Abstract
Purpose
To examine the relationship between country-level age norms for sexual initiation timing and early sexual initiation (ESI) among adolescent boys and girls.
Methods
Nationally-representative data from 17 countries that participated in the 2006/07 European Social Survey (ESS-3, n=33,092) and the 2005/06 Health Behaviour in School-Aged Children Study (HBSC, n=27,702) were analyzed. Age norms were measured as the average country-level response to an item asking the age at which ESS respondents believed someone is too young to have sexual intercourse. HBSC respondents (aged 14-16) self-reported age at sexual initiation which we defined as early (<15 years) or not (≥15 years or no initiation). Control variables included age, family affluence, perceived socioeconomic status, family living arrangement, substance use, school attachment, and country-level legal age of consent. Multivariable three-level logistic models with random intercepts were run separately by sex.
Results
In multivariable analyses, higher overall age norms were associated with reduced likelihood of ESI among girls (AOR 0.60, 95% CI 0.45-0.79); associations with ESI were stronger for parent cohort (ages 31-65) norms (AOR 0.37, 95% CI 0.23-0.58) than for peer cohort (ages 15-20) norms (AOR 0.60, 95% CI 0.49-0.74). For boys, overall norms were also significantly negatively associated with ESI (AOR 0.68, 95% CI 0.46-0.99), as were parent cohort norms (AOR 0.66, 95% CI 0.45-0.96). Peer cohort norms were not significantly related to boys’ ESI.
Conclusion
Macro-level cultural norms may impact adolescents’ sexual initiation timing. Research exploring the sexual health outcomes of early initiators in countries with contrasting age norms is warranted.
Keywords: Europe, adolescents, sexual behavior, cross-national comparison, culture, multilevel modeling
INTRODUCTION
Sexual initiation is a significant transitional point for adolescents,1 and its timing can impact their health and well-being. Earlier sexual initiation (ESI) has been linked with increased risk for sexually transmitted infections (STIs) and pregnancy during adolescence, short-term increases in depressive symptoms among girls, lower educational attainment by early adulthood, greater sex partner accumulation, and risky sexual practices in adulthood.2-7 Younger initiators’ increased STI and pregnancy risk is largely due to their more inconsistent use of condoms and other contraceptives.8 Although many studies document individual, familial and peer group risk factors for ESI, rates of ESI differ considerably across countries after accounting for these factors.9-11 A 2005/06 study revealed that among 14 to 16 year olds in Europe, Canada and Israel, prevalence of ever having sexual intercourse varied from a low of 12% in Slovakia to 61% in Greenland.12 Research explaining this cross-national variation is scarce.
Researchers have speculated that differences in socio-cultural norms may contribute to cross-national variation in adolescent sexual initiation timing.9,10,13 Although studies have found that individual-level perceived sexual behavior norms (i.e., individual perceptions of peer sexual involvement and peer beliefs about appropriate timing for sexual initiation) are among the strongest predictors of ESI,14,15 no studies have examined norms at a macro-social level. According to ecological systems theory,16 adolescent behavior is influenced by contextual factors at different levels, including macro-system influences such as cultural values, customs, and laws. The macro system likely influences youths’ sexual socialization through shaping cultural values expressed by socialization agents such as family and peers.
Two qualitative studies have examined the influence of country-level cultural norms on adolescent reproductive health, including ESI. Although substantive differences between the U.S. and other (primarily European) nations were found in the acceptability of adolescent sexual behavior, similarity across countries in the proportion of adolescents who were sexually active seemed to suggest cultural norms had little influence on adolescent sexual behavior.13,17 However, these studies only included a small number of countries (two and five, respectively), and mainly focused on contrasting Western and Northern European nations with the U.S.
As highlighted by Bronfenbrenner,16 socio-cultural norms at the macro-level may interact with individual factors, such as biological sex, in influencing behavior. Recent studies highlight a persistent sexual double standard – boys are permitted greater sexual freedom than girls.18 Given this double-standard, societal norms may be more strongly related to sexual initiation timing for girls versus boys.
In the present study, we examined the influence of socio-cultural norms on adolescents’ likelihood of early sexual initiation. We conducted a test of macro-level processes and influences on adolescent behavior, one that is rarely encountered in the empirical literature.19 We addressed the following research questions:
Do norms about the age at which it is acceptable for adolescents to become sexually active explain cross-national variability in ESI among adolescents?
Does this association vary by biological sex?
METHODS
Data
Data from the European Social Survey (ESS; 2006-07) and the Health Behaviour in School-aged Children Study (HBSC; 2005-06) were utilized. The ESS is a multi-country cross-sectional survey conducted biennially designed to track the attitudes, beliefs and behavior of Europeans.20 Twenty-five countries participated in 2006-07. Probability samples of the resident national population aged 15 or older living in private households were drawn in each country, and face-to-face interviews were conducted. Across the 17 countries included in this analysis, the median response rate was 64.5% (range 46%-73%).
The HBSC study was conducted in 41 primarily European nations in 2005-06 in collaboration with the World Health Organization Regional Office for Europe. HBSC was designed to examine the health and health behaviors of adolescents across country contexts.12 Cluster sampling, with classes as the primary sampling units, was used to select nationally representative samples (in most countries). Data collection was conducted during the 2005-06 school year; students completed anonymous questionnaires within classrooms. All countries adhered to a strict international protocol for sampling and data collection. School response rates varied by country (from 47% to 100%, but >70% for 14 of 18 included countries). Student response rates also varied by country but were >70% for most of the countries included here.21 The present analysis was deemed exempt from review by the Tulane University Institutional Review Board.
Our sample is limited to the seventeen European countries that participated in both HBSC 2005-06 and ESS-3 (see Table 1). The separate HBSC country surveys conducted in England, Scotland and Wales were combined into Great Britain for the current analysis to align with the ESS, which was conducted in the United Kingdom as a whole. Sampled countries vary in culture, religion, economic systems, adolescent access to sexual health services, and adolescent sexual behavior.12,22
Table 1.
Female | Age | High Family Affluence | High Perceived SES | Living With Both Biological Parents | Girls’ Early Sexual Initiationa | Boys’ Early Sexual Initiationa | Girl vs. Boy difference in ESI prevalenceb | Substance Use | School attachment | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
% | Mean | % | % | % | % | % | % | Mean | Mean | |
TOTAL | 53.1 | 15.1 | 37.9 | 49.1 | 71.3 | 13.5 | 15.4 | -1.9 | -0.3 | -0.1 |
| ||||||||||
North | 51.9 | 15.2 | 45.3 | 52.8 | 66.4 | 18.3 | 14.6 | 3.7 | -0.2 | -0.1 |
| ||||||||||
Denmark | 52.0 | 15.2 | 51.1 | 21.4 | 54.4 | 36.0 | 31.8 | 4.2 | -0.0 | 0.2 |
Estonia | 50.0 | 15.3 | 24.0 | 55.6 | 64.0 | 8.5 | 10.9 | -2.4 | -0.2 | -0.1 |
Finland | 55.2 | 15.3 | 38.7 | 64.2 | 69.6 | 14.9 | 11.2 | 3.7 | -0.4 | -0.3 |
Great Britain | 51.4 | 15.2 | 49.2 | 51.0 | 67.9 | 17.3 | 12.3 | 5.0 | -0.1 | -0.1 |
Sweden | 51.6 | 15.0 | 58.5 | 73.7 | 72.0 | 19.2 | 13.2 | 6.0 | -0.9 | 0.2 |
| ||||||||||
East | 56.8 | 15.1 | 11.3 | 51.8 | 69.5 | 8.5 | 17.8 | -9.3 | -0.1 | -0.5 |
| ||||||||||
Bulgaria | 56.8 | 15.1 | 14.0 | 36.5 | 78.6 | 13.8 | 25.6 | -11.8 | 0.0 | -0.8 |
Hungary | 55.4 | 15.0 | 21.9 | 29.7 | 74.8 | 9.5 | 11.8 | -2.3 | 0.3 | -0.2 |
Russia | 58.0 | 15.1 | 8.6 | 92.2 | 61.2 | 7.3 | 16.8 | -9.5 | -0.3 | -0.6 |
Ukraine | 56.3 | 15.3 | 5.7 | 28.8 | 68.7 | 4.9 | 16.2 | -11.3 | 0.0 | -0.4 |
| ||||||||||
South | 55.7 | 15.1 | 40.3 | 47.5 | 82.4 | 7.5 | 13.2 | -5.7 | -0.6 | 0.3 |
| ||||||||||
Portugal | 58.0 | 15.1 | 34.9 | 40.6 | 80.9 | 9.2 | 16.1 | -6.9 | -0.8 | 0.3 |
Slovenia | 53.7 | 15.1 | 45.0 | 54.0 | 83.7 | 5.7 | 10.8 | -5.1 | -0.3 | 0.2 |
| ||||||||||
West | 51.2 | 15.0 | 46.4 | 44.3 | 74.3 | 13.9 | 15.6 | -1.7 | -0.4 | 0.1 |
| ||||||||||
Austria | 55.3 | 14.7 | 40.1 | 57.5 | 76.8 | 19.7 | 22.0 | -2.3 | 0.1 | 0.2 |
Belgium | 48.5 | 15.0 | 44.7 | 30.8 | 71.2 | 11.6 | 13.2 | -1.6 | -0.4 | -0.1 |
France | 50.5 | 15.1 | 49.4 | 54.5 | 72.7 | 12.0 | 19.7 | -7.7 | -0.5 | -0.2 |
Germany | 50.8 | 14.9 | 46.9 | 51.0 | 73.0 | 15.1 | 13.8 | 1.3 | -0.5 | 0.2 |
Netherlands | 51.7 | 15.0 | 48.8 | 24.2 | 78.2 | 15.5 | 12.0 | 3.5 | -0.4 | 0.2 |
Switzerland | 52.3 | 15.0 | 47.8 | 42.1 | 77.3 | 10.2 | 12.9 | -2.7 | -0.6 | 0.3 |
Sexual initiation before age 15
Negative numbers indicate greater prevalence of boys’ early initiation, positive numbers indicate greater prevalence of girls’ early initiation
Measures
Outcome – Early Sexual Initiation
Early sexual initiation was defined as initiation before age 15. Comparative research suggests that adolescent girls under fifteen are physiologically unprepared for pregnancy; their cervixes are more vulnerable to STI infection; and they have “lower cognitive capacity for making safe, informed, and voluntary decisions” due to immaturity of the prefrontal cortex below age 15.23 Defining initiation before age 15 as early initiation is consistent with the UNAIDS definition.24 This variable was constructed based on HBSC respondents’ reports of ever having sexual intercourse and reported age at first intercourse.
Predictors – Age Norms
Age norms for sexual initiation were based on an ESS question: “At what age is someone too young to have sexual intercourse?” Participants responded with an age in years. We created three different age norms variables: an overall measure (average of all responses to this question within each country), a youth cohort measure (average response among respondents ages 15-20), and a parent cohort measure (average response among respondents ages 31-65). Higher responses indicated less tolerance for ESI. Using formulae provided by Ludtke et al., we found acceptable levels of reliability within countries for age norms.25,a
Individual-level controls
Age was included as a continuous variable, since likelihood of sexual initiation increases with age. The HBSC Family Affluence Scale (FAS), based on respondents’ household asset reports, was used to capture family affluence.26 Affluence has been inversely related to likelihood of adolescents’ ESI.9 Scores ranged from zero to seven, categorized as low, medium and high affluence, based on published guidelines.26 Perceived socioeconomic status, was based on respondents’ reports of how well-off their family is on a five-point scale (very well off to not at all well off), collapsed to create a three-level variable (better off, average, worse off). Family living arrangement (both biologic parents, stepfamily, single parent, or other) was included because living with two biologic parents, contrasted to other living arrangements, has been associated with decreased odds of ESI among adolescents.9 School attachment was based on a polychoric principal components analysis of five items describing students’ perceptions of classmates and the general school environment (4- to 5-point likert scales for five items, Cronbach’s α=0.71); higher scores indicate higher school attachment. Substance use was based on a polychoric principal components analysis of frequency of alcohol use (5-point likert scale), frequency of tobacco use (5-point likert scale), and frequency of getting drunk (5-point likert scale, Cronbach’s α=0.65); higher scores indicate greater substance use. School attachment has been linked with a lower odds of ESI, while substance use has been linked with a higher odds.9
Country-level control
We included one country-level control variable, legal age of consent, coded based on a 2000 article,27 but updated for countries where laws changed between 1999-2006.28 The correlation between legal age of consent and age norms was moderate (r=0.45, p=0.06).
Analysis Sample
We applied three inclusion criteria. First, we limited to students targeted for the 15-year-old sample, as those were the only students asked about sexual behavior (n=67,872). Second, we limited to HBSC participants in the 17 countries where the ESS-3 was also conducted (n=33,912). We then limited to students who had complete data on all analysis covariates (n=27,702). Missingness varied across variables: ESI (12.3%); age (0%); family living arrangement (0%); family affluence (3.0%); perceived socioeconomic status (2.0%); substance use (1.6%); and school attachment (3.4%). Compared to those excluded, included respondents were significantly more likely to report living with both biologic parents (71.3% vs. 61.1%), high family affluence (37.9% vs. 34.1%), and less likely to report high perceived socioeconomic status (49.1% vs. 53.7%) and ESI (14.4% vs. 19.7%). Included respondents also indicated higher school attachment (-0.10 vs. -0.15) and lower substance use (-0.31 vs. -0.09).
Analysis
All analyses were performed in Stata version 10 (StataCorp LP, College Station, Texas), with individuals (n=27,702) as the units of analysis. Individuals were nested in schools (n=2,177), which were nested in countries (n=17). We began by examining univariate distributions of variables across countries (i.e., proportions and means). Stratifying by biological sex, we then examined crude bivariate relationships between ESI and covariates. For individual-level variables, we used single-level binary logistic models with standard error corrections for non-independence within schools and countries. For country-level variables, we used multilevel models with random intercepts for schools and countries.
In the last step, we conducted multivariable multilevel logistic regression analyses separately for boys and girls. First, a null or empty model quantified the amount of variability in ESI at the individual, school and country levels. Using an estimated level-one variance of π2/3,29 we calculated country-level intra-class correlations (ICC). We also calculated a Median Odds Ratio (MOR), which represents the increased risk that (in median) one would have if moving to another area with a higher risk when randomly picking two areas.30 In the second model, we added individual-level controls. In the third model, we added overall age norms to assess their relationship with ESI after controlling for individual factors. We then repeated this step for peer cohort age norms and parent cohort age norms separately (models 4 and 5). Any controls not significantly associated with ESI in bivariate analyses for both boys and girls were not included in multivariable models.
RESULTS
Descriptive Results
Individual-level characteristics of HBSC study respondents and country characteristics are presented in Table 1. The sample was 53.1% female with an average age of 15.1 years. Nearly 40% were classified as high affluence, 49.1% reported high perceived socioeconomic status and 71.3% lived with both biologic parents. Across countries, 13.5% of girls and 15.4% of boys reported having experienced ESI. Prevalence of ESI ranged from 4.9% in Ukraine to 36% in Denmark for girls, and from 10.8% in Slovenia to 31.8% in Denmark for boys. Perceptions of when a person is too young to have sex varied from 15.2 years in Austria to 17.3 in the Ukraine (Table 2). Across countries, norms among parent cohorts were more conservative than youth cohorts’ norms. Legal age of consent varied from 13 to16, with a mean of 14.8.
Table 2.
Age Norms for Sexual Initiation Timinga Overall Mean | Age Norms for Sexual Initiation Timingb Youth Cohort Mean | Age Norms for Sexual Initiation Timingc Parent Cohort Mean | Legal Age of Consent | |
---|---|---|---|---|
TOTAL | 16.3 | 15.5 | 16.3 | 14.8 |
| ||||
North | 16.4 | 15.5 | 16.4 | 15.2 |
| ||||
Denmark | 15.9 | 15.1 | 15.8 | 15 |
Estonia | 16.7 | 15.8 | 16.7 | 14 |
Finland | 16.7 | 15.6 | 16.7 | 16 |
Great Britain | 16.6 | 15.9 | 16.6 | 16 |
Sweden | 16.1 | 14.9 | 16.1 | 15 |
| ||||
East | 16.9 | 16.4 | 16.9 | 15.0 |
| ||||
Bulgaria | 16.6 | 15.9 | 16.6 | 14 |
Hungary | 16.7 | 16.3 | 16.8 | 14 |
Russia | 16.8 | 16.3 | 16.9 | 16c |
Ukraine | 17.3 | 17.0 | 17.1 | 16 |
| ||||
South | 16.2 | 15.3 | 16.1 | 15.0 |
| ||||
Portugal | 15.9 | 15.1 | 15.7 | 14 |
Slovenia | 16.4 | 15.4 | 16.5 | 15d |
| ||||
West | 15.9 | 15.2 | 15.9 | 15.0 |
| ||||
Austria | 15.2 | 14.5 | 15.4 | 14 |
Belgium | 16.3 | 15.3 | 16.3 | 16 |
France | 16.1 | 15.3 | 16.2 | 15 |
Germany | 15.4 | 14.8 | 15.4 | 14 |
Netherlands | 16.2 | 15.8 | 16.1 | 16 |
Switzerland | 16.0 | 15.2 | 15.9 | 16 |
Average within-country response to ESS question “At what age is someone is too young to have sexual intercourse?”
Average within-country response to ESS question “At what age is someone is too young to have sexual intercourse?” among those age 15-19
Average within-country response to ESS question “At what age is someone is too young to have sexual intercourse?” among those age 31-65
In 2003, Russia changed the legal age of consent from 14 to 16
In 1999, Slovenia raised their legal age of consent from age 14 to age 15
In 1999, Spain raised their legal age of consent from age 12 to 13
Bivariate Results
Crude associations are presented in Table 3. For both boys and girls, increased odds of ESI were associated with younger age, family structures other than two biologic parents, lower perceived socioeconomic status, and higher substance use. For boys, higher perceived socioeconomic status was associated with increased odds of ESI. For girls, both lower affluence and school attachment were associated with reduced odds of ESI. Girls living in countries with higher age norms had lower odds of ESI (OR 0.53, 95% CI 0.33-0.83); parent cohort norms (OR 0.50, 95% CI 0.32-0.78) were slightly more negatively related to girls’ ESI than were youth cohort norms (OR 0.58, 95% CI 0.40-0.83). None of the age norms variables were significantly associated with boys’ odds of ESI in crude analyses. Country-level legal age of consent was also unrelated to ESI for both girls and boys.
Table 3.
Adolescent Girls (n=14,697) | Adolescent Boys (n=13,005) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
N ESI/N Total Sample (% ESI) | Crude OR (95% CI) | N ESI/ N Total Sample (% ESI) | Crude OR (95%CI) | |
Individual covariatesa | ||||
Age | ||||
14 years old | 102 / 601 (17.0) | 1.30(1.03-1.64)* | 110 / 537 (20.5) | 1.41 (1.12-1.78)** |
15 years old | 1,643 / 12,078 (13.6) | Ref. | 1,618 / 10,494 (15.4) | Ref. |
16 years old | 245 / 2,018 (12.1) | 0.88(0.76-1.02) | 280 / 1,947 (14.2) | 0.91 (0.79-1.05) |
Family affluence | ||||
Low | 342 / 3,293 (10.4) | 0.70 (0.62—0.80)*** | 328 / 2,277 (14.4) | 0.91 (0.79—1.05) |
Medium | 896 / 6,330 (14.2) | Ref. | 825 / 5,304 (15.6) | Ref. |
High | 752 / 5,074 (14.8) | 1.06 (0.94—1.18) | 855 / 5,424 (15.8) | 1.02 (0.91—1.14) |
Perceived socioeconomic status | ||||
Lower than average | 243 / 1,265 (19.4) | 1.55 (1.32—1.83)*** | 168 / 892 (18.8) | 1.42 (1.18—1.71)*** |
Average | 881 / 6,639 (12.6) | Ref. | 743 / 5,293 (14.0) | Ref. |
Better off than average | 866 / 6,793 (12.9) | 0.95(0.92—1.15) | 1,097 / 6,820 (16.1) | 1.17 (1.05—1.31)** |
Family living structure | ||||
With both bio. parents | 1,106 / 10,325 (10.7) | Ref. | 1,308 / 9,413 (13.9) | Ref. |
Stepfamily | 355 / 1,535 (23.1) | 2.50 (2.21—2.84)*** | 203 / 1,066 (19.0) | 1.46 (1.23—1.72)*** |
Single parent | 412 / 2,319 (17.8) | 1.80 (1.60—2.03)*** | 394 / 2,043 (19.3) | 1.48 (1.31—1.67)*** |
Other | 117 / 518 (22.6) | 2.43 (1.92—3.09)*** | 103 / 483 (21.3) | 1.68 (1.36—2.08)*** |
Substance use (Mean (sd)) | ||||
ESI | 0.67 (1.01) | 3.07 (2.90—3.25)*** | 0.59 (1.14) | 2.22 (2.12—2.34)*** |
Not ESI | -0.53 (0.95) | Ref. | -0.41 (1.03) | |
School attachment (Mean (sd)) | ||||
ESI | -0.31 (1.34) | 0.88 (0.85—0.92)*** | -0.10 (1.32) | 0.97 (0.93—1.01) |
Not ESI | -0.10 (1.29) | Ref. | -0.05 (1.21) | |
County-level predictorsb | ||||
Overall age norms (Mean (sd)) | ||||
ESI | 16.22 (0.49) | 0.53 (0.33—0.83)** | 16.26 (0.51) | 0.80 (0.56—1.14) |
Not ESI | 16.34 (0.50) | Ref. | 16.32 (0.49) | Ref. |
Youth cohort age norms (Mean (sd)) | ||||
ESI | 15.45(0.55) | 0.58 (0.40—0.83)** | 15.53(0.61) | 0.90 (0.66—1.21) |
Not ESI | 15.61(0.62) | Ref. | 15.57(0.59) | Ref. |
Parent cohort age norms (Mean (sd)) | ||||
ESI | 16.22(0.49) | 0.50 (0.32—0.78)** | 16.28(0.51) | 0.79 (0.55—1.13) |
Not ESI | 16.36(0.50) | Ref. | 16.33(0.49) | Ref. |
Legal age of consent | ||||
13/14 | 592/4,559 (13.0) | Ref. | 637/3,899 (16.3) | Ref. |
15 | 510/2,949 (17.3) | 1.34 (0.66— 2.70) | 520/2,748 (18.9) | 1.13 (0.71—1.80) |
16 | 888/7,189 (12.4) | 0.86 (0.47—1.57) | 851/6,358 (13.4) | 0.79 (0.53—1.17) |
NOTES: ESI = Early sexual initiation; OR = Odds Ratio; CI = Confidence Interval
p<.05
p<.01
p<.001
OR estimates derived using single-level logistic regression analyses
OR estimates derived using three-level logistic regression analyses
Multivariable Results - Girls
Multivariable results for girls are presented in Table 4. In the null model, the ICC indicates that approximately 9% of the variability in girls’ ESI is due to differences across country contexts. The MOR indicates that a girl’s odds of ESI would, on median, increase by 71% if moving from a lower risk to a higher risk country picked randomly. In the second model, we added individual-level covariates. Living in a stepfamily, with a single parent, or in another living situation were all associated with increased odds of ESI versus living with both biological parents. Lower perceived socioeconomic status and substance use were significantly positively related to girls’ odds of ESI, while school attachment was significantly negatively related to ESI. After incorporating these variables, unexplained between-country variance in ESI increased by 40.6%. In the third model, overall country-level age norms were negatively associated with girls’ ESI (AOR 0.60, 95% CI 0.45-0.79). Overall norms explained the between-country variance gained from adding the individual-level variables in Model 2. Parent cohort norms (AOR 0.37, 95% CI 0.23-0.58) were more strongly negatively related to girls’ ESI than were peer cohort norms (AOR 0.60, 95% CI 0.49-0.74) (models 4 and 5), with parent cohort norms also explaining more between country variance in ESI (38.0% vs. 18.8%).
Table 4.
Model 1: Null | Model 2: Individual controls | Model 3: Individual controls + overall age norms | Model 4: Individual controls + youth cohort age norms | Model 5: Individual controls + parent cohort age norms | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
AOR (95% CI) | AOR (95% CI) | AOR (95% CI) | AOR (95% CI) | AOR (95% CI) | |
Individual Predictors | |||||
Age | -- | 0.75(0.64—0.86)*** | 0.76 (0.65—0.88)*** | 0.76 (0.65—0.88)*** | 0.76 (0.65—0.88)*** |
Family Affluence | -- | ||||
Low | 0.92(0.78—1.09) | 0.92 (0.78—1.09) | 0.93 (0.78—1.09) | 0.92 (0.78—1.09) | |
Medium | Ref. | Ref. | Ref. | Ref. | |
High | 0.99(0.87—1.13) | 0.99 (0.87—1.13) | 0.99 (0.87—1.13) | 0.99 (0.87—1.13) | |
Perceived SES | -- | ||||
Lower than average | 1.28(1.06—1.56)* | 1.28 (1.05—1.56)* | 1.28 (1.05—1.56)* | 1.28 (1.05—1.56)* | |
Average | Ref. | Ref. | Ref. | Ref. | |
Better off than average | 1.11(0.97—1.26) | 1.11 (0.97—1.26) | 1.11 (0.97—1.26) | 1.11 (0.97—1.26) | |
Living Arrangement | |||||
Both bio. parents | -- | Ref. | Ref. | Ref. | Ref. |
Stepfamily | 1.69(1.44—1.99)*** | 1.70 (1.44—2.00)*** | 1.70 (1.44—2.00)*** | 1.69 (1.44—1.99)*** | |
Single parent | 1.48(1.27—1.73)*** | 1.48 (1.27—1.73)*** | 1.48 (1.27—1.73)*** | 1.48 (1.28—1.73)*** | |
Other | 1.87(1.41—2.48)*** | 1.87 (1.41—2.49)*** | 1.88 (1.42—2.49)*** | 1.87 (1.41—2.48)*** | |
School attachment | -- | 0.89(0.85—0.93)*** | 0.89 (0.85—0.93)*** | 0.89 (0.85—0.93)*** | 0.89 (0.85—0.93)*** |
Substance use | -- | 3.32(3.12—3.53)*** | 3.32 (3.12—3.52)*** | 3.32 (3.12—3.52)*** | 3.32 (3.12—3.53)*** |
Country Predictors | |||||
Overall age Norms | -- | 0.60 (0.45—0.79)*** | -- | -- | |
Youth cohort age norms | -- | -- | 0.60 (0.49—0.74)*** | -- | |
Parent cohort age norms | -- | -- | -- | 0.37 (0.23—0.58)*** | |
Variance Estimates | |||||
School Intercept (se) | 0.40 (0.06) | 0.31 (0.06) | 0.31 (0.06) | 0.31 (0.06) | 0.31 (0.06) |
Country Intercept (se) | 0.32 (0.12) | 0.45 (0.15) | 0.32 (0.11) | 0.26 (0.09) | 0.20 (0.07) |
Statistics | |||||
ICC (Country) | 0.09 | 0.12 | 0.09 | 0.07 | 0.05 |
% change in country intercept variancea | -- | +40.6% | 0% | -18.8% | -38% |
Median Odds Ratio(MOR) | 1.71 | 1.89 | 1.71 | 1.62 | 1.53 |
NOTES: ESI = Early sexual initiation; se=standard error; AOR = Adjusted Odds Ratio; CI = Confidence Interval
p<.05
p<.01
p<.001
Relative to Null model
Multivariable Results - Boys
Results for boys are presented in Table 5. In the null model, the ICC indicates that approximately 4% of the variability in ESI is due to differences across country contexts. The MOR indicates that a boy’s odds of ESI would, on median, increase by 43% if moving from a lower risk to a higher risk country picked randomly. In the second model, age was significantly negatively related to ESI. Living with a single parent, compared to living with both biologic parents, was associated with increased odds. Lower and higher than average perceived socioeconomic status were both associated with increased odds of ESI, while low, compared to medium, affluence was negatively related. After accounting for these individual-level differences, estimated between-country variability in boys’ ESI increased by 28.6%. In the third model, overall country-level age norms were negatively related to boys’ ESI (AOR 0.68, 95% CI 0.46-0.99), and explained the between-country variance gained from adding the individual-level variables in Model 2. When differentiating between effects of youth versus parent cohort norms (Models 4 and 5), only parent cohort norms were negatively related to boys’ ESI (AOR 0.66, 95% CI 0.45-0.96). Parent cohort norms did not explain additional between-country variance in ESI above and beyond that explained by overall age norms.
Table 5.
Model 1: Null | Model 2: Individual controls | Model 3: Individual controls + overall age norms | Model 4: Individual controls + youth cohort age norms | Model 5: Individual controls + parent cohort age norms | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
AOR (95% CI) | AOR (95% CI) | AOR (95% CI) | AOR (95% CI) | AOR (95% CI) | |
Individual Predictors | |||||
Age | -- | 0.74(0.64—0.84)*** | 0.74(0.65—0.84)*** | 0.74(0.65—0.85)*** | 0.74(0.65—0.85)*** |
Family Affluence | -- | ||||
Low | 0.80(0.68—0.95)* | 0.81(0.69—0.96)* | 0.81(0.69—0.96)* | 0.81(0.69—0.96)* | |
Medium | Ref. | Ref. | Ref. | Ref. | |
High | 0.98(0.86—1.11) | 0.97(0.86—1.11) | 0.98(0.86—1.11) | 0.97(0.86—1.10) | |
Perceived SES | -- | ||||
Lower than average | 1.45(1.17—1.79)** | 1.44(1.16—1.78)** | 1.45(1.16—1.78)** | 1.44(1.16—1.78)** | |
Average | Ref. | Ref. | Ref. | Ref. | |
Better off than average | 1.29(1.14—1.46)*** | 1.29(1.14—1.46)*** | 1.29(1.14—1.46)*** | 1.29(1.14—1.47)*** | |
Living Arrangement | |||||
Both bio. parents | -- | Ref. | Ref. | Ref. | Ref. |
Stepfamily | 1.18(0.97—1.42) | 1.18(0.97—1.42) | 1.17(0.97—1.42) | 1.18(0.97—1.42) | |
Single parent | 1.41(1.21—1.63)*** | 1.41(1.21—1.63)*** | 1.41(1.21—1.63)* | 1.41(1.21—1.63)* | |
Other | 1.30(0.99—1.71) | 1.31(0.99—1.72) | 1.31(0.99—1.72) | 1.31(0.99—1.72) | |
School attachment | -- | 1.00(0.96—1.05) | 1.00(0.96—1.05) | 1.00(0.96—1.05) | 1.00(0.96—1.05) |
Substance use | -- | 2.42(2.29—2.55)*** | 2.42(2.30—2.55)*** | 2.42(2.30—2.56)*** | 2.42(2.30—2.56)*** |
Country Predictors | |||||
Overall age Norms | -- | 0.68(0.46—0.99)* | -- | -- | |
Youth cohort age norms | -- | -- | 0.76(0.55—1.04) | -- | |
Parent cohort age norms | -- | -- | -- | 0.66(0.45—0.96)* | |
Variance Estimates | |||||
School Intercept (se) | 0.40 (0.06) | 0.43 (0.07) | 0.42 (0.07) | 0.42 (0.07) | 0.42 (0.07) |
Country Intercept (se) | 0.14 (0.05) | 0.18 (0.07) | 0.14 (0.06) | 0.15 (0.06) | 0.14 (0.05) |
Statistics | |||||
ICC (Country) | 0.04 | 0.05 | 0.04 | 0.04 | 0.04 |
% change in country intercept variancea | -- | +28.6% | 0% | +7.1% | 0% |
Median Odds Ratio(MOR) | 1.43 | 1.50 | 1.43 | 1.44 | 1.43 |
NOTES: ESI = Early sexual initiation; se=standard error; AOR = Adjusted Odds Ratio; CI = Confidence Interval
p<.05
p<.01
p<.001
Relative to Null model
DISCUSSION
Similar to past findings, we found significant differences between European countries in the prevalence of ESI.9,10,31 Particularly noteworthy was the high rate of ESI observed in Denmark (34.1%), which is consistent with past HBSC studies of adolescent sexual activity.12 Although age norms for sexual initiation timing explained part of the between-country differences, substantial between-country variability remained after adjustment. It is possible that age norms are part of a larger cultural system that influences the timing of adolescents’ sexual initiation. It is also possible that health system and demographic factors that vary across countries impact sexual initiation timing. Research exploring possible determinants of age norms, such as country-level religiosity and country GDP and/or income inequality, is warranted.
After controlling for multiple individual-level characteristics, higher overall age norms were associated with reduced odds of ESI, and explained substantial between-country variability in ESI. The impact of such norms on specific health outcomes, however, is unclear. More culturally conservative contexts may constrain some precocious sexual behavior, but also restrict adolescents’ access to sexual health education and clinical services that could prevent negative sexual health outcomes. In a recent analysis of U.S. states, those with higher religiosity and political conservatism evidenced higher teen birth rates.32 Although research is mixed in terms of the effect of individual-level religiosity on contraceptive or condom use,33,34 interventions that promote religiosity to influence sexual behavior may have negative effects on condom and contraceptive use at the macro-level.35 Further research examining the implications of macro-level cultural norms on other adolescent sexual health outcomes, such as contraceptive use, teen pregnancy and STI is warranted.
We found sex differences in the influence of norms on ESI: all three norms measures were more strongly negatively related to girls’ ESI than to boys’. This finding offers further support for differences in sexual standards for adolescent girls and boys identified in past studies.18 Such a double-standard could lead to the negative sequelae of ESI observed for girls but not boys in some contexts, most notably negative psychological symptoms,5,31 peer judgment and rejection.3 These double-standards also may lessen young women’s power in sexual encounters, including their ability to request male partners to use condoms and to refuse unwanted sexual contact.36 Further research should explore the role of sexual norms in producing sex-disparate outcomes of ESI, and their implications for young women’s sexual health.
For both boys and girls, parental cohort norms were more strongly negatively related to adolescents’ ESI than were peer cohort norms. These results are consistent with studies that found adolescents’ perceptions of maternal values for abstinence predict delayed sexual onset, and that relationships and communication with parents about sexual matters can buffer peer influences on adolescents’ sexual initiation.37,38 Our results also extend these findings since we are measuring the general normative context within which parents operate, which may or may not concur with parents’ own values. Future studies that examine the interactive influence of both individual parents’ values and general normative context could further elucidate how and under what conditions social norms at various contextual levels influence adolescent sexual behavior.
Sex differences in associations between peer cohort norms and ESI are consistent with a number of studies which find that group membership is more important for girls than for boys, and that girls are more susceptible than boys to peer norms and peer pressure in other risk behaviors.39,40 However, the association between peer cohort norms and ESI for boys is trending in the expected direction; therefore, lack of statistical significance may merely reflect lack of power at the country level. Future analyses including more countries could further test these sex differences.
Legal age of consent was only borderline correlated with country-level age norms, and was unassociated with ESI for both boys and girls. It is possible that the narrow range of legal ages of consent (13-16) contributed to this. However, this may also indicate that laws are not consistent with popular opinion. Findings emphasize the importance of measuring both structural and social aspects of norms, which may differentially impact health outcomes.
Despite the study’s strengths, such as the use of large, nationally-representative datasets, inclusion of more countries compared to past qualitative studies, and the use of multilevel analyses to quantitatively estimate the effects of age norms on ESI, findings should be interpreted with knowledge of its limitations. First, although we incorporated data from a much larger set of countries than previously studied,13,17 our country sample size may result in an under-estimate of between-country variance, and an overestimate of the standard errors for age norms. Second, our study is cross-sectional, and therefore causal inference should be made with caution. However, given that we measure norms reported by respondents other than the adolescents reporting sexual behavior, we do avoid some potential problems of endogeneity (i.e., ESI influencing adolescents’ perceptions of norms). Third, age at sexual initiation is self-reported and subject to reporting biases potentially related to cultural norms. If ESI is under-reported in countries with higher age norms, then the association between age norms and ESI may be overestimated. Fourth, we focus on a simple measure of sexual initiation timing, without exploring subsequent sexual behavior or the couple context of initiation. Future studies that examine whether sociocultural norms are related to post-initiation sexual behavior trajectories and/or whether first sex is voluntary are warranted. Fifth, although the question was meant to capture vaginal penetrative sex acts, it is possible some adolescents interpreted this question differently. Sixth, use of a complete case analysis may result in selectivity biases. Seventh, the age norms measure did not distinguish between boys and girls, for whom norms may be different. Also the norms question is negatively worded (i.e., when someone is “too young to have sex,”) which may result in different responses than a positively-worded question would (i.e., when someone is “old enough to have sex.”) Finally, we were unable to control for some potentially important individual confounders, such as childhood sexual abuse or whether the respondent’s mother was a teen mother. If future datasets become available that include such measures, replications of our analyses including these factors would be warranted.
In conclusion, likelihood of ESI was lower among adolescents living in countries with older age norms for sexual initiation timing compared to adolescents living in countries with younger age norms. Additionally, norms among parent-aged adults seem especially influential for teens, compared to teen norms. Results support the influence of macro-level cultural norms on the sexual behavior of adolescents. Future research should examine whether such norms increase the risk associated with earlier sexual initiation through, for example, reduced condom and contraceptive use. Further, studying interactions between macro-level norms and more proximal determinants of adolescent sexual behavior (e.g., parent-reported norms) may inform intervention opportunities across a variety of sociocultural contexts.
IMPLICATIONS AND CONTRIBUTION.
Adolescents’ likelihood of initiating sex early (<15 years) is lower in countries with higher age norms for sexual initiation timing, with parent cohort norms (ages 31-65) more strongly related than youth cohort (ages 15-20) norms. This study contributes knowledge of the relationship between macro-level social environments and adolescents’ sexual behavior.
Acknowledgments
Health Behaviour in School-Aged Children (HBSC) is an international study carried out in collaboration with the World Health Organization Regional Office for Europe. The International Coordinator of the 2005–2006 study was Candace Currie, University of St Andrews, Scotland; the Data Bank Manager was Oddrun Samdal, University of Bergen, Norway. The study included data from the following countries (whose principal investigators appear in parentheses): Austria (Rosemarie Felder-Puig), Belgium (Carine Vereecken & Danielle Piette), Bulgaria (Lidiya Vasileva), Denmark (Pernille Due), England (Antony Morgan), Estonia (Katrin Aasvee), Finland (Jorma Tynjälä), France (Emmanuelle Godeau), Germany (Petra Kolip), Hungary (Ágnes Németh), The Netherlands (Wilma A.M. Vollebergh), Portugal (Margarida Gaspar de Matos), Russian Federation (Alexander Malinin), Scotland (Candace Currie), Slovenia (Helena Jericek), Spain (Carmen Moreno), Sweden (Lilly Augustine), Switzerland (Emmanuel Kuntsche), Ukraine (Olga Balakireva), and Wales (Chris Roberts). This research was supported in part by grant T76MC04927 from the Maternal and Child Health Bureau of the Health Resources and Services Administration and by grant HHSN2672008000009C from the Intramural Research Program of the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development and the Maternal and Child Health Bureau of the Health Resources and Services Administration.
Footnotes
For age norms we calculated the ICC(2) to equal 0.99. Ludke, et al. (2006) suggest an ICC(2) above 0.70 indicates acceptable reliability.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Carpenter L. Virginity Lost: An Intimate Portrait of First Sexual Experiences. New York: New York University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Kaestle CE, Halpern CT, Miller WC, Ford CA. Young age at first sexual intercourse and sexually transmitted infections in adolescents and young adults. Am J Epidemiol. 2005 Apr 15;161(8):774–780. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwi095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Shoveller JA, Johnson JL, Langille DB, Mitchell T. Socio-cultural influences on young people’s sexual development. Soc Sci Med. 2004 Aug;59(3):473–487. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2003.11.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Spriggs AL, Halpern CT. Timing of sexual debut and initiation of postsecondary education in early adulthood. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health. 2008;40(3):152–161. doi: 10.1363/4015208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Spriggs AL, Halpern CT. Sexual debut timing and depressive symptoms in emerging adulthood. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2008;37(9):1085–1096. doi: 10.1007/s10964-008-9303-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Coker AL, Richter DL, Valois RF, McKeown RE, Garrison CZ, Vincent ML. Correlates and consequences of early initiation of sexual intercourse. J Sch Health. 1994 Nov;64(9):372–377. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.1994.tb06208.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Sandfort TGM, Orr M, Hirsch JS, Santelli J. Long-Term Health Correlates of Timing of Sexual Debut: Results From a National US Study. American Journal of Public Health. 2008 Jan 01;98(1):155–161. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2006.097444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.O’Donnell L, O’Donnell CR, Stueve A. Early sexual initiation and subsequent sex-related risks among urban minority youth: the reach for health study. Fam Plann Perspect. 2001 Nov-Dec;33(6):268–275. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Madkour AS, Farhat T, Halpern CT, Godeau E, Nic Gabhainn S. Early adolescent sexual initiation as a problem behavior: A comparative study of five nations. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2010;47(4):389–398. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.02.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Madkour AS, Farhat T, Halpern CT, Nic Gabhainn S, Godeau E. Parents’ Support and Knowledge of Their Daughters’ Lives, and Females’ Early Sexual Initiation In Nine European Countries. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health. 2012;44(3):167–175. doi: 10.1363/4416712. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Raj A, Silverman J, Amaro H. The Relationship Between Sexual Abuse and Sexual Risk Among High School Students: Findings from the 1997 Massachusetts Youth Risk Behavior Survey. Matern Child Health J. 2000 Jun 01;4(2):125–134. doi: 10.1023/a:1009526422148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Currie C, Nic Gabhainn S, Godeau E, et al. Inequalities in young people’s health: Health Behaviour in School-aged Children (HBSC) international report from the 2005/2006 survey. Copenhagen, Denmark: WHO Regional Office for Europe; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Darroch JE, Frost JJ, Singh S. Teenage sexual and reproductive health in developed countries: Can more progress be made? New York: The Alan Guttmacher Institute; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 14.O’Donnell L, Myint UA, O’Donnell CR, Stueve A. Long-term influence of sexual norms and attitudes on timing of sexual initiation among urban minority youth. J Sch Health. 2003 Feb;73(2):68–75. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2003.tb03575.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Santelli JS, Kaiser J, Hirsch L, Radosh A, Simkin L, Middlestadt S. Initiation of sexual intercourse among middle school adolescents: the influence of psychosocial factors. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2004;34(3):200–208. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2003.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bronfenbrenner U. The Ecology of Human Development: Experiments by Nature and Design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1979. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Schalet AT. Must we fear adolescent sexuality? Medscape General Medicine. 2004:44. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kraeger DA, Staff J. The sexual double standard and adolescent peer acceptance. Social Psychological Quarterly. 2009;72:143–164. doi: 10.1177/019027250907200205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Vazsonyi AT, Chen P, Jenkins DD, Burcu E, Torrente G, Sheu CJ. Jessor’s problem behavior theory: Cross-national evidence from Hungary, the Netherlands, Slovenia, Spain, Switzerland, Taiwan, Turkey, and the United States. Developmental Psychology. 2010;46:1779–1791. doi: 10.1037/a0020682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.ESS Round 3: European Social Survey, ESS-3 2006 Documentation Report. Bergen: European Social Survey Data Archive, Norwegian Social Science Data Services; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Roberts C, Freeman J, Samdal O, et al. The Health Behaviour in School-aged Children (HBSC) study: methodological developments and current tensions. International Journal of Public Health. 2009;54(0):140–150. doi: 10.1007/s00038-009-5405-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kontula O. Reproductive health behaviour of young Europeans: The role of education and information. Council of Europe Publishing; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Dixon-Mueller R. How Young is “Too Young”? Comparative Perspectives on Adolescent Sexual, Marital, and Reproductive Transitions. Studies in Family Planning. 2008;39(4):247–262. doi: 10.1111/j.1728-4465.2008.00173.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS (UNAIDS) UNAIDS Report on the global AIDS epidemic. Geneva: UNAIDS; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Lüdtke O, Trautwein U, Kunter M, Baumert J. Reliability and agreement of student ratings of the classroom environment: A reanalysis of TIMSS data. Learning Environments Research. 2006;9(3):215–230. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Currie C, Molcho M, Boyce W, Holstein B, Torsheim T, Richter M. Researching health inequalities in adolescents: the development of the HBSC Family Affluence Scale. Social Science and Medicine. 2008;66(6):1429–1436. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.11.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Graupner H. Sexual consent: The criminal law in Europe and overseas. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2000;29(5):415–461. doi: 10.1023/a:1001986103125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. [7/10/2013];Ages of consent in Europe (Wikipedia) http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Ages_of_consent_in_Europe.
- 29.Snijders TAB, Bosker RJ. Multilevel analysis: an introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. 1. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Merlo J, Chaix B, Ohlsson H, et al. A brief conceptual tutorial of multilevel analysis in social epidemiology: using measures of clustering in multilevel logistic regression to investigate contextual phenomena. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006 Apr;60(4):290–297. doi: 10.1136/jech.2004.029454. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Madkour AS, Farhat T, Halpern CT, Godeau E, Nic Gabhainn S. Early adolescent sexual initiation and physical/psychological symptoms: A comparative analysis of five nations. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2010;39(10):1211–1225. doi: 10.1007/s10964-010-9521-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Cavazos-Rehg PA, Krauss MJ, Spitznagel EL, et al. Associations Between Sexuality Education in Schools and Adolescent Birthrates A State-Level Longitudinal Model. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine. 2012 Feb;166(2):134–140. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.657. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.McCree DH, Wingood GM, DiClemente R, Davies S, Harrington KF. Religiosity and risky sexual behavior in African-American adolescent females. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2003 Jul;33(1):2–8. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(02)00460-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Rosenbaum JE. Patient teenagers? A comparison of the sexual behavior of virginity pledgers and matched nonpledgers. Pediatrics. 2009 Jan;123(1):e110–120. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-0407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Rosenbaum JE. Patient Teenagers? A Comparison of the Sexual Behavior of Virginity Pledgers and Matched Nonpledgers. Pediatrics. 2009 Jan;123(1):E110–E120. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-0407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hird MJ, Jackson S. Where ‘angels’ and ‘wusses’ fear to tread: sexual coercion in adolescent dating relationships. Journal of Sociology. 2001 Mar 1;37(1):27–43. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Fasula AM, Miller KS. African-American and Hispanic adolescents’ intentions to delay first intercourse: parental communication as a buffer for sexually active peers. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2006;38(3):193–200. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Dittus PJ, Jaccard J. Adolescents’ perceptions of maternal disapproval of sex: relationship to sexual outcomes. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2000;26(4):268–278. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(99)00096-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Brown BB. Peer groups and peer cultures. In: Feldman SS, Elliot GR, editors. At the threshold: The developing adolescent. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Press; 1990. pp. 171–196. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Rudolph KD, Conley CS. The Socioemotional Costs and Benefits of Social-Evaluative Concerns: Do Girls Care Too Much? Journal of Personality. 2005;73(1):115–138. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00306.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]