Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Community Psychol. 2014 Jan;42(1):61–79. doi: 10.1002/jcop.21594

Does neighborhood collective efficacy for families change over time? The Boston Neighborhood Survey

Nicole M Schmidt 1,*, Eric J Tchetgen Tchetgen 2, Amy Ehntholt 3, Joanna Almeida 4, Quynh C Nguyen 1, Beth E Molnar 1,5,6, Deborah Azrael 6, Theresa L Osypuk 5,7
PMCID: PMC4066733  NIHMSID: NIHMS572028  PMID: 24976653

Abstract

There is an increased interest in how neighborhood social processes, such as collective efficacy, may protect mental health. Yet little is known about how stable these neighborhood processes are over time, or how to change them to influence other downstream factors. We used a population-based, repeat cross-sectional study of adults (n=5135) to assess stability of collective efficacy for families in 38 Boston neighborhoods across 4 years (2006, 2008, 2010) (the Boston Neighborhood Survey). We test temporal stability of collective efficacy for families across and within neighborhoods using 2-level random effects linear regression, fixed effects linear regression, T-tests, and Wilcoxon rank tests. Across the different methods, neighborhood collective efficacy for families remained stable across 4 years, after adjustment for neighborhood composition. If neighborhood collective efficacy is measured within 4 years of the exposure period of interest, assuming temporal stability may be valid.

Keywords: Neighborhood, neighborhood effects, neighborhood change, collective efficacy, multilevel models

Introduction

A burgeoning literature has documented the association between individual mental health and neighborhood social and structural factors (Kim, 2008; Mair, Roux, & Galea, 2008; Osypuk, Schmidt, et al., 2012; Osypuk, Tchetgen Tchetgen, et al., 2012; Simons et al., 2002; Xue, Leventhal, Brooks-Gunn, & Earls, 2005). Researchers posit three ways in which neighborhoods can affect mental health: 1) norms/social processes, such as neighborhood collective efficacy, minimize negative processes that could affect mental health (Ingoldsby & Shaw, 2002; Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, 2000); 2) institutional resources, such as youth programs, facilitate healthy development (Leventhal, Dupéré, & Gunn, 2009; McLoyd, Jayaratne-Epstein, Ceballo, & Borquez, 1994); or 3) neighborhood context shapes parental well-being, household processes, and parenting styles, such as parental monitoring, that affect child and adolescent development (Caldwell, Rafferty, Reischl, De Loney, & Brooks, 2010; Earls & Carlson, 2001).

The bulk of this literature is relatively new, especially in regards to measuring social processes. Neighborhood collective efficacy is one such social process that has recently been linked to mental health. Studies have shown that collective efficacy interacts with other variables to predict depression (Vega, Ang, Rodriquez, & Finch, 2011) and suicide attempts (Maimon, Browning, & Brooks-Gunn, 2010), and indirectly relates to internalizing behaviors (Dupéré, Leventhal, & Vitaro, 2012). To the extent that neighborhood processes like collective efficacy remain stable over time, it could mean lasting poor mental health outcomes among residents in affected neighborhoods. Prior studies have not demonstrated how to change collective efficacy or other social processes, so even if they causally influence mental health or other outcomes, it may represent a fruitless direction for upstream prevention.

A first step in understanding this causal process is to assess whether neighborhood collective efficacy exposures change over time, including to move beyond the predominantly cross-sectional research evidence. Some evidence of neighborhood effects on mental (and physical) health comes from longitudinal studies (see e.g., Cerdá, Diez-Roux, Tchetgen Tchetgen, Gordon-Larsen, & Kiefe, 2010; Nandi et al., 2010), including one experimental study of housing mobility (see e.g., Ludwig et al., 2011; Orr et al., 2003; Osypuk, Schmidt, et al., 2012; Osypuk, Tchetgen Tchetgen, et al., 2012). However, the majority of studies have been cross-sectional (Kim, 2008; Mair, et al., 2008).

Theoretically, changes in neighborhood exposures may be generated by virtue of residents themselves moving to different neighborhoods (residential mobility), by place-based fluctuations in resident composition (driven by residential turnover), or by place-based physical or social changes (e.g. neighborhood revitalization), indicating several different approaches for modeling change. Although prior work has demonstrated that most neighborhoods are stable social systems that change slowly unless disrupted by ‘triggering events’ such as disinvestment or demolition (Skogan, 1986), researchers testing the stability of neighborhood characteristics have primarily examined sociodemographic features (Alba, Denton, Leung, & Logan, 1995; Jackson & Mare, 2007; Rohe & Stewart, 1996) facilitated by the collection and dissemination of decennial census data in administrative units (e.g., census tracts) used as proxies for neighborhoods (Chaix, 2009; Messer & Kaufman, 2006). Given the intrinsic limitations of census data for capturing the social characteristics of neighborhoods as they are experienced by residents, many studies of neighborhood effects on health have shifted away from a sole reliance on such administrative data operationalizing neighborhood context using compositional variables (e.g., poverty rate or ethnic composition of census tracts), towards using resident surveys to operationalize neighborhood context. Indeed, such survey-based data may represent the most nuanced means of measuring social processes and social resources in neighborhoods, including collective efficacy (Raudenbush & Sampson, 1999).

Collective efficacy is a measure of how confident residents are that their neighbors trust each other and will work together for the good of the neighborhood (Sampson, Raudenbush, & Earls, 1997). Collective efficacy has roots in Albert Bandura’s work on perceived self-efficacy; extending the notion of “efficacy” to communities, Bandura postulated that the strength of a community could be at least partially attributed to the residents’ beliefs that collective problems can be solved by working together. The stronger this belief, the more effort is invested in collective behaviors for the good of the community (Bandura, 1995). A lesser studied component of collective efficacy that may be particularly relevant to raising children is intergenerational closure, defined as the degree to which parents and children in a neighborhood know and interact with each other (Sampson, Morenoff, & Earls, 1999). We include intergenerational closure in our operationalization of collective efficacy, which we call collective efficacy for families, because we are interested in facets of interconnectedness between adults and youth in communities that are not captured in the traditional measure.

Researchers applying ecometric methods (Raudenbush & Sampson, 1999) to understand neighborhood health effects have documented that neighborhood-level social exposure variables derived from resident surveys are associated with individual outcomes. Empirically, higher neighborhood collective efficacy has been associated with better outcomes for youth and families across a number of domains, including decreased violence in neighborhoods with similar socioeconomic status (Morenoff, Sampson, & Raudenbush, 2001; Sampson, et al., 1997), less gun carrying by youth (Molnar, Miller, Azrael, & Buka, 2004), less delinquency and aggression among youth (Molnar, Cerda, Roberts, & Buka, 2008; Odgers et al., 2009), decreased intimate partner homicide and non-lethal partner violence (Browning, 2002), and decreased perpetration of adolescent dating violence (Jain, Buka, Subramanian, & Molnar, 2010; Rothman et al., 2011). Collective efficacy has also been associated with multiple educational and health-related outcomes (Kawachi & Berkman, 2003), including higher passing rates on standardized math tests among low-income adolescents (Emory, Caughy, Harris, & Franzini, 2008), improved school and substance use outcomes (Coley, Morris, & Hernandez, 2004), later onset of first sexual activity (Browning, Leventhal, & Brooks-Gunn, 2004), better internalizing behavior scores (Xue, et al., 2005), higher birth weight (Buka, Brennan, Rich-Edwards, Raudenbush, & Earls, 2003), better self-rated health (Browning & Cagney, 2002; Wen, Browning, & Cagney, 2003), and reduction in asthma (Cagney & Browning, 2004).

Neighborhood collective efficacy may be dynamic across time as social relationships change or people in the neighborhood move, yet most studies using neighborhood resident surveys to operationalize social context are cross-sectional (Sampson, Morenoff, & Gannon-Rowley, 2002), and are rarely contemporaneous with the measurement of the health outcomes. For example, Buka et al. (2003), Cagney and Browning (2004), Xue et al. (2005), Maimon et al. (2010), and Dupéré et al. (2012) all use data from the Project for Human Development in Chicago Neighborhoods (PHDCN), which collected exposure measures of neighborhood social processes in 1994–1995 across neighborhoods in the city of Chicago. Outcomes, including mental health, whether from PHDCN or alternative sources, were measured anywhere from one to five years after the neighborhood data, requiring the assumption that either 1) neighborhood social context is stable over time, or, 2) that the neighborhood exposure impacts the health outcome across that particular latency period. However, these assumptions have not been tested to our knowledge, and if they are invalid, conclusions drawn from studies that rely on these assumptions may be dubious.

To date, only one study has examined the stability of neighborhood social processes over time. In a recent book on Chicago neighborhoods using data from the PHDCN, Sampson examined whether collective efficacy of Chicago neighborhoods remained stable or changed from 1995 to 2002 using two waves of data (Sampson, 2012). Sampson found that collective efficacy remained stable over this 7-year period, as did the structural factors (e.g., inequality) that are hypothesized to drive changes in collective efficacy over time. However, having only two waves of data inherently constrained Sampson’s ability to assess neighborhood stability over time and neighborhood processes or structure in Chicago may differ markedly from those in other US cities (Osypuk, Galea, McArdle, & Acevedo-Garcia, 2009).

The overarching objective of this paper is to test whether neighborhood collective efficacy for families (Sampson, et al., 1999; Sampson, et al., 1997) remains stable across time using a unique source of population-based neighborhood resident survey data in Boston, MA, comprised of three waves of data across a 4-year period. This study makes several contributions to the literature. First, the availability of three waves of data allows us to employ an array of simple to sophisticated methodological techniques to test our hypotheses, over and above the correlations reported in Sampson’s analysis of Chicago neighborhoods, thereby providing evidence beyond that of cross-sectional or pre-post designs. Second, we examine a frequently studied social construct --collective efficacy-- that has been linked to mental health outcomes, in a city other than Chicago. Finally, we include an additional validated scale (intergenerational closure) to operationalize collective efficacy that is specific for neighborhood processes influencing children and families.

Methods

Data for this study come from the 2006, 2008, and 2010 Boston Neighborhood Survey (BNS). The BNS is a repeat cross-sectional, random-digit-dial telephone survey conducted by the opinion research firm Fact Finders, Inc (http://www.factfinders.com/) on behalf of the Harvard Injury Control Research Center. One adult 18 years or older was randomly selected from each household to answer questions about neighborhood norms and processes (Azrael et al., 2009) (n=1707 adults in 2006, n=1710 in 2008, and n=1718 in 2010 for a total of 5135). Potential respondents were stratified by each of Boston’s 16 larger neighborhoods (e.g., Roxbury, Dorchester, South Boston), with sampling proportional to neighborhood population size (Azrael, et al., 2009). Interviewers administered the survey in English and Spanish. Interviewers obtained verbal informed consent prior to administering the survey.

Measures

Collective efficacy for families was measured by combining three validated neighborhood scales (social cohesion, informal social control, and intergenerational closure) (Sampson, et al., 1999; Sampson, et al., 1997). Table 1 lists the coding scheme, number and content of scale items, scale internal consistency reliability, and response categories for each item. Responses of don’t know/no opinion were coded as missing because we could not differentiate between a respondent with a neutral opinion/no opinion and one who did not know enough about their neighborhood to respond.

Table 1.

Collective Efficacy Scale & Composite Scales Variable Coding

Scale Higher Values Indicate No. of
Items
Cronbach's
Alpha
Items Rating Scale
All Years
Social Cohesion More cohesion 5 0.85 In my neighborhood: people can be trusted; people are willing to help their neighbors; people know and like each other; people get along with each other; people share the same beliefs about what is right and wrong 1=strongly agree to 4=strongly disagree; reverse coded
Informal Social Control More social control 5 0.82 In your neighborhood, how likely is it that your neighbors would: organize together to keep a fire station open that was going to close because of budget cuts; do something about neighborhood children skipping school and hanging out on a street corner; do something about a child showing disrespect to an adult; do something about a child spray-painting graffiti on a local building; do something if there was a fight in your neighborhood and someone was being beaten or threatened 1=very likely to 4=very unlikely; reverse coded
Intergenerational Closure More connection between adults/children 4 0.82 In my neighborhood: there are adults that children can look up to; you can count on adults to watch out that children and teenagers are safe and stay out of trouble; parents know one another; parents know their children's friends 1=strongly agree to 4=strongly disagree; reverse coded
Collective Efficacy for families More collective efficacy 14 0.93 All items described above Described above*

Table Note

*

Scores for the Collective Efficacy for Families scale were recoded from the original scale items using IRT methods for the final models.

To confirm that the 14 items across the 3 scales were unidimensional and reliable, we ran a factor analysis and calculated Cronbach’s alpha. Internal consistency reliability was very high, α=.93; moreover, items loaded on a single factor, with the first factor explaining 96% of the variance.

We then applied Item Response Theory (IRT) methods to calculate the collective efficacy measure, combining the three ordinal scales into a factor score with an approximately standard normal distribution. As such, a one-unit change in collective efficacy in our models corresponds to an approximately one standard deviation (SD) change (mean(SD)= −1.38E-04(.95)). IRT is a more flexible latent variable method for creating scales compared with simply summing items; IRT gives greater weight to items having a stronger relationship to the underlying construct and therefore decreases measurement error compared to simple mean scales (Hambleton & Swaminathan, 1985). We estimated the IRT score using Mplus software and had very little missing data on our outcome (0.2%).

We adjusted for year linearly (2006=0, 2008=1, 2010=2), sex, age, race/ethnicity, nativity (foreign-born vs. US-born), education, income, length of time in neighborhood (neighborhood tenure), and home ownership in multivariate analyses. All covariates had <2% missing except income (19%), which was modeled using a missing indicator. Cases with missing covariate data (except income), missing outcome data, or missing a neighborhood level identifier were excluded from multivariate analyses (n=232, 4.5% of sample).

Neighborhood Definition

The City of Boston conventionally divides its geography into 16 large neighborhoods. These divisions, which range in size up to 90,000 residents, are far too large for understanding the relationships of social processes such as collective efficacy, which are hypothesized to operate at smaller spatial scales. As such, as part of the larger project in which the BNS is embedded, the research team worked with key informants in sub-neighborhoods of the city who inspected maps and used their local knowledge to define 38 socially relevant “neighborhood clusters” comprised of multiple contiguous census blocks. Details of this neighborhood formulation process are described elsewhere (Azrael, et al., 2009). The BNS survey achieved a mean sample size of 45 respondents in each of these 38 neighborhoods (SD=42; median=28–31 respondents across years), with a minimum neighborhood sample size of 4 and a maximum of 203, representing sufficient sample sizes for estimating neighborhood ecometric constructs (Raudenbush & Sampson, 1999).

Analysis

We applied multiple methods to test the temporal stability of neighborhood collective efficacy for families, including comparing means over time, between and within neighborhoods. All hypothesis tests were conducted at nominal type 1 error=0.05. Individual-level data were used for some analyses; neighborhood-level measures were created for other analyses by aggregating data to calculate a mean collective efficacy value for each of the 38 neighborhood clusters for each year (2006, 2008, and 2010). We began with a basic test of difference across time, using a paired t-test to assess the change in collective efficacy from 2006 to 2010 across neighborhoods (i.e., using neighborhood-level data) and then separate t-tests within each of the 38 neighborhoods (i.e., using individual-level data stratified by neighborhood). Next, we used a Wilcoxon rank sum test to assess whether the rank ordering of neighborhoods changed for collective efficacy from 2006 to 2010 (i.e., using neighborhood-level data). Although these analyses provide simple tests of the null hypothesis that neighborhood collective efficacy for families remained stable over time, these analyses fail to adjust for covariates that might influence respondents’ reporting of their neighborhood social environment.

To obtain a more powerful test of neighborhood change, we employed a two-level random effects multi-level model, which explicitly accounted for the nesting of individuals within neighborhoods, to test whether there was neighborhood variation in change in collective efficacy over time. For each neighborhood, we had measurements of collective efficacy in 2006, 2008, and 2010, so we estimated a two-level model using SAS proc mixed, with respondents (level 1) nested in neighborhoods (level 2). We estimated both a crude model and a covariate-adjusted model to test for compositional effects of neighborhoods. The model contained a random intercept for neighborhoods, a fixed effect for year, and a random slope for year, which allows for neighborhood-specific rates of change that captures heterogeneity across neighborhoods. The equation for this model is:

Collective Efficacy for Familiesij=γ00+γ10Yearij+ζ0i+ζ1iYearij+εij (1)

where γ00 is the overall neighborhood intercept, γ10 is the overall rate of change (fixed effect of year; a one unit change in this time variable is equal to a 2-year change), ζ0i is the level 2 neighborhood random intercept variance, ζ1i is the level 2 year random slope (a one unit change in this time variable is equal to a 2-year change), and Ɛij is the level 1 residual error. The parameters of interest are the fixed effect of year and the variance-covariance matrix of random effects. We are particularly interested in whether there is evidence of moderate to large heterogeneity in the rate of change across neighborhoods, which would indicate that the time trend for some neighborhoods is significantly different from the average (fixed effect) of the time trend in collective efficacy for all of Boston.

The random effects model is a powerful framework for evaluating change, but it relies on the assumptions that unobserved heterogeneity is uncorrelated with other variables in the model and that the residuals are normally distributed (Gardiner, Luo, & Roman, 2009; Snijders, 2005). We tested multilevel model assumptions (functional form, normality, and homoscedasticity) (Singer & Willett, 2003), and we found no evidence of a violation of the assumptions. However, it is conceivable that any unobserved heterogeneity is not independent of the explanatory variables, which could lead to inconsistent random effects (Gardiner, et al., 2009). As a supplemental analysis, we next estimated a neighborhood fixed effects model, which effectively removes unobserved heterogeneity from the model by including interactions between the explanatory variable (year) and the level 2 units (neighborhoods) (Snijders, 2005). We first estimated a crude model, and then a covariate-adjusted model, to test for compositional effects, which included the aforementioned covariates, a linear variable for survey year, an indicator for each of the 37 neighborhoods (vs. a reference 38th neighborhood), and an interaction term between year and each neighborhood indicator. The focus here was on the joint test of significance for the interaction terms, which tests the null hypothesis that there is no overall change across years across neighborhoods.

Results

Table 2 provides descriptive information on the pooled year sample (see Appendix Table 1 for descriptive information by year). The sample was predominately female (59%) and the average age of respondents was approximately 53 years. The sample was 20% black, 63% white, 9% Hispanic, and 8% other race; 21% of respondents were foreign born. The modal education level was some graduate level education (30%). Of those who reported income, the modal income level was $40–80,000 (26%). Respondents reported living in their neighborhood for an average of 20 years; 59% were homeowners and 37% were renters. The distribution of some demographic variables significantly changed from 2006 to 2010 (see Appendix Table 2); in 2010, the sample was more female, slightly older, reported slightly higher income, had lived in their neighborhood longer, and were more likely to be homeowners.

Table 2.

Individual-level descriptive statistics for Boston Neighborhoods Survey - 2006, 2008, and 2010

Variable Subgroup
n
Total n Mean SD Min Max
Male 2123 5135 0.41 0.49 0 1
Female 3012 5135 0.59 0.49 0 1
Age -- 5074 53.39 16.61 18 99
Race/Ethnicity
Black 1013 5032 0.20 0.40 0 1
White 3145 5032 0.63 0.48 0 1
Hispanic 462 5032 0.09 0.29 0 1
Other 412 5032 0.08 0.27 0 1
Foreign Born 1078 5126 0.21 0.41 0 1
Education
< HS 364 5096 0.07 0.26 0 1
High school diploma or GED 1049 5096 0.21 0.40 0 1
Some College 1205 5096 0.24 0.42 0 1
Bachelors 971 5096 0.19 0.39 0 1
Graduate Study 1507 5096 0.30 0.46 0 1
Annual Income
<20K 790 5135 0.15 0.36 0 1
20–40K 875 5135 0.17 0.38 0 1
40–80K 1096 5135 0.21 0.41 0 1
80–100K 452 5135 0.09 0.28 0 1
>100K 944 5135 0.18 0.39 0 1
Missing Income 978 5135 0.19 0.39 0 1
Neighborhood Tenure -- 5119 20.02 17.59 0 94
Home Owner 3016 5090 0.59 0.49 0 1
Renter 1865 5090 0.37 0.48 0 1
Social Cohesion a -- 5135 3.63 0.66 1 5
Informal Social Control a -- 5135 3.81 0.84 1 5
Intergenerational Closure a -- 5135 3.61 0.67 1 5
Collective Efficacy for Families IRT
Score -- 5125 −1.38E-04 0.95 −3.59 2.41
a

Before creating the mean score, items were recoded to a 5-point scale and missing data was imputed to the row-column mean (on average, 13% of scale items were missing).

Neighborhood Stability Analyses

We began with t-tests to examine differences in collective efficacy for families using data from 2006 and 2010, both between and within neighborhood clusters (Table 3). Across neighborhood clusters (i.e., Panel 3a, using neighborhood-level data), there was a significant increase in collective efficacy in Boston from 2006 to 2010 (paired t-test p=.04). Within neighborhood clusters (i.e., Panel 3b, using individual-level data stratified by neighborhood cluster), we found a statistically significant difference in 3 neighborhood clusters (8%), without correcting for multiple testing (i.e., running 38 separate t-tests; this difference was greater than the 5% expected by chance). However, once we adjusted for multiple testing using a Bonferroni correction (adjusted p=.05/38=.001), none of the t-tests for neighborhood collective efficacy change achieved the Bonferroni-corrected significance level.

Table 3.

T-tests between and within 38 Boston neighborhoods; Testing change in collective efficacy for families 2006 to 2010

3a. Between Neighborhoods Paired t-test; N=38

Mean (SD)
2006 2010 t p-value p
−.13(.42) −.02(.37) −2.15 0.04 *
3b. Within Neighborhoods t-test; N=5125, stratified by neighborhood

Without Bonferroni With Bonferroni
# % p-value # % p-value
significant tests (of 38) significant tests significant tests (of 38) significant tests
3 8% 0.05 0.00 0% 0.001
#

p<.10

*

p<.05

**

p<.01

***

p<.001

Bonferroni correction is .05 divided by the number of tests; for each neighborhood cluster (n=38) we are testing one outcome for 38 neighborhoods, so .05 is divided by 38.

NOTES: The sample size for section 3a is 38, because the t-tests are done using neighborhood level data; the sample size for section 3b is 5125, because the t-tests are done at the individual level, but stratified by neighborhood -- the neighborhood specific sample sizes range from 4 to 203 cases.

A Wilcoxon rank sum test examined whether any neighborhoods differed in rank ordering on collective efficacy from 2006 to 2010, using neighborhood-level means. The test was non-significant (z-statistic= −1.12, p=.26), indicating that neighborhoods achieved no significant change in their neighborhood ranking across Boston during this time.

Table 4 presents the results from crude and covariate-adjusted two-level random effects models. There was a significant random intercept for neighborhood (adjusted σ02(SE)=.049(.018)), p=.004) indicating that neighborhoods significantly varied from one another in their 2006 cross-sectional collective efficacy score. However, we found no statistical evidence of a random slope for year (adjusted σ12(SE)=.001(.003), p=.370), and no significant covariance between intercept and year slope (adjusted σ01(SE)=-.002(.006), p=.805), indicating no change in collective efficacy across neighborhoods over time. We did find a statistically significant crude estimate of the fixed effect of year (b(SE)=.029(.015), p=.05) on collective efficacy, which suggests collective efficacy did change in Boston across this time; however, this effect disappeared upon adjusting for individual-level covariates (b(SE)=.004(.017), p=.827). Ten percent of the variation in the outcome was due to differences between neighborhood clusters (Intra-cluster correlation (ICC)=.103; calculated from unadjusted random intercept model). Although this is a larger ICC for a neighborhood study, it does indicate that most of the variation in collective efficacy for families was due to differences between individuals within neighborhoods, in line with other neighborhood ecometric studies (Hox, 2002; Morenoff, 2003; Morenoff, Diez Roux, Osypuk, & Hansen, 2006; Raudenbush & Sampson, 1999; Snijders & Bosker, 1999).

Table 4.

Two-level random effects model; Testing change in collective efficacy for families 2006, 2008, and 2010

Unadjusted Model Covariate-Adjusted Model

b SE p-value p b SE p-value p
Variance Parameter Estimates
Random Neighborhood Intercept 0.097 0.029 0.001 *** 0.049 0.018 0.004 **
Random Year Slope −0.001 0.002 0.602 0.001 0.003 0.370
Covariance between Intercept-Slope −0.001 0.007 0.907 −0.002 0.006 0.805
Residual 0.829 0.017 <.0001 *** 0.772 0.016 <.0001 ***
Fixed Effects
Year 0.029 0.015 0.05 * 0.004 0.017 0.827
AIC 13102.9 12806.1
N 4903 4903
#

p<.10

*

p<.05

**

p<.01

***

p<.001

NOTE: Covariate-adjusted model adjusted for gender, age, race/ethnicity, foreign born status, education, income, neighborhood tenure, and home ownership

Finally, we estimated fixed effects unadjusted and covariate-adjusted linear regression models, with an indicator for year, for each neighborhood, and for neighborhood-year interactions. Table 5 summarizes the F-test comparing the full (with neighborhood-year interactions) and reduced (without interactions) models. The F-test was not significant in the unadjusted models (p=.22). However the F-test was marginally significant for the covariate adjusted model (p=.07), suggesting that, after controlling for all unmeasured time-stable characteristics of neighborhoods, as well as differences in measured respondent characteristics, the neighborhoods marginally differed in the slope change of collective efficacy for families from 2006 to 2010.

Table 5.

Neighborhood fixed effects model; Testing change in collective efficacy for families 2006, 2008, and 2010

Unadjusted Model Covariate-Adjusted Model
Full Reduced Full Reduced
Linear Effect of Year b(SE) −.016(.046) .031(.016) −0.064(.045) .001(.016)
Linear Effect of Year p-value 0.74 .05 # 0.16 0.93
R2 0.09 0.09 0.16 0.15
Joint F-Test of Interactions 1.17 1.36
p-value 0.22 .07 #
N 4907 4907
#

p<.10

*

p<.05

**

p<.01

***

p<.001

NOTES: Covariate-adjusted model adjusted for gender, age, race/ethnicity, foreign born status, education, income, neighborhood tenure, and home ownership. The F-test above is comparing a full model to a reduced model. The null hypothesis test is that the interaction terms do not significantly improve the model. In other words, the joint effect of the year*neighborhood interactions is equal to zero. The full model was estimated with post-estimation commands that give a sub-group analysis comparing, within each neighborhood, the effect of time on the construct of interest. The critical value with F(α=.05; df=37, 4907)=1.41. We tested for nonlinearity of year using a squared term and found no evidence of a non-linear effect of year.

Discussion

Our analysis of population-based neighborhood survey data in Boston tested whether collective efficacy for families remained stable across time, which has implications for causal questions regarding how neighborhood collective efficacy can potentially prevent mental health problems or other individual outcomes. Overall, our analyses could not reject the null hypothesis that collective efficacy for families in Boston neighborhoods remained stable over a 4-year period.

Although we found that Boston residents reported significantly higher collective efficacy in 2010 vs. 2006 in unadjusted analyses (mean comparison and fixed effect of year in multilevel regression), the time pattern was accounted for by compositional differences across time, which may be due to true population change across time, or to changes in BNS sample response patterns across time. Using covariate-adjusted regression, we found no statistical evidence of significant change in collective efficacy for families overall across neighborhood clusters. This suggests that it is important to adjust for population composition when assessing change not due to variation in person-level characteristics. However there also may be some argument for not adjusting for population composition. For example, if changing the population composition is one feasible way to increase (or decrease) collective efficacy, (suggesting that changing population composition is a cause of collective efficacy), then researchers may want to model it in its naturally occurring form.

The neighborhood fixed effects model was suggestive of potential variation across neighborhood cluster-years in change in collective efficacy for families 2006–2010 given the marginally-significant F-test result, but this was not confirmed by the random effects model. The fixed and random effects models are closely related in that they both account for the clustered nature of the data and incorporate the fact that observations within clusters are likely to be correlated. They differ, however, in one important respect: the random effects model incorporates intra-cluster correlation and assumes that it is uncorrelated with other variables in the model, and makes across neighborhood comparisons (Petersen, 2004). It also is drawn from a common random effect distribution, suggesting that random effect-neighborhoods are exchangeable (i.e., that the effects are unconfounded) (Oakes, 2004), which may or may not be a reasonable assumption. The fixed effects model, on the other hand, removes all of the unobserved, time stable characteristics of neighborhoods from the model, and makes within neighborhood comparisons (Petersen, 2004). This model adjusts for possible lack of exchangeability by removing neighborhood level confounding (measured and unmeasured) that is stable over time. Although this provides a more consistent test of the stability of collective efficacy for families, it also estimates the random slope for year as if it were a fixed value and thus, to the extent that there is variability in change across neighborhoods, it may be underestimating the variance of this effect.

Given the different assumptions and treatment of unobserved heterogeneity in fixed versus random effects models, it is not surprising that we found slightly divergent results in our models. Despite the divergence, the neighborhood-time fixed effect test was only marginally significant, so we cannot rule out that it may be due to chance. Taken together, these results suggest that collective efficacy for families is stable over this 4-year, 3-wave period in the city of Boston. This is consistent with recent findings for collective efficacy over a 7-year, 2-wave period in Chicago (Sampson, 2012).

One potential explanation for our finding that collective efficacy for families was stable across time is that four years may be insufficient time for the construct to change, suggesting that studies that assume the stability of collective efficacy – at least over relatively short periods of time – may be justified in doing so. Higher levels of collective efficacy have been linked to positive neighborhood structural factors, such as having more parks and fewer alcohol outlets, after controlling for tract-level disadvantage and sociodemographic variables of individuals (Cohen, Inagami, & Finch, 2008). Thus, collective efficacy may be stable because of its covariation with structural components that do not change dramatically over a short period of time. This explanation is supported by Sampson’s recent work (2012) that found stability in both collective efficacy and the underlying structural components that influence collective efficacy (e.g., concentrated poverty).

Collective efficacy is thought to be a modifiable neighborhood process (Earls, Raviola, & Carlson, 2008). Indeed, interventions aimed at increasing collective efficacy are being attempted in some communities, including, most recently, Sub-Saharan Africa (Carlson, Brennan, & Earls, 2012; Earls, et al., 2008). However, to our knowledge only one study has documented that collective efficacy can indeed be changed with explicit, directed intervention. Even then, this intervention changed one very specific dimension of collective efficacy – youth collective efficacy related to HIV communication (as perceived by adults in the neighborhood) – but the intervention did not change a more general, global measure of neighborhood collective efficacy, although it attempted to do so (Carlson, et al., 2012). Aside from this intervention, most current evidence on collective efficacy derives from studies that typically examine between-neighborhood cross-sectional differences in the construct. As the field moves toward interventions, more evaluation studies are needed to assess whether efforts to mobilize the participation of community members for the collective good can modify collective efficacy.

Researchers commonly operationalize collective efficacy with two scales: social cohesion and informal social control (Sampson, et al., 1997). In sensitivity analyses, we used a traditional 10-item measure of collective efficacy (i.e., comprised of social cohesion and informal social control, Cronbach’s α=.89; see Sampson et al., 1997). The results were comparable. In this study, however, we used a more inclusive measure, incorporating a scale of intergenerational closure in order to tap an aspect of collective efficacy relevant to families and raising children (Sampson, et al., 1999). Intergenerational closure assesses the degree to which adults and children in a neighborhood are interconnected, which could have important consequences for the health of neighborhood residents, both children and adults. The three scales were very highly correlated (at .93) and loaded clearly on one factor, so adding this third scale to collective efficacy may improve measurement.

Limitations

We used a population-based survey, but cooperation rates (CR) were low, e.g. 39% in 2006, 32% in 2008, and 33% in 2010, calculated thus:

(CR=completedcompleted+partial+refused) (2)

We recognize that low cooperation rates have the potential to bias results, especially with respect to external validity (Galea & Tracy, 2007). However these cooperation rates are consistent with a strong secular decreasing trend in survey participation (Galea & Tracy, 2007). With this limitation in mind, we assessed the generalizability of the BNS sample to the city of Boston by comparing demographic characteristics of BNS respondents with those obtained for the city of Boston from the 2005–2009 American Community Survey (ACS) conducted by the US Census Bureau (see Appendix Table 2). We then adjusted for all covariates in our sample that exhibited significantly different distributions from ACS estimates of the Boston population distributions. Thus we can still generalize our findings to Boston conditional on these covariates provided that these covariates account for generalizability differences between the BNS and ACS. Notably, empirical research has demonstrated that nonresponse bias is unlikely to substantially bias measures of interest (Galea & Tracy, 2007). Moreover, there is evidence that even studies that use volunteers, rather than random samples, can provide samples representative at the group level to study neighborhood effects (Oakes, Forsyth, Hearst, & Schmitz, 2009).

Our study also may not be generalizable to cities other than Boston. However, our finding that collective efficacy was stable in Boston neighborhoods across the 4-year period of our study, coupled with Sampson’s similar finding in Chicago over 7 years (Sampson 2012), suggests that studies that assume that collective efficacy is stable over relatively short periods of time may be justified in doing so.

Analyzing change with mean-difference tests carries with it some problems (Cronbach & Furby, 1970), including regression to the mean and the association between raw scores and random/measurement error. They also cannot account for covariates or for clustered data. We address these issues by also estimating more sophisticated neighborhood random and fixed effects models, which overcome many of those limitations.

Conclusion

Prior literature has provided very little information on how, and if, neighborhood social processes change over time (Sampson, et al., 2002), which is one criteria for demonstrating that such processes may be upstream causes of mental health or other individual outcomes (Hill, 1965). This study attempts to fill this gap by explicitly testing the stability of collective efficacy, a frequently studied neighborhood social process. We applied a range of tests, ranging from simple to more sophisticated methods including multi-level analysis, and we find that collective efficacy for families remains stable in Boston neighborhoods over a four-year period. These findings suggest that using existing cross-sectional community surveys to examine the links between social processes and health outcomes may be a reasonable alternative to developing community-level panel data (Bellair, 2000) to study the population level effects of community processes on mental and physical health.

Supplementary Material

Acknowledgment

Support for this publication was provided by NIH grant 1R01MD006064-01 (Dr. Osypuk, PI) and an award from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (U49-CE00740, Dr. Hemenway, PI). The Boston Youth Survey was conducted by the Harvard Youth Violence Prevention Center in collaboration with the City of Boston (the Honorable Thomas M. Menino, Mayor), the Boston Public Health Commission (Barbara Ferrer, Executive Director) and the Boston Public Schools.

Footnotes

Conflicts of Interest: The authors do not have any conflicts of interest to declare.

Author Contributions: Nicole M. Schmidt, PhD and Theresa L. Osypuk, ScD, SM conceived the hypotheses and interpreted results, drafted and revised the manuscript, and Dr. Schmidt additionally analyzed the data. Eric Tchetgen Tchetgen, PhD advised on the statistical analysis and interpretation of results, and edited the manuscript. Amy Ehntholt, MPH contributed to the data analysis and manuscript editing. Joanna Almeida, ScD and Quynh C. Nguyen, PhD contributed to interpreting results and editing. Beth E. Molnar, ScD and Deborah Azrael, PhD contributed to the design of the BNS study, the acquisition of the data for these analyses, interpretation of results, and editing. All authors approved the final submitted version.

References

  1. Alba RD, Denton NA, Leung S-yJ, Logan JR. Neighborhood change under conditions of mass immigration: The New York City region, 1970–1990. International Migration Review. 1995;29(3):625–656. [Google Scholar]
  2. Azrael D, Johnson RM, Molnar BE, Vriniotis M, Dunn EC, Duncan DT, Hemenway D. Creating a youth violence data system for Boston, Massachusetts. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Criminology. 2009;42:406–421. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bandura A. Exercise of personal and collective efficacy in changing societies. In: Bandura A, editor. Self-efficacy in Changing Societies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1995. pp. 1–45. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bellair PE. Informal surveillance and street crime: A complex relationship. Criminology. 2000;38(1):137–169. [Google Scholar]
  5. Browning CR. The span of collective efficacy: Extending social disorganization theory to partner violence. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 2002;64(4):833–850. [Google Scholar]
  6. Browning CR, Cagney KA. Neighborhood structural disadvantage, collective efficacy, and self-rated physical health in an urban setting. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2002;43(4):383–399. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Browning CR, Leventhal T, Brooks-Gunn J. Neighborhood context and racial differences in early adolescent sexual activity. Demography. 2004;41(4):697–720. doi: 10.1353/dem.2004.0029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Buka SL, Brennan RT, Rich-Edwards JW, Raudenbush SW, Earls F. Neighborhood support and the birth weight of urban infants. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2003;157(1):1–8. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cagney KA, Browning CR. Exploring neighborhood-level variation in asthma and other respiratory diseases: The contribution of neighborhood social context. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2004;19:229–236. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2004.30359.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Caldwell CH, Rafferty J, Reischl TM, De Loney EH, Brooks CL. Enhancing Parenting Skills Among Nonresident African American Fathers as a Strategy for Preventing Youth Risky Behaviors. American Journal of Community Psychology. 2010;45(1–2):17–35. doi: 10.1007/s10464-009-9290-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Carlson M, Brennan RT, Earls F. Enhancing adolescent self-efficacy and collective efficacy through public engagement around HIV/AIDS competence: A multilevel, cluster randomized-controlled trial. Social Science & Medicine. 2012;75:1078–1087. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.04.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cerdá M, Diez-Roux AV, Tchetgen Tchetgen EJ, Gordon-Larsen P, Kiefe C. The relationship between neighborhood poverty and alcohol use: Estimation by marginal structural models. Epidemiology. 2010;21(4):482–489. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e3181e13539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chaix B. Geographic life environments and coronary heart disease: A literature review, theoretical contributions, methodological updates, and a research agenda. Annual Review of Public Health. 2009;30:81–105. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.031308.100158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cohen DA, Inagami S, Finch B. The built environment and collective efficacy. Health & Place. 2008;14(2):198–208. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2007.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Coley RL, Morris JE, Hernandez D. Out-of-school care and problem behavior trajectories among low-income adolescents: Individual, family, and neighborhood characteristics as added risks. Child Development. 2004;75(3):948–965. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00716.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cronbach LJ, Furby L. How we should measure "change" - or should we? Psychological Bulletin. 1970;74(1):68–80. [Google Scholar]
  17. Dupéré V, Leventhal T, Vitaro F. Neighborhood processes, self-efficacy, and adolescent mental health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2012;53(2):183–198. doi: 10.1177/0022146512442676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Earls F, Carlson M. The social ecology of child health and well-being. Annual Review of Public Health. 2001;22(1):143–166. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.22.1.143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Earls F, Raviola GJ, Carlson M. Promoting child and adolescent mental health in the context of the HIV/AIDS pandemic with a focus on sub-Saharan Africa. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2008;49(3):295–312. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01864.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Emory R, Caughy M, Harris TR, Franzini L. Neighborhood social processes and academic achievement in elementary school. Journal of Community Psychology. 2008;36(7):885–898. [Google Scholar]
  21. Galea S, Tracy M. Participation rates in epidemiologic studies. Annals of Epidemiology. 2007;17(9):643–653. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2007.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gardiner JC, Luo Z, Roman LA. Fixed effects, random effects and GEE: What are the differences? Statistics in Medicine. 2009;28:221–239. doi: 10.1002/sim.3478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hambleton RK, Swaminathan H. Item Response Theory: Principles and Applications. Boston, MA: Kluwer-Nijhoff Publishing; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  24. Hill AB. The environment and disease: Association or causation? Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine. 1965;58:295–300. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hox J. Multilevel Analysis: Techniques and Applications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  26. Ingoldsby EM, Shaw DS, editors. Clinical Child & Family Psychology Review. Springer Science & Business Media B.V; 2002. Neighborhood Contextual Factors and Early-Starting Antisocial Pathways [Article] (Writers) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Jackson MI, Mare RD. Cross-sectional and longitudinal measurements of neighborhood experience and their effects on children. Social Science Research. 2007;36:590–610. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2007.02.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Jain S, Buka SL, Subramanian SV, Molnar BE. Neighborhood predictors of dating violence victimization and perpetration in young adulthood: A multilevel study. American Journal of Public Health. 2010;100(9):1737–1744. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.169730. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kawachi I, Berkman LF. Neighborhoods and Health. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  30. Kim D. Blues from the neighborhood? Neighborhood characteristics and depression. Epidemiologic Reviews. 2008;30(1):101–117. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxn009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Leventhal T, Brooks-Gunn J. The Neighborhoods They Live In: The Effects of Neighborhood Residence on Child and Adolescent Outcomes. Psychological Bulletin. 2000;126(2):309–337. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Leventhal T, Dupéré V, Gunn JB. Chapter 12: Neighborhood Influences on Adolescent Development. In: Lerner RM, Steinberg L, editors. Handbook of Adolescent Psychology, Volume 2, Contextual Influences on Adolescent Development, 3rd Edition. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2009. pp. 411–443. [Google Scholar]
  33. Ludwig J, Sanbonmatsu L, Gennetian L, Adam E, Duncan GJ, Katz LF, McDade TW. Neighborhoods, obesity, and diabetes - A randomized social experiment. The New England Journal of Medicine. 2011;365(16):1509–1519. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa1103216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Maimon D, Browning CR, Brooks-Gunn J. Collective efficacy, family attachment, and urban adolescent suicide attempts. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2010;51(3):307–324. doi: 10.1177/0022146510377878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Mair C, Roux AVD, Galea S. Are neighbourhood characteristics associated with depressive symptoms? A review of evidence. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health. 2008;62(11):940–946. doi: 10.1136/jech.2007.066605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. McLoyd V, Jayaratne-Epstein T, Ceballo R, Borquez J. Unemployment and work interruption among African American single mothers: Effects on parenting and adolescent socioemotional functioning. Child Development. 1994;65:562–589. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Messer L, Kaufman JS. Using census data to approximate neighborhood effects. In: Oakes JM, Kaufman JS, editors. Methods in Social Epidemiology. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 2006. pp. 209–236. [Google Scholar]
  38. Molnar BE, Cerda M, Roberts AL, Buka SL. Effects of neighborhood resources on aggressive and delinquent behaviors among urban youths. American Journal of Public Health. 2008;98(6):1086–1093. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2006.098913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Molnar BE, Miller MJ, Azrael D, Buka SL. Neighborhood predictors of concealed firearm carrying among children and adolescents: Results from the Project for Human Development in Chicago Neighborhoods. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine. 2004;158(7):657–664. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.158.7.657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Morenoff JD. Neighborhood mechanisms and the spatial dynamics of birth weight. American Journal of Sociology. 2003;108(5):976–1017. doi: 10.1086/374405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Morenoff JD, Diez Roux AV, Osypuk T, Hansen B. National Poverty Center Working Paper. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan; 2006. Residential Environments and Obesity: What Can We Learn about Policy Interventions from Observational Studies? [Google Scholar]
  42. Morenoff JD, Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW. Neighborhood inequality, collective efficacy, and the spatial dynamics of urban violence. Criminology. 2001;39(3):517–559. [Google Scholar]
  43. Nandi A, Glass TA, Cole SR, Chu H, Galea S, Celentano DD, Mehta SH. Neighborhood poverty and injection cessation in a sample of injection drug users. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2010;171(4):391–398. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwp416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Oakes JM. The (mis)estimation of neighborhood effects: causal inference for a practicable social epidemiology. Social Science & Medicine. 2004;58(10):1929–1952. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2003.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Oakes JM, Forsyth A, Hearst MO, Schmitz KH. Recruiting participants for neighborhood effects research: Strategies and outcomes of the Twin Cities Walking Study. Environment and Behavior. 2009;41(6):787–805. [Google Scholar]
  46. Odgers CL, Moffitt TE, Tach LM, Sampson RJ, Taylor A, Matthews CL, Caspi A. The protective effects of neighborhood collective efficacy on British children growing up in deprivation: A developmental analysis. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45(4):942–957. doi: 10.1037/a0016162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Orr L, Feins JD, Jacob R, Beecroft E, Sanbonmatsu L, Katz LF, Kling JR. Moving to Opportunity for Fair Housing Demonstration Program: Interim Impacts Evaluation. Washington, DC: US Dept of HUD; 2003. p. 341. [Google Scholar]
  48. Osypuk TL, Galea S, McArdle N, Acevedo-Garcia D. Quantifying Separate and Unequal: Racial-Ethnic Distributions of Neighborhood Poverty in Metropolitan America. Urban Affairs Review. 2009;45(1):25–65. doi: 10.1177/1078087408331119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Osypuk TL, Schmidt NM, Bates LM, Tchetgen-Tchetgen EJ, Earls FJ, Glymour MM. Gender and crime victimization modify neighborhood effects on adolescent mental health. Pediatrics. 2012;130(3):472–481. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Osypuk TL, Tchetgen Tchetgen EJ, Acevedo-Garcia D, Earls FJ, Lincoln A, Schmidt NM, Glymour MM. Differential mental health effects of neighborhood relocation among youth in vulnerable families. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2012;69(12):1284–1294. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Petersen T. Analyzing panel data: Fixed- and random-effects models. In: Hardy M, Bryman A, editors. Handbook of Data Analysis. London: SAGE Publications; 2004. pp. 331–345. [Google Scholar]
  52. Raudenbush SW, Sampson RJ. Ecometrics: Toward a science of assessing ecological settings, with application to the systematic social observation of neighborhoods. Sociological Methodology. 1999;29:1–41. [Google Scholar]
  53. Rohe WM, Stewart LS. Homeownership and neighborhood stability. Housing Policy Debate. 1996;7(1):37–81. [Google Scholar]
  54. Rothman EF, Johnson RM, Young R, Weinberg J, Azrael D, Molnar BE. Neighborhood-level factors associated with physical dating violence perpetration: Results of a representative survey conducted in Boston, MA. Journal of Urban Health. 2011;88(2):201–213. doi: 10.1007/s11524-011-9543-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Sampson RJ. Great American City: Chicago and the Enduring Neighborhood Effect. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  56. Sampson RJ, Morenoff JD, Earls F. Beyond social capital: Spatial dynamics of collective efficacy for children. American Sociological Review. 1999;64(5):633–660. [Google Scholar]
  57. Sampson RJ, Morenoff JD, Gannon-Rowley T. Assessing"neighborhood effects": Social processes and new directions in research. Annual Review of Sociology. 2002;28:443–478. [Google Scholar]
  58. Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW, Earls F. Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science. 1997;277(5328):918–924. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5328.918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Simons RL, Murry V, McLoyd V, Lin KH, Cutrona C, Conger RD. Discrimination, crime, ethnic identity, and parenting as correlates of depressive symptoms among African American children: a multilevel analysis. Development & Psychopathology. 2002;14(2):371–393. doi: 10.1017/s0954579402002109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Singer JD, Willett JB. Applied Longitudinal Data Analysis: Modeling Change and Event Occurrence. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  61. Skogan W. Fear of crime and neighborhood change. Crime and Justice. 1986;8:203–229. [Google Scholar]
  62. Snijders TAB. Fixed and random effects. In: Everitt BS, Howell DC, editors. Encyclopedia of Statistics in Behavioral Science. Vol. 2. Chicester: Wiley; 2005. pp. 664–665. [Google Scholar]
  63. Snijders TAB, Bosker R. Multilevel Analysis: An Introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  64. Vega WA, Ang A, Rodriquez MA, Finch BK. Neighborhood protective effects on depression in Latinos. American Journal of Community Psychology. 2011;47:114–126. doi: 10.1007/s10464-010-9370-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Wen M, Browning CR, Cagney KA. Poverty, affluence, and income inequality: Neighborhood economic structure and its implications for health. Social Science & Medicine. 2003;57:843–860. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(02)00457-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Xue Y, Leventhal T, Brooks-Gunn J, Earls FJ. Neighborhood residence and mental health problems of 5- to 11-year-olds. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62(5):554–563. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.5.554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

RESOURCES