Summary
Foraging insect pollinators such as bees must find and identify flowers in a complex visual environment. Bees use skylight polarization patterns for navigation [1–3], a capacity mediated by the polarization-sensitive dorsal rim area (DRA) of their eye [4, 5]. While other insects use polarization sensitivity to identify appropriate habitats [6], oviposition sites, and food sources [7], to date no nonnavigational functions of polarization vision have been identified in bees. Here we investigated the ability of bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) to learn polarization patterns on artificial “flowers” in order to obtain a food reward. We show that foraging bumblebees can learn to discriminate between two differently polarized targets, but only when the target artificial “flower” is viewed from below. A context for these results is provided by polarization imaging of bee-pollinated flowers, revealing the potential for polarization patterns in real flowers. Bees may therefore have the ability to use polarization vision, possibly mediated by their polarization-sensitive DRA, both for navigation and to learn polarization patterns on flowers, the latter being the first nonnavigational function for bee polarization vision to be identified.
Highlights
-
•
Bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) learn polarization patterns on artificial “flowers”
-
•
Polarization patterns were only learned from downward-facing, pendant “flowers”
-
•
Polarization vision in bumblebees is not restricted to sun-compass navigation
-
•
Polarization patterns of petals may be a component of floral signaling
Foraging bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) learn polarization patterns on downward-facing artificial “flowers” to find food, demonstrating that their polarization vision is not restricted to sun-compass navigation. Polarization patterns occur on the petals of real flowers and may be a, hitherto overlooked, component of floral signaling.
Results and Discussion
Sensitivity to the polarization of light is a common visual ability in insects [8, 9] and other arthropods, which use it for a variety of behaviors, including sun-compass navigation [2, 4, 10], motion detection [11], detecting bodies of water [6], and determining oviposition sites [12]. Honeybees (Apis mellifera) were among the first animals to be identified as being polarization sensitive [1] and to use the polarization-sensitive dorsal rim area (DRA) of their eye to identify the position of the sun for sun-compass navigation [2, 4, 10]. The role of polarization sensitivity in insect foraging, however, is far less well known, although the butterfly Papilio xuthus can learn to associate certain angles of polarization with food [7] (albeit confounding polarization with color), and polarization vision has not hitherto been suggested in the context of bee foraging. Both pollinator and plant fitness are greatly dependent on the ability of pollinators to discriminate flowers accurately, and bees have been shown to be able to use a wide range of floral cues, including color, shape, texture, volatiles, and temperature, to improve identification and recognition of flowers [13, 14]. Recent findings have added floral humidity and electric fields as additional modalities with which pollinators can discriminate flowers [15, 16], and it is advantageous for a plant to produce a multimodal array of signals that a pollinator can utilize effectively [17]. Could polarization sensitivity therefore function as an additional floral cue for bees?
Orientation of Artificial Flowers Affects Learning Ability for Polarization Patterns
We investigated whether bumblebees could learn polarization patterns of artificial “flowers” and found that their ability to distinguish between two differently polarized targets was dependent on viewing direction. Freely foraging bumblebees flying in an experimental arena learned to differentiate between rewarding (sucrose solution providing) and aversive (quinine solution providing) artificial “flowers” (Figure 1) in a single learning session. Patterns of polarization (Figure 2) were either rewarding, type “contrast” (in which the two linearly polarized components of the bull’s-eye-patterned target were oriented at 90° to each other), or aversive, type “plain” (having the polarized components oriented in the same direction). In one treatment “flowers” were oriented to be upward facing (UF), such that they were viewed by the ventral region of the bee’s eye, and in another treatment “flowers” were downward facing (DF), such that they were viewed by the dorsal part of the bee’s eye, including the polarization-sensitive DRA. For DF “flowers” in two experiments (DF1 and DF2), nested mixed-effects models of learning for repeated experiments provided significantly better fits than null models (DF1: Δdeviance = 13.142, degrees of freedom [df] = 1, p < 0.01; DF2: Δdeviance = 21.761, df = 1, p < 0.01) and had lower Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) values (DF1: AIC 923.25 versus 912.11; DF2: AIC 1288.8 versus 1,269.1), indicating that foraging experience led to a greater proportion of rewarding “flowers” being chosen (Figure 3). For UF “flowers,” this was not the case in two experiments, UF1 and UF2 (UF1: AIC 1,251.3 versus 1,252.4, Δdeviance = 0.8733, df = 1, p = 0.35; UF2: AIC 1,390.3 versus 1,392.3, Δdeviance = 0.0137, df = 1, p = 0.9067), and the task was not learned (Figure 3). The number of trials required to reach a maximum proportion of correct responses (100 trials) is longer than that required by bees to learn most colors and shapes in choice experiments [18–21], indicating that bees find this task difficult (this number of trials is equivalent to that needed for bees to learn a “very high” color similarity [19]). In nature, however, polarization patterns may be learned more easily in concert with other cues as part of a multicomponent signal [22, 23].
Many insect species possess polarization sensitive areas in the ventral and lateral regions of their eyes [6, 24], or indeed throughout their eye [7, 12], in addition to the DRA. In honeybees, polarization sensitivity of the compound eyes tends to be limited to the DRA [2, 4, 10] and to be UV mediated. Nevertheless, ommatidia in the DRA of honeybees express both UV- and green-sensitive visual pigments [25], and the DRA photoreceptors of bumblebees express more than one UV pigment [26], potentially broadening spectral sensitivity. In addition, there is electrophysiological evidence that bumblebees have both blue and UV polarization sensitivity in their main retina [27], as well as polarization-sensitive ocelli [28]. Consequently, we used light sources that included the full bee-visible spectrum, including the UV, and employed linear polarizers that polarized effectively across the whole of this spectrum (Figure S1 available online). In the color-sensitive regions of the bee’s compound eye, adaptations such as twisted rhabdoms [10] reduce polarization sensitivity to avoid confounding color and polarization [29], as occurs in the butterfly Papilio [7, 12], which lacks twisted rhabdoms. Since bumblebees were trained successfully to distinguish between two polarization patterns, but only when presented dorsally (DF “flowers”), our data suggest that flower discrimination can be mediated by polarization sensitivity, but only when the flowers are viewed by the dorsal region of the eye. A further requirement may be that the pattern is simple and spatially course as the spatial resolution of the DRA is low in most polarization sensitive insects [5], including bumblebees [30], although we cannot eliminate the possibility that they could detect “finer” polarization patterns with higher spatial frequencies than we presented. The results of previous attempts to demonstrate polarization sensitivity behaviorally in Bombus spp. have been somewhat ambiguous and related to polarotaxis rather than target identification in foraging (e.g., [31]), and this study represents the first clear evidence that bumblebees may be trained to polarization patterns.
Learning of Polarization Patterns Is Independent of Intensity Cues
In a further experiment, bees were again presented with downward-facing “flowers” with a polarization pattern that indicated whether the “flower” was rewarding or aversive, but with an added intensity contrast pattern that was unrelated to the rewarding status of the “flower” (downward facing with intensity contrast, DFIC), such that there were four types of “flower”: condition 1, polarization contrast with darker outer ring (rewarding); condition 2, polarization contrast with lighter outer ring (rewarding); condition 3, polarization plain with darker outer ring (aversive); and condition 4, polarization plain with lighter outer ring (aversive). Nested mixed-effects models of learning effects again provided significantly better fits than null models and had lower AIC values (AIC 1,431.8 versus 1,426.6; Δdeviance = 7.2162, df = 1, p < 0.01). Not only were the bumblebees capable of learning to differentiate between the two polarization patterns, but intensity contrast type (darker outer or inner ring) did not affect the choice of polarization pattern (AIC 4,817.9 versus 4,819.6; Δdeviance = 0.3283, df = 1, p = 0.5667), indicating that they can learn to ignore intensity patterns in favor of polarization patterns when these patterns are visible to the frontodorsal region of the eye, including the DRA. This result suggests that the polarization cues are not simply interpreted as patterns of intensity contrasts by the bees.
Polarimetry of Downward-Facing Flowers
Our results suggest that bees may be able to learn polarization patterns on downward-facing, pendant, natural flowers. The orientation of pendant flowers will impart directionality to pollinators as they approach [32], and it has been estimated that 53% of flower species do not face upward [33]. The occurrence of polarization patterns presented in such a way as to be visually accessible to the dorsofrontal region of the eye, including the bees’ DRA, could therefore be widespread, particularly as various mechanisms such as internal reflections [34] and specular reflection [35] have the potential to produce polarization patterns in petals. Petals in particular have a range of epidermal morphologies, at both macro and micro scale, that could contribute to polarization signals [36]. These occur in patterns [36, 37], and these patterns of petal surface structure were found to correspond to patterns of polarization in three phylogenetically distinct species (the gentian Eustoma russellianum, family Gentianaceae; the garden anemone Anemone coronaria, family Ranunculaceae; and the tree mallow Lavatera × clementii “Rosea”, family Malaceae; Figure 4). Since these patterns in petal surface structure have previously been described in several other floral species (for example, flower-of-an-hour Hibiscus trionum, family Malvacea; Chilean bellflower Nolana paradoxa, family Solanaceae; the tulip Tulipa humilis, family Liliaceae; and the meadow vetchling Lathyrus pratensis, family Fabaceae) and are thought to be widespread in multimodal flowers [36, 37], the corresponding patterns in polarization are likely to be equally prevalent. Basic image processing to calculate angle of polarization, as presented here and elsewhere (e.g., [8] and references therein), can provide valuable information about surface, orientation, and curvature. Bumblebees are known to take flower orientation into account when foraging as prior understanding of flower orientation affects flower handling [38], the minimization of handling time enhancing foraging efficiency. In addition, light reflected from downward-facing flowers has the potential to contrast with skylight polarization patterns, potentially facilitating bees’ detection and identification of flowers.
Conclusions
In addition to the well-established use of polarization sensitivity for sun-compass navigation, we show that bumblebees are able to learn to identify polarization patterns associated with a food source when foraging. Visual polarization information is interpreted independently from intensity information as, when only polarization indicates profitability, intensity patterns are ignored. While this capacity is apparently restricted to the frontodorsal field of view in bumblebees (and so possibly mediated by the DRA), bee-pollinated pendant (downward-facing) flowers represent a high proportion of angiosperm species. Our polarimetric imaging of flowers (Figure 4) indicates that petals produce polarized reflections due to their surface cellular organization and that these patterns are presented in such a way as to be available to pollinators approaching flowers from the below. Flower polarization patterns may therefore provide important information about flower shape and petal orientation that can be learned by bees, as well as providing cues for the discrimination of rewarding and nonrewarding food sources. Floral polarization patterns may therefore serve as signals to pollinators and rank alongside color and intensity patterns as foraging cues, extending the use of polarization vision by bumblebees from purely navigational purposes to the critical assessment of salient objects in their multisensory landscape.
Experimental Procedures
Artificial Flower Design
Artificial “flower” targets were constructed from black microcentrifuge tubes (10 mm mouth diameter, 2 ml volume) on top of which were mounted 38 mm diameter rings of 5-mm-thick UV-visible transmitting Perspex acrylic sheet. Two concentric rings of linear polarizing filter (HN22; Knight Optical), with 38 and 24 mm outer diameters, were affixed to the underside of the Perspex (Figure 1). For UF “flowers”, the acrylic surface was horizontal, facing upward, and the upturned lid of a microcentrifuge tube acted as a feeding reservoir for foraging bees; for the DF targets, the acrylic surface was 0°–30° from horizontal, facing downward, and the lid of the microcentrifuge tube was left attached and open. Between trials, bees were allowed to feed from similar “flowers” in which rings of neutral-density filter replaced the polarizing filter. Two types of target, arranged in pseudorandom grids, were used for differential conditioning: contrast targets with polarizer rings that transmitted perpendicular polarization angles, and plain targets with parallel-oriented polarizer rings (Figure 2). The feeding reservoir on each target contained either a 30% w/v sucrose solution, on the contrast targets, or 0.12% w/v quinine hemisulphate hydrate (C20H24N2O2 ⋅ 0.5H2O4S ⋅ H2O) solution, on the plain targets. The former acted as a reward, and the latter as a distasteful deterrent to foraging from these targets, differential conditioning having been shown to be more effective in honeybees than reward alone [19, 21, 39].
Downward-Facing “Flowers” with Intensity Contrasts
Targets contrasting in intensity and polarization were constructed by addition of a further two concentric rings (with the same diameters as the polarization filters) of 0.3 and 0.9 neutral-density filters (209 and 211 Lee Filters) on top of the polarizing filters (Figure 1). These targets were of the four different conditions described above (see the Results and Discussion). Four “flowers” of each condition were used in each trial and were arranged in a four-by-four Latin square. So that the effect of spatial learning of the target positions could be reduced, when the forager returned to the nest between foraging bouts, targets were rearranged such that (1) no target was in its previous place, (2) no place was occupied by a target of the same condition as previously, and (3) the four different conditions all formed a Latin square.
Differential Conditioning Experiments
The procedure for differential conditioning in B. terrestris was based on methods used to great effect in studies of sensitivity to color, surface microstructure, and electric field strength [16, 18, 38]. One hundred choices were recorded for each individual as the choice of either a rewarded or aversive target for a minimum of nine motivated foragers per treatment. All experiments were conducted under the guidelines and rules laid down by the University of Bristol. Further details of the differential conditioning procedure can be found in the Supplemental Experimental Procedures.
Bumblebee Colony Conditions and Flight Arena
Animal husbandry methods are detailed in the Supplemental Experimental Procedures. Naive B. terrestris colonies were supplied by Syngenta Bioline. The flight arena (72 cm wide × 104 cm long × 30 cm high) was covered with UV-visible transmitting acrylic (Perspex) sheet. All work was conducted under the University of Bristol guidelines for animal experiments.
Lights and Light Levels
Illumination for behavioral experiments was provided by six Sylvania Activa 172 Professional 36 W fluorescent tubes powered by Phillips high-frequency ballasts to have a flicker frequency greater than 1,200 Hz and run on a 12:12 hr daily light:dark regime. Light levels in the flight arena were measured using a Hanastech Quantitherm Lightmeter and averaged 20.3 μmol/m2/s (SEM 0.214), and the spectrum was measured with a USB2000 Spectrophotometer (Ocean Optics), P600-10-UV/VIS optical fiber (Ocean Optics), and a CC-3-UV-T cosine-corrected irradiance probe (Ocean Optics). Illumination encompassed the whole bee-visible spectrum, including the UV (Figure S1).
Statistical Analysis
Insect learning curves associated with each treatment were obtained by pooling of data from all individuals. Since there were strong between-subject differences in both ability to learn and rate of acquisition of learning, logistic mixed-effects models were chosen to account for random between-subject effects. Logistic regression models, as employed here, are commonly used to fit relationships between binomial response data and factors such as experience and condition [17, 23, 40] and may be used as a link function for fitting generalized linear mixed-effects models [41, 42]. For the DFIC treatment, a logistic mixed-effects model was also fitted for the effect of the interaction of intensity contrast type with experience (i.e., number of choices) on the proportion of correct responses, to investigate whether intensity contrasts contributed to learning. All nested models were compared with a simpler random-effects model using the change in deviance on removal of a term from the model, as well as the Akaike Information Criterion [43], to test for each model’s ability to describe the data. Logistic mixed-effects models were fitted with the lme4 package (version 1.0-5) in R 3.0.2 [41].
Polarization Imaging
Color-coded images showing the degree of polarization and angle of polarization for each pixel were calculated from digital photographs of artificial “flowers” and natural bee-pollinated flowers provided by the University of Bristol Botanic Gardens. Images were recorded with a Nikon D70 DSLR and stored as NEF (RAW) files. The camera was mounted on a heavy tripod, and a subject flower was held immobile in a darkroom laboratory under controlled, directional, invariant illumination provided from a fiber optic illuminator (Schott KL1500 Electronic Light Source). A sequence of eight images of each flower was recorded: seven with a linear polarizing filter rotated in 20° increments and a final “dark” image recorded with a lens cap in place. RAW images were transferred to computer and converted to uncompressed TIFF files using open-source RAW image-decoding software (DCRAW, [44]), maintaining pixel bit number linearity with pixel exposure, and keeping the four Bayer-masked CCD channels separate. These RGGB color channels were separated using a MATLAB (version 8.1.0.604; MathWorks) program and saved as uncompressed TIFF files, with one color channel being selected for further analysis. The corresponding TIFF images were aligned using ImageJ [45] with the Turboreg and Stackreg plugins [46]. A further MATLAB program was used to calculate the Stokes parameters for each pixel across the series of aligned images and produced images representing, variously, predominant angle of polarization, degree of polarization, and predominant angle of polarization with pixel brightness weighted by the degree of polarization, displayed as a color-coded image (Figures 2, 4, and S2).
Acknowledgments
We thank I. Cuthill for statistical advice and R. Woodfield, W. Amos, and J. Ruśčić for assistance with experiments. We are grateful for funding from BBSRC grants BB/H01635X/1 (to N.W.R. and J.C.P.) and BB/G022917/1 (to N.W.R.) and from ERC project number 260920 (to H.M.W.).
Footnotes
This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/).
Supplemental Information
References
- 1.von Frisch K. Die Polarisation des Himmelslichtes als orientierender Faktor bei den Tänzen der Bienen. Experientia. 1948;5:142–148. doi: 10.1007/BF02174424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Rossel S., Wehner R., Lindauer M. E-vector orientation in bees. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 1978;125:1–12. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kraft P., Evangelista C., Dacke M., Labhart T., Srinivasan M.V. Honeybee navigation: following routes using polarized-light cues. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2011;366:703–708. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Menzel R., Snyder A. Polarised light detection in the bee, Apis mellifera. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 1974;88:247–270. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Labhart T., Meyer E.P. Detectors for polarized skylight in insects: a survey of ommatidial specializations in the dorsal rim area of the compound eye. Microsc. Res. Tech. 1999;47:368–379. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0029(19991215)47:6<368::AID-JEMT2>3.0.CO;2-Q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Horváth G., Kriska G. Polarization vision in aquatic insects and ecological traps for polarotactic insects. In: Lancaster J., Briers R.A., editors. Aquatic Insects: Challenges to Populations. CABI; Wallingford: 2008. pp. 204–229. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kelber A., Thunell C., Arikawa K. Polarisation-dependent colour vision in Papilio butterflies. J. Exp. Biol. 2001;204:2469–2480. doi: 10.1242/jeb.204.14.2469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Horváth G., Varjú D. Springer-Verlag; Heidelberg: 2004. Polarized Light in Animal Vision. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Roberts N.W., Porter M.L., Cronin T.W. The molecular basis of mechanisms underlying polarization vision. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2011;366:627–637. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wehner R., Bernard G.D., Geiger E. Twisted and non-twisted rhabdoms and their significance for polarization detection in the bee. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 1975;104:225–245. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Glantz R.M., Schroeter J.P. Polarization contrast and motion detection. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 2006;192:905–914. doi: 10.1007/s00359-006-0127-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kelber A. Why ‘false’ colours are seen by butterflies. Nature. 1999;402:251. doi: 10.1038/46204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Raguso R.A. Flowers as sensory billboards: progress towards an integrated understanding of floral advertisement. Curr. Opin. Plant Biol. 2004;7:434–440. doi: 10.1016/j.pbi.2004.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Leonard A.S., Papaj D.R. ‘X’ marks the spot: the possible benefits of nectar guides to bees and plants. Funct. Ecol. 2011;25:1–9. [Google Scholar]
- 15.von Arx M., Goyret J., Davidowitz G., Raguso R.A. Floral humidity as a reliable sensory cue for profitability assessment by nectar-foraging hawkmoths. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2012;109:9471–9476. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121624109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Clarke D., Whitney H., Sutton G., Robert D. Detection and learning of floral electric fields by bumblebees. Science. 2013;340:66–69. doi: 10.1126/science.1230883. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kaczorowski R.L., Leonard A.S., Dornhaus A., Papaj D.R. Floral signal complexity as a possible adaptation to environmental variability: a test using nectar-foraging bumblebees, Bombus impatiens. Anim. Behav. 2012;83:905–913. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Dyer A.G., Chittka L. Fine colour discrimination requires differential conditioning in bumblebees. Naturwissenschaften. 2004;91:224–227. doi: 10.1007/s00114-004-0508-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Dyer A.G., Chittka L. Bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) sacrifice foraging speed to solve difficult colour discrimination tasks. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 2004;190:759–763. doi: 10.1007/s00359-004-0547-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Giurfa M. Conditioning procedure and color discrimination in the honeybee Apis mellifera. Naturwissenschaften. 2004;91:228–231. doi: 10.1007/s00114-004-0530-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Avarguès-Weber A., de Brito Sanchez M.G., Giurfa M., Dyer A.G. Aversive reinforcement improves visual discrimination learning in free-flying honeybees. PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e15370. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Rowe C. Receiver psychology and the evolution of multicomponent signals. Anim. Behav. 1999;58:921–931. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1999.1242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kulahci I.G., Dornhaus A., Papaj D.R. Multimodal signals enhance decision making in foraging bumble-bees. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2008;275:797–802. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2007.1176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Wernet M.F., Velez M.M., Clark D.A., Baumann-Klausener F., Brown J.R., Klovstad M., Labhart T., Clandinin T.R. Genetic dissection reveals two separate retinal substrates for polarization vision in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 2012;22:12–20. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wakakuwa M., Kurasawa M., Giurfa M., Arikawa K. Spectral heterogeneity of honeybee ommatidia. Naturwissenschaften. 2005;92:464–467. doi: 10.1007/s00114-005-0018-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Spaethe J., Briscoe A.D. Molecular characterization and expression of the UV opsin in bumblebees: three ommatidial subtypes in the retina and a new photoreceptor organ in the lamina. J. Exp. Biol. 2005;208:2347–2361. doi: 10.1242/jeb.01634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Meyer-Rochow V.B. Electrophysiology and histology of the eye of the bumblebee Bombus hortorum (L.) (Hymenoptera: Apidae) J. R. Soc. N. Z. 1981;11:123–153. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Ribi W., Warrant E., Zeil J. The organization of honeybee ocelli: regional specializations and rhabdom arrangements. Arthropod Struct. Dev. 2011;40:509–520. doi: 10.1016/j.asd.2011.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Wehner R., Bernard G.D. Photoreceptor twist: a solution to the false-color problem. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1993;90:4132–4135. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.9.4132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Macuda T., Gegear R.J., Laverty T.M., Timney B. Behavioural assessment of visual acuity in bumblebees (Bombus impatiens) J. Exp. Biol. 2001;204:559–564. doi: 10.1242/jeb.204.3.559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Jacobs-Jessen U.F. Zur Orientierung der Hummeln und Einiger Anderer Hymenopteren. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 1959;41:597–641. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Fenster C.B., Armbruster W.S., Dudash M.R. Specialization of flowers: is floral orientation an overlooked first step? New Phytol. 2009;183:502–506. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8137.2009.02852.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Rands S.A., Glover B.J., Whitney H.M. Floral epidermal structure and flower orientation: getting to grips with awkward flowers. Arthropod-Plant Interact. 2011;5:279–285. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Kay Q., Daoud H., Stirton C. Pigment distribution, light reflection and cell structure in petals. Bot. J. Linn. Soc. 1981;83:57–84. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Horváth G., Gál J., Labhart T., Wehner R. Does reflection polarization by plants influence colour perception in insects? Polarimetric measurements applied to a polarization-sensitive model retina of Papilio butterflies. J. Exp. Biol. 2002;205:3281–3298. doi: 10.1242/jeb.205.21.3281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Whitney H.M.H.M., Kolle M., Andrew P., Chittka L., Steiner U., Glover B.J. Floral iridescence, produced by diffractive optics, acts as a cue for animal pollinators. Science. 2009;323:130–133. doi: 10.1126/science.1166256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Whitney H.M., Kolle M., Alvarez-Fernandez R., Steiner U., Glover B.J. Contributions of iridescence to floral patterning. Commun. Integr. Biol. 2009;2:230–232. doi: 10.4161/cib.2.3.8084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Whitney H.M., Chittka L., Bruce T.J.A., Glover B.J. Conical epidermal cells allow bees to grip flowers and increase foraging efficiency. Curr. Biol. 2009;19:948–953. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.04.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Chittka L., Dyer A.G., Bock F., Dornhaus A. Psychophysics: bees trade off foraging speed for accuracy. Nature. 2003;424:388. doi: 10.1038/424388a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Hartz S.M., Ben-Shahar Y., Tyler M. Logistic growth curve analysis in associative learning data. Anim. Cogn. 2001;3:185–189. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Bolker, B.M. (2013). Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using Eigen and S4. https://github.com/lme4/lme4/.
- 42.Bates, D. (2011). Linear Mixed Model Implementation in lme4. http://www.icesi.edu.co/CRAN/web/packages/lme4/vignettes/Implementation.pdf.
- 43.Akaike H. A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Trans. Automat. Contr. 1974;19:716–723. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Coffin, D. (2013) Decoding Raw Digital Images in Linux. http://www.cybercom.net/∼dcoffin/dcraw/.
- 45.Schneider C.A., Rasband W.S., Eliceiri K.W. NIH Image to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis. Nat. Methods. 2012;9:671–675. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Thévenaz P., Ruttimann U.E., Unser M. A pyramid approach to subpixel registration based on intensity. IEEE Trans. Image Process. 1998;7:27–41. doi: 10.1109/83.650848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.