Skip to main content
Pediatrics logoLink to Pediatrics
. 2014 Jul;134(1):e63–e71. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-3928

Autism Spectrum Disorders and Race, Ethnicity, and Nativity: A Population-Based Study

Tracy A Becerra a, Ondine S von Ehrenstein b, Julia E Heck a, Jorn Olsen a, Onyebuchi A Arah a, Shafali S Jeste c, Michael Rodriguez d, Beate Ritz a,
PMCID: PMC4067639  PMID: 24958588

Abstract

OBJECTIVE:

Our understanding of the influence of maternal race/ethnicity and nativity and childhood autistic disorder (AD) in African Americans/blacks, Asians, and Hispanics in the United States is limited. Phenotypic differences in the presentation of childhood AD in minority groups may indicate etiologic heterogeneity or different thresholds for diagnosis. We investigated whether the risk of developing AD and AD phenotypes differed according to maternal race/ethnicity and nativity.

METHODS:

Children born in Los Angeles County with a primary AD diagnosis at ages 3 to 5 years during 1998–2009 were identified and linked to 1995–2006 California birth certificates (7540 children with AD from a cohort of 1 626 354 births). We identified a subgroup of children with AD and a secondary diagnosis of mental retardation and investigated heterogeneity in language and behavior.

RESULTS:

We found increased risks of being diagnosed with AD overall and specifically with comorbid mental retardation in children of foreign-born mothers who were black, Central/South American, Filipino, and Vietnamese, as well as among US-born Hispanic and African American/black mothers, compared with US-born whites. Children of US African American/black and foreign-born black, foreign-born Central/South American, and US-born Hispanic mothers were at higher risk of exhibiting an AD phenotype with both severe emotional outbursts and impaired expressive language than children of US-born whites.

CONCLUSIONS:

Maternal race/ethnicity and nativity are associated with offspring’s AD diagnosis and severity. Future studies need to examine factors related to nativity and migration that may play a role in the etiology as well as identification and diagnosis of AD in children.

Keywords: autistic disorder, emigration and immigration, epidemiology, continental population groups


What’s Known on This Subject:

Autism prevalence is reported to be highest among non-Hispanic white children, lower in Hispanic and African American/black children, and highly variable in Asian/Pacific Islanders. More comorbid intellectual disability and delays in expressive language have been observed among Hispanic and African American children.

What This Study Adds:

Maternal nativity is a risk factor for childhood autism in US populations. We observed higher risk of severe autism phenotypes in children of foreign-born black, Central/South American, Filipino, and Vietnamese mothers and US-born African Americans and Hispanics compared with US-born whites.

For 2 decades, autism prevalence has risen in the United States and now reaches ∼147 per 10 000 children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) by 8 years of age and 21 per 10 000 children with autistic disorder (AD).1,2 The associated disabilities are characterized by atypical development of socialization and communication and the presence of restricted, repetitive interests and behaviors beginning in early childhood.1,3 Lower ASD prevalence in Hispanic and African American/black children (henceforth “black”) than in non-Hispanic white children (henceforth “white”) and variations in prevalence from 30 to 210 per 10 000 among Asians/Pacific Islanders (Asians/PIs) have been reported.2,4,5 Autism phenotype differences with regard to intellectual and language disabilities across race/ethnic groups in the United States may suggest differences in ASD etiology and disparities in diagnostic and treatment-related factors.4,68

The prenatal period is strongly implicated in ASD etiology911; yet, except for parental age and some pregnancy complications, evidence is insufficient for many potential prenatal risk factors, including recently reported associations with immigration status of the mother.10,12 Epidemiology has a long tradition of using migration studies to understand how environmental and genetic factors contribute to disease risk in populations.13,14 The fact that 22% of children <6 years old born in the United States have immigrant parents opens a unique opportunity to consider the influence of nativity and race/ethnicity on the etiology of ASD.15

We hypothesized that the prevalence of AD (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Fourth Edition [DSM- IV-R]) and specific phenotypes, ie, comorbid mental retardation (MR), expressive language, and emotional/behavioral deficits, differs by maternal race/ethnicity and nativity. We focused our investigation on children born and diagnosed in Los Angeles (LA) County, California, a highly diverse metropolitan region home to black (400 000 in 2010),16 Hispanic (4.7 million in 2009),17 and Asian/PI (1.4 million in 2012)18 residents with a high proportion of immigrants (30% in 2011)19 and 92 languages spoken by students of the LA Unified School District.20 Our findings have clinical implications for early identification and treatment of ASD and research implications that raise innovative hypotheses, not only for different racial/ethnic groups but specifically for large and diverse immigrant communities who vary in risk, protective factors, and access to health care.

Methods

We studied children born to mothers who resided in LA County, California, between 1995 and 2006. Children included in the current study had birth certificate information on maternal race/ethnicity and maternal nativity and a plausible gestational age (21–46 weeks) and birth weight (500–6800 g). Maternal race/ethnicity and nativity were determined on the basis of mother’s self-reported Hispanic origin (yes or no), race (non-Hispanic white, non-Hispanic black, Hispanic, and non-Hispanic Asian), and maternal nativity (US-born or foreign-born). California birth certificates also asked Hispanic mothers to identify their ethnic ancestry and specified country of origin for several Asian and Latin American nations and US territories. Asian ethnic ancestry for US-born mothers was available, but investigating AD in US-born Asians by country of origin was not possible due to small numbers (138 cases among 21 678 births).

In LA County, children needing assessment and treatment of ASD are seen at a network of 7 regional centers contracted by the California Department of Developmental Services (DDS). Referrals to regional centers are made according to residential address by pediatricians, other clinical providers, and schools; parents can also self-refer their children. During our study period, DDS services were available to all children free of charge irrespective of income, type of health insurance, or immigration status. The diagnosis of AD was based on the DSM- IV-R (International Classification of Diseases, Ninth Revision, Clinical Modification [ICD-9-CM] code 299.00),3 as reported on the DDS Client Development Evaluation Report (CDER). The AD diagnoses for these children would likely hold under the new ASD diagnostic criteria in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5).21 Validation studies have established the reliability and validity of the CDER.22,23

Trained psychologists usually make the diagnostic determination of AD/ASD at 7 DDS regional centers. Best practice suggests that primary care providers provide a comprehensive medical assessment including a health history, physical examination, and developmental/neurologic examination. California published guidelines in 2002 to establish a comprehensive diagnostic evaluation for ASD that encompasses the following: (1) review of relevant background information, (2) parent/caregiver interview, (3) comprehensive medical evaluation, (4) cognitive assessment, (5) measures of adaptive functioning, and (6) direct observation, in which the Autism Diagnostic Observation Scale was 1 recommended tool.24

From DDS records, we identified 10 821 children with a primary AD diagnosis at 36 to 71 months of age during 1998–2009 and were able to link 8600 (79.5%) to a California birth record by using a probabilistic linkage program, as described in a previous article.25,26 The main reason for nonlinkage of 2221 DDS records was missing/incomplete information on the birth or DDS records.26 In total, 1 626 354 births of which 7540 children had an AD diagnosis were eligible according to our criteria and had a mother of white, black, Hispanic, or Asian/PI race/ethnicity.

We also identified a subgroup of children with AD and a secondary diagnosis of MR (AD-MR), according to DSM-IV terminology from the CDER (ICD-9-CM codes: 317, mild; 318.0, moderate; 318.1, severe; 318.2, profound; 319, MR unspecified). Although we will use the term “mental retardation” in the context of the actual diagnoses, we recognize the changes in terminology to “intellectual disability” in the DSM-5 and will refer to the more accepted term when speaking more broadly.27 Although cognitive assessment methods were not described explicitly on the CDER, standard and informal procedures are recommended to assess both verbal and nonverbal cognitive functioning.24 To investigate heterogeneity in phenotype for language and behavior, we restricted our data to 5-year-olds, limiting variation due to age-dependent development (n = 1 340 850). From DDS evaluation records, we identified 4 subgroups with overlap between language and behavior subtypes, measured by caregiver interview, observation, or demonstration23: 2 subgroups with either “impaired” or “less impaired” expressive language (AD-impaired expressive language: child does not use words, says simple words or 2-word sentences; AD-less-impaired expressive language: child uses sentences of ≥3 words or at least can engage in basic conversation) and 2 subgroups with “severe” or “less severe” emotional outburst behavior (AD-severe outbursts: child has daily or weekly tantrums requiring restraint; AD-less-severe outburst: child has no tantrums or weekly or less than weekly tantrums without needing restraint). This research was approved by the University of California Los Angeles and the California Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects.

In unconditional logistic regression models we estimated crude and adjusted odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals and report relative risks for the following outcomes: AD, AD-MR, AD-impaired expressive language, AD-less-impaired expressive language, AD-severe outbursts, and AD-less-severe outbursts according to maternal race/ethnicity and nativity with US-born white mothers as the reference.

To examine the influence of AD risk factors likely to vary between race/ethnic groups and by nativity, we present several adjusted models. Our purpose was to quantify associations while adjusting for multiple factors, although some risk factors may be on the causal pathway(s) between race/ethnicity/nativity and AD. First, we adjusted for maternal age, the strongest known risk factor.10 We did not adjust for paternal age due to a high percentage of missing information, especially for children of black mothers (19%). Among those with information available, the correlation between mother’s and father’s age was strong (r = 0.73). Second, we additionally adjusted for child’s gender, birth year, type of birth, parity, gestational age, birth weight, the trimester that prenatal care began, and pregnancy complications. Third, we adjusted for maternal education and insurance type, which was previously observed to be a reasonable indicator of socioeconomic status (SES).28 Finally, to consider possible diagnostic variability, we controlled for DDS regional center catchment area (for controls, regional center was according to residential address during birth).

Results

The mean age of children entering the DDS system differed by no more than 6 months by maternal race/ethnicity and nativity (range: 3 years 1 month to 3 years 7 months); children identified earliest were born to US-born Asian, foreign-born Japanese, foreign-born black, and US-born white mothers, whereas those diagnosed later were children of mothers who immigrated from Central/South America and Vietnam.

Crude rates of being diagnosed with AD varied by race/ethnicity and nativity (from 32 to 93 per 10 000 births), and fully adjusted risks were 76% higher in children of foreign-born black mothers, 43% higher in foreign-born Vietnamese, 25% higher in foreign-born Filipinos, 26% higher in foreign-born Central/South Americans, and 13% to 14% higher in US-born Hispanics and blacks compared with US-born white mothers (Table 1). Children whose mothers were born in China and Japan were at ∼30% lower risk of AD compared with white US-born children, and adjustment for all available risk factors changed estimates only minimally. Additional adjustment for regional center changed the risk of AD only for children of US-born black mothers in a substantive manner and suggested an increased risk.

TABLE 1.

Maternal Race/Ethnicity and Nativity in Relation to Children's Diagnosis of AD in LA County

Maternal Race/Ethnicity and Nativity Mean (SD) Age at Diagnosis, y Case/Cohort, n Rate, per 10 000 Births Crude RR (95% CI) Maternal Age–Adjusted RR (95% CI) Adjusted RRa (95% CI) Additionally Adjusted RRb (95% CI) RR Additionally Adjusted by Regional Center (95% CI)
White
 US-born 3.4 (0.9) 1477/236 347 62.5 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
 Foreign-born 3.3 (0.9) 420/63 464 66.2 1.06 (0.95–1.18) 1.06 (0.95–1.19) 1.02 (0.91–1.14) 1.05 (0.94–1.17) 1.04 (0.93–1.16)
Black
 US-born 3.4 (0.9) 526/123 316 42.6 0.68 (0.62–0.75) 0.84 (0.76–0.93) 1.00 (0.90–1.10) 1.04 (0.94–1.15) 1.14 (1.02–1.26)
 Foreign-born 3.2 (0.9) 92/10 093 91.2 1.46 (1.18–1.80) 1.49 (1.20–1.84) 1.59 (1.28–1.96) 1.65 (1.33–2.05) 1.76 (1.41–2.18)
Hispanic
 US-born 3.5 (0.8) 1376/316 565 43.5 0.70 (0.65–0.75) 0.97 (0.90–1.05) 1.08 (1.00–1.17) 1.15 (1.06–1.24) 1.13 (1.04–1.22)
 Foreign-born 3.5 (0.9) 2594/711 825 36.4 0.58 (0.55–0.62) 0.69 (0.64–0.73) 0.85 (0.79–0.91) 1.05 (0.97–1.14) 1.06 (0.98–1.15)
  Mexico 3.5 (0.9) 1792/548 977 32.6 0.52 (0.49–0.56) 0.61 (0.57–0.66) 0.76 (0.70–0.82) 0.95 (0.86–1.05) 0.95 (0.86–1.05)
  Central/South America 3.6 (0.9) 760/157 147 48.4 0.77 (0.71–0.84) 0.89 (0.81–0.97) 1.08 (0.98–1.19) 1.25 (1.13–1.39) 1.26 (1.14–1.40)
Asian/PI
 US-born 3.1 (0.8) 138/21 678 63.7 1.02 (0.85–1.21) 1.08 (0.91–1.29) 1.03 (0.86–1.22) 1.02 (0.85–1.21) 1.04 (0.87–1.24)
 Foreign-born 3.5 (0.9) 917/143 066 64.1 1.03 (0.94–1.11) 1.01 (0.93–1.10) 0.98 (0.90–1.06) 1.04 (0.95–1.13) 1.02 (0.93–1.11)
  China 3.5 (0.9) 145/29 666 48.9 0.78 (0.66–0.93) 0.74 (0.62–0.87) 0.66 (0.56–0.79) 0.74 (0.62–0.89) 0.69 (0.57–0.83)
  Japan 3.2 (0.7) 29/5815 49.9 0.82 (0.57–1.18) 0.76 (0.53–1.09) 0.71 (0.49–1.02) 0.72 (0.50–1.04) 0.70 (0.48–1.03)
  Korea 3.4 (0.9) 136/22 206 61.2 0.98 (0.82–1.16) 0.98 (0.82–1.18) 0.92 (0.77–1.10) 0.95 (0.80–1.14) 0.97 (0.81–1.17)
  Philippines 3.5 (1.0) 266/35 306 75.3 1.21 (1.06–1.37) 1.21 (1.06–1.37) 1.23 (1.08–1.40) 1.23 (1.08–1.41) 1.25 (1.09–1.43)
  Vietnam 3.6 (0.9) 179/19 287 92.8 1.48 (1.27–1.74) 1.49 (1.27–1.74) 1.45 (1.24–1.70) 1.58 (1.35–1.86) 1.43 (1.21–1.68)

CI, confidence interval; RR, risk ratio.

a

Adjusted for maternal age (≤18, 19–25, 26–30, 31–35, or >35 years), type of birth (single or twin+), parity (1, 2, 3, or >3 children), infant gender (male or female), year of birth (1995–2006), gestational age (<37 weeks or ≥37 weeks), birth weight (<2500, 2500–4500, or >4500 g), trimester start of prenatal care (no care, first, second, or third trimester), and any pregnancy complication (hypertension, renal, lung, or cardiac disease, asthma, pyelonephritis, diabetes, gestational diabetes, Rh sensitivity, hemoglobinopathy, uterine bleeding, hydramnios, incomplete cervix, sexually transmitted diseases, hepatitis B, rubella, other infections, prenatal tobacco use, and large fibroids).

b

Additionally adjusted for maternal education (less than high school, high school, more than high school) and insurance type (Medi-Cal, private insurance, other).

AD-MR

Among 806 children diagnosed with AD-MR (Table 2), the highest crude rates were observed in children of foreign-born black, Vietnamese, and Filipino mothers. Adjustment for other risk factors strengthened estimates with more than twofold increased risks compared with whites. For all children, adjusted risk estimates were >1 (95% confidence intervals included the null values for smaller groups), regardless of maternal nativity, except for Mexican-born mothers.

TABLE 2.

Maternal Race/Ethnicity and Nativity in Relation to Children's Diagnosis of AD-MR

Maternal Race/Ethnicity and Nativity Mean (SD) Age at Diagnosis, y Case/Cohort, n Rate, per 10 000 Births Crude RR (95% CI) Maternal Age–Adjusted RR (95% CI) Adjusted RRa (95% CI) Additionally Adjusted RRb (95% CI) RR Additionally Adjusted by Regional Center (95% CI)
White
 US-born 3.2 (0.8) 122/236 347 5.2 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
 Foreign-born 3.6 (1.1) 35/63 464 5.5 1.07 (0.73–1.56) 1.06 (0.73–1.54) 1.06 (0.73–1.55) 1.08 (0.74–1.57) 1.20 (0.82–1.75)
Black
 US-born 3.6 (1.0) 78/123 316 6.3 1.23 (0.92–1.63) 1.38 (1.04–1.84) 1.42 (1.06–1.90) 1.47 (1.09–1.97) 1.52 (1.11–2.06)
 Foreign-born 3.4 (0.9) 13/10 093 12.9 2.49 (1.41–4.42) 2.50 (1.41–4.42) 2.56 (1.44–4.53) 2.67 (1.50–4.74) 2.63 (1.44–4.78)
Hispanic
 US-born 3.6 (0.9) 161/316 565 5.1 0.99 (0.78–1.25) 1.20 (0.94–1.53) 1.30 (1.02–1.66) 1.35 (1.04–1.73) 1.53 (1.18–1.98)
 Foreign-born 3.6 (0.9) 281/711 825 3.9 0.76 (0.62–0.95) 0.83 (0.67–1.03) 0.91 (0.73–1.14) 1.01 (0.79–1.30) 1.16 (0.90–1.50)
  Mexico 3.6 (0.9) 190/548 977 3.5 0.67 (0.53–0.84) 0.71 (0.56–0.89) 0.79 (0.62–1.01) 0.94 (0.70–1.26) 1.04 (0.77–1.41)
  Central/South America 3.7 (0.9) 86/157 147 5.5 1.06 (0.80–1.40) 1.11 (0.84–1.46) 1.20 (0.90–1.60) 1.42 (1.03–1.96) 1.66 (1.20–2.28)
Asian/PI
 US-born 3.6 (1.1) 14/21 678 6.5 1.25 (0.72–2.18) 1.30 (0.75–2.26) 1.31 (0.75–2.27) 1.30 (0.75–2.27) 1.48 (0.85–2.58)
 Foreign-born 3.6 (0.9) 102/143 066 7.1 1.38 (1.06–1.80) 1.35 (1.04–1.76) 1.36 (1.04–1.77) 1.41 (1.08–1.84) 1.66 (1.26–2.18)
  China 3.6 (1.0) 19/29 666 6.4 1.24 (0.76–2.01) 1.19 (0.74–1.94) 1.19 (0.73–1.93) 1.25 (0.76–2.05) 1.40 (0.84–2.35)
  Japan 3.7 (0.3) 2/5815 3.4 0.67 (0.16–2.69) NA NA NA NA
  Korea 3.0 (0.5) 6/22 206 2.7 0.52 (0.23–1.19) NA NA NA NA
  Philippines 3.8 (1.0) 35/35 306 9.9 1.92 (1.32–2.80) 1.90 (1.31–2.77) 1.92 (1.31–2.80) 1.98 (1.36–2.90) 2.27 (1.54–3.34)
  Vietnam 3.6 (0.9) 20/19 287 10.4 2.01 (1.25–3.22) 1.96 (1.22–3.15) 1.96 (1.22–3.16) 2.07 (1.28–3.35) 2.45 (1.50–4.01)

CI, confidence interval; NA, not applicable; RR, risk ratio.

a

Adjusted for maternal age (≤18, 19–25, 26–30, 31–35, or >35 years), type of birth (single or twin+), parity (1, 2, 3, or >3 children), infant gender (male or female), year of birth (1995–2006), gestational age (<37 weeks or ≥37 weeks), birth weight (<2500, 2500–4500, or >4500 g), trimester start of prenatal care (no care, first, second, or third trimester), and any pregnancy complication (hypertension, renal, lung, or cardiac disease, asthma, pyelonephritis, diabetes, gestational diabetes, Rh sensitivity, hemoglobinopathy, uterine bleeding, hydramnios, incomplete cervix, sexually transmitted diseases, hepatitis B, rubella, other infections, prenatal tobacco use, and large fibroids).

b

Additionally adjusted for maternal education (less than high school, high school, more than high school) and insurance type (Medi-Cal, private insurance, other).

AD: Expressive Language Skills and Emotional Outburst Phenotypes

For 5-year old children with impaired AD expressive language abilities (Table 3), most race/ethnicity and nativity subgroups were at higher risk of an impaired expressive language phenotype, which persisted when restricting to children who were diagnosed early (ie, at 2 or 3 years of age; results not shown). Fewer differences for children with a less impaired language phenotype were observed (ie, risks were comparable to US-born whites, except for children of foreign-born Mexican, Chinese, and Korean mothers who were at lower risk).

TABLE 3.

Maternal Race/Ethnicity and Nativity in Relation to Child's Diagnosis of AD by Expressive Language Skills at 5 Years of Age

Maternal Race/Ethnicity and Nativity Impaired Expressive Language Less-Impaired Expressive Language
Case/Cohort, n Rate, per 10 000 Births Crude RR (95% CI) Adjusted RRa (95% CI) Case/Cohort, n Rate, per 10 000 Births Crude RR (95% CI) Adjusted RRa (95% CI)
White
 US-born 349/197 992 17.6 1.00 1.00 509/197 992 25.7 1.00 1.00
 Foreign-born 109/51 540 21.1 1.16 (0.94–1.44) 1.24 (1.00–1.54) 117/51 540 22.7 0.88 (0.72–1.08) 0.91 (0.74–1.11)
Black
 US-born 189/103 643 18.2 1.04 (0.87–1.24) 1.37 (1.13–1.66) 106/103 643 10.2 0.40 (0.32–0.49) 0.84 (0.68–1.05)
 Foreign-born 38/8331 45.6 2.55 (1.83–3.57) 2.76 (1.96–3.89) 14/8331 16.8 0.65 (0.38–1.11) 0.90 (0.53–1.54)
Hispanic
 US-born 429/250 825 17.1 0.92 (0.80–1.06) 1.28 (1.09–1.49) 309/250 825 12.3 0.48 (0.42–0.55) 1.01 (0.86–1.19)
 Foreign-born 993/595 680 16.7 0.95 (0.84–1.07) 1.36 (1.17–1.57) 451/595 680 7.6 0.30 (0.26–0.33) 0.83 (0.71–0.97)
  Mexico 700/460 141 15.2 0.86 (076–0.98) 1.12 (0.95–1.33) 301/460 141 6.5 0.25 (0.22–0.29) 0.78 (0.64–0.95)
  Central/South America 275/130 579 21.1 1.18 (1.01–1.39) 1.57 (1.31–1.89) 142/130 579 10.9 0.42 (0.35–0.51) 1.03 (0.84–1.28)
Asian/PI
 US-born 42/16 886 24.9 1.31 (0.95–1.81) 1.34 (0.97–1.86) 42/16 886 24.9 0.97 (0.71–1.33) 1.01 (0.74–1.39)
 Foreign-born 348/115 953 30.0 1.65 (1.42–1.91) 1.58 (1.35–1.84) 182/115 953 15.7 0.61 (0.51–0.72) 0.66 (0.55–0.79)
  China 56/23 536 23.8 1.28 (0.96–1.70) 1.06 (0.78–1.42) 38/23 536 16.1 0.63 (0.45–0.87) 0.61 (0.43–0.87)
  Japan 13/4646 28.0 1.51 (0.87–2.63) 1.34 (0.75–2.40) 5/4646 10.8 0.42 (0.17–1.01) NA
  Korea 54/17 766 30.4 1.65 (1.24–2.19) 1.82 (1.34–2.43) 20/17 766 11.3 0.44 (0.28–0.68) 0.52 (0.33–0.81)
  Philippines 94/28 901 32.5 1.80 (1.43–2.26) 1.86 (1.48–2.34) 55/28 901 19.0 0.74 (0.56–0.98) 0.82 (0.62–1.10)
  Vietnam 73/16 070 45.4 2.56 (1.99–3.30) 2.06 (1.58–2.70) 34/16 070 21.1 0.82 (0.58–1.16) 0.96 (0.67–1.37)

CI, confidence interval; NA, not applicable; RR, risk ratio.

a

Adjusted for maternal age (≤18, 19–25, 26–30, 31–35, or >35 years), type of birth (single or twin+), parity (1, 2, 3, or >3 children), infant gender (male or female), year of birth (1995–2006), gestational age (<37 weeks or ≥37 weeks), birth weight (<2500, 2500–4500, or >4500 g), trimester start of prenatal care (no care, first, second, or third trimester), any pregnancy complication (hypertension, renal, lung, or cardiac disease, asthma, pyelonephritis, diabetes, gestational diabetes, Rh sensitivity, hemoglobinopathy, uterine bleeding, hydramnios, incomplete cervix, sexually transmitted diseases, hepatitis B, rubella, other infections, prenatal tobacco use, and large fibroids), maternal education (less than high school, high school, more than high school), insurance type (Medi-Cal, private insurance, other), and regional center. Language information is presented for cases 5 years of age at evaluation; cohort is for 1995–2004 births, N = 1 340 850.

Considering the 1396 with a “severe” emotional outburst behavior phenotype among 4197 total 5-year-old children with AD, those born to US-born Hispanic and foreign-born Central/South American mothers were at higher risk only in the severe outburst category (Table 4). However, higher risk of severe outbursts only persisted in conjunction with impaired expressive language (results not shown). Children of foreign-born black mothers were at increased risk of both phenotypes, whereas Vietnamese and Filipino offspring exhibited slightly higher risks of AD with the less severe outburst phenotype.

TABLE 4.

Maternal Race/Ethnicity and Nativity in Relation to Child's Diagnosis of AD With Severe or Less-Severe Emotional Outburst Behavior at 5 Years of Age

Maternal Race/Ethnicity and Nativity Severe Emotional Outbursts Less-Severe Emotional Outbursts
Case/Cohort, n Rate, per 10 000 Births Crude RR (95% CI) Adjusted RRa (95% CI) Case/Cohort, n Rate, per 10 000 Births Crude RR (95% CI) Adjusted RRa (95% CI)
White
 US-born 272/197 992 13.7 1.00 1.00 586/197 992 29.6 1.00 1.00
 Foreign-born 66/51 540 12.8 0.93 (0.71–1.22) 0.98 (0.74–1.28) 154/51 540 29.9 0.98 (0.82–1.17) 1.04 (0.87–1.24)
Black
 US-born 84/103 643 8.1 0.59 (0.46–0.75) 0.93 (0.72–1.20) 209/103 643 20.2 0.68 (0.58–0.80) 1.10 (0.93–1.31)
 Foreign-born 17/8331 20.4 1.49 (0.91–2.43) 1.72 (1.03–2.86) 34/8331 40.8 1.38 (0.96–1.92) 1.63 (1.15–2.31)
Hispanic
 US-born 279/250 825 11.1 0.81 (0.69–0.96) 1.29 (1.07–1.55) 453/250 825 18.1 0.58 (0.51–0.65) 0.99 (0.86–1.13)
 Foreign-born 508/595 680 8.5 0.62 (0.54–0.72) 1.10 (0.92–1.32) 925/595 680 15.5 0.52 (0.47–0.58) 1.01 (0.89–1.15)
  Mexico 345/460 141 7.5 0.55 (0.47–0.64) 0.93 (0.75–1.16) 649/460 141 14.1 0.48 (0.43–0.53) 0.89 (0.77–1.04)
  Central/South America 160/130 579 12.2 0.89 (0.73–1.08) 1.47 (1.17–1.85) 253/130 579 19.4 0.65 (0.56–0.75) 1.12 (0.94–1.32)
Asian/PI
 US-born 26/16 886 15.4 1.12 (0.75–1.68) 1.18 (0.79–1.77) 57/16 886 33.8 1.14 (0.87–1.50) 1.11 (0.84–1.46)
 Foreign-born 144/115 953 12.4 0.90 (0.74–1.11) 0.97 (0.78–1.19) 383/115 953 33 1.12 (0.98–1.27) 1.07 (0.93–1.22)
  China 25/23 536 10.6 0.77 (0.51–1.16) 0.71 (0.46–1.11) 68/23 536 28.9 0.98 (0.76–1.25) 0.79 (0.61–1.03)
  Japan 3/4646 6.5 0.47 (0.15–1.47) NA 15/4646 32.3 1.09 (0.65–1.82) 0.84 (0.48–1.45)
  Korea 22/17 766 12.4 0.90 (0.58–1.39) 1.13 (0.73–1.76) 51/17 766 28.7 0.97 (0.73–1.29) 0.99 (0.74–1.33)
  Philippines 40/28 901 13.8 1.01 (0.72–1.40) 1.11 (0.79–1.55) 108/28 901 37.4 1.26 (1.03–1.55) 1.29 (1.05–1.59)
  Vietnam 29/16 070 18 1.31 (0.89–1.93) 1.22 (0.81–1.84) 78/16 070 48.5 1.64 (1.29–2.08) 1.53 (1.19–1.96)

CI, confidence interval; NA, not applicable; RR, risk ratio.

a

Adjusted for maternal age (≤18, 19–25, 26–30, 31–35, or >35 years), type of birth (single or twin+), parity (1, 2, 3, or >3 children), infant gender (male or female), year of birth (1995–2006), gestational age (<37 weeks or ≥37 weeks), birth weight (<2500, 2500–4500, or >4500 g), trimester start of prenatal care (no care, first, second, or third trimester), any pregnancy complication (hypertension, renal, lung, or cardiac disease, asthma, pyelonephritis, diabetes, gestational diabetes, Rh sensitivity, hemoglobinopathy, uterine bleeding, hydramnios, incomplete cervix, sexually transmitted diseases, hepatitis B, rubella, other infections, prenatal tobacco use, and large fibroids), maternal education (less than high school, high school, more than high school), insurance type (Medi-Cal, private insurance, other), and regional center. Behavioral information is presented for cases 5 years of age at evaluation; cohort is for 1995–2004 births, N = 1 340 850.

Discussion

We investigated the influence of maternal race/ethnicity and nativity on AD diagnosis in LA County, a racially diverse area with a high percentage of recent immigrants. We found compelling evidence that, compared with children born to white US-born mothers, children of foreign-born black, Filipino, and Vietnamese mothers had higher risks of developing or being diagnosed with AD, specifically with MR and impaired expressive language. Previously, immigration status was reported to not be associated with high risk of AD in California,5 possibly due to the lower risks we observed among offspring of foreign-born Mexican women, who make up the majority of the foreign-born population in California (53%).19 However, we found foreign-born black mothers to be at highest risk of having a child with AD and AD-MR and impaired language abilities, consistent with findings of a higher autism risk among children born to refugee mothers from Africa and the Caribbean living in the United Kingdom and Sweden.2932 Differences in AD risk by race/ethnicity and nativity could imply variation in etiologic factors across racial/ethnic/nativity groups, which might affect fetal or early childhood development, including stress, diet, toxic environmental exposures, or infections.33,34 These differences could also point to disparities in accessing and receiving appropriate diagnosis and treatment.

US minority groups are historically believed to be underidentified for ASD, and it is possible that children with milder symptoms did not go to a regional center and were missed during our study period.1,4,5 This view is supported by our finding that crude rates of AD in US-born Hispanics and black children were lower than in whites. However, when we assessed maternal race/ethnicity subgroups by nativity and adjusted for well-known risk factors such as maternal age, similar or higher AD risks were estimated. SES and location of diagnosis may be related to environmental risk factors and may also be a source of identification and diagnosis variability. Adjustment for these factors changed a no-risk to a higher-risk estimate for US-born blacks and a lower risk to a no-risk estimate for children of Mexican-born mothers. Thus, ascertainment bias is possible, and improved early identification is needed in these 2 groups. Ascertainment bias is also supported by the observation that for more severe AD phenotypes that are more likely to prompt identification and treatment, such as the AD-MR and impaired language phenotypes of AD, the crude rates were indeed similar in US-born Hispanic, black, and white children but higher after adjustment for risk factors. Language and cultural barriers (ie, low access to psychologists from minority groups, appropriately standardized and language-sensitive assessment instruments)3538 could also have caused underascertainment of AD or misclassification of language skills, a category in which children from all minority groups were at higher risk compared with those with US-born white mothers. Underutilization of formal health services was reported in Chinese and other Asian populations, and underdiagnosis of milder forms of AD at a young age was reported in black children.8,3941

Information about AD incidence or prevalence in lower income countries in Africa,42 Latin America, and South East Asia43 is limited, although rates of ASD are reportedly increasing in Vietnam,44 making it difficult to determine whether the observed AD risk reflects rates from country of origin or if it is a migratory phenomenon. Voluntary migration involves physical relocation and is often preceded by uncertainty, with refugees sometimes facing life-threatening situations; after arriving, immigrants may end up in low-SES neighborhoods, becoming socially vulnerable. Women from Central American nations (Guatemala, El Salvador) who migrated seeking asylum in the 1980s may have a history of trauma from civil war, violence, and displacement.4547 Maternal life event stress and psychiatric disorders, possibly related to experiences of escaping wars and disasters, as well as nutritional deficiencies from famine, may be possible explanations for the increased risks observed in Central American, Vietnamese, and some African immigrant groups,4853 and are considered risk factors for low-functioning autism in offspring.54,55 Dietary factors such as folic acid and vitamin D deficiencies, common among US black and Hispanic women56,57 and in women from Vietnam and the Philippines,58,59 could explain some of the increased risk of AD and AD-MR.6063 In comparison, folic acid deficiency in pregnant women is less common in China’s southern regions, from where a majority of US Chinese immigrants originate.64,65 Also, being born and raised in a foreign country may result in less immunity against local host country pathogens, increasing susceptibility to infections common in the United States. Maternal infections can affect fetal brain development, and some authors suggested that influenza and prolonged episodes of fever increase ASD risk.10,6668 This hypothesis may apply to Filipino mothers, of whom a large proportion (30%) are employed in health care–related occupations in which the risk of exposure to infections is high.69 Studying this common infection hypothesis in more depth, however, is necessary.

Contrary to Central/South American immigrants, children born to immigrant Mexican women were at similar risk to US-born whites for AD and AD-MR. These children were also at lower risk of being diagnosed with “less impaired” language, possibly suggesting underdiagnosis of milder forms of AD. Alternatively, AD differences favoring Mexican immigrants compared with US-born Hispanic Americans, despite similarly lower income and education, resemble the “Latina paradox” of healthier birth outcomes.7072 Evidence for a Latina paradox is supported by a higher adjusted risk of AD diagnosis among US-born Hispanics, who also had higher risks of more severe phenotypes, including MR, impaired language, and severe emotional outburst behaviors as also seen in an earlier report.73

Although the large and diverse LA population allowed us to assess AD risk in offspring of mothers who migrated from different regions of the world, a comparison with US-born women from the same backgrounds was limited due to smaller subgroup sample size. Because the comparison group was US-born white women, differences in prevalence may also be due to genetic variation according to race/ethnicity. The DDS counts of persons with autism likely underestimate the actual California population because it is estimated that 75% to 80% of the total population of persons in California with autism are enrolled in the developmental service system.74

Although our large sample size did not allow us to validate diagnoses, the diagnostic stability of AD is considered good within the studied age group (the diagnostic consistency for ASD between ages 2 and 9 years is 90%).22,75 Furthermore, our study included children with a diagnosis of AD, which is more likely to hold under DSM-5 criteria (sensitivity: 0.76) than other ASD groups (ie, Asperger syndrome; pervasive developmental disorder, not otherwise specified) (sensitivity: 0.25–0.28).21 Expressive language skills and social-emotional behavior assessment partially relied on caregiver report. Although the parent interview is considered to be critical given the challenges of direct observation of the child for diagnostically assessing language and behavior,24 it might be hampered by the parent’s ability to understand and report such observations accurately.

Conclusions

Our results underscore the importance of ASD research in diverse racial/ethnic populations that considers nativity to inform clinical practice. Systematic exploration of risk and protective factors related to living circumstances before and after migration is sorely needed and may lead to clinical and public health interventions (ie, mental health, environmental, or dietary interventions). Exploring infections and immunologic profiles across immigrant subgroups, their exposures to stress and environmental factors, as well as the role of acculturation and adoption of the new culture’s diet may be particularly important and informative. Equally important is the need to improve quality of and access to health care for diverse populations to improve autism diagnoses and timeliness of treatment.

Glossary

AD

autistic disorder

ASD

autism spectrum disorder

CDER

Client Development Evaluation Report

DDS

Department of Developmental Services

DSM-IV-R

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Fourth Edition

DSM-5

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition

LA

Los Angeles

MR

mental retardation

PI

Pacific Islander

SES

socioeconomic status

Footnotes

Dr Becerra conceptualized and designed the study, carried out the analyses, and drafted the initial manuscript; Dr von Ehrenstein contributed to the conceptualization of the study and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr Heck critically reviewed and revised the manuscript; Drs Olsen, Arah, Jeste, and Rodriguez provided content and critical feedback on the manuscript; Dr Ritz coordinated, conceptualized, and designed the study, as well as contributed to drafting and editing the manuscript; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted.

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

FUNDING: This research was sponsored by the University of California Los Angeles (UCLA) Graduate Division, the California Center for Population Research, UCLA, supported by infrastructure grant R24HD041022 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development and by NIEHS of the National Institutes of Health under award number R21ES022389. Funded by the National Institutes of Health (NIH).

POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

References

  • 1.Baio J. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders—Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 14 sites, United States, 2008. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2012;61(3):1–19 [PubMed]
  • 2.Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network Surveillance Year 2010 Principal Investigators. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years—Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 sites, United States, 2010. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2002;2014(63 suppl 2):1–21 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed, text revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000 [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Pedersen A, Pettygrove S, Meaney FJ, et al. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders in Hispanic and non-Hispanic white children. Pediatrics. 2012;129(3). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/129/3/e629 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Croen LA, Grether JK, Selvin S. Descriptive epidemiology of autism in a California population: who is at risk? J Autism Dev Disord. 2002;32(3):217–224 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Cuccaro ML, Brinkley J, Abramson RK, Hall A, Wright HH, Hussman JP, Gilbert JR, Pericak-Vance MA. Autism in African American Families: Clinical-Phenotypic Findings. Am J Med Genet. 2007; Part B 144B:1022–1026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Chaidez V, Hansen RL, Hertz-Picciotto I. Autism spectrum disorders in Hispanics and non-Hispanics. Autism. 2012;16(4):381–397 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Jarquin VG, Wiggins LD, Schieve LA, Van Naarden-Braun K. Racial disparities in community identification of autism spectrum disorders over time: Metropolitan Atlanta, Georgia, 2000-2006. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2011;32(3):179–187 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Courchesne E, Yeung-Courchesne R, Press GA, Hesselink JR, Jernigan TL. Hypoplasia of cerebellar vermal lobules VI and VII in autism. N Engl J Med. 1988;318(21):1349–1354 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Gardener H, Spiegelman D, Buka SL. Prenatal risk factors for autism: comprehensive meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195(1):7–14 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Stoner R, Chow ML, Boyle MP, et al. Patches of disorganization in the neocortex of children with autism. N Engl J Med. 2014;370(13):1209–1219 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kolevzon A, Gross R, Reichenberg A. Prenatal and perinatal risk factors for autism: a review and integration of findings. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161(4):326–333 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Hjern A. Migration and public health: health in Sweden. The National Public Health Report 2012. Chapter 13. Scand J Public Health. 2012;40(9 suppl):255–267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hamilton TG, Hummer RA. Immigration and the health of U.S. black adults: does country of origin matter? Soc Sci Med 1982. 2011;73(10):1551–1560 [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 15.Urban Institute. Children of immigrants: facts and figures. 2006. Available at: www.urban.org/publications/900955.html. Accessed May 8, 2013
  • 16.Rastogi S, Johnson TD, Hoeffel EM, Drewery MPJ. The Black Population: 2010 US Census Bureau; 2011. Available at: www.census.gov/prod/cen2010/briefs/c2010br-06.pdf. Accessed May 7, 2013
  • 17.US Census Bureau. Hispanic Americans by the numbers. Available at: www.infoplease.com/spot/hhmcensus1.html. Accessed May 8, 2013
  • 18.US Census Bureau. Los Angeles County QuickFacts. Available at: http://quickfacts.census.gov/qfd/states/06/06037.html. Accessed July 10, 2013
  • 19.Johnson H, Cuellar Mejia M. Immigrants in California San Francisco, CA: Public Policy Institute of California; 2013. Available at: www.ppic.org/main/publication_show.asp?i=258. Accessed November 25, 2012
  • 20.Los Angeles Almanac. Language spoken at home by Los Angeles communities. 2000. Available at: www.laalmanac.com/LA/la10b.htm. Accessed July 22, 2013
  • 21.McPartland JC, Reichow B, Volkmar FR. Sensitivity and specificity of proposed DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51(4):368–383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.California Department of Developmental Services. Autism spectrum disorders: changes in the California caseload, an update: June 1987–June 2007. Available at: www.dds.ca.gov/Autism/Home.cfm. Accessed October 23, 2011
  • 23.State of California Department of Developmental Services. Diagnostic information and instruments. 1986. Available at: www.dds.ca.gov/FactsStats/Diagnostic_Main.cfm. Accessed October 23, 2011
  • 24.California Department of Developmental Services. Autism spectrum disorders: best practice guidelines for screening, diagnosis and assessment. 2002. Available at: www.dds.ca.gov/Autism/docs/ASD_Best_Practice2002.pdf. Accessed October 23, 2011
  • 25.US Department of Health and Human Services; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion. Registry Plus, a suite of publicly available software programs for collecting and processing cancer registry data. 2010. Available at: www.cdc.gov/cancer/npcr/. Accessed November 15, 2010
  • 26.Becerra TA, Wilhelm M, Olsen J, Cockburn M, Ritz B. Ambient air pollution and autism in Los Angeles County, California. Environ Health Perspect. 2013;121(3):380–386 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.American Psychiatric Association. Intellectual disability. 2013. Available at: www.dsm5.org/Documents/Intellectual%20Disability%20Fact%20Sheet.pdf. Accessed February 13, 2014
  • 28.Ritz B, Wilhelm M, Hoggatt KJ, Ghosh JKC. Ambient air pollution and preterm birth in the environment and pregnancy outcomes study at the University of California, Los Angeles. Am J Epidemiol. 2007;166(9):1045–1052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Gillberg C, Steffenburg S, Börjesson B, Andersson L. Infantile autism in children of immigrant parents: a population-based study from Göteborg, Sweden. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;150(6):856–858 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Keen DV, Reid FD, Arnone D. Autism, ethnicity and maternal immigration. Br J Psychiatry. 2010;196(4):274–281 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Magnusson C, Rai D, Goodman A, et al. Migration and autism spectrum disorder: population-based study. Br J Psychiatry. 2012;201(2):109–115 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Barnevik-Olsson M, Gillberg C, Fernell E. Prevalence of autism in children born to Somali parents living in Sweden: a brief report. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2008;50(8):598–601 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Miyake K, Hirasawa T, Koide T, Kubota T. Epigenetics in autism and other neurodevelopmental diseases. Neurodegener Dis. 2012;724:91–98 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Woods R, LaSalle JM. Epigenetic epidemiology of autism and other neurodevelopmental disorders. In: Michels KB, ed. Epigenetic Epidemiology Netherlands: Springer Netherlands; 2012:321–342. Available at: http://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-94-007-2495-2_17. Accessed May 7, 2013
  • 35.Michalski D, Mulvey T, Kohout J. 2008 APA Survey of Psychology Health Service Providers. 2010. Available at: www.apa.org/workforce/publications/08-hsp/index.aspx. Accessed May 22, 2013
  • 36.Lord C, Rutter M, Goode S, et al. Autism diagnostic observation schedule: a standardized observation of communicative and social behavior. J Autism Dev Disord. 1989;19(2):185–212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, et al. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30(3):205–223 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.McInnes LA, González PJ, Manghi ER, et al. A genetic study of autism in Costa Rica: multiple variables affecting IQ scores observed in a preliminary sample of autistic cases. BMC Psychiatry. 2005;5(1):1–11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Yeargin-Allsopp M, Rice C, Karapurkar T, Doernberg N, Boyle C, Murphy C. Prevalence of autism in a US metropolitan area. JAMA. 2003;289(1):49–55 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Chung I. Changes in the sociocultural reality of Chinese immigrants: challenges and opportunities in help-seeking behaviour. Int J Soc Psychiatry. 2010;56(4):436–447 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Yamashiro G, Matsuoka JK. Help-seeking among Asian and Pacific Americans: a multiperspective analysis. Soc Work. 1997;42(2):176–186 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Bakare MO, Munir KM. Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) in Africa: a perspective. Afr J Psychiatry (Johannesbg). 2011;14(3):208–210 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Elsabbagh M, Divan G, Koh Y-J, et al. Global prevalence of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders. Autism Res. 2012;5(3):160–179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Yi Y. Children with autism on the rise in Vietnam. 2013. Available at: http://news.xinhuanet.com/english/health/2013-03/14/c_132234027.htm. Accessed April 21, 2013
  • 45.Smith J. Guatemala: economic migrants replace political refugees. Migration Information Source April 1, 2006. Available at: www.migrationinformation.org/Profiles/display.cfm?ID=392. Accessed April 24, 2013
  • 46.Terrazas A. Salvadoran immigrants in the United States. Migration Information Source January 5, 2010. Available at: www.migrationinformation.org/usfocus/display.cfm?ID=765. Accessed May 14, 2013
  • 47.Gammage S. El Salvador: despite end to civil war, emigration continues. Migration Information Source July 26, 2007. Available at: www.migrationinformation.org/Profiles/display.cfm?ID=636. Accessed April 24, 2013
  • 48.Campagna AM, Settgast AM, Walker PF, DeFor TA, Campagna EJ, Plotnikoff GA. Effect of country of origin, age, and body mass index on prevalence of vitamin D deficiency in a US immigrant and refugee population. Mayo Clin Proc. 2013;88(1):31–37 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Pfortmueller CA, Graf F, Tabbara M, Lindner G, Zimmermann H, Exadaktylos AK. Acute health problems in African refugees: ten years’ experience in a Swiss emergency department. Wien Klin Wochenschr. 2012;124(17–18):647–652 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Jaranson JM, Butcher J, Halcon L, et al. Somali and Oromo refugees: correlates of torture and trauma history. Am J Public Health. 2004;94(4):591–598 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Lindert J, Ehrenstein OS von, Priebe S, Mielck A, Brähler E. Depression and anxiety in labor migrants and refugees—a systematic review and meta-analysis. Soc Sci Med 1982. 2009;69(2):246–257 [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 52.Hinton WL, Chen YC, Du N, et al. DSM-III-R disorders in Vietnamese refugees: prevalence and correlates. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1993;181(2):113–122 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Wagner J, Burke G, Kuoch T, Scully M, Armeli S, Rajan TV. Trauma, healthcare access, and health outcomes among Southeast Asian refugees in Connecticut. J Immigr Minor Health. 2013;15(6):1065–1072 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Roberts AL, Lyall K, Rich-Edwards JW, Ascherio A, Weisskopf MG. Association of maternal exposure to childhood abuse with elevated risk for autism in offspring. JAMA Psychiatry 2013;70(5):508–515 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Yirmiya N, Shaked M. Psychiatric disorders in parents of children with autism: a meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46(1):69–83 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Yetley EA. Assessing the vitamin D status of the US population. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;88(2):558S–564S [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Grant WB, Peiris AN. Possible role of serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D in black-white health disparities in the United States. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2010;11(9):617–628 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Cavalli-Sforza T. Effectiveness of weekly iron-folic acid supplementation to prevent and control anemia among women of reproductive age in three Asian countries: development of the master protocol and implementation plan. Nutr Rev. 2005;63(12 pt 2):S77–S80 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Smitasiri S, Solon FS. Implementing preventive iron-folic acid supplementation among women of reproductive age in some Western Pacific countries: possibilities and challenges. Nutr Rev. 2005;63(12 pt 2):S81–S86 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Schmidt RJ, Tancredi DJ, Ozonoff S, et al. Maternal periconceptional folic acid intake and risk of autism spectrum disorders and developmental delay in the CHARGE (CHildhood Autism Risks from Genetics and Environment) case-control study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;96(1):80–89 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Surén PRC, Roth C, Bresnahan M, et al. Association between maternal use of folic acid supplements and risk of autism spectrum disorders in children. JAMA. 2013;309(6):570–577 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Kočovská E, Fernell E, Billstedt E, Minnis H, Gillberg C. Vitamin D and autism: clinical review. Res Dev Disabil. 2012;33(5):1541–1550 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Yang Q-H, Carter HK, Mulinare J, Berry RJ, Friedman JM, Erickson JD. Race-ethnicity differences in folic acid intake in women of childbearing age in the United States after folic acid fortification: findings from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 2001-2002. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;85(5):1409–1416 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Skeldon R. Migration from China. J Int Aff. 1996;49(2):434
  • 65.Ren A, Zhang L, Hao L, Li Z, Tian Y, Li Z. Comparison of blood folate levels among pregnant Chinese women in areas with high and low prevalence of neural tube defects. Public Health Nutr. 2007;10(8):762–768 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Zerbo O, Iosif A-M, Walker C, Ozonoff S, Hansen RL, Hertz-Picciotto I. Is maternal influenza or fever during pregnancy associated with autism or developmental delays? Results from the CHARGE (CHildhood Autism Risks from Genetics and Environment) study. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43(1):25–33 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Atladóttir HÓ, Henriksen TB, Schendel DE, Parner ET. Autism after infection, febrile episodes, and antibiotic use during pregnancy: an exploratory study. Pediatrics. 2012;130(6). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/130/6/e1447 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Patterson PH. Immune involvement in schizophrenia and autism: etiology, pathology and animal models. Behav Brain Res. 2009;204(2):313–321 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Terrazas A. Filipino immigrants in the United States. Migration Information Source April 7, 2010. Available at: http://migrationinformation.org/USfocus/display.cfm?id=694#13. Accessed July 22, 2013
  • 70.Acevedo-Garcia D, Soobader M-J, Berkman LF. Low birthweight among US Hispanic/Latino subgroups: the effect of maternal foreign-born status and education. Soc Sci Med 1982. 2007;65(12):2503–2516 [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 71.Flores MES, Simonsen SE, Manuck TA, Dyer JM, Turok DK. The “Latina epidemiologic paradox”: contrasting patterns of adverse birth outcomes in U.S.-born and foreign-born Latinas. Womens Health Issues. 2012;22(5):e501–e507 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Romero CX, Duke JK, Dabelea D, Romero TE, Ogden LG. Does the epidemiologic paradox hold in the presence of risk factors for low birth weight infants among Mexican-born women in Colorado? J Health Care Poor Underserved. 2012;23(2):604–614 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Schieve LA, Boulet SL, Blumberg SJ, et al. Association between parental nativity and autism spectrum disorder among US-born non-Hispanic white and Hispanic children, 2007 National Survey of Children’s Health. Disabil Health J. 2012;5(1):18–25 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Croen LA, Grether JK, Hoogstrate J, Selvin S. The changing prevalence of autism in California. J Autism Dev Disord. 2002;32(3):207–215 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Lord C, Risi S, DiLavore PS, Shulman C, Thurm A, Pickles A. Autism from 2 to 9 years of age. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(6):694–701 [DOI] [PubMed]

Articles from Pediatrics are provided here courtesy of American Academy of Pediatrics

RESOURCES