Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Apr 10.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Med. 2013 Oct 10;44(10):2223–2229. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713002493

The heritability of clinically diagnosed Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder across the life span

Henrik Larsson 1, Zheng Chang 1, Brian M D’Onofrio 2, Paul Lichtenstein 1
PMCID: PMC4071160  NIHMSID: NIHMS586459  PMID: 24107258

Abstract

Background

No prior twin study has explored the heritability of clinically diagnosed ADHD. Such studies are needed to resolve conflicting results regarding the importance of genetic effects for ADHD in adults. We aimed to estimate the relative contribution of genetic and environmental influences for clinically diagnosed ADHD across the life span with a specific focus on ADHD in adults.

Method

Information about zygosity and sex was obtained from 59,514 twins born between 1959 and 2001 included in the nation-wide population-based Swedish Twin Register. Clinical data of ADHD diagnosis was obtained from the Prescribed Drug (i.e., stimulant or non-stimulant medication for ADHD) and National Patient Registers (i.e., ICD-10 diagnosis of ADHD). Twin methods were applied to clinical data of ADHD diagnosis using structural equation modeling with monozygotic and dizygotic twins.

Results

The best-fitting model revealed a high heritability of ADHD (0.88; 95% CI, 0.83–0.92) for the entire sample. Shared environmental effects, on the other hand, were non-significant and of minimal importance. The heritability of ADHD in adults was also substantial (0.72; 95% CI, 0.56–0.84).

Conclusion

This study showed that the heritability of clinically diagnosed ADHD is high across the life span. Our finding of high heritability for clinically diagnosed ADHD in adults indicate that the previous reports of low heritability is best explained by rater effects and that gene-identification studies of ADHD in adults needs to consider pervasiveness (e.g., multiple raters) and developmentally (e.g., childhood onset criteria) informative data.

Keywords: ADHD, Twin study, heritability, Adults

INTRODUCTION

Many twin studies have explored the heritable nature of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), but none of these have been based on clinically diagnosed cases. Such a study is needed to clarify how genetic factors influence ADHD across different levels of severity, (Larsson et al 2012) and to resolve inconsistent results regarding the heritability of ADHD in adults (Franke et al., 2012).

Twin studies using parent or teacher ratings indicate that continuous measure of ADHD is highly heritable (~60–90) (Burt, 2009, Nikolas and Burt, 2010, Faraone et al., 2005). The few available twin studies using categorical measures of ADHD (see Table 1) suggest equally high heritability estimates (Lichtenstein et al., 2010, Sherman et al., 1997, Thapar et al., 2000, Larsson et al., 2011), but these studies suffer from limitations. In particular, they applied broad categories that contain milder cases which would not meet syndromal criteria for ADHD, and all studies lacked information on the age of onset or impairment criteria used in the DSM and ICD diagnostic definition. Thus, more stringent diagnostic methods and narrow definitions may generate different heritability estimates.

Table 1.

Twin studies exploring the heritability using categorical measures of ADHD.

Authors Twin sample Measure Informant % meeting cut-off criteria for ADHD Heritability
Larsson et al 2011 Population: children/adolescents; 1,450 twin pairs; aged 8–17 years; Swedish Longitudinal DSM-IV ADHD symptoms Parents
  1. High trajectory of Hyperactivity

  2. High trajectory of inattention

  1. 80%

  2. 72%

Lichtenstein et al 2010 Population: 10,895 twin pairs; aged 12 years; Swedish DSM-IV ADHD symptoms Parents 1.8% 79%
Sherman et al 1997 Population: 288 twin pairs; aged 11–12 years; USA DSM-III-R ADHD symptoms Teachers and parents, combined 18% 79%
Thapar et al 2000 Population: 2,082 twin pairs; aged 11–12 years; UK DSM-IV ADHD symptoms Teachers and parents, combined - 80%

Twin studies suggest substantially lower heritability estimates (~30–40%) for ADHD in adults (Boomsma et al., 2010, Larsson et al., 2013a, Reiersen et al., 2008, van den Berg et al., 2006), but it has been difficult to resolve if the drop in heritability reflect true developmental changes or relate to rater effects (Franke et al., 2012). This is because twin studies on ADHD in adults have used self-ratings, while studies on children have used other informants (i.e., parent and teacher ratings). The few studies on this topic suggest that the heritability of self-rated ADHD is low in both adults and adolescents (Kan et al., 2013, Merwood et al., 2013), and that the heritability of ADHD in adults is substantial when both self and parent ratings are combined into a composite index of ADHD (Chang et al., 2013). Together, these findings suggest that the low heritability for ADHD in adults may be best explained by rater effects. Nevertheless, more rigorously characterized twin samples are desired, including clinically stringent measures of ADHD, to more firmly establish the heritability of ADHD in adults.

In the present report we use data from 59,514 twins born between 1959 and 2001 included in the Swedish Twin Register (Lichtenstein et al., 2006). Clinical data on ADHD from Swedish national registers was obtained. We aimed to estimate the relative contribution of genetic, shared environmental and non-shared environmental influences for clinically diagnosed ADHD across the life-span with a specific focus on ADHD in adults. Based on previously observed similarities between continuous trait measures and broad categorical definitions, we predicted similar heritability estimates for clinically diagnosed ADHD. Based on the recent cross-informant twin studies (Chang et al., 2013), we predicted high heritability also for ADHD in adults. This is because both cross-informant measures and clinical diagnosis focus on pervasive symptoms.

METHODS

SWEDISH TWIN REGISTER

We obtained the data from the nationwide population-based Swedish Twin Register (Lichtenstein et al., 2006). The target population in this study were all twins born in Sweden between 1959 and 2001 (N=89,174). Excluded from the analyses were twin pairs with unknown zygosity (N=21,714), twin pairs in which one or both died (N=3,250) or emigrated (N=4,696) before the start of follow-up in 2001; thus, the final sample of 59,514 twins (29,177 male twins, 49%) represented 67% of the targeted population. Twin analyses on ADHD in adults was based on a sub-sample of 37,714 twins (18,092 male twins, 48%) born 1959–1991.

Zygosity was established using DNA testing or standard zygosity questionnaires concerning twin similarity and confusion (Lichtenstein et al., 2006). When zygosity was determined based on questionnaire data, only twins with more than 95% probability of being correctly classified were assigned a zygosity. In the twin analyses, 17,026 were monozygotic (MZ), 16,554 were dizygotic (DZ), and 25,934 were opposite sex DZ twins.

NATIONAL REGISTERS

Data from the Swedish Twin Register was linked to the population based Prescribed Drug and Patient Registers, using each individual’s unique personal identification number.

The Swedish Prescribed Drug Register (PDR) is a national healthcare register (administered by the National Board of Health and Welfare) with data on dispensed pharmaceuticals. Information regarding drug identity according to the Anatomical Therapeutic Chemical classification system, quantity and dosage of the prescribed drug, and date of prescription has been registered since July 2005. The register covers the entire population of Sweden, and the identity of the patients is available for > 99.7% of the population (Wettermark et al., 2007).

The Patient Register (held by the National Board of Health and Welfare) has nationwide coverage for psychiatric outpatient care since 2001. Every record has a discharge date, a primary discharge diagnosis, and up to seven secondary diagnoses assigned by the treating medical doctor according to WHO’s International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10) (WHO, 1992).

VARIABLES

Twins treated with stimulant or non-stimulant medication for ADHD (methylphenidate [N06BA04]; atomoxetin [N06BA09]; amphetamine [N06BA01]; dexamphetamine [N06BA02]) at any time between July 2005 and July 2010 were identified via PDR. National guidelines for medication of ADHD, issued by the National Board of Health and Welfare in 2002, stated that medication should be reserved for cases where other supportive interventions have failed, indicating that pharmacological ADHD treatment most likely represents an indicator of the more severe cases of ADHD. The authority to prescribe ADHD drugs in Sweden is restricted to specialist physicians familiar with the treatment of this disorder. In the total sample of 59,514 twins, 730 (1.23%) were treated for ADHD/hyperkinetic disorder at any time between 2005 and 2010.

Twins obtaining a diagnosis of hyperkinetic disorder between 2001 and 2009 were identified via the Patient Register (ICD-10: F90). In the total sample of 59,514 twins, 525 (0.88%) had received a diagnosis of hyperkinetic disorder from outpatient care. A substantial number of these twins (n=392, 74.7%) were treated with stimulant or non-stimulant medication at any time between 2005 and 2010.

To maximize power in the twin model fitting analyses, the present study applied an “or” approach to define clinically diagnosed ADHD resulting in 863 twins (1.45%) who either meet criteria for hyperkinetic disorder at any time between 2001 and 2009 or received stimulant or non-stimulant treatment at any time between 2005 and 2010. We have recently reported high specificity for this register-based definition of ADHD (Larsson et al., 2013b).

Twin model fitting on ADHD in adults was based on the sub-sample of 37,714 twins born between 1959 and 1991 (i.e., 18 years old or older at the end of follow-up) resulting in 241 (0.64%) twins with a clinical diagnosis of ADHD in adults; our definition only considered twins diagnosed with ADHD at the age of 18 years or older.

The study was approved by the research ethics committee at Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden.

STATISTICAL ANALYSES

The concordance rate (i.e., the risk of ADHD for the cotwin of a twin with ADHD) was calculated as the proportion of individuals belonging to concordantly affected twin pairs out of all twins with the disorder. Correlation of liability (tetrachoric within-twin pair correlation) was also estimated for each sex-zygosity group. Higher concordance rates and correlations of liability in MZ than in DZ twins indicate a genetic contribution to the manifestation of disease. Shared environmental influences were inferred if the DZ correlation was greater than half of the MZ correlation (the DZ correlation higher than expected from sharing 50% of their segregating genes).

Biometric twin analyses were conducted on raw ordinal data using the Mx program (Neale, 2003) to determine the relative contribution of additive genetic factors (A, Heritability) reflecting additive effects of different alleles, shared environmental factors (C) reflecting environmental influences that make twin siblings similar to each other and non-shared environmental factors (E) reflecting non-genetic influences that make twin pairs dissimilar (Rijsdijk and Sham, 2002). In twin models based on ordinal raw data, each individual is coded as having the disease or not, and the threshold (z score) corresponds to the rate of the disease.

The following combinations of variance components were considered in the twin models: ACE and AE. Three sex-limitation models were fitted to the data. The full sex-limitation model allows quantitative and qualitative differences in the parameter estimates between males and females. The common effects sex-limitation model allows quantitative sex differences between males and females, but no qualitative differences. The null model equates all genetic and environmental parameter estimates for males and females, testing the hypothesis that there are no sex differences. Goodness of fit for the different twin models was assessed by Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC); a lower AIC value indicates better fit of the model to the observed data.

The quantitative genetic models were performed under the usual assumptions of the classical twin designs: random mating, no gene–environment interaction, and equal environments of MZ and DZ twin pairs (Rijsdijk and Sham, 2002).

RESULTS

Concordance rates and tetrachoric correlations for the full sample (59,514 twins born 1959–2001) are shown in Table 2. Tetrachoric DZ correlations were half of the MZ correlations, suggesting genetic, but not shared environmental influences on ADHD. MZ correlations were less than 1, suggesting non-shared environmental influences (including measurement error). Tetrachoric twin correlations were similar for males and females, which suggest no quantitative sex-differences in the genetic and environmental contribution. In addition, twin correlations were similar for same-sexed DZ and opposite-sex DZ twins (DZOS), which suggest no qualitative sex-differences (Table 2).

Table 2.

Concordance rates and tetrachoric correlations with 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) for clinical diagnosis of ADHD in 59,514 Swedish twins, by Sex and Zygosity

MZM DZM MZF DZF Opposite-sex twins
Case=Male Case=Female
Clinical diagnosis of ADHD
 No. of concordant affected pairs 35 13 11 5 29 29
 No. discordant pairs 55 139 38 68 277 100
 Concordance rates (95% CI) 0.56 (0.45–0.66) 0.16 (0.09–0.25) 0.37 (0.21–0.54) 0.13 (0.04–0.26) 0.17 (0.12–0.24) 0.37 (0.27–0.47)
 Tetrachoric correlations (95% CI) 0.90 (0.84–0.94) 0.48 (0.33–0.61) 0.81 (0.68–0.90) 0.50 (0.28–0.67) 0.49 (0.40–0.58)

MZM, monozygotic male; MZF, monozygotic female; DZM, dizygotic male; DZF, dizygotic female.

Table 3 displays the age-adjusted model fitting results of ACE and AE sex-limitation models compared to the saturated model using the full twin sample (59,514 twins born 1959–2001). In all these models, thresholds were equated across twin 1 and twin 2, for MZ and DZ twins, but not across males (z score, 1.46) and females (z score, 2.06). As can be seen, the AE null model had the lowest AIC value (Table 3). That is, a model that constrained the genetic and environmental parameter estimates to be equal across sex, and excluded variance in liability due to the shared environmental factor provided the most parsimonious fit of the data. This best-fitting model estimated the heritability and non-shared environment contribution as 0.88 (95% confidence interval [CI], 0.83–0.92) and 0.12 (95% CI, 0.08–0.17), respectively.

Table 3.

Model fitting results of univariate analysis of ADHD.

Fit of model compared to saturated model

Model -2LL df χ2 Δdf AIC
Saturated model 8092.5 59489 - - -
1. ACE Univariate
 Full sex-limitation modela 8112.0 59505 19.5 16 −12.5
 Common effects sex-limitation model b 8112.0 59506 19.5 17 −14.5
 Null modelc 8113.5 59509 21.0 20 −19.0
2. AE Univariate
 Full sex-limitation modela 8112.5 59507 20.0 18 −16.0
 Common effects sex-limitation model b 8112.5 59508 20.0 19 −18.0
Null modelc 8113.7 59510 21.3 21 20.7

Best-fitting model indicated in bold. -2LL, likelihood fit statistic; df, degrees of freedom; χ2, the difference in -2LL between the saturated and restricted model; Δdf, difference in df between the saturated and restricted model; AIC, Akaike’s Information Criterion;

a

The full sex-limitation model allows quantitative and qualitative differences in the parameter estimates between males and females;

b

The common effects sex-limitation model allows quantitative sex differences between males and females, but no qualitative differences;

c

The null model equates all genetic and environmental parameter estimates for males and females, testing the hypothesis that there are no sex differences

Similar results were obtained when refitting all twin models using an “AND” approach (i.e., clinically diagnosed ADHD defined as meeting criteria for hyperkinetic disorder and received stimulant/non-stimulant treatment). The best fitting model (AE null model) estimated the heritability and non-shared environment contribution as 0.89 (95% CI, 0.82–0.94) and 0.11 (95% CI, 0.06–0.18), respectively. Similar genetic and environmental parameter estimates were also obtained when the twin modeling were restricted to either ADHD cases identified through ICD diagnoses (A=0.89; 95% CI, 0.83–0.93; E=0.11; 95% CI, 0.07–0.17) or pharmacological ADHD treatments (A=0.88; 95% CI, 0.83–0.92; E=0.12; 95% CI, 0.08–0.17), providing converging evidence across different outcome definitions Table 3.

Heritability of ADHD in adults

Analyses were also conducted on twins born between 1959 and 1991 (N=37,714 twins) to estimate the heritability of ADHD in adults. These twins were on average 23.0 years old at the start of the register follow-up. Importantly, we only considered twins diagnosed with ADHD at the age of 18 years or older. The best fitting model (AE null model) estimated the heritability and non-shared environment contribution as 0.72 (95% CI, 0.56–0.84) and 0.28 (95% CI, 0.16–0.44), respectively.

DISCUSSION

This study showed that the heritability of clinically diagnosed ADHD is high across the life span. The finding of high heritability for clinically diagnosed ADHD in adults indicate that the previous reports of low heritability is best explained by rater effects and that gene-identification studies of ADHD in adults should not rely on self-ratings only.

In this study, using data from representative national registers, we estimated the heritability of clinically diagnosed ADHD at 88%. Our result is in line with the large number of twin studies using continuous trait measures of ADHD based on parent or teacher ratings (Burt, 2009, Faraone et al., 2005), and the few studies that have explored the genetic impact on categorically defined ADHD (Lichtenstein et al., 2010, Sherman et al., 1997, Thapar et al., 2000, Larsson et al., 2011). There are noteworthy differences between the present study and these prior twin studies, in particular related to differing assessment methodologies. In the present study subjects were diagnosed with ICD-10. These diagnoses are performed by clinicians, and thus based on structured interviews covering age of onset of the impairing symptoms and presence of impairment in multiple settings, while prior twin studies were based on quantitative measures of ADHD symptoms without addressing the childhood criteria and not systematically assessing the impairment in multiple settings criteria. Yet, remarkably similar heritability estimates for ADHD were obtained across studies suggesting strong genetic effects in ADHD regardless of whether it is assessed using continuous trait measures, broad categorical definitions or narrow diagnostic definitions, which in turn provides further support for ADHD as a quantitative extreme of genetic and environmental factors operating dimensionally throughout the distribution of ADHD symptoms (Chen et al., 2008, Larsson et al., 2012, Levy et al., 1997).

One novel finding of this study was that clinically diagnosed ADHD in adults was highly heritable. The finding is in line with a recent cross-informant twin study reporting that the heritability of ADHD in 19–20 year olds was 78%, when both self and parent ratings were combined into a composite index of ADHD symptoms to adjust for rater bias (Chang et al., 2013) and also with prior family studies suggesting a high familial loading on ADHD in adults (Biederman et al., 1996, Biederman et al., 1995, Faraone et al., 2000, Faraone, 2004), but is inconsistent with what has been reported in twin studies of self-rated ADHD in adults (Reiersen et al., 2008, Boomsma et al., 2010, van den Berg et al., 2006, Larsson et al., 2013a). One increasingly recognized explanation to previous reports of low heritability for ADHD in adults is increased contribution of measurement error (reflecting accuracy of the measures) associated with the use of self-ratings (Kan et al., 2013, Franke et al., 2012, Merwood et al., 2013, Chang et al., 2013). Another explanation is that the clinical diagnosis of ADHD in adults reflects persistence of the childhood disorder (i.e., childhood onset), whereas cross-sectional self-ratings may also reflect adult-onset ADHD-like symptoms (i.e., phenocopies) involving different genetic and environmental processes. Taken together, this indicates that self-ratings may not be the best measure to use in gene-identification studies of ADHD in adults and that other factors such as childhood onset, pervasiveness and impairment should be taken into account.

Limitations

This study had some limitations. First, it was not possible to classify ADHD cases according to the three DSM-IV ADHD subtypes (i.e., combined, primarily hyperactive-impulsive and primarily inattentive type), since these were not recorded across the registers. However, prior twin research suggests similar heritability estimates for the inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive component of ADHD (Nikolas and Burt, 2010, Larsson et al., 2006).

Second, the validity of the non-standardized register diagnoses has not been explored using comparisons with research diagnoses based on independent semi-structured interviews and/or medical records. Even though our own validity checks of ADHD support high specificity for the register-based diagnosis, we could not rule out false negatives (i.e., individuals with ADHD who had never been recorded in any registers). This is because the Patient Register provided coverage of outpatient care after 2001 and the Swedish Prescribed Drug Register provided data on twins treated with stimulant or non-stimulant medication after 2005. However, bias due to such outcome miss-classification most probably applies equally to MZ and DZ pairs and would therefore not introduce a significant upward bias of the heritability estimate.

Third, the ascertainment of ADHD cases was predominantly based on ICD-10 diagnosis of hyperkinetic disorder and prescribed medication unique for the treatment of ADHD. The ICD-10 definition of ADHD is stricter compared with that in DSM-IV, and the Swedish National guidelines for medication of ADHD stated that medication should be reserved for cases where other supportive interventions have failed, indicating that our measure most likely represent an indicator of the more severe cases of ADHD. Thus, generalizations should be made with caution.

Fourth, even with a nationwide twin cohort born 1959–2001 the absolute numbers of clinically diagnosed concordant or discordant twin pairs remained limited, resulting in wide confidence intervals and limited power to detect shared environmental influences. Point estimates, especially for ADHD in adults, should therefore be interpreted with caution.

Conclusions

We have demonstrated that heritability estimates for clinically diagnosed ADHD is high across the life span. This indicates that the previous reports of low heritability for ADHD symptoms in adults are best explained by rater effects giving rise to measurement error, rather than by distinct developmental changes in the importance of non-shared environmental factors.

Acknowledgments

Financial support: This research was supported by grants from the Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research and from the Swedish Research Council (2010-3184; 2011-2492).

This research was supported by grants from the Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research and from the Swedish Research Council (2010-3184; 2011-2492).

Footnotes

Declaration of Interest: None

All authors had full access to all the data in the study. Henrik Larsson takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. No competing interest to report.

References

  1. BIEDERMAN J, FARAONE S, MILBERGER S, CURTIS S, CHEN L, MARRS A, OUELLETTE C, MOORE P, SPENCER T. Predictors of persistence and remission of ADHD into adolescence: results from a four-year prospective follow-up study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1996;35:343–51. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199603000-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. BIEDERMAN J, FARAONE SV, MICK E, SPENCER T, WILENS T, KIELY K, GUITE J, ABLON JS, REED E, WARBURTON R. High risk for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder among children of parents with childhood onset of the disorder: a pilot study. The American journal of psychiatry. 1995;152:431–5. doi: 10.1176/ajp.152.3.431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. BOOMSMA DI, SAVIOUK V, HOTTENGA JJ, DISTEL MA, DE MOOR MH, VINK JM, GEELS LM, VAN BEEK JH, BARTELS M, DE GEUS EJ, WILLEMSEN G. Genetic epidemiology of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD index) in adults. PLoS One. 2010;5:e10621. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. BURT SA. Rethinking environmental contributions to child and adolescent psychopathology: a meta-analysis of shared environmental influences. Psychological bulletin. 2009;135:608–37. doi: 10.1037/a0015702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. CHANG Z, LICHTENSTEIN P, ASHERSON PJ, LARSSON H. Developmental twin study of attention problems: high heritabilities throughout development. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70:311–8. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. CHEN W, ZHOU K, SHAM P, FRANKE B, KUNTSI J, CAMPBELL D, FLEISCHMAN K, KNIGHT J, ANDREOU P, ARNOLD R, ALTINK M, BOER F, BOHOLST MJ, BUSCHGENS C, BUTLER L, CHRISTIANSEN H, FLIERS E, HOWE-FORBES R, GABRIELS I, HEISE A, KORN-LUBETZKI I, MARCO R, MEDAD S, MINDERAA R, MULLER UC, MULLIGAN A, PSYCHOGIOU L, ROMMELSE N, SETHNA V, UEBEL H, MCGUFFIN P, PLOMIN R, BANASCHEWSKI T, BUITELAAR J, EBSTEIN R, EISENBERG J, GILL M, MANOR I, MIRANDA A, MULAS F, OADES RD, ROEYERS H, ROTHENBERGER A, SERGEANT J, SONUGA-BARKE E, STEINHAUSEN HC, TAYLOR E, THOMPSON M, FARAONE SV, ASHERSON P. DSM-IV combined type ADHD shows familial association with sibling trait scores: a sampling strategy for QTL linkage. American journal of medical genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric genetics : the official publication of the International Society of Psychiatric Genetics. 2008;147B:1450–60. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. FARAONE SV. Genetics of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2004;27:303–21. doi: 10.1016/S0193-953X(03)00090-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. FARAONE SV, BIEDERMAN J, MONUTEAUX MC. Toward guidelines for pedigree selection in genetic studies of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Genetic Epidemiology. 2000;18:1–16. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2272(200001)18:1<1::AID-GEPI1>3.0.CO;2-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. FARAONE SV, PERLIS RH, DOYLE AE, SMOLLER JW, GORALNICK JJ, HOLMGREN MA, SKLAR P. Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57:1313–23. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. FRANKE B, FARAONE SV, ASHERSON P, BUITELAAR J, BAU CH, RAMOS-QUIROGA JA, MICK E, GREVET EH, JOHANSSON S, HAAVIK J, LESCH KP, CORMAND B, REIF A. The genetics of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults, a review. Molecular psychiatry. 2012;17:960–87. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. KAN KJ, DOLAN CV, NIVARD MG, MIDDELDORP CM, VAN BEIJSTERVELDT CE, WILLEMSEN G, BOOMSMA DI. Genetic and environmental stability in attention problems across the lifespan: evidence from the Netherlands twin register. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2013;52:12–25. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. LARSSON H, ANCKARSATER H, RASTAM M, CHANG Z, LICHTENSTEIN P. Childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder as an extreme of a continuous trait: a quantitative genetic study of 8,500 twin pairs. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines. 2012;53:73–80. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02467.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. LARSSON H, ASHERSON P, CHANG Z, LJUNG T, FRIEDRICHS B, LARSSON JO, LICHTENSTEIN P. Genetic and environmental influences on adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms: a large Swedish population-based study of twins. Psychological medicine. 2013a;43:197–207. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712001067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. LARSSON H, DILSHAD R, LICHTENSTEIN P, BARKER ED. Developmental trajectories of DSM-IV symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: genetic effects, family risk and associated psychopathology. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines. 2011;52:954–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02379.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. LARSSON H, LICHTENSTEIN P, LARSSON JO. Genetic contributions to the development of ADHD subtypes from childhood to adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2006;45:973–81. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000222787.57100.d8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. LARSSON H, RYDEN E, BOMAN M, LANGSTROM N, LICHTENSTEIN P, LANDEN M. Risk of bipolar disorder and schizophrenia in relatives of people with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. The British journal of psychiatry: the journal of mental science. 2013b;203:103–106. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.112.120808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. LEVY F, HAY DA, MCSTEPHEN M, WOOD C, WALDMAN I. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a category or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997;36:737–44. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199706000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. LICHTENSTEIN P, CARLSTROM E, RASTAM M, GILLBERG C, ANCKARSATER H. The genetics of autism spectrum disorders and related neuropsychiatric disorders in childhood. The American journal of psychiatry. 2010;167:1357–63. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10020223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. LICHTENSTEIN P, SULLIVAN PF, CNATTINGIUS S, GATZ M, JOHANSSON S, CARLSTROM E, BJORK C, SVARTENGREN M, WOLK A, KLARESKOG L, DE FAIRE U, SCHALLING M, PALMGREN J, PEDERSEN NL. The Swedish Twin Registry in the third millennium: an update. Twin research and human genetics : the official journal of the International Society for Twin Studies. 2006;9:875–82. doi: 10.1375/183242706779462444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. MERWOOD A, GREVEN CU, PRICE TS, RIJSDIJK F, KUNTSI J, MCLOUGHLIN G, LARSSON H, ASHERSON PJ. Different heritabilities but shared etiological influences for parent, teacher and self-ratings of ADHD symptoms: an adolescent twin study. Psychological medicine. 2013;43:1973–84. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712002978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. NEALE MC, BOKER SM, XIE G, MAES HH. Mx Statistical Modeling. VCU Box 900126, Richmond, VA 23298: Department of Psychiatry; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  22. NIKOLAS MA, BURT SA. Genetic and environmental influences on ADHD symptom dimensions of inattention and hyperactivity: a meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2010;119:1–17. doi: 10.1037/a0018010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. REIERSEN AM, CONSTANTINO JN, GRIMMER M, MARTIN NG, TODD RD. Evidence for shared genetic influences on self-reported ADHD and autistic symptoms in young adult Australian twins. Twin research and human genetics : the official journal of the International Society for Twin Studies. 2008;11:579–85. doi: 10.1375/twin.11.6.579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. RIJSDIJK FV, SHAM PC. Analytic approaches to twin data using structural equation models. Briefings in bioinformatics. 2002;3:119–33. doi: 10.1093/bib/3.2.119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. SHERMAN DK, MCGUE MK, IACONO WG. Twin concordance for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a comparison of teachers’ and mothers’ reports. The American journal of psychiatry. 1997;154:532–5. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.4.532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. THAPAR A, HARRINGTON R, ROSS K, MCGUFFIN P. Does the definition of ADHD affect heritability? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2000;39:1528–36. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200012000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. VAN DEN BERG SM, WILLEMSEN G, DE GEUS EJ, BOOMSMA DI. Genetic etiology of stability of attention problems in young adulthood. American journal of medical genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric genetics : the official publication of the International Society of Psychiatric Genetics. 2006;141B:55–60. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. WETTERMARK B, HAMMAR N, FORED CM, LEIMANIS A, OTTERBLAD OLAUSSON P, BERGMAN U, PERSSON I, SUNDSTROM A, WESTERHOLM B, ROSEN M. The new Swedish Prescribed Drug Register--opportunities for pharmacoepidemiological research and experience from the first six months. Pharmacoepidemiology and drug safety. 2007;16:726–35. doi: 10.1002/pds.1294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. WHO. International Classification of Diseases. World Health Organization WHO; Geneva: 1992. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES