the mammalian circadian clock. Science 286: 768–771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Sangoram AM, Saez L, Antoch MP, Gekakis N, Staknis D, et al. (1998) Mammalian circadian autoregulatory loop: a timeless ortholog and mPer1 interact and negatively regulate CLOCK-BMAL1-induced transcription. Neuron 21: 1101–1113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Sato TK, Yamada RG, Ukai H, Baggs JE, Miraglia LJ, et al. (2006) Feedback repression is required for mammalian circadian clock function. Nat Genet. 38: 312–319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Gerhart-Hines Z, Feng D, Emmett MJ, Everett LJ, Loro E, et al. (2013) The nuclear receptor Rev-erbα controls circadian thermogenic plasticity. Nature 503: 410–413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Chappuis S, Ripperger JA, Schnell A, Rando G, Jud C, et al. (2013) Role of the circadian clock gene Per2 in adaptation to cold temperature. Mol Metab 2: 183–193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Saini C, Morf J, Stratmann M, Gos P, Schibler U (2012) Simulated body temperature rhythms reveal the phase-shifting behavior and plasticity of mammalian circadian oscillators. Genes Dev. 26: 567–580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Brown SA, Zumbrunn G, Fleury-Olela F, Preitner N, Schibler U (2002) Rhythms of mammalian body temperature can sustain peripheral circadian clocks. Curr Biol 12: 1574–1583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Buhr ED, Yoo SH, Takahashi JS (2010) Temperature as a universal resetting cue for mammalian circadian oscillators. Science 330: 379–385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Tamaru T, Hattori M, Honda K, Benjamin I, Ozawa T, et al. (2011) Synchronization of circadian Per2 rhythms and HSF1-BMAL1: CLOCK interaction in mouse fibroblasts after short-term heat shock pulse. PLoS one 6: e24521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Morf J, Rey G, Schneider K, Stratmann M, Fujita J, et al. (2012) Cold-inducible RNA-binding protein modulates circadian gene expression posttranscriptionally. Science 338: 379–383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Francis AJ, Coleman GJ (1988) The effect of ambient temperature cycles upon circadian running and drinking activity in male and female laboratory rats. Physiol Behav 43: 471–477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Goldman BD, Goldman SL, Riccio AP, Terkel J (1997) Circadian patterns of locomotor activity and body temperature in blind mole-rats, Spalax ehrenbergi. J Biol Rhythms. 12: 348–361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Lindberg RG, Hayden P (1974) Thermoperiodic entrainment of arousal from torpor in the little pocket mouse, Perognathus longimembris. Chronobiologia 1: 356–361. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Pohl HH (1998) Temperature cycles as zeitgeber for the circadian clock of two burrowing rodents, the normothermic antelope ground squirrel and the heterothermic Syrian hamster. Biol Rhythm Res 29: 311–325. [Google Scholar]
- 21. Rajaratnam SM, Redman JR (1998) Entrainment of activity rhythms to temperature cycles in diurnal palm squirrels. Physiol Behav 63: 271–277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Tahara Y, Kuroda H, Saito K, Nakajima Y, Kubo Y, et al. (2012) In vivo monitoring of peripheral circadian clocks in the mouse. Curr Biol 22: 1029–1034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Yoo SH, Yamazaki S, Lowrey PL, Shimomura K, Ko CH, et al. (2004) PERIOD2:: LUCIFERASE real-time reporting of circadian dynamics reveals persistent circadian oscillations in mouse peripheral tissues. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101: 5339–5346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Reebs SG, Mrosovsky N (1989) Effects of induced wheel running on the circadian activity rhythms of Syrian hamsters: entrainment and phase response curve. J Biol Rhythms 4: 39–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Marchant EG, Mistlberger RE (1996) Entrainment and phase shifting of circadian rhythms in mice by forced treadmill running. Physiol Behav 60: 657–663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Hara R, Wan K, Wakamatsu H, Aida R, Moriya T, et al. (2001) Restricted feeding entrains liver clock without participation of the suprachiasmatic nucleus. Genes Cells 6: 269–278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Refinetti R, Cornélissen G, Halberg F (2007) Procedures for numerical analysis of circadian rhythms. Biol Rhythm Res 38: 275–325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Tahara Y, Otsuka M, Fuse Y, Hirao A, Shibata S (2011) Refeeding after fasting elicits insulin-dependent regulation of Per2 and Rev-erbα with shifts in the liver clock. J Biol Rhythms 26: 230–240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Itokawa M, Hirao A, Nagahama H, Otsuka M, Ohtsu T, et al. (2013) Time-restricted feeding of rapidly digested starches causes stronger entrainment of the liver clock in PER2:: LUCIFERASE knock-in mice. Nutr Res 33: 109–119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Balsalobre A, Brown SA, Marcacci L, Tronche F, Kellendonk C, et al. (2000) Resetting of circadian time in peripheral tissues by glucocorticoid signaling. Science 289: 2344–2347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Refinetti R (2010) Entrainment of circadian rhythm by ambient temperature cycles in mice. J Biol Rhythms 25: 247–256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Roenneberg T, Hut R, Daan S, Merrow M (2010) Entrainment concepts revisited. J Bio Rhythms 25: 329–339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Sujino M, Furukawa K, Koinuma S, Fujioka A, Nagano M, et al. (2012) Differential entrainment of peripheral clocks in the rat by glucocorticoid and feeding. Endocrinology 153: 2277–2286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.