Abstract
Affiliative social relationships (e.g., among spouses, family members, and friends) play an essential role in human society. These relationships affect psychological, physiological, and behavioral functions. As positive and enduring bonds are critical for the overall well-being of humans, it is not surprising that considerable effort has been made to study the neurobiological mechanisms that underlie social bonding behaviors. The present review details the involvement of the nonapeptides, oxytocin (OT), and arginine vasopressin (AVP), in the regulation of social bonding in mammals including humans. In particular, we will discuss the role of OT and AVP in the formation of social bonds between partners of a mating pair as well as between parents and their offspring. Furthermore, the role of OT and AVP in the formation of interpersonal bonding involving trust is also discussed.
Keywords: pair bond, affiliation, social recognition, oxytocin, vasopressin
Introduction
Lasting, positive, and affiliative social relationships (e.g., among spouses, family members, and friends) play an essential role in human society (for review see Baumeister and Leary, 1995). These relationships affect psychological, physiological, and behavioral functions (for review see Baumeister and Leary, 1995). In particular, the enduring attachments between socio-sexual partners (e.g., marital relationships) seem to have profound impacts on the cognitive, social, emotional, and physical well-being. For example, social bonds that provide a sense of social belonging (social connectedness) seem to protect against feelings of loneliness, depression, and even anxiety (Lee and Robbins, 1998, 2000; Williams and Galliher, 2006). Furthermore, stable and positive marital relationships have been implicated to increase the life expectancy compared to that of non-attached singles (House et al., 1988; Lillard and Waite, 1995; Drefahl, 2012). There are also strong positive associations between high levels of marital satisfaction and immune function as well as cardiovascular health (Kiecolt-Glaser and Newton, 2001; Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2010). Close parent relationships positively impact the psychological well-being of both parents and their children (Silverstein and Bengtson, 1991; Graziano et al., 2009). The presence of positive, secure attachments between parents and children provide protection against depression and anxiety in the child as well as lead to higher levels of child academic success (Bögels and Phares, 2008; Sarkadi et al., 2008; Graziano et al., 2009).
On the contrary, the disruption of social bonds (e.g., caused by marital problems, confrontations, isolation, or neglect) can have a negative impact on mental and physical health (Steptoe, 1991; Curtis, 1995). For example, low levels of intimacy between partners as well as perceived loneliness have been associated with negative psychological states, such as depression and depressive symptoms (Kiecolt-Glaser and Newton, 2001; Alpass and Neville, 2003; Adams et al., 2004). In addition, relationship conflict and interpersonal stress are associated with alterations in immune function (Kiecolt-Glaser and Newton, 2001; Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2010; Jaremka et al., 2013). Similarly, adverse childhood experiences (such as parental separation or divorce, abuse, or neglect) have detrimental effects on the child's cognitive, physical, social, and emotional well-being. Early adverse childhood experiences are associated with poor child health at an early age (e.g., increased risk for asthma as well as cardio-respiratory and infectious diseases) as well as with the adoption of unhealthy lifestyles and medical problems (including but not limited to obesity, cancer, liver disease, chronic lung disease, and cardiovascular disease) in adulthood (Flaherty et al., 2006; Lanier et al., 2010; Leeb et al., 2011; Shonkoff et al., 2012). Adverse childhood experiences may also lead to reduced academic (i.e., failing in school) and post-academic (i.e., unemployment, poverty, and homelessness) success (Shonkoff et al., 2012). The experience of interpersonal distress during childhood is also correlated with depression, aggression, and drug misuse (e.g., cigarette smoking, alcohol consumption, and illegal drug use) later in life (DeFronzo and Pawlak, 1993; Anda et al., 1999; Nation and Heflinger, 2006; Leeb et al., 2011; Shonkoff et al., 2012).
It has been shown that abnormal social attachments and deficits in social interactions are core features of most psychopathologies, including autism spectrum disorder, social phobia, obsessive-compulsive disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder, and borderline personality disorder (reivewed by Bartz and Hollander, 2006). As social bonds play important roles in the psychological, physiological, and behavioral functioning and deficits in social bonding are core features of various psychopathologies, the research investigating the neural correlates of social bond formation is highly important. Research has provided ample evidence to suggest that social interactions (including the bond observed between socio-sexual partners and between parents and offspring, as well as trust in social interactions) seem to share similar neural mechanism (e.g., Curley and Keverne, 2005).
The structurally similar neuropeptides, oxytocin (OT), and arginine vasopressin (AVP), have repeatedly been implicated in the regulation of social cognition and behavior (including social recognition, affiliation, aggression, pair bonding, as well as parental behavior) (Lim and Young, 2006; Donaldson and Young, 2008; Heinrichs et al., 2009). OT and AVP may act as neurotransmitters, if released centrally via the hypothalamus (namely the paraventricular and supraoptic nuclei of the hypothalamus), or as neurohormones, if released peripherally via the pituitary gland (Ludwig and Leng, 2006; Debiec, 2007). It is believed that the behavioral effects of OT and AVP are primarily due to the central release from the parvocellular neurons of the paraventricular nucleus (Debiec, 2007). It should be noted that various other neurochemicals, including dopamine, endogenous opioids, adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH), gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA), may also play a role in the regulation of social cognition and behavior (reviewed by Carter et al., 1995).
The following review will focus on the role of the neuropeptides OT and AVP in mediating social attachments between partners and between parents and their offspring. Such selective and enduring relationships are complex and likely depend on distinct processes, including approach behaviors, formation of social recognition memory, and subsequently the formation and maintenance of the bond. Therefore, we will discuss the role of OT and AVP in partner bonds and parent-offspring bonds focusing in particular on these aspects of social attachments. In addition, the influence of OT and AVP on relationships between close individuals, particularly trust between peers, will be discussed. We will focus exclusively on the mammalian literature, including evidence from studies in rodents, non-human primates, and humans. Finally, we will discuss how our current knowledge may shape future research to better understand the neural correlates of social bonding and its implications in pathologies.
Social bonds between partners in a mating pair
Romantic or passionate love, the intense attraction between mates, is an essential component of human social behavior and often precedes the formation of enduring preferential associations between two sexual partners. Such pair bonds are a type of social bond that occurs in nearly all human societies. While there are various definitions, a pair bond is commonly defined, across species, as an enduring preferential association between two sexually mature adults. The established social bond—a complex social behavior—is characterized by affiliative behaviors, copulation, and the selective preference toward the partner over a stranger (partner preference) (Gubernick, 1994). The bond is also often associated with mate guarding and bi-parental care of the offspring (Kleiman, 1977; Buss, 1988; Fraley et al., 2005). The significance of positive, enduring social bonds between human partners has been documented cross-culturally. For example, stable marital relationships are positively correlated with higher life expectancy compared to the life expectancy displayed by individuals who are single (House et al., 1988; Lillard and Waite, 1995; Drefahl, 2012). There are also strong positive associations between high levels of marital satisfaction and immune function as well as cardiovascular health (Kiecolt-Glaser and Newton, 2001; Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2010). As enduring social bonds between partners are important to the psychological, physiological, and behavioral well-being (for review see Baumeister and Leary, 1995), considerable efforts have been made to identify the neurobiological mechanisms underlying pair bond formation. It should be mentioned that traditional laboratory rodents usually do not display pair bond formation, a behavioral characteristic that is displayed by less than 5% of mammalian species (Kleiman, 1977; Dewsbury, 1988). Therefore, various nontraditional animal models, which display pair bond formation—including marmosets (Callithrix penicillata), titi monkeys (Callicebus cupreus), California mice (Peromyscus californicus), and prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster)—have emerged as better alternative animal models to study the neural correlates of pair bond formation (Carter et al., 1995; Bester-Meredith et al., 1999; Bales et al., 2007; de Jong et al., 2009; Smith et al., 2010). The nonapeptides, OT, and AVP, have been implicated in the regulation of social recognition and the display of prosocial/proximity-seeking behaviors—processes important for the formation of social bonds—as well as the formation of the bond itself (reviewed by Insel, 1992; Dluzen et al., 2001; Young, 2002; Bielsky and Young, 2004; Winslow and Insel, 2004; Wacker and Ludwig, 2012).
Using neuropharmacological manipulations, results suggest that OT and AVP play a role in pair bond formation across various mammalian species, including humans. Social recognition, an essential process to allow the formation of social bonds (Carter et al., 1995), allows members in a mating pair to distinguish between the familiar partner and an unfamiliar stranger. Several studies have implicated OT and AVP in mediating social recognition (reviewed by Engelmann et al., 1996; Dluzen et al., 2001; Bielsky and Young, 2004; Winslow and Insel, 2004; Wacker and Ludwig, 2012). In particular, the peripheral, central, and site-specific (e.g., into the lateral septum, main olfactory bulbs, and medial preoptic area) administration of OT facilitates social recognition in rats by increasing the retention time in a social discrimination test (Popik and van Ree, 1991; Popik et al., 1992a,b; Dluzen et al., 1998a,b, 2000), whereas the administration of a selective OT receptor, OTR, antagonist blocks short-term social recognition in rats and mice (Popik and van Ree, 1991; Engelmann et al., 1998; Dluzen et al., 2000; Ferguson et al., 2000). OT administration is also able to reverse the deficits in social memory observed in OT knockout mice (Ferguson et al., 2000, 2001). Similarly to OT, peripheral, central, and site-specific (e.g., into the lateral septum and main olfactory bulbs) AVP administration increases social recognition memory in rats (Dantzer et al., 1987, 1988; Le Moal et al., 1987; Popik et al., 1992b; Dluzen et al., 1998a,b) and rescues the social recognition impairment observed in Brattleboro rats, which carry a genetic mutation causing the lack of endogenous AVP (Engelmann and Landgraf, 1994). On the contrary, the peripheral, central, or site-specific (e.g., into the hippocampus, lateral septum, or main olfactory bulbs) administration of a non-selective AVP receptor antagonist, selective AVP receptor (V1aR) antagonist, or V1aR antisense oligodeoxynucleotide impairs social recognition memory in mice and rats (Dantzer et al., 1987, 1988; Bluthe et al., 1990; Popik et al., 1992b; Engelmann and Landgraf, 1994; Landgraf et al., 1995; van Wimersma Greidanus and Maigret, 1996; Everts and Koolhaas, 1997, 1999; Bielsky et al., 2005; Tobin et al., 2010). Rats treated site-specifically into the lateral septum with antisense oligodeoxynucleotides to the mRNA of V1aR investigate a familiar and a novel juvenile similarly, while rats treated with vehicle or scrambled-antisense oligodeoxynucleotide investigate the familiar significantly less than the novel juvenile—an indication of impaired social recognition (Landgraf et al., 1995). Similarly, the treatment with a V1aR antagonist, but not saline, into the lateral septum blocks the reduction in social investigation between a familiar and novel juveniles in rats (Dantzer et al., 1988; Popik et al., 1992b; Engelmann and Landgraf, 1994; Everts and Koolhaas, 1997, 1999; Bielsky et al., 2005). Site-specific treatment with anti-AVP serum into the ventral as well as dorsal hippocampus prevents the reduction of social investigation time between initial and second encounter of a juvenile as observed in control rats (van Wimersma Greidanus and Maigret, 1996). Treatment with an AVP antagonist into the main olfactory bulb inhibits the discrimination of a familiar vs. novel juvenile (Tobin et al., 2010). In extension to the memory-enhancing effects of OT in rats, intranasal OT administration also enhances the short- and long-term memory performance of both men and women in a face recognition test (Savaskan et al., 2008; Rimmele et al., 2009). However, it should be noted that additional research is necessary to interpret the effects of peripheral manipulations of the OT and AVP systems in affecting social recognition. In particular, research needs to systematically assess whether extrahypothalamic OT and AVP neurons are part of the neuronal circuit that is involved in social memory.
Further evidence for the involvement of OT and AVP in social bonds comes from neuropharmacological studies showing that OT and AVP play a role in the regulation of prosocial and affiliative behaviors. Such regulation is another essential component for the formation of a pair bond. For example, female prairie voles display higher levels of social behavior (including more side-by-side contact and less aggression) following a peripheral or central OT administration (Witt et al., 1990). Similarly, chronic OT administration increases social interactions of male rats with a female (as shown by an increase in the amount of side-by-side contact displayed and the duration of anogenital investigation) (Witt et al., 1992). The central OT administration increases approach, affiliative grooming (an important component of social interactions in various primate species) (Hinde and Berman, 1983), and huddle behavior toward a female in subordinate, but not dominant, male squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) (Winslow and Insel, 1991). In monogamous marmosets (C. penicillata), OT treatment increases the frequency of contact behavior, decreases the partner approach latency, and increases food sharing with the partner; whereas the treatment with a selective OTR antagonist reduces the frequency of contact behavior and decreased food sharing with the partner (Smith et al., 2010). Using adeno-viral vector-mediated (AAV) gene transfer, mice, rats, and meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus, a promiscuous and nonsocial rodent species) can be engineered to express a similar V1aR pattern as the prairie vole. In turn, this receptor expression pattern in male mice, rats, and meadow voles significantly increases the affiliative behavior toward conspecific females (Young et al., 1999; Landgraf et al., 2003; Lim et al., 2004). Furthermore, intranasal AVP administration results in an increase in affiliative behavior (e.g., higher frequency of contact time) in male titi monkeys (C. cupreus) toward their female partner (Jarcho et al., 2011). Recent studies in humans have also provided evidence for the involvement of OT in the regulation of prosocial and affiliative behaviors. Using the Relationship Closeness Induction Task (a social psychology method used to introduce strangers to each other), intranasal OT administration increases conversational intimacy in women, but not men (Liu et al., 2012). Conversational intimacy is an index of human social approach behavior (Liu et al., 2012). Furthermore, intranasal OT treatment increases positive communication behavior in relation to negative behavior during a couple conflict discussion (Ditzen et al., 2009). In addition, researchers comparing plasma OT levels between new lovers (3 months after initiation of their romantic relationship) and non-attached singles, found that OT levels do not differ between men and women, are individually stable, and are higher in new lovers than singles (Schneiderman et al., 2012). Interestingly, the OT levels were positively correlated with the level of interactive reciprocity (i.e., affectionate touch) (Schneiderman et al., 2012). Similarly to plasma OT levels, plasma AVP levels also correlate positively with social functioning. In particular, higher levels of plasma AVP levels are associated with fewer negative marital interactions (Gouin et al., 2012).
Neuropharmacological manipulations of the OT and AVP systems, in particular using the socially monogamous prairie vole as an ideal model to study the neural correlates of pair bond formation between socio-sexual partners (Dewsbury, 1975; Williams et al., 1992b), provide further evidence that OT and AVP play an essential role in pair bond formation. Central or site-specific (e.g., into the nucleus accumbens) OT treatment, compared to artificial cerebrospinal fluid treatment, results in the formation of partner preference, an index of bond formation (Insel and Hulihan, 1995), in female prairie voles (Williams et al., 1992a; Insel and Hulihan, 1995; Cho et al., 1999; Liu and Wang, 2003). While mating (and/or cohabitation) is an essential component for pair bond formation (Getz et al., 1981; Williams et al., 1992b), OT-induced partner preference formation occurs even in the absence of mating. However, when females are pre-treated with a selective OTR antagonist, the OT treatment does not result in a partner preference (Williams et al., 1992a; Insel and Hulihan, 1995; Cho et al., 1999). In addition, a selective OTR antagonist given centrally before mating or in combination with OT also inhibits the formation of a partner preference in female prairie voles without affecting mating per se (Insel and Hulihan, 1995; Cho et al., 1999). The blockade of partner preference formation due to a selective OTR antagonist indicates that OT is working via an OTR-mediated mechanism. Interestingly, the effect of central OT seems to be gender-specific as OT administration does not result in partner preference formation and selective OTR antagonist administration does not block partner preference formation in males (Williams et al., 1992a; Winslow et al., 1993; Insel and Hulihan, 1995; but see Cho et al., 1999). Similarly to OT, AVP also plays a role in the formation of partner preference in prairie voles. In particular, central or site-specific (e.g., ventral pallidum) AVP treatment increases partner preference formation in male prairie voles in the absence of mating, while the treatment with a non-selective AVP receptor antagonist or a selective V1aR antagonist blocks the formation of partner preference formation (Winslow et al., 1993; Insel and Winslow, 1998; Cho et al., 1999; Lim and Young, 2004). It should be noted that such AVP-induced partner preference formation in female prairie voles is only observed when using a high dose of AVP (Insel, 1997; Cho et al., 1999).
Studies using genetic manipulations have also provided evidence indicating that social bonds (including the underlying components of social recognition, social affiliation, and, in turn, pair bond formation) are regulated in part by the neuropeptides, OT and AVP. For example, mice and Brattleboro rats that are null-mutants of the OT or AVP gene, respectively, show deficits in a social recognition task; but social recognition can be restored by OT and AVP administration (Ferguson et al., 2000, 2001; Winslow and Insel, 2002; Choleris et al., 2006; Crawley et al., 2007; Macbeth et al., 2009). In particular, OT treatment site-specifically into the medial amygdala is able to restore social recognition in OT knockout mice, indicating that amygdala OT plays an essential role in social recognition (Ferguson et al., 2001). Profound impairments in social recognition are also observed in OTR and V1aR knockout mouse species (Bielsky et al., 2004, 2005; Takayanagi et al., 2005; Macbeth et al., 2009). The re-expression of V1aR (using AAV) in the lateral septum of V1aR knockout mice can fully restore social recognition (Bielsky et al., 2005). In addition, the overexpression of the V1aR in the lateral septum of wild-type mice and rats can potentiate social recognition (Landgraf et al., 2003; Bielsky et al., 2005). Interestingly, CD38 knockout mice also show deficits in social recognition. CD38 knockout mice lack CD38, a type-II transmembrane protein. This protein is involved in the mobilization of intracellular Ca2+ via activation of cyclic ADP ribose (cADPR), a cellular messenger for calcium signaling (Jin et al., 2007). In turn, intracellular calcium signaling plays a key role in the central release of OT (Lopatina et al., 2013). Consequently, CD38 knockout mice have reduced levels of OT and display impairments in social recognition (Jin et al., 2007; Lopatina et al., 2013).
Genetic tools have also shown the involvement of OT and AVP in the display of prosocial and affiliative behaviors. Increasing the V1aR density via AAV-mediated gene transfer increases social affiliation and also shortens the length of cohabitation necessary for partner preference formation in male prairie voles (Pitkow et al., 2001). Interestingly, transgenic mice, expressing V1aR in a pattern resembling prairie vole V1aR expression, show an increase in affiliative behaviors toward females (including olfactory investigation and grooming) (Young et al., 1999). Prosocial behavior is also potentiated in male rats and prairie voles in response to the overexpression of V1aR in the lateral septum and ventral pallidum, respectively, (Pitkow et al., 2001; Landgraf et al., 2003). Furthermore, a recent study in humans indicates that single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) of the OTR gene, namely the variants rs13316193, rs2254298, rs1042778, rs2268494, and rs226849, are associated with impairments in empathic communication (including less support-giving interactions and affective congruence) at the beginning of a romantic relationship. These impairments suggest that OT mediates human affiliative/prosocial behavior as an essential component of pair bond formation (Schneiderman et al., 2013).
Using genetic tools, researchers have also acquired evidence that OT and AVP play a role in the formation of the pair bond. For example, the overexpression of the V1aR in the ventral pallidum results in a strong partner preference formation in male prairie voles even in the absence of mating (Pitkow et al., 2001). Similarly, AAV-mediated V1aR gene transfer into the ventral forebrain of the promiscuous meadow vole (M. pennsylvanicus) enhances partner preference formation, a behavior that is not typically displayed by meadow voles (Lim et al., 2004). On the contrary, the knockdown of V1aR in the ventral pallidum of male prairie voles causes a deficit in partner preference formation (Barrett et al., 2013). Lastly, a number of recent findings suggest that OT and AVP may also play a role in the regulation of social bonding in humans. In particular, researchers assessed whether genetic variations in the OT and AVP receptor genes are associated with pair bonding and social behavior. The SNP in the OTR gene (rs7632287) seems to be associated with pair bonding in women. Women carrying one or two A-alleles score lower on the partner bonding scale and the relationship quality survey as well as are more likely to report martial problems than woman carrying two G alleles (Walum et al., 2012). The variation in the V1aR gene, AVPR1A, also seems to contribute to differences in human pair bonding. The allelic variant, RS3 334, is associated in men, but not women, with a lower bonding quality with the partner (characterized by lower scores on the partner bonding scale and a greater likelihood of reporting martial problems) (Walum et al., 2008). Interestingly, the association between the gene variants of the OTR and AVPR1A and the quality of pair bonding is also detected in the partner's perception of marital satisfaction (Walum et al., 2008, 2012).
Parental bonding
Most mammalian offspring, including human children, are dependent on parental care for survival (Bowlby, 1951; Ainsworth, 1979; Nowak et al., 2000). For the young, parental care is essential to their individual survival; for the parent, parental care—characterized by one or both parents providing offspring with nutrition, warmth, shelter, as well as predatory protection—is an essential component of mammalian fitness (de Jong et al., 2009). Positive, enduring bonds between parents and their children play a significant role for the psychological, physiological, and behavioral functions of both parents and children (Silverstein and Bengtson, 1991; Graziano et al., 2009). For example, the presence of positive, secure attachments between parents and children provide protection against depression and anxiety in the child as well as lead to higher levels of child academic success (Bögels and Phares, 2008; Sarkadi et al., 2008; Graziano et al., 2009). On the contrary, the lack of positive, enduring parental bonding is associated with poor child health at an early age (e.g., increased risk for asthma, as well as cardio-respiratory and infectious diseases) as well as with the adoption of unhealthy lifestyles and medical problems (including but not limited to obesity, cancer, liver disease, chronic lung disease, and cardiovascular disease) in adulthood (Flaherty et al., 2006; Lanier et al., 2010; Leeb et al., 2011; Shonkoff et al., 2012). In addition, the lack of positive parental bonds can also lead to reduced academic success, depression, aggression, and drug misuse (DeFronzo and Pawlak, 1993; Anda et al., 1999; Nation and Heflinger, 2006; Leeb et al., 2011; Shonkoff et al., 2012). As positive social bonds between parents and their children are essential for the psychological, physiological, and behavioral well-being of both parents and child, considerable efforts have been expanded to identify the neurobiological mechanisms underlying parental bond formation. Various animal models, primarily rats and sheep, have been used to study the neural correlates of maternal behavior (Kendrick, 2000). Very little is known about paternal behavior, which is relatively rare among mammals and is displayed by less than 5% of mammalian species that are monogamous, including humans (Kleiman, 1977). More recently, untraditional animal models including the male California mouse and the male prairie vole have emerged to study the neurobiological mechanism underlying paternal behavior (Kentner et al., 2010). Parental bonding depends on the approach (including the display of prosocial behavior), social recognition of the offspring, and subsequently the formation of a selective and enduring bond between parent and offspring. This complex social behavior seems to be regulated in part by OT and AVP (Kendrick, 2000; de Jong et al., 2009; Bosch and Neumann, 2012; Nagasawa et al., 2012).
Through neuropharmacological manipulations, evidence has accumulated suggesting that OT and AVP play a role in parental bond formation across various different mammalian species, including humans. The initiation of parental behavior depends on the approach and display of prosocial behavior, a process that seems to be facilitated in part by the neuropeptides, OT, and AVP. Central OT administration reduces the latency of female rats to show maternal behavior when exposed to pups (Pedersen and Prange, 1979; Pedersen et al., 1982; Fahrbach et al., 1984) and increases maternal behavior in mice (McCarthy, 1990). The central administration of the OTR agonist, Thr4, Fly7-OT, has a similar potency to OT in stimulating maternal behavior (Kendrick, 2000). In addition, central, but not peripheral, OT administration enhances the interest of nulliparous ewes toward newborns, increases the display of maternal behavior, and induces maternal responsiveness even without the experience of natural parturition (Kendrick et al., 1987; Keverne and Kendrick, 1992; Levy et al., 1992). Administration of an OTR antagonist site-specifically into the nucleus accumbens inhibits “spontaneous” maternal behavior in sexually naïve female prairie voles, which provides further evidence that OT may play a role in maternal behavior (Olazabal and Young, 2006). Furthermore, a correlational study in humans suggests the involvement of OT in maternal behavior. In particular, an increase in plasma OT level from the first to the third trimester is linked with maternal bonding to the fetus during the third trimester (Levine et al., 2007). Additionally, high levels of OT in the first trimester predict the amount of postpartum maternal behavior (including gaze, positive affect, and “motherese” vocalizations) (Feldman et al., 2007). Similarly to OT, central AVP administration also induces maternal behavior in rats (Pedersen et al., 1982). Chronic central AVP administration or overexpression of V1aR in the medial preoptic area (using AAV) increases arched back nursing behavior in female rats (Bosch and Neumann, 2008). On the contrary, antagonizing OT (via the administration of a selective OTR antagonist, antisera to OT, or an analog OT antagonist) centrally or site-specifically (e.g., into the medial preoptic area, olfactory bulb, or ventral tegmental area) can delay or block the onset of maternal behavior in rats (Fahrbach et al., 1985; Pedersen et al., 1985, 1994; van Leengoed et al., 1987; Yu et al., 1996) as well as reduce the display of arched back nursing behavior in rats (Bosch and Neumann, 2008). Similarly, the central or site-specific (e.g., into the medial preoptic area) administration of AVP antisera, V1aR antisense oligodeoxynucleotide, or V1aR antagonist decreases maternal behavior (Pedersen et al., 1985, 1994; Bosch and Neumann, 2008). Unfortunately due to the lack of an appropriate animal model to study the neural correlates of paternal behavior, only recently have researchers started to acquire knowledge about the underlying neural mechanisms. Congruent with the OT and AVP regulation of maternal affiliative behaviors toward pups (reviewed by Bosch and Neumann, 2012), these neuropeptides may also play a role in the onset of paternal affiliative behaviors toward pups. For example, parentally naïve male California mice show higher levels of OT when housed with a pregnant female than sexually naïve males or new fathers (Gubernick et al., 1995). There is also a transient increase of OT in male prairie voles that have been exposed to pups (Kenkel et al., 2012). Central treatment with OT, but not vehicle, in male common marmosets reduces the frequency of refusal of food transfer to the infant (Saito and Nakamura, 2011). As food transfer from mother or father to infant is considered caretaking behavior, the OT-induced reduction in food transfer refusal indicates an increase in parental behavior. There is evidence to suggest that AVP may also be involved. In early prairie vole studies, new fathers, compared to sexually naïve males, showed an increase in AVP mRNA expression in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (Wang et al., 1994b), but showed a decreased density of AVP-immunoreactive fibers in the lateral septum (Bamshad et al., 1993). These changes in the AVP system suggest an increased AVP release in the lateral septum, which is associated with the enhanced display of paternal behavior in the father voles (Wang et al., 1998). In a pharmacological experiment, the administration of AVP into the lateral septum of sexually naïve male prairie voles enhances paternal behavior (including grooming, crouching over, and contacting pups); whereas the administration of an AVP receptor antagonist inhibits paternal behavior (Wang et al., 1994a; Bales et al., 2004). These data demonstrate the functional role of central AVP in paternal behavior. Interestingly, central AVP treatment can also induce facultative paternal behavior in the typically non-paternal meadow vole, which can be blocked by the central treatment with a selective V1aR antagonist (Parker and Lee, 2001). In addition to the evidence that AVP plays a role in mediating paternal behavior in rodents, a study using male marmosets (C. jacchus) showed that fathers, both first time and experienced ones, have greater dendritic spine density on pyramidal neurons in the prefrontal cortex as compared to non-father marmosets (Kozorovitskiy et al., 2006). Most interestingly, the density of AVP V1aR and the proportion of V1aR-labeled dendritic spines increased significantly in fathers compared to non-fathers (Kozorovitskiy et al., 2006). These data indicate that AVP is involved in the neural reorganization associated with the experience of fatherhood in a bi-parental primate species. There is also evidence to suggest that OT and AVP interact in the regulation of paternal care. Bales et al. (2004) noted that only the combined treatment with OT and AVP antagonists fully reduces paternal care (i.e., inhibit kyphosis, pup approach, nonkyphotic contact, licking/grooming, and retrieving) in naïve male prairie voles. Furthermore, there is evidence indicating that OT and AVP may play a role in human parental behavior. In particular, the OT concentration in the mother's cerebrospinal fluid is significantly increased following birth (Takagi et al., 1985) and plasma OT levels after birth are correlated with maternal behavior (including gaze, vocalizations, positive affect, and affectionate touch) (Feldman et al., 2007). In addition, plasma OT levels show intra-individual stability across the first 6 months of parenthood and are positively correlated with maternal and paternal behavior (including “motherese” vocalizations, positive affect, and affectionate touch) (Feldman et al., 2010, 2011; Gordon et al., 2010a,c; Apter-Levi et al., 2013). Similarly, plasma AVP levels predict maternal and paternal behavior (particularly joint attention to inanimate objects and stimulatory contact) to their infant (Apter-Levi et al., 2013). Furthermore, triadic synchrony (defined as moments in which physical proximity and affectionate touch is displayed between parents and between parent and infant) is correlated with plasma OT (Gordon et al., 2010b).
Pharmacological manipulations suggest that the nonapeptides, OT and AVP, not only play a role in the initiation of maternal behavior, but also have an important role in mediating social recognition, another vital component for parental bonding. Indeed, social recognition between mothers and their offspring has been shown in various species including sheep (Ferreira et al., 2000), goats (Poindron et al., 2003), Southern pig-tailed macaques (Macaca nemestrina) (Jensen, 1965), Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus) (Hammerschmidt and Fischer, 1998), rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) (Jovanovic et al., 2000), and even rodents (Ostermeyer and Elwood, 1983). Using the sheep as a model to study the neural correlates for maternal bonding, OT has been shown to play a critical role in the olfactory recognition of the lamb (Kendrick et al., 1997). Once a ewe has formed such social recognition memory, it can distinguish its lamb from others and will reject all lambs other than their own—indicating the formation of a mother-offspring bond (Kendrick, 2000). Nephew and Bridges (2008) showed that the site-specific administration (into the medial amygdala, but not the lateral ventricles) of a V1aR antagonist blocks social recognition in female rats. While only very few studies have been performed to assess parent-offspring bonding in humans, it is still plausible to hypothesize that such parent-child bonding does occur in humans. In particular, human mothers are capable of recognizing their own infant within 30 min after birth (Porter et al., 1983).
Additional support of the involvement of OT and AVP in the initiation of parental behavior, comes from genetic studies. OTR and paternally expressed gene 3 (peg3) knockout mice display deficits in maternal behavior including nurturing of pups, nest building, pup retrieval, and crouching behavior (Li et al., 1999; Ragnauth et al., 2005; Pedersen et al., 2006; Champagne et al., 2009). The gene peg3 codes for a large zinc finger protein, a transcription factor that has been implicated in p53-mediated apoptosis (Deng and Wu, 2000; Relaix et al., 2000), which may account for the reduction in hypothalamic OT neurons and OTR in peg3 knockout mice (Relaix et al., 1998; Li et al., 1999). Further, Brattleboro rats, which are genetically incapable of producing AVP, also show deficits in maternal care evidenced by a lower survival rate of offspring (Wideman and Murphy, 1990). Female and male CD38 knockout mice, which exhibit low central OT levels in comparison to wild-type mice, as mentioned above, also display deficits in parental behavior (i.e., lower rate of pup retrieval and crouching over pups) (Jin et al., 2007; Akther et al., 2013). The re-expression of CD38 (using AAV) in the nucleus accumbens and OT administration in CD38 knockout mice can restore paternal behavior (including retrieval, pup grooming, crouching, and huddling) (Akther et al., 2013). In addition, the SNP of the prairie vole V1aR gene (avpr1a) is associated with parental behavior. Specifically, male prairie voles with the allele coding for a long avpr1a microsatellite display higher levels of licking/grooming behavior than male prairie voles with the short avpr1a microsatellite (Hammock and Young, 2005). Microsatellites, or short tandem repeats, consisting of one to six nucleotides, which are repeated several times and therefore contribute to genetic variation (Guichoux et al., 2011).
In humans, SNPs in the genes for the OTR and CD38 have been implicated in parental behavior. In particular, the OTR gene alleles rs2254298 and rs1042778 as well as the CD38 gene allele rs3796863 are associated with lower plasma OT levels, and in turn, lower rates of parental care (i.e., shorter duration of parent-infant gaze synchrony and less amount of touch toward the infant) (Feldman et al., 2012). Future studies are needed to address whether genetic manipulations can show the involvement of OT and AVP in the paternal recognition of offspring and the parental-offspring bond formation per se.
Interpersonal bonding
Similarly to the importance of attachment between partners of a mating pair and between parents and their offspring, mutually cooperative interactions involving the display of trust are an essential aspect of human society (Luhmann, 1979; Coleman, 1990). Trust is essential in love, families, and friendships. While a lot of effort has been expanded to investigate the neural correlates of social behavior, the neural mechanisms underlying trust are just beginning to be examined. Interestingly, it appears that the neuropeptide OT is not only involved in facilitating various social behaviors, such as pair bonding (Dluzen and Carter, 1979; Insel and Shapiro, 1992; Insel, 1997; Carter, 1998) and maternal attachment (Insel and Young, 2001; Bosch and Neumann, 2012), but has also been implicated in playing a role in trust (reviewed by De Dreu et al., 2010).
Research on the role of OT in regulating the display of trust revealed that OT levels increase in response to the display of intentional trust in a trust game with real monetary stakes. In particular, the level of peripheral OT (as assessed using blood) increases in subjects who received an intentional trust signal (Zak et al., 2004, 2005). However, it should be noted that currently the relationship between peripheral OT and central OT in humans is still unknown. Nevertheless, the findings of increased peripheral OT in response to intentional trust should be considered in the light of the hyperfunctional magnetic resonance imaging (hyperfMRI) study by Krueger et al. (2007). The authors report significant activity in the septal area when subjects display trust intentionally. Based on evidence from rodent studies, the septal area is involved in the regulation of social behavior (including pair bonding and paternal behavior) (Young et al., 2005; Skuse and Gallagher, 2009). In addition, the lateral septum via its connections to the nucleus accumbens may also represent a potential link between social behavior and reward circuitries (Young et al., 2005; Skuse and Gallagher, 2009). Furthermore, intranasal administration of OT increases interpersonal trust (see review by Van Ijzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2012), without affecting non-social risk-taking behaviors (Kosfeld et al., 2005). Specifically, 13 out of 29 subjects who received nasal spray containing OT showed the maximal trust level (i.e., sending all the money to another person in the “trust game” with real monetary stakes), whereas only 6 out of 29 subjects who received placebo showed maximal trust (Kosfeld et al., 2005). It should be noted that OT-induced trust only increases in the absence of cues that the partner is untrustworthy (Mikolajczak et al., 2010a). Exogenous administration of OT also increases the trust displayed toward one's own group (in-group trust). Using the intergroup prisoner's dilemma-maximizing difference game, it has been shown that intranasal OT increases in-group trust as displayed by an increase in the (monetary) contributions to the in-group, without affecting out-group distrust (De Dreu et al., 2010). In addition to the trust-increasing effects of OT in a monetary scenario, exogenous OT also increases trust in non-monetary scenarios. It has been observed that intranasal OT decreases the perceived risk of betrayal and increased trust in a scenario involving intimate and confidential information (Mikolajczak et al., 2010b). In particular, 60% of the participants who received OT, compared to only 3% of participants who received placebo, left a letter containing intimate and confidential information unsealed (Mikolajczak et al., 2010b). In addition, intranasal OT increases a person's willingness to share emotions, without affecting level of talkativeness (Lane et al., 2013).
Additional evidence for the involvement of OT in the regulation of trust comes from genetic studies (reviewed by Donaldson and Young, 2008). It should be noted that trust behavior appears to be heritable, indicating that a specific gene may be responsible for the variations in trust observed across individuals (Cesarini et al., 2008). In particular, using genetic screening for the OTR gene and the “trust game” showed that the rs53576, rs1042778, rs2268490, and rs237887 SNPs of the OTR gene is associated with greater trusting behavior (Israel et al., 2009; Krueger et al., 2012). Similarly to OT, AVP seems to play a role in mediating trust as well. In particular, the AVPR1A promoter region length (rs3 microsatellite polymorphism) is associated with greater money allocation, suggesting greater trusting behavior, in the “trust game” (Knafo et al., 2008).
Conclusion
Over the past decades, the understanding of the neurobiological mechanism underlying bonding behaviors has substantially increased. In particular, studies using various animal models have provided evidence of the involvement of the nonapeptides, OT and AVP, in the regulation of social bonding (including bonding between mates in a mating pair and bonding between parent and offspring) (see Table 1). In addition, experimental techniques (such as intranasal OT administration and gene sequencing) in humans have resulted in evidence suggesting that these nonapeptides may also play a role in the regulation of bonding in humans. However, it should be noted that other neurochemicals, such as prolactin, sex hormones, catecholamines, endogenous opiates, and GABA may also play a role in the regulation of social behaviors. Thus, future research is needed to determine the interplay of these various neurochemicals to regulate social bonding across various species. It is also of importance to mention that there are instances of sexual dimorphisms in the regulation of social bonding by OT and AVP (e.g., OT and AVP differently regulate social bonds in female and male prairie voles). Therefore, sex differences in the regulation of social bonding warrant further investigation.
Table 1.
Type of bond | Treatment | Species | Effect | References |
---|---|---|---|---|
PAIR BOND | ||||
Social recognition | Peripheral OT | Rat | ↑ | Popik et al., 1992a |
Central OT | OT KO mouse | ↑ | Ferguson et al., 2000 | |
Site-specific OT (e.g., AMY, LS, MPOA, OB) | OT KO mouse, rat | ↑ | Popik and van Ree, 1991; Popik et al., 1992b; Dluzen et al., 1998a,b, 2000; Ferguson et al., 2001; Winslow and Insel, 2002 | |
Intranasal OT | Human | ↑ | Savaskan et al., 2008; Rimmele et al., 2009 | |
Central OTR-A | Rat, mouse | ↓ | Engelmann et al., 1998; Ferguson et al., 2000 | |
Site-specific OTR-A (e.g., MPOA, OB) | Rat | ↓ | Popik and van Ree, 1991; Dluzen et al., 2000 | |
Peripheral AVP | Rat | ↑ | Dantzer et al., 1987 | |
Central AVP | Rat | ↑ | Le Moal et al., 1987 | |
Site-specific AVP (e.g., LS, OB) | Brattleboro rat, rat, V1aR KO mouse | ↑ | Dantzer et al., 1988; Popik et al., 1992b; Engelmann and Landgraf, 1994; Dluzen et al., 1998a,b; Landgraf et al., 2003; Bielsky et al., 2005 | |
Central AVP-A | Rat | ↓ | Bluthe et al., 1990; van Wimersma Greidanus and Maigret, 1996 | |
Site-specific AVP antisense, AVP-A, or V1aR-A (e.g., hippocampus, LS, OB) | Mouse, rat | ↓ | Dantzer et al., 1988; Popik et al., 1992b; Engelmann and Landgraf, 1994; Landgraf et al., 1995; van Wimersma Greidanus and Maigret, 1996; Everts and Koolhaas, 1997, 1999; Bielsky et al., 2005; Tobin et al., 2010 | |
Prosocial behavior | Peripheral OT | Rat, prairie vole | ↑ | Witt et al., 1990 |
Central OT | Prairie vole, rat, marmoset, squirrel monkey | ↑ | Witt et al., 1990, 1992; Winslow and Insel, 1991; Smith et al., 2010 | |
Intranasal OT | Human | ↑ | Liu et al., 2012 | |
Central OTR-A | Marmoset | ↓ | Smith et al., 2010 | |
Intranasal AVP | Titi monkey | ↑ | Jarcho et al., 2011 | |
Bond | Central OT | Prairie vole | ↑ | Williams et al., 1992a; Insel and Hulihan, 1995; Cho et al., 1999 |
Site-specifc OT (e.g., NAcc) | Prairie vole | ↑ | Liu and Wang, 2003 | |
Central OTR-A | Prairie vole | ↓ | Insel and Hulihan, 1995; Cho et al., 1999 | |
Central AVP | Prairie vole | ↑ | Winslow et al., 1993 | |
Central AVPR-A or V1aR-A | Prairie vole | ↓ | Winslow et al., 1993; Insel and Winslow, 1998; Cho et al., 1999 | |
Site-specific V1aR-A (e.g., VP) | Prairie vole | ↓ | Lim and Young, 2004 | |
PARENTAL BOND | ||||
Social recognition | OT or OTR-A | na | na | na |
AVP or AVPR-A | na | na | na | |
Prosocial behavior | Central OT | ♂ common marmoset, ♀ mice, ♀ rat, ♀ sheep | ↑ | Pedersen and Prange, 1979; Pedersen et al., 1982; Fahrbach et al., 1984; Kendrick et al., 1987; McCarthy, 1990; Keverne and Kendrick, 1992; Levy et al., 1992; Saito and Nakamura, 2011 |
Site-specific OT (e.g., NAcc) | ♀♂ CD38KO mouse | ↑ | Akther et al., 2013 | |
Central OTR-A | ♀ rats | ↓ | Fahrbach et al., 1985; Pedersen et al., 1985; van Leengoed et al., 1987 | |
Site-specific OTR-A (e.g., MPOA, NAcc, OB, and VTA) | ♀ prairie voles, ♀ rats | ↓ | Pedersen et al., 1994; Yu et al., 1996; Olazabal and Young, 2006 | |
Central AVP | ♂ meadow voles, ♀ rats | ↑ | Pedersen et al., 1982; Parker and Lee, 2001 | |
Site-specific AVP (e.g., LS) | ♂ prairie vole | ↑ | Wang et al., 1994a | |
Central AVPR-A | ♂ meadow voles, ♀ rats | ↓ | Pedersen et al., 1985; Parker and Lee, 2001; Bosch and Neumann, 2008 | |
Site-specific AVPR-A or V1aR-A (e.g., MPOA and LS) | Rats, ♂ prairie vole | ↓ | Pedersen et al., 1994; Wang et al., 1994a | |
Central AVP-A and OTR-A | ♂ prairie vole | ↓ | Bales et al., 2004 | |
Bond | OT or OTR-A | na | na | na |
AVP or AVPR-A | na | na | na | |
TRUST | Intranasal OT | Human | ↑ | Kosfeld et al., 2005; De Dreu et al., 2010; Mikolajczak et al., 2010a,b |
Abbreviations used: AMY, amygdala; AVP, argenine vasopressin; AVP-A, non-selective AVP receptor antagonist; LS, lateral septum; MPOA, medial preoptic area; NAcc, nucleus accumbens; OB, olfacotry bulbs; OT, oxytocin, OTR-A, oxytocin receptor antagonist; VP, ventral pallidum; VTA, ventral tegmental area.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to Hans Jorgensen for his critical reading of the manuscript. This manuscript was supported by NIH grant MHR01-058616.
Glossary
Abbreviations
- AAV
adeno-associated viral vector
- AVP
arginine vasopressin
- GABA
gamma-aminobutyric acid
- OT
oxytocin
- OTR
oxytocin receptor
- V1aR
selective AVP receptor 1
- SNP
single nucleotide polymorphism.
References
- Adams K. B., Sanders S., Auth E. A. (2004). Loneliness and depression in independent living retirement communities: risk and resilience factors. Aging Ment. Health 8, 475–485 10.1080/13607860410001725054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ainsworth M. S. (1979). Infant–mother attachment. Am. Psychol. 34, 932–937 10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.932 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Akther S., Korshnova N., Zhong J., Liang M., Cherepanov S. M., Lopatina O., et al. (2013). CD38 in the nucleus accumbens and oxytocin are related to paternal behavior in mice. Mol. Brain 6, 1–10 10.1186/1756-6606-6-41 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alpass F. M., Neville S. (2003). Loneliness, health and depression in older males. Aging Ment. Health 7, 212–216 10.1080/1360786031000101193 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anda R. F., Croft J. B., Felitti V. J., Nordenberg D., Giles W. H., Williamson D. F., et al. (1999). Adverse childhood experiences and smoking during adolescence and adulthood. JAMA 282, 1652–1658 10.1001/jama.282.17.1652 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Apter-Levi Y., Zagoory-Sharon O., Feldman R. (2013). Oxytocin and vasopressin support distinct configurations of social synchrony. Brain Res. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.1010.1052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bales K. L., Kim A. J., Lewis-Reese A. D., Sue Carter C. (2004). Both oxytocin and vasopressin may influence alloparental behavior in male prairie voles. Horm. Behav. 45, 354–361 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.01.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bales K. L., Mason W. A., Catana C., Cherry S. R., Mendoza S. P. (2007). Neural correlates of pair-bonding in a monogamous primate. Brain Res. 1184, 245–253 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.09.087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bamshad M., Novak M. A., De Vries G. J. (1993). Sex and species differences in the vasopressin innervation of sexually naive and parental prairie voles, Microtus ochrogaster and meadow voles, Microtus pennsylvanicus. J. Neuroendocrinol. 5, 247–255 10.1111/j.1365-2826.1993.tb00480.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett C. E., Keebaugh A. C., Ahern T. H., Bass C. E., Terwilliger E. F., Young L. J. (2013). Variation in vasopressin receptor (Avpr1a) expression creates diversity in behaviors related to monogamy in prairie voles. Horm. Behav. 63, 518–526 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.01.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartz J. A., Hollander E. (2006). The neuroscience of affiliation: forging links between basic and clinical research on neuropeptides and social behavior. Horm. Behav. 50, 518–528 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.06.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baumeister R. F., Leary M. R. (1995). The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol. Bull. 117, 497–529 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bester-Meredith J. K., Young L. J., Marler C. A. (1999). Species differences in paternal behavior and aggression in peromyscus and their associations with vasopressin immunoreactivity and receptors. Horm. Behav. 36, 25–38 10.1006/hbeh.1999.1522 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bielsky I. F., Hu S.-B., Ren X., Terwilliger E. F., Young L. J. (2005). The V1a vasopressin receptor is necessary and sufficient for normal social recognition: a gene replacement study. Neuron 47, 503–513 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.06.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bielsky I. F., Hu S. B., Szegda K. L., Westphal H., Young L. J. (2004). Profound impairment in social recognition and reduction in anxiety-like behavior in vasopressin V1a receptor knockout mice. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 483–493 10.1038/sj.npp.1300360 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bielsky I. F., Young L. J. (2004). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and social recognition in mammals. Peptides 25, 1565–1574 10.1016/j.peptides.2004.05.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bluthe R.-M., Schoenen J., Dantzer R. (1990). Androgen-dependent vasopressinergic neurons are involved in social recognition in rats. Brain Res. 519, 150–157 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90073-K [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bögels S., Phares V. (2008). Fathers' role in the etiology, prevention and treatment of child anxiety: a review and new model. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 28, 539–558 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.07.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bosch O. J., Neumann I. D. (2008). Brain vasopressin is an important regulator of maternal behavior independent of dams' trait anxiety. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 17139–17144 10.1073/pnas.0807412105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bosch O. J., Neumann I. D. (2012). Both oxytocin and vasopressin are mediators of maternal care and aggression in rodents: from central release to sites of action. Horm. Behav. 61, 293–303 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowlby J. (1951). Maternal Care and Mental Health. Geneva: World Health Organization; [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buss D. M. (1988). From vigilance to violence: tactics of mate retention in American undergraduates. Ethol. Sociobiol. 9, 291–317 10.1016/0162-3095(88)90010-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Carter C. S. (1998). Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 779–818 10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00055-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter C. S., DeVries A. C., Getz L. L. (1995). Physiological substrates of mammalian monogamy: the prairie vole model. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 19, 303–314 10.1016/0149-7634(94)00070-H [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cesarini D., Dawes C. T., Fowler J. H., Johannesson M., Lichtenstein P., Wallace B. (2008). Heritability of cooperative behavior in the trust game. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 3721–3726 10.1073/pnas.0710069105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Champagne F. A., Curley J. P., Swaney W. T., Hasen N. S., Keverne E. B. (2009). Paternal influence on female behavior: the role of Peg3 in exploration, olfaction, and neuroendocrine regulation of maternal behavior of female mice. Behav. Neurosci. 123, 469–480 10.1037/a0015060 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cho M. M., DeVries A. C., Williams J. R., Carter C. S. (1999). The effects of oxytocin and vasopressin on partner preferences in male and female prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Behav. Neurosci. 113, 1071–1079 10.1037/0735-7044.113.5.1071 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choleris E., Ogawa S., Kavaliers M., Gustafsson J. A., Korach K. S., Muglia L. J., et al. (2006). Involvement of estrogen receptor α, β and oxytocin in social discrimination: a detailed behavioral analysis with knockout female mice. Genes Brain Behav. 5, 528–539 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2006.00203.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coleman J. (1990). Foundations of Social Theory. Cambridge, MA: Belknap [Google Scholar]
- Crawley J. N., Chen T., Puri A., Washburn R., Sullivan T. L., Hill J. M., et al. (2007). Social approach behaviors in oxytocin knockout mice: comparison of two independent lines tested in different laboratory environments. Neuropeptides 41, 145–163 10.1016/j.npep.2007.02.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curley J. P., Keverne E. B. (2005). Genes, brains and mammalian social bonds. Trends Ecol. Evol. 20, 561–567 10.1016/j.tree.2005.05.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curtis R. (1995). Stress, personality and illness: the move from generality to specificity in current research trends. Irish J. Psychol. 16, 299–321 10.1080/03033910.1995.10558067 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dantzer R., Bluthe R. M., Koob G. F., Le Moal M. (1987). Modulation of social memory in male rats by neurohypophyseal peptides. Psychopharmacology(Berl) 91, 363–368 10.1007/BF00518192 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dantzer R., Koob G. F., Bluthe' R.-M., Le Moal M. (1988). Septal vasopressin modulates social memory in male rats. Brain Res. 457, 143–147 10.1016/0006-8993(88)90066-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Debiec J. (2007). From affiliative behaviors to romantic feelings: a role of nanopeptides. FEBS Lett. 581, 2580–2586 10.1016/j.febslet.2007.03.095 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Dreu C. K., Greer L. L., Handgraaf M. J., Shalvi S., Van Kleef G. A., Baas M., et al. (2010). The neuropeptide oxytocin regulates parochial altruism in intergroup conflict among humans. Science 328, 1408–1411 10.1126/science.1189047 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeFronzo J., Pawlak R. (1993). Effects of social bonds and childhood experiences on alcohol abuse and smoking. J. Soc. Psychol. 133, 635–642 10.1080/00224545.1993.9713918 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Jong T. R., Chauke M., Harris B. N., Saltzman W. (2009). From here to paternity: neural correlates of the onset of paternal behavior in California mice (Peromyscus californicus). Horm. Behav. 56, 220–231 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deng Y., Wu X. (2000). Peg3/Pw1 promotes p53-mediated apoptosis by inducing Bax translocation from cytosol to mitochondria. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 12050–12055 10.1073/pnas.97.22.12050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dewsbury D. A. (1975). Diversity and adaptation in rodent copulatory behavior. Science 190, 947–954 10.1126/science.1188377 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dewsbury D. A. (1988). The Comparative Psychology of Monogamy. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press; [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ditzen B., Schaer M., Gabriel B., Bodenmann G., Ehlert U., Heinrichs M. (2009). Intranasal oxytocin increases positive communication and reduces cortisol levels during couple conflict. Biol. Psychiatry 65, 728–731 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dluzen D. E., Carter C. S. (1979). Ovarian hormones regulating sexual and social behaviors in female prairie voles, Microtus ochrogaster. Physiol. Behav. 23, 597–600 10.1016/0031-9384(79)90063-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dluzen D. E., Muraoka S., Engelmann M., Ebner K., Landgraf R. (2000). Oxytocin induces preservation of social recognition in male rats by activating alpha-adrenoceptors of the olfactory bulb. Eur. J. Neurosci. 12, 760–766 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00952.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dluzen D. E., Muraoka S., Engelmann M., Landgraf R. (1998a). The effects of infusion of arginine vasopressin, oxytocin, or their antagonists into the olfactory bulb upon social recognition responses in male rats. Peptides 19, 999–1005 10.1016/S0196-9781(98)00047-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dluzen D. E., Muraoka S., Landgraf R. (1998b). Olfactory bulb norepinephrine depletion abolishes vasopressin and oxytocin preservation of social recognition responses in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 254, 161–164 10.1016/S0304-3940(98)00691-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dluzen D. E., Shang Y., Landgraf R. (2001). Oxytocin, norepinephrine and olfacotry bulb mediated recogntion, in Chemical Signals in Vertebrates Nine, eds Marchlewska-Koj A., Lepri J. J., Müller-Schwarze D. (New York, NY: Springer US; ), 107–115 10.1007/978-1-4615-0671-3_14 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Donaldson Z. R., Young L. J. (2008). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of sociality. Science 322, 900–904 10.1126/science.1158668 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drefahl S. (2012). Do the married really live longer? The role of cohabitation and socioeconomic status. J. Marriage Fam. 74, 462–475 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.00968.x14666221 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Engelmann M., Ebner K., Wotjak C. T., Landgraf R. (1998). Endogenous oxytocin is involved in short-term olfactory memory in female rats. Behav. Brain Res. 90, 89–94 10.1016/S0166-4328(97)00084-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engelmann M., Landgraf R. (1994). Microdialysis administration of vasopressin into the septum improves social recognition in Brattleboro rats. Physiol. Behav. 55, 145–149 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90022-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engelmann M., Wotjak C. T., Neumann I., Ludwig M., Landgraf R. (1996). Behavioral consequences of intracerebral vasopressin and oxytocin: focus on learning and memory. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 20, 341–358 10.1016/0149-7634(95)00059-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everts H. G., Koolhaas J. M. (1997). Lateral septal vasopressin in rats: role in social and object recognition? Brain Res. 760, 1–7 10.1016/S0006-8993(97)00269-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everts H. G., Koolhaas J. M. (1999). Differential modulation of lateral septal vasopressin receptor blockade in spatial learning, social recognition, and anxiety-related behaviors in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 99, 7–16 10.1016/S0166-4328(98)00004-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fahrbach S. E., Morrell J. I., Pfaff D. W. (1984). Oxytocin induction of short-latency maternal behavior in nulliparous, estrogen-primed female rats. Horm. Behav. 18, 267–286 10.1016/0018-506X(84)90016-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fahrbach S. E., Morrell J. I., Pfaff D. W. (1985). Possible role for endogenous oxytocin in estrogen-facilitated maternal behavior in rats. Neuroendocrinology 40, 526–532 10.1159/000124125 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman R., Gordon I., Schneiderman I., Weisman O., Zagoory-Sharon O. (2010). Natural variations in maternal and paternal care are associated with systematic changes in oxytocin following parent–infant contact. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 1133–1141 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.01.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman R., Gordon I., Zagoory-Sharon O. (2011). Maternal and paternal plasma, salivary, and urinary oxytocin and parent–infant synchrony: considering stress and affiliation components of human bonding. Dev. Sci. 14, 752–761 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01021.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman R., Weller A., Zagoory-Sharon O., Levine A. (2007). Evidence for a neuroendocrinological foundation of human affiliation: plasma oxytocin levels across pregnancy and the postpartum period predict mother-infant bonding. Psychol. Sci. 18, 965 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02010.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman R., Zagoory-Sharon O., Weisman O., Schneiderman I., Gordon I., Maoz R., et al. (2012). Sensitive parenting is associated with plasma oxytocin and polymorphisms in the OXTR and CD38 genes. Biol. Psychiatry 72, 175–181 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson J. N., Aldag J. M., Insel T. R., Young L. J. (2001). Oxytocin in the medial amygdala is essential for social recognition in the mouse. J. Neurosci. 21, 8278–8285 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson J. N., Young L. J., Hearn E. F., Matzuk M. M., Insel T. R., Winslow J. T. (2000). Social amnesia in mice lacking the oxytocin gene. Nat. Genet. 25, 284–288 10.1038/77040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferreira G., Terrazas A., Poindron P., Nowak R., Orgeur P., Lévy F. (2000). Learning of olfactory cues is not necessary for early lamb recognition by the mother. Physiol. Behav. 69, 405–412 10.1016/S0031-9384(00)00211-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flaherty E. G., Thompson R., Litrownik A. J., Theodore A., English D. J., Black M. M., et al. (2006). Effect of early childhood adversity on child health. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 160, 1232–1238 10.1001/archpedi.160.12.1232 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fraley R. C., Brumbaugh C. C., Marks M. J. (2005). The evolution and function of adult attachment: a comparative and phylogenetic analysis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 89, 731–746 10.1037/0022-3514.89.5.751 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Getz L. L., Carter C. S., Gavish L. (1981). The mating system of the prairie vole Microtus ochrogaster: field and laboratory evidence for pair-bonding. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 8, 189–194 10.1007/BF00299829 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon I., Zagoory-Sharon O., Leckman J. F., Feldman R. (2010a). Oxytocin and the development of parenting in humans. Biol. Psychiatry 68, 377–382 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon I., Zagoory-Sharon O., Leckman J. F., Feldman R. (2010b). Oxytocin, cortisol, and triadic family interactions. Physiol. Behav. 101, 679–684 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.08.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon I., Zagoory-Sharon O., Leckman J. F., Feldman R. (2010c). Prolactin, oxytocin, and the development of paternal behavior across the first six months of fatherhood. Horm. Behav. 58, 513–518 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.04.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gouin J.-P., Carter C. S., Pournajafi-Nazarloo H., Malarkey W. B., Loving T. J., Stowell J., et al. (2012). Plasma vasopressin and interpersonal functioning. Biol. Psychol. 91, 270–274 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.07.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graziano F., Bonino S., Cattelino E. (2009). Links between maternal and paternal support, depressive feelings and social and academic self-efficacy in adolescence. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 6, 241–257 10.1080/17405620701252066 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gubernick D. J. (1994). Biparental Care and Male-Female Relations in Mammals. Chur: Harwood Academic Publishers [Google Scholar]
- Gubernick D. J., Winslow J. T., Jensen P., Jeanotte L., Bowen J. (1995). Oxytocin changes in males over the reproductive cycle in the monogamous, biparental California mouse, Peromyscus californicus. Horm. Behav. 29, 59–73 10.1006/hbeh.1995.1005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guichoux E., Lagache L., Wagner S., Chaumeil P., Leger P., Lepais O., et al. (2011). Current trends in microsatellite genotyping. Mol. Ecol. Res. 11, 591–611 10.1111/j.1755-0998.2011.03014.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammerschmidt K., Fischer J. (1998). Maternal discrimination of offspring vocalizations in Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus). Primates 39, 231–236 10.1007/BF02557735 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hammock E. A. D., Young L. J. (2005). Microsatellite instability generates diversity in brain and sociobehavioral traits. Science 308, 1630–1634 10.1126/science.1111427 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heinrichs M., von Dawans B., Domes G. (2009). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and human social behavior. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 30, 548–557 10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.05.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinde R. A., Berman C. M. (1983). Primate Social Relationships: An Integrated Approach. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific [Google Scholar]
- House J. S., Landis K. R., Umberson D. (1988). Social relationships and health. Science 241, 540–545 10.1126/science.3399889 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insel T. R. (1992). Oxytocin–a neuropeptide for affiliation: evidence from behavioral, receptor autoradiographic, and comparative studies. Psychoneuroendocrinology 17, 3–35 10.1016/0306-4530(92)90073-G [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insel T. R. (1997). A neurobiological basis of social attachment. Am. J. Psychiatry 154, 726–735 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insel T. R., Hulihan T. J. (1995). A gender-specific mechanism for pair bonding: oxytocin and partner preference formation in monogamous voles. Behav. Neurosci. 109, 782–789 10.1037/0735-7044.109.4.782 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insel T. R., Shapiro L. E. (1992). Oxytocin receptor distribution reflects social organization in monogamous and polygamous voles. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 89, 5981–5985 10.1073/pnas.89.13.5981 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insel T. R., Winslow J. T. (1998). Serotonin and neuropeptides in affiliative behaviors. Biol. Psychiatry 44, 207–219 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00094-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insel T. R., Young L. J. (2001). The neurobiology of attachment. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 129–136 10.1038/35053579 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Israel S., Lerer E., Shalev I., Uzefovsky F., Riebold M., Laiba E., et al. (2009). The oxytocin receptor (OXTR) contributes to prosocial fund allocations in the dictator game and the social value orientations task. PLoS ONE 4:e5535 10.1371/journal.pone.0005535 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarcho M. R., Mendoza S. P., Mason W. A., Yang X., Bales K. L. (2011). Intranasal vasopressin affects pair bonding and peripheral gene expression in male Callicebus cupreus. Gene Brain Behav. 10, 375–383 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.00677.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaremka L. M., Fagundes C. P., Peng J., Bennett J. M., Glaser R., Malarkey W. B., et al. (2013). Loneliness promotes inflammation during acute stress. Psychol. Sci. 24, 1089–1097 10.1177/0956797612464059 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jensen G. D. (1965). Mother-infant relationship in the monkey Macaca nemestrina: development of specificity of maternal response to own infant. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 59, 305–308 10.1037/h0021832 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jin D., Liu H.-X., Hirai H., Torashima T., Nagai T., Lopatina O., et al. (2007). CD38 is critical for social behaviour by regulating oxytocin secretion. Nature 446, 41–45 10.1038/nature05526 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jovanovic T., Megna N. L., Maestripieri D. (2000). Early maternal recognition of offspring vocalizations in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Primates 41, 421–428 10.1007/BF02557653 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendrick K. M. (2000). Oxytocin, motherhood and bonding. Exp. Physiol. 85, 111S–124S 10.1111/j.1469-445X.2000.tb00014.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendrick K. M., Da Costa A. P. C., Broad K. D., Ohkura S., Guevara R., Lévy F., et al. (1997). Neural control of maternal behaviour and olfactory recognition of offspring. Brain Res. Bull. 44, 383–395 10.1016/S0361-9230(97)00218-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendrick K. M., Keverne E. B., Baldwin B. A. (1987). Intracerebroventricular oxytocin stimulates maternal behaviour in the sheep. Neuroendocrinology 46, 56–61 10.1159/000124796 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kenkel W. M., Paredes J., Yee J. R., Pournajafi-Nazarloo H., Bales K. L., Carter C. S. (2012). Neuroendocrine and behavioural responses to exposure to an infant in male prairie voles. J. Neuroendocrinol. 24, 874–886 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2012.02301.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kentner A. C., Abizaid A., Bielajew C. (2010). Modeling dad: animal models of paternal behavior. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 438–451 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.08.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keverne E. B., Kendrick K. M. (1992). Oxytocin facilitation of maternal behavior in sheep. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 652, 83–101 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb34348.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiecolt-Glaser J. K., Gouin J. P., Hantsoo L. (2010). Close relationships, inflammation, and health. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 33–38 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.09.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiecolt-Glaser J. K., Newton T. L. (2001). Marriage and health: his and hers. Psychol. Bull. 127, 472–503 10.1037/0033-2909.127.4.472 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleiman D. G. (1977). Monogamy in mammals. Q. Rev. Biol. 52, 39–69 10.1086/409721 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knafo A., Israel S., Darvasi A., Bachner−Melman R., Uzefovsky F., Cohen L., et al. (2008). Individual differences in allocation of funds in the dictator game associated with length of the arginine vasopressin 1a receptor RS3 promoter region and correlation between RS3 length and hippocampal mRNA. Genes Brain Behav. 7, 266–275 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00341.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kosfeld M., Heinrichs M., Zak P. J., Fischbacher U., Fehr E. (2005). Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature 435, 673–676 10.1038/nature03701 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kozorovitskiy Y., Hughes M., Lee K., Gould E. (2006). Fatherhood affects dendritic spines and vasopressin V1a receptors in the primate prefrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1094–1095 10.1038/nn1753 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krueger F., McCabe K., Moll J., Kriegeskorte N., Zahn R., Strenziok M., et al. (2007). Neural correlates of trust. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 20084–20089 10.1073/pnas.0710103104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krueger F., Parasuraman R., Iyengar V., Thornburg M., Weel J., Lin M., et al. (2012). Oxytocin receptor genetic variation promotes human trust behavior. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:4 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landgraf R., Frank E., Aldag J. M., Neumann I. D., Sharer C. A., Ren X., et al. (2003). Viral vector-mediated gene transfer of the vole V1a vasopressin receptor in the rat septum: improved social discrimination and active social behaviour. Eur. J. Neurosci. 18, 403–411 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02750.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landgraf R., Gerstberger R., Montkowski A., Probst J. C., Wotjak C. T., Holsboer F., et al. (1995). V1 vasopressin receptor antisense oligodeoxynucleotide into septum reduces vasopressin binding, social discrimination abilities, and anxiety-related behavior in rats. J. Neurosci. 15, 4250–4258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lane A., Luminet O., Rimé B., Gross J. J., de Timary P., Mikolajczak M. (2013). Oxytocin increases willingness to socially share one's emotions. Int. J. Psychol. 48, 676–681 10.1080/00207594.2012.677540 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lanier P., Jonson-Reid M., Stahlschmidt M. J., Drake B., Constantino J. (2010). Child maltreatment and pediatric health outcomes: a longitudinal study of low-income children. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 35, 511–522 10.1093/jpepsy/jsp086 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee R. M., Robbins S. B. (1998). The relationship between social connectedness and anxiety, self-esteem, and social identity. J. Counsel. Psychol. 45, 338–345 10.1037/0022-0167.45.3.33816688743 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lee R. M., Robbins S. B. (2000). Understanding social connectedness in college women and men. J. Couns. Dev. 78, 484–491 10.1002/j.1556-6676.2000.tb01932.x21463854 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Leeb R. T., Lewis T., Zolotor A. J. (2011). A review of physical and mental health consequences of child abuse and neglect and implications for practice. Am. J. Lifestyle Med. 5, 454–468 10.1177/155982761141026611061791 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moal Le, Dantzer M. R., Michaud B., Koob G. F. (1987). Centrally injected arginine vasopressin (AVP) facilitates social memory in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 77, 353–359 10.1016/0304-3940(87)90527-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levine A., Zagoory-Sharon O., Feldman R., Weller A. (2007). Oxytocin during pregnancy and early postpartum: individual patterns and maternal–fetal attachment. Peptides 28, 1162–1169 10.1016/j.peptides.2007.04.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy F., Kendrick K. M., Keverne E. B., Piketty V., Poindron P. (1992). Intracerebral oxytocin is important for the onset of maternal behavior in inexperienced ewes delivered under peridural anesthesia. Behav. Neurosci. 106, 427–432 10.1037/0735-7044.106.2.427 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li L.-L., Keverne E. B., Aparicio S. A., Ishino F., Barton S. C., Surani M. A. (1999). Regulation of maternal rehavior and offspring growth by paternally expressed peg3. Science 284, 330–334 10.1126/science.284.5412.330 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lillard L. A., Waite L. J. (1995). Til death do us part: marital disruption and mortality. Am. J. Sociol. 100, 1131–1156 10.1086/230634 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lim M. M., Wang Z., Olazabal D. E., Ren X., Terwilliger E. F., Young L. J. (2004). Enhanced partner preference in a promiscuous species by manipulating the expression of a single gene. Nature 429, 754–757 10.1038/nature02539 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lim M. M., Young L. J. (2004). Vasopressin-dependent neural circuits underlying pair bond formation in the monogamous prairie vole. Neuroscience 125, 35–45 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.12.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lim M. M., Young L. J. (2006). Neuropeptidergic regulation of affiliative behavior and social bonding in animals. Horm. Behav. 50, 506–517 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.06.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu J. C. J., Guastella A. J., Dadds M. R. (2012). Effects of oxytocin on human social approach measured using intimacy equilibriums. Horm. Behav. 62, 585–591 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu Y., Wang Z. X. (2003). Nucleus accumbens oxytocin and dopamine interact to regulate pair bond formation in female prairie voles. Neuroscience 121, 537–544 10.1016/S0306-4522(03)00555-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lopatina O., Inzhutova A., Salmina A. B., Higashida H. (2013). The roles of oxytocin and CD38 in social or parental behaviors. Front. Neurosci. 6:182 10.3389/fnins.2012.00182 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ludwig M., Leng G. (2006). Dendritic peptide release and peptide-dependent behaviours. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 126–136 10.1038/nrn1845 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luhmann N. (1979). Trust and Power. New York, NY: John Willey & Sons [Google Scholar]
- Macbeth A. H., Lee H. J., Edds J., Young W. S. (2009). Oxytocin and the oxytocin receptor underlie intrastrain, but not interstrain, social recognition. Genes Brain Behav. 8, 558–567 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2009.00506.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy M. M. (1990). Oxytocin inhibits infanticide in female house mice (Mus domesticus). Horm. Behav. 24, 365–375 10.1016/0018-506X(90)90015-P [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mikolajczak M., Gross J. J., Lane A., Corneille O., de Timary P., Luminet O. (2010a). Oxytocin makes people trusting, not gullible. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1072–1074 10.1177/0956797610377343 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mikolajczak M., Pinon N., Lane A., de Timary P., Luminet O. (2010b). Oxytocin not only increases trust when money is at stake, but also when confidential information is in the balance. Biol. Psychol. 85, 182–184 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.05.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nagasawa M., Okabe S., Mogi K., Kikusui T. (2012). Oxytocin and mutual communication in mother-infant bonding. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:31 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nation M., Heflinger C. A. (2006). Risk factors for serious alcohol and drug use: the role of psychosocial variables in predicting the frequency of substance use among adolescents. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse 32, 415–433 10.1080/00952990600753867 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nephew B. C., Bridges R. S. (2008). Arginine vasopressin V1a receptor antagonist impairs maternal memory in rats. Physiol. Behav. 95, 182–186 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.05.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nowak R., Porter R. H., Levy F., Orgeur P., Schaal B. (2000). Role of mother-young interactions in the survival of offspring in domestic mammals. Rev. Reprod. 5, 153–163 10.1530/ror.0.0050153 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olazabal D. E., Young L. J. (2006). Oxytocin receptors in the nucleus accumbens facilitate “spontaneous” maternal behavior in adult female prairie voles. Neuroscience 141, 559–568 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.04.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ostermeyer M. C., Elwood R. W. (1983). Pup recognition in Mus musculus: parental discrimination between their own and alien young. Dev. Psychobiol. 16, 75–82 10.1002/dev.420160202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parker K. J., Lee T. M. (2001). Central vasopressin administration regulates the onset of facultative paternal behavior in Microtus pennsylvanicus (meadow voles). Horm. Behav. 39, 285–294 10.1006/hbeh.2001.1655 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pedersen C. A., Ascher J. A., Monroe Y. L., Prange A. J., Jr. (1982). Oxytocin induces maternal behavior in virgin female rats. Science 216, 648–650 10.1126/science.7071605 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pedersen C. A., Caldwell J. D., Johnson M. F., Fort S. A., Prange A. J., Jr. (1985). Oxytocin antiserum delays onset of ovarian steroid-induced maternal behavior. Neuropeptides 6, 175–182 10.1016/0143-4179(85)90108-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pedersen C. A., Caldwell J. D., Walker C., Ayers G., Mason G. A. (1994). Oxytocin activates the postpartum onset of rat maternal behavior in the ventral tegmental and medial preoptic areas. Behav. Neurosci. 108, 1163–1171 10.1037/0735-7044.108.6.1163 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pedersen C. A., Prange A. J., Jr. (1979). Induction of maternal behavior in virgin rats after intracerebroventricular administration of oxytocin. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 76, 6661–6665 10.1073/pnas.76.12.6661 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pedersen C. A., Vadlamudi S. V., Boccia M. L., Amico J. A. (2006). Maternal behavior deficits in nulliparous oxytocin knockout mice. Genes Brain Behav. 5, 274–281 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00162.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitkow L. J., Sharer C. A., Ren X., Insel T. R., Terwilliger E. F., Young L. J. (2001). Facilitation of affiliation and pair-bond formation by vasopressin receptor gene transfer into the ventral forebrain of a monogamous vole. J. Neurosci. 21, 7392–7396 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poindron P., Gilling G., Hernandez H., Serafin N., Terrazas A. (2003). Early recognition of newborn goat kids by their mother: I. Nonolfactory discrimination. Dev. Psychobiol. 43, 82–89 10.1002/dev.10123 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Popik P., van Ree J. M. (1991). Oxytocin but not vasopressin facilities social recognition following injection into the medial preoptic area of the rat brain. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 1, 555–560 10.1016/0924-977X(91)90010-R [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Popik P., Vetulani J., van Ree J. M. (1992a). Low doses of oxytocin facilitate social recognition in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 106, 71–74 10.1007/BF02253591 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Popik P., Vos P. E., Van Ree J. M. (1992b). Neurohypophyseal hormone receptors in the septum are implicated in social recognition in the rat. Behav. Pharmacol. 3, 351–358 10.1097/00008877-199208000-00011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Porter R. H., Cernoch J. M., McLaughlin F. J. (1983). Maternal recognition of neonates through olfactory cues. Physiol. Behav. 30, 151–154 10.1016/0031-9384(83)90051-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ragnauth A. K., Devidze N., Moy V., Finley K., Goodwillie A., Kow L. M., et al. (2005). Female oxytocin gene-knockout mice, in a semi-natural environment, display exaggerated aggressive behavior. Genes Brain Behav. 4, 229–239 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00118.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Relaix F., Wei X. J., Li W., Pan J., Lin Y., Bowtell D. D., et al. (2000). Pw1/Peg3 is a potential cell death mediator and cooperates with Siah1a in p53-mediated apoptosis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 2105–2110 10.1073/pnas.040378897 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Relaix F., Wei X. J., Wu X., Sassoon D. A. (1998). Peg3/Pw1 is an imprinted gene involved in the TNF-NFkappaB signal transduction pathway. Nat. Genet. 18, 287–291 10.1038/ng0398-287 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rimmele U., Hediger K., Heinrichs M., Klaver P. (2009). Oxytocin makes a face in memory familiar. J. Neurosci. 29, 38–42 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4260-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saito A., Nakamura K. (2011). Oxytocin changes primate paternal tolerance to offspring in food transfer. J. Comp. Physiol. A 197, 329–337 10.1007/s00359-010-0617-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sarkadi A., Kristiansson R., Oberklaid F., Bremberg S. (2008). Fathers' involvement and children's developmental outcomes: a systematic review of longitudinal studies. Acta Paediatr. 97, 153–158 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2007.00572.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Savaskan E., Ehrhardt R., Schulz A., Walter M., Schächinger H. (2008). Post-learning intranasal oxytocin modulates human memory for facial identity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 368–374 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.12.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneiderman I., Kanat-Maymon Y., Ebstein R. P., Feldman R. (2013). Cumulative risk on the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) underpins empathic communication difficulties at the first stages of romantic love. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1093/scan/nst142 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneiderman I., Zagoory-Sharon O., Leckman J. F., Feldman R. (2012). Oxytocin during the initial stages of romantic attachment: relations to couples' interactive reciprocity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 1277–1285 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.12.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shonkoff J. P., Garner A. S. Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Heath, Committee on Early Childhood, Adoption, and Dependent Care, and Section on Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. (2012). The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics 129, e232–e246 10.1542/peds.2011-2663 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silverstein M., Bengtson V. L. (1991). Do close parent-child relations reduce the mortality risk of older parents? J. Health Soc. Behav. 32, 382–395 10.2307/2137105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skuse D. H., Gallagher L. (2009). Dopaminergic-neuropeptide interactions in the social brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 27–35 10.1016/j.tics.2008.09.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith A. S., Agmo A., Birnie A. K., French J. A. (2010). Manipulation of the oxytocin system alters social behavior and attraction in pair-bonding primates, Callithrix penicillata. Horm. Behav. 57, 255–262 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.12.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steptoe A. (1991). The links between stress and illness. J. Psychosom. Res. 35, 633–644 10.1016/0022-3999(91)90113-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takagi T., Tanizawa O., Otsuki Y., Sugita N., Haruta M., Yamaji K. (1985). Oxytocin in the cerebrospinal fluid and plasma of pregnant and nonpregnant subjects. Horm. Metab. Res. 17, 308–310 10.1055/s-2007-1013526 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takayanagi Y., Yoshida M., Bielsky I. F., Ross H. E., Kawamata M., Onaka T., et al. (2005). Pervasive social deficits, but normal parturition, in oxytocin receptor-deficient mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 16096–16101 10.1073/pnas.0505312102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tobin V. A., Hashimoto H., Wacker D. W., Takayanagi Y., Langnaese K., Caquineau C., et al. (2010). An intrinsic vasopressin system in the olfactory bulb is involved in social recognition. Nature 464, 413–417 10.1038/nature08826 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Ijzendoorn M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg M. J. (2012). A sniff of trust: meta-analysis of the effects of intranasal oxytocin administration on face recognition, trust to in-group, and trust to out-group. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 438–443 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.07.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Leengoed E., Kerker E., Swanson H. H. (1987). Inhibition of post-partum maternal behaviour in the rat by injecting an oxytocin antagonist into the cerebral ventricles. J. Endocrinol. 112, 275–282 10.1677/joe.0.1120275 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Wimersma Greidanus T. B., Maigret C. (1996). The role of limbic vasopressin and oxytocin in social recognition. Brain Res. 713, 153–159 10.1016/0006-8993(95)01505-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wacker D. W., Ludwig M. (2012). Vasopressin, oxytocin, and social odor recognition. Horm. Behav. 61, 259–265 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.08.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walum H., Lichtenstein P., Neiderhiser J. M., Reiss D., Ganiban J. M., Spotts E. L., et al. (2012). Variation in the oxytocin receptor gene is associated with pair-bonding and social behavior. Biol. Psychiatry 71, 419–426 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.09.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walum H., Westberg L., Henningsson S., Neiderhiser J. M., Reiss D., Igl W., et al. (2008). Genetic variation in the vasopressin receptor 1a gene (AVPR1A) associates with pair-bonding behavior in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 14153–14156 10.1073/pnas.0803081105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Z., Ferris C. F., De Vries G. J. (1994a). Role of septal vasopressin innervation in paternal behavior in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 91, 400–404 10.1073/pnas.91.1.400 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Z., Smith W., Major D. E., De Vries G. J. (1994b). Sex and species differences in the effects of cohabitation on vasopressin messenger RNA expression in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) and meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus). Brain Res. 650, 212–218 10.1016/0006-8993(94)91784-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Z., Young L. J., De Vries G. J., Insel T. R. (1998). Voles and vasopressin: a review of molecular, cellular, and behavioral studies of pair bonding and paternal behaviors. Prog. Brain Res. 119, 483–499 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)61589-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wideman C. H., Murphy H. M. (1990). Vasopressin, maternal behavior, and pup well-being. Curr. Psychol. 9, 285–295 10.1007/BF02686866 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Williams J. R., Carter C. S., Insel T. (1992a). Partner preference development in female prairie voles is facilitated by mating or the central infusion of oxytocin. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 652, 487–489 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb34393.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams J. R., Catania K. C., Carter C. S. (1992b). Development of partner preferences in female prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster): the role of social and sexual experience. Horm. Behav. 26, 339–349 10.1016/0018-506X(92)90004-F [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams K. L., Galliher R. V. (2006). Predicting depression and self-esteem from social connectedness, support, and competence. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 25, 855–874 10.1521/jscp.2006.25.8.855 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Winslow J. T., Hastings N., Carter C. S., Harbaugh C. R., Insel T. R. (1993). A role for central vasopressin in pair bonding in monogamous prairie voles. Nature 365, 545–548 10.1038/365545a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winslow J. T., Insel T. R. (1991). Social status in pairs of male squirrel monkeys determines the behavioral response to central oxytocin administration. J. Neurosci. 11, 2032–2038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winslow J. T., Insel T. R. (2002). The social deficits of the oxytocin knockout mouse. Neuropeptides 36, 221–229 10.1054/npep.2002.0909 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winslow J. T., Insel T. R. (2004). Neuroendocrine basis of social recognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 14, 248–253 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Witt D. M., Carter C. S., Walton D. M. (1990). Central and peripheral effects of oxytocin administration in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 37, 63–69 10.1016/0091-3057(90)90042-G [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Witt D. M., Winslow J. T., Insel T. R. (1992). Enhanced social interactions in rats following chronic, centrally infused oxytocin. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 43, 855–861 10.1016/0091-3057(92)90418-F [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young L. J. (2002). The neurobiology of social recognition, approach, and avoidance. Biol. Psychiatry 51, 18–26 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01268-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young L. J., Murphy Young A. Z., Hammock E. A. D. (2005). Anatomy and neurochemistry of the pair bond. J. Comp. Neurol. 493, 51–57 10.1002/cne.20771 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young L. J., Nilsen R., Waymire K. G., MacGregor G. R., Insel T. R. (1999). Increased affiliative response to vasopressin in mice expressing the V1a receptor from a monogamous vole. Nature 400, 766–768 10.1038/23650 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu G. Z., Kaba H., Okutani F., Takahashi S., Higuchi T. (1996). The olfactory bulb: a critical site of action for oxytocin in the induction of maternal behaviour in the rat. Neuroscience 72, 1083–1088 10.1016/0306-4522(95)00600-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zak P. J., Kurzban R., Matzner W. T. (2004). The neurobiology of trust, in Biobehavioral Stress Response: Protective and Damaging Effects, eds Yehuda R., McEwen B. (New York, NY: New York Academy of Sciences; ), 224–227 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zak P. J., Kurzban R., Matzner W. T. (2005). Oxytocin is associated with human trustworthiness. Horm. Behav. 48, 522–527 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]