Abstract
This study examined differences in predictors of marijuana use versus quantity of marijuana use across the high school years, using annual assessments from the Oregon Youth Study (OYS) and a two-part model for semicontinuous data. The OYS is a community sample of at-risk boys followed from age 10 years. In order to capture dynamic prediction effects, change scores of predictors, as well as baseline scores, were included. Baseline predictors predominantly showed associations with the intercepts but not with the slopes of growth models. Change scores for parental monitoring, peer substance use, and antisocial behavior and deviant associations were associated with both parts of the model. Findings highlight the importance of looking at marijuana use compared to quantity of marijuana use.
Keywords: marijuana, growth, antisocial behavior
Influences on Boys’ Marijuana Use in High School: A Two-Part Random Intercept Growth Model
Marijuana has long been the most commonly used illicit drug in the U.S. (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2010) and in recent years has shown increases in prevalence among students in Grades 8, 10, and 12 (Johnston, O'Malley, Bachman, & Schulenberg, 2010). The Monitoring the Future Study found that in 2009 only 17% of students in Grade 8 had tried marijuana (Johnston, et al., 2010), but by Grade 12, the proportion had increased over 2.5 times to 44%. Along with the increasing numbers of youth who transition to marijuana use, there is growth in the quantity of use among users (Johnston, et al., 2010). Further cause for concern regarding adolescent marijuana use is that there is evidence of lasting negative effects of use on the developing adolescent brain (Arseneault, Cannon, Witton, & Murray, 2004; Bossong & Niesink, 2010), such that more use is associated with greater risk for mental illness (Moore et al., 2007). Finally, there is evidence of negative socioeconomic consequences in early adulthood of adolescent marijuana use (Broman, 2009). Thus, there is both increasing prevalence of marijuana use among adolescents and increasing evidence of the negative consequences of such use. Therefore, a better understanding of factors related to the onset and escalation of marijuana use across adolescence is of critical importance and will inform the development of preventive interventions.
The current study uses a theoretical approach based in a Dynamic Developmental Systems (DDS) framework (Capaldi, Shortt, & Kim, 2005) that focuses on the interactions between developmental history associated with general risk for problems (including marijuana use) – particularly as indicated by conduct problem behaviors – and outcome-specific risk, mainly from proximal social influences (e.g., parental and peer substance use) in the etiology and course of risk behaviors (Capaldi, Stoolmiller, Kim, & Yoerger, 2009). A similar theoretical approach has been taken by others to examining the emergence of substance use problems (J. O. Lee et al., 2011). By using such a framework to guide the selection of predictors of both use versus nonuse and quantity of marijuana use, and by including repeated measures of marijuana use outcomes for an at-risk sample of boys (in the Oregon Youth Study; OYS), the present study makes a novel contribution to the substance use literature. In the current study, the hypothesized general developmental risk pathway predictors included parental monitoring and boys’ depressive symptoms, antisocial behavior, and deviant peer associations; whereas the more proximal and specific social influence pathway factors were parent marijuana use and peer substance use.
The value of the study is further increased by examining the effects on the marijuana use outcomes of changes across adolescence in these social influences within a two-part semicontinuous growth model. Within the context of the growth model, this identifies how changes in predictors directly influence both use and quantity of use without the need for additional growth models for the predictors. This method allows for conducting several key tests simultaneously for the predictors, and thus makes a novel contribution to understanding adolescent marijuana use. Specifically, we tested whether key predictors of marijuana use were more associated with the intercepts and growth in use versus nonuse of marijuana compared with the quantity of marijuana used. We also tested the difference between associations of static baseline predictors and change score versions across time points of those same predictors. This approach, which we have previously used to examine prediction to growth in alcohol use (Capaldi, et al., 2009), addresses the dynamic nature of the associations that general and outcome-specific risk factors have with marijuana use and quantity of use. In addition, the study makes a substantial contribution over prior studies with the OYS data set that have included examination of peer and family factors associated with substance use in midadolescence (Dishion, Capaldi, Spracklen, & Li, 1995); prediction to age of onset of use through age 16 years only, involving time-invariant predictors (Dishion, Capaldi, & Yoerger, 1999); and examination of reciprocal associations between observed social interactions with a friend and substance use from early adolescence to young adulthood (Dishion & Owen, 2002).
Characteristics of Marijuana Use at Adolescence
There have been numerous studies of the associations of both general and outcome-specific risk factors with a range of marijuana outcomes (use, frequency, latent classes). However, few studies have adequately modeled key distributional characteristics of marijuana use in adolescence – namely the typically skewed nature of the distribution of quantity of marijuana used and, in particular, that many individuals are nonusers. Relatedly, the notion that different predictor pathways may apply to use versus nonuse in comparison to quantity of use among users has generally not been well addressed, even though it is well established that different factors should influence the onset and occurrence versus the maintenance or escalation of youth substance use. The two-part random intercepts model (Olsen & Schafer, 2001) addresses these issues by permitting simultaneous prediction to (a) use versus nonuse and (b) to quantity of use given any use. This approach has been used in numerous studies to examine the etiology and growth in alcohol use at adolescence (Blozis, Feldman, & Conger, 2007; Brown, Catalano, Fleming, Haggerty, & Abbott, 2005; Capaldi, et al., 2009). Prior studies that have used the two-part models of marijuana use have focused on either the program effects of an intervention (Brown, et al., 2005; Dembo, Wareham, Greenbaum, Childs, & Schmeidler, 2009) or on ethnic differences in growth (C. Lee, Mun, White, & Simon, 2010). This study is the first, to our knowledge, to use general and outcome-specific risk factors to predict to growth in both marijuana use and quantity of use in the high school years.
General Risk Factors for Marijuana Use
Youth antisocial behavior and association with deviant peers are strongly predictive of a cluster of problem behaviors in adolescence, including marijuana use (Dishion, et al., 1999; Tarter, Kirisci, Ridenour, & Vanyukov, 2008), and thus represent a general risk pathway to such problem outcomes. Specifically, Flory, Lynam, Milich, Leukefeld, and Clayton (2004) showed that adolescents with symptoms of conduct disorder were more likely to be in either of the marijuana use groups they identified as opposed to the nonuser group. Windle and Wiesner (2004) also found that initial levels of delinquent behaviors were significantly lower for nonusers than for all classes of users they identified based on growth patterns. Several studies also document that associations with deviant peers increases risk for later substance use (Dishion, et al., 1995; Kirisci, Mezzich, Reynolds, Tarter, & Aytaclar, 2009), and marijuana use specifically (Mauricio et al., 2009). Antisocial behavior and deviant peer association are highly associated in adolescence (Dishion & Patterson, 2006) and intimately associated with the general risk developmental pathway; therefore, in the current study, they were combined in the prediction models.
A second general pathway risk factor included in the model is depressive symptoms. There is mixed and often contradictory evidence for an association between depressive symptoms and marijuana use for adolescents (Degenhardt, Hall, & Lynskey, 2003). Effects of depressive symptoms on later marijuana use would be expected based on the self-medication hypothesis, which posits that substances may be used to alleviate negative affect or to provide some positive stimulation in the context of anhedonia (Khantzian, 1997). Though Degenhardt and colleagues did not find support for the self-medication hypothesis, others have. Windle and Wiesner (2004) found significant relations between depressive symptoms and marijuana use growth patterns in adolescence. Fleming, Mason, Mazza, Abbott, and Catalano (2008) found similar results for a main effect on using marijuana. It is thus important to examine this association in a further study, at the same time controlling for associated risk factors (e.g., antisocial behavior).
Poor parental monitoring is a well-established risk factor for a wide range of problem behaviors at adolescence, including delinquency, sexual risk behavior (Capaldi, Crosby, & Stoolmiller, 1996), and substance use (Bahr, Hoffmann, & Yang, 2005); thus, good parental monitoring (involving parental or other adult supervision and knowledge of the youth’s activities) is an important family protective factor for problem behaviors in adolescence (Dishion & McMahon, 1998; Snyder, 2002). A link between low parental monitoring and marijuana use has been identified (Lac & Crano, 2009; Martins, Storr, Alexandre, & Chilcoat, 2008), which suggests parental monitoring could have a protective effect. It is thus hypothesized that, in the multivariate two-part model, parental monitoring will relate to lower marijuana use, particularly given that it is an illicit drug.
Outcome-Specific Risk Factors for Marijuana Use
A key aspect of the DDS model of the etiology of substance use is to examine the contribution of outcome-specific risk in the context of the contribution of general risk factors, and parental and peer substance use are hypothesized to be the most important outcome-specific risk factors for marijuana use in adolescence. The influence of parental substance use on their children’s substance use is well documented (Bailey, Hill, Oesterle, & Hawkins, 2006; Li, Pentz, & Chou, 2002; Reinherz, Giaconia, Hauf, Wasserman, & Paradis, 2000) and likely involves multiple mechanisms, including shared genetic risk, modeling, and increased access to substances. We are concerned here with the specific influence of parent marijuana use, which has also been reported to directly influence adolescent marijuana use (Kandel, Griesler, Lee, Davies, & Schaffran, 2001).
Association with peers who use illicit substances (including alcohol and tobacco for minors) is a more specific peer influence on marijuana use than is deviant peer association in general. Perceived peer use of marijuana has been found to predict use of marijuana (Creemers et al., 2010; D'Amico & McCarthy, 2006), as has smoking tobacco with peers (Duncan, Tildesley, Duncan, & Hops, 1995; Kiesner, Poulin, & Dishion, 2010). To help clarify the role of peer substance use versus general deviance (antisocial behavior and deviant peer association combined), both risk factors were included in the model.
Issues in Prediction to Quantity of Use
The etiology of levels and changes in quantity of marijuana use is much less clear than that of use versus nonuse for adolescents; as studies generally do not distinguish between these concepts, they are often confounded. Yet understanding the etiology of higher levels of use versus occasional use at adolescence is critically important from a prevention standpoint, because adolescents who use more marijuana are more likely to have impairment and long-term problems (Arseneault, et al., 2004; Bossong & Niesink, 2010; Moore, et al., 2007). The use of the two-part growth model in the current study will help clarify the role of the theoretical predictors in both use and growth in quantity of use of marijuana across the high school years.
Change in Risk Across the High School Years
The use of change scores (from one assessment to the next) of risk factors is an underutilized approach to studying dynamic associations within growth models (Brook, Whiteman, Finch, Morojele, & Cohen, 2000). This approach accounts for the fact that the predictors are changing with development but is a manageable approach for a multivariate prediction model (versus, for example, trying to examine multiple simultaneous growth curve models). Prediction from change scores provides stronger evidence of a likely causal association than use of predictors from one time point, and it also provides evidence of strength of associations at particular developmental stages (in the current case, across high school). This may be particularly informative for the design of prevention programs.
Hypotheses of the Current Study
Our theorized model is presented in Figure 1. We expected that all of the baseline scores of risk factors would significantly predict the intercepts of both parts of the model, namely use versus nonuse of marijuana and quantity of use. We also expected that the association between the change scores and the outcomes would be significant across time. Given that the predicted direct associations between the change scores and the outcomes across time were expected to be relatively strong, particularly for prediction from changes in peer substance use, we considered it unlikely that the Grade 8–9 predictors would be significantly associated with the slopes of either parts of the model (hence the dashed lines in the model).
Method
Sample
Schools in neighborhoods with higher incidences of juvenile delinquency were identified in a medium-sized metropolitan area (Eugene–Springfield, Oregon). Boys in Grade 4 (ages 9–10 years) of those schools were invited to participate in the study with their families (the study did not include girls). The recruitment rate was 74.4% (N = 206), and retention was at least 97% at each wave through the senior year of high school (Capaldi, Chamberlain, Fetrow, & Wilson, 1997). The sample size of boys was 202 (98%) in Grade 8 and 201 (98%) in Grade 12. The sample was predominantly White (90%), and 75% were of lower socioeconomic status. At Grade 8, family composition included intact parents (38%), single parent (19%), stepparent (29%), and multiple parental transitions (14%). The proportion of single-parent families was between 14% and 17% through Grade 12. Parents living with the youth were invited to participate at each wave. At Grade 8, 191 mothers and 140 fathers participated, and at Grade 12, 188 mothers and 128 fathers participated. Both parents participated in 53% to 64% of the families, and at least one parent participated in 95% to 99% of the families across the period.
Procedure
The OYS involved yearly data collection with alternating major (Grades 4, 6, 8, 10, and 12) and minor waves. In line with the theoretical framework of DDS, the major assessments were multimethod and multiagent involving predictors and outcome measures (Capaldi, et al., 1997). This allowed for a more ecological examination of participants behavior, using key natural raters (Kellam, Rebok, Mayer, Ialongo, & Kalodner, 1994) in their lives as well as self- reports and avoided the issues of common method bias (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003). Minor waves were more limited in scope and focused mainly on the dependent variables, including measures of marijuana use. The data in the current study were taken from Wave 5 (ages 13–14 years, Grade 8) through Wave 9 (ages 17–18 years, Grade 12) over a period of 5 years, with the dependent variables taken from Wave 6 to Wave 9. Parents (and OYS men as adults) provided informed consent and all procedures were approved by the IRB of the Oregon Social Learning Center. Participants were compensated for their time at each assessment wave. Family members were reimbursed at a rate of approximately $10 per hour for their participation in each of the assessment protocols.
Interviews and questionnaires
The parent (or parents) and adolescent boys were interviewed separately. The interviews lasted 45 minutes to 1 hour each. The boys were asked questions concerning problem behavior and substance use, and the interviewers completed a ratings checklist after each interview.
Schools
Teachers completed questionnaires rating the study boys on academic, emotional, and behavioral adaptations to school, using the Teacher Report Form (TRF) (Achenbach, 1991).
Measures
Dependent variable
Marijuana use was coded on the basis of any reported use in the past year. The quantity of marijuana usage was calculated using a formula based on the reported number of times participants smoked marijuana in the last year. Two questions were asked of each participant, “How many times have you used marijuana in the last year?” and “When using marijuana, how much do you usually use?” Participants gave an estimated number of times used in the last year. For how much they usually use, they had four options: answered that they share a joint, have one joint, have two joint, or gave an amount themselves. With the assumption that an average joint of marijuana was equal to one gram (World Health Organization, 1997), the multiplication of these variables gives an estimate in grams of marijuana of the amount of marijuana used in the last year. Given that the two-part analysis assumes normality in the second part (i.e., quantity of marijuana use), the variable was log transformed after subtracting a constant to minimize skewness.
Independent variables
The risk factors or independent variables were initially assessed at Grade 8 (ages 13–14 years) for all factors except antisocial behavior and deviant peer associations and depressive symptoms, which were available at Grade 9, and these scores were used to predict the intercept and slope of marijuana use and quantity of use across high school (see Table 1 for more information). The general strategy for building predictor constructs for this study has been described by Capaldi and Patterson (1989) and Patterson, Reid, and Dishion (1992). Several potential indicators were developed for each construct and combined as follows:
The internal consistency of the a priori items associated with each scale or indicator was established using criteria of an alpha of at least .6 (Cronbach, 1951) and an item-total correlation of .2 (p < .05).
The convergent validity of the indicators for a construct was examined within a principal component factor analysis. Items with factor loadings for the one-factor solution of at least .3 were retained.
The indicator scores were standardized to ensure equal weight and aggregated by taking the mean of the scales for the final score.
Four of the predictors were coded so that a higher score represented a more problematic behavior or situation, with only parental monitoring scored so that a high score involved stronger monitoring.
Table 1.
Construct | Assessment instrument |
Respondent | Number of items |
Sample item | Cronbach’s alpha |
Pearson corr. |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Antisocial behavior | 0.74 | |||||
CBC-L, overt | M,F | 7,7 | Disobedient at home. | .82, .74 | 0.65 | |
CBC-L, covert | M, F | 8,8 | Destroys others things. | .84, .87 | 0.73 | |
Peers q'naire | M, F | 1, 1 | How often does your son get in conflicts with other kids around the home? | 0.24 | ||
CBC-L, overt | Teacher | 11 | Cruelty, bullying, meanness to others. | 0.93 | ||
CBC-L, covert | Teacher | 8 | Lying or cheating. | 0.86 | ||
TPRSK | Teacher | 1 | How often does he exert negative influence on his friends? | |||
Interview, ratings | Interviewer | 1 | How likely is it that this boy will have future trouble with the police? | |||
Deviant peer association | 0.76 | |||||
CBC-L + peers q'naire | M,F | 1+2, 1+2 | Hangs out with kids who get in trouble. | .83, .84 | 0.71 | |
CBC-L + TPRSK | Teacher | 1+3 | Does this student associate with kids involved in stealing or vandalism? | 0.92 | ||
Interview + describing | Youth | 10+5 | During the past year, how many of your friends stole something worth < $5? | 0.86 | ||
friends q'naire | ||||||
Antisocial behavior + deviant peers | 0.78 | |||||
Depressive symptoms | ||||||
CESD-D | Youth | 20 | During the past week, I felt sad. | 0.86 | ||
Parent monitoring | 0.75 | |||||
Interview, parent monitoring |
M,F | 12, 15 | On the average, how many hrs per week is your son alone or with siblings only? |
.82, .79 | 0.73 | |
Interview, impressions | Interviewer | 1, 1 | This parent seemed to monitor the child carefully. | 0.77 | ||
Interview | Youth | 9 | Your parents let you go any place you please without asking. | 0.79 | ||
Interview, impressions | Interviewer | 1 | This boy seems to be well supervised by his parents. | |||
Parent marijuana use | ||||||
Substance use q'naire | M,F | 1, 1 | How often do you smoke pot or hash? | 0.88 | ||
Peer substance use | 0.9 | |||||
Interview | Youth | 10 | How often do your friends drink? | 0.84 | ||
Interview | M, F | 3,3 | Son hangs out with kids who smoke? | .88, .87 |
Note: M stands for Mother, F stands for Father.
The correlation matrix for the predictor variables – namely parent marijuana use, peer substance use, participant’s antisocial behavior and deviant associations, depressive symptoms, and parental monitoring – is shown in Table 2. Although significantly associated, the independent variables were not so strongly associated as to cause concern of multicollinearity.
Table 2.
A | B | C | D | E | F | G | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
A. Use versus nonuse | 1 | ||||||
B. Quantity of use† | NA | 1 | |||||
C. Antisocial behavior & deviant peer | 0.44*** | 0.19 | 1 | ||||
D. Depressive symptoms | 0.16* | 0.21 | 0.29*** | 1 | |||
E. Parental monitoring | −0.40*** | −0.17 | −0.46*** | −0.14 | 1 | ||
F. Parent marijuana use | 0.30*** | 0.20 | 0.18* | −0.07 | −0.25*** | 1 | |
G. Peer substance use | 0.41*** | 0.35* | 0.52*** | 0.23*** | −0 44*** | 0.21** | 1 |
All who did not use are set to missing.
p< .05.
p<.01.
p<.001.
Youth antisocial behavior and deviant association
Scales for youth antisocial behavior were created from three sources: parents, teachers, and the interviewer. None of the items pertained to substance use or illegal behavior directly related to substance use (e.g., selling drugs). Parent questions came from two questionnaires, Child Behavior Check List (CBC-L) (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1983) and Peers Questionnaire (Oregon Social Learning Center, 1982–2012), with 15 questions from the externalizing scale of the CBCL and 1 question from the Peers Questionnaire. The scores from the mother and father were checked separately for construct validity and then combined. Teachers also filled out two questionnaires, the TRF (Achenbach, 1991) and the Teachers Peers Social Skills Questionnaire [TPRSK] (Dishion & Capaldi, 1985; Walker & McConnell, 1988), with 19 items from the TRF and 1 item from the TPRSK. A final item was the interviewer ratings from the Youth Interview. Cronbach’s alpha for all of these indicators was .74.
Deviant peer association was assessed by two parent questionnaires (CBC-L and Peers Questionnaire), with one item from the CBC-L and two items from the Peers Questionnaire. Like the measure of antisocial behavior, the construct was validated for each parent then combined. Teacher reports from two questionnaires were also included (TRF and TPRSK), with one item from TRF and three items from the TPRSK. The final indicator of the construct came from youth report in an interview and questionnaire (Describing Friends Questionnaire), with 10 items from the interview and 5 questions from the questionnaire. Cronbach’s alpha for the indicators was .76. The antisocial behavior and deviant peer association constructs were highly associated (r = .78, p < .001) and were standardized and combined to avoid problems of multicollinearity in the final analysis.
Youth depressive symptoms
A single indictor involving the youth’s self-report of 20 items regarding depressive symptoms (CES-D, Radloff, 1977) was used. Cronbach’s alpha for the scale was .86.
Parental monitoring
The parental monitoring measure was created from the Parent Interview, parent interviewer ratings, Youth Interview, and youth interviewer ratings. Mothers were asked 12 questions and fathers 15 questions that were separately validated and then combined. These were combined with interviewer rating items regarding monitoring by the mother and father. In a similar fashion, the boy answered nine questions about parental monitoring in the interview, and the interviewer answered one question about the boy. Cronbach’s alpha for the indicators was .75.
Frequency of parent marijuana use
Parental reports from the Substance Use Questionnaire were used. Following Capaldi et al. (2009), parent marijuana frequency was a standardized average across the two parents. The correlation of the two indicators was .88 (p < .001).
Peer substance use
A total of 10 items from the Youth Interview, with an additional 3 questions that both parents answered in the Parent Interview, were used to assess peer substance use. Cronbach’s alpha for the indicators was .90.
Multiple Imputation in Stata
To utilize the full sample, multiple imputation was used to obtain 20 datasets of sample size of 204 with full information on the predictors. Two participants had no data on the outcome and were excluded. A total sample size of 176 out of 204 would have resulted from using only the available information on covariates, losing about 14% of the sample. Although each variable was only missing – at most, 11 observations (5%) and, on average, 5 missing observations (2%) – the combined missingness over time resulted in the 14% loss of the sample. The multiple imputation was run with all of the variables in the model, as well as a series of demographic variables that were related to the missingness of outcome and increased prediction of the covariates (Engels & Diehr, 2003). The outcome was returned to the original state of missingness for the modeling as missing data on the outcome was modeled using full information maximum likelihood (FIML).
Analytic Plan
The data on marijuana use for the high school years of the sample were analyzed using a single semicontinuous two-part analysis in Mplus using the maximum likelihood with robust standard errors (MLR) estimator to handle better the missing data in the second part of the model (Olsen & Schafer, 2001). This analysis addresses two different aspects of marijuana usage (see Figure 1) by two parallel growth models that are estimated simultaneously with change scores on the outcomes. The use of change scores allows the predictors to have a time-varying effect on the outcomes, in a fashion that allows for the development and changes in behavior of the participants, peers, and parents to influence the outcomes. The first is a binary growth model for use versus nonuse of marijuana, and the second is a growth model of quantity used, with not using marijuana considered missing in the second part. The two-part analysis allows for analysis of continuous data with a preponderance of zeros, in a similar fashion to a zero-inflated Poisson model for discrete data. Given that multiple imputation, a two-part growth model, and the MLR estimator were used for missing data, no fit statistics beyond an R-squared are available for the model from Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2010). However, given the common lack of fit statistics for growth models in the literature, we consider that the use of R-squared is adequate in this case.
The model was estimated with a simple linear slope and random intercepts only for both parts of the model. The use of a linear slope is due to the fact that only four time points were available for high school in our sample. The residuals for the continuous part of the model were held constant across time for parsimony, and the intercept of the growth model in Part 1 and the outcome variables in Part 2 were fixed at 0 for identification purposes. The intercept and slope of both parts of the model were regressed on the five baseline (either Grade 8 or 9) predictors.
Regarding prediction from change scores, the dependent variables of both parts (i.e., use and quantity of use of marijuana) at the last three time points were simultaneously regressed on the change scores between assessments for each predictor, thus allowing for a much stronger test of the association of the predictors to the marijuana outcomes across this period of rapid growth in both marijuana use versus nonuse and quantity of use. For parsimony, the effect of the change score across time points was held constant, which results in a single parameter estimate for the link between the change scores and each of the two dependent variables. Two of the predictors, antisocial behavior and deviant associations and depressive symptoms, were assessed in all 4 years of high school; thus, change scores from Grades 9 to 10, 10 to 11, and 11 to 12 were calculated as predictors. For the remaining variables, change scores from Grades 8 to 10 were used to predict the dependent variables at Grades 10 and 11, and change scores from Grades 10 to 12 were used to predict the dependent variables at Grade 12. This design, although complex, made the best use of the multiple assessments to test the hypotheses regarding prediction to growth.
Results
Prevalence and Quantity of Marijuana Use
Shown in Table 3 are prevalence rates for the prior 12-month period and the N by grade for first marijuana use, any marijuana use, and also average quantity of use [untransformed with test of normality for the transformed variable (D'agostino, Belanger, & D'Agostino Jr, 1990)], with zero use (i.e., nonusers) excluded for the latter score. Just over 20% of the sample indicated some marijuana use before high school, with 17% using in Grade 9, increasing to 35% by Grade 12. Similarly, the quantity of marijuana used increased substantially over time, with a particularly large jump from Grade 9 to Grade 10 (more than three times the Grade 9 average). In preparation for analysis, the quantity of marijuana use was log transformed to minimize skew, and all waves of the variable showed nonsignificant p-values for a test of non-normality.
Table 3.
First recorded usea |
Any recorded use |
Average quantity useb |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | Grade | % N | Count | % N | Count | M | SD | P |
201 | 9 | 5% | 11 | 17% | 34 | 52.74 | 143.03 | 0.159 |
199 | 10 | 6% | 12 | 20% | 39 | 171.40 | 452.49 | 0.212 |
202 | 11 | 8% | 17 | 28% | 57 | 176.06 | 476.78 | 0.108 |
201 | 12 | 10% | 21 | 35% | 70 | 150.88 | 530.81 | 0.440 |
A total of 20.32% of the sample reported first use before high school.
Quantity of use in grams.
Quantity of use is log transformed to minimize skew.
Part 1: Prediction to Use versus Nonuse
The analytic model contained two separate but parallel growth models that had identical prediction models associated with them (see Table 4 and 5). Part 1 (first main column in Table 4) was the predicted growth in the probability of smoking marijuana. The intercept of the use of marijuana (i.e., higher probability of use at Grade 9) was predicted by the general risk predictor of boy's antisocial behavior and deviant associations and the outcome-specific risk predictors of parent marijuana use and peer substance use. As shown in Table 4 (intercept of latent slope), the slope of use indicated a significant increase across time in the probability of marijuana use. None of the variables assessed at Grade 8 or 9 significantly predicted the slope of use. However, as shown in Table 4 (change score prediction), relative increases in peer substance use were associated with an increased probability of marijuana use; whereas relative increases in parental monitoring were associated with a decreased probability of use.
Table 4.
Part 1: |
Part 2: |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Use versus nonuse |
Quantity of use |
|||||
B | S.E. | β | B | S.E. | β | |
Prediction to latent intercept | ||||||
Intercept of latent intercept | 0 | NA | −0.76 | 0.46 | ||
Antisocial behavior and deviant peers | 1.32** | 0.43 | 0.39 | 0.51 | 0.34 | 0.23 |
Depressive symptoms | 0.29 | 0.39 | 0.09 | 0.60* | 0.25 | 0.27 |
Parental monitoring | −0.60 | 0.36 | −0.18 | −0.21 | 0.24 | −0.09 |
Parent marijuana use | 0.60* | 0.28 | 0.18 | 0.25 | 0.23 | 0.11 |
Peer substance use | 1.09** | 0.34 | 0.32 | 0.86** | 0.32 | 0.38 |
Prediction to latent slope | ||||||
Intercept of latent slope | 0.86*** | 0.15 | 0.73*** | 0.14 | ||
Antisocial behavior and deviant peers | −0.30 | 0.16 | −0.55 | 0.19 | 0.13 | 0.54 |
Depressive symptoms | −0.09 | 0.15 | −0.17 | −0.33** | 0.11 | −0.92 |
Parental monitoring | 0.12 | 0.16 | 0.22 | 0.03 | 0.08 | 0.08 |
Parent marijuana use | 0.03 | 0.12 | 0.06 | −0.11 | 0.08 | −0.32 |
Peer substance use | −0.20 | 0.15 | −0.38 | −0.12 | 0.13 | −0.34 |
Change score prediction to outcome | ||||||
Antisocial behavior and deviant peers | 0.06 | 0.20 | 0.01 | 0.56*** | 0.16 | 0.15 |
Depressive symptoms | −0.17 | 0.16 | −0.05 | 0.11 | 0.15 | 0.04 |
Parental monitoring | −0.57** | 0.18 | −0.16 | −0.10 | 0.15 | −0.04 |
Parent marijuana use | 0.12 | 0.16 | 0.03 | −0.21 | 0.14 | −0.06 |
Peer substance use | 0.89*** | 0.25 | 0.23 | 0.64** | 0.21 | 0.20 |
Note: The intercepts of the outcome for Part 2 and the intercept of the intercept of Part 1 are fixed to 0.
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
Table 5.
B | S.E. | |
---|---|---|
Correlations | ||
Pt. 1 Intercept with pt. 2 intercept | 0.41* | 0.20 |
Residual variance | ||
Quantity of use | 2.41*** | 0.39 |
Pt. 1 intercept | 3.58*** | 1.12 |
Pt. 2 intercept | 2.36*** | 0.48 |
R-Square | ||
Use grade 9 | 0.77*** | 0.05 |
Use grade 10 | 0.73*** | 0.05 |
Use grade 11 | 0.68*** | 0.06 |
Use grade 12 | 0.65*** | 0.06 |
Quantity of use grade 9 | 0.68*** | 0.06 |
Quantity of use grade 10 | 0.66*** | 0.05 |
Quantity of use grade 11 | 0.65*** | 0.05 |
Quantity of use grade 12 | 0.66*** | 0.06 |
Pt. 1 intercept | 0.68*** | 0.07 |
Pt. 2 intercept | 0.54*** | 0.10 |
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
Part 2: Prediction to Quantity of Use
The second part of the growth model (second main column of Table 4) concerned predictors of the quantity of marijuana used by the youth who used in a given year. The intercept of quantity of use at Grade 9 was predicted by Grades 8 and 9 depressive symptoms and peer substance use; both were associated with increased quantities of use. The intercept of the quantity of use was not itself significant, but the slope parameter was significant, showing a trend toward increased quantity of use over time. Depressive symptoms were significantly negatively associated with the slope, possibly indicating some recovery from the initially higher levels of quantity of use at ages 14–15 years associated with depressive symptoms. The effects of the change scores on the quantity of marijuana used indicated that relative increases between assessments in the boy’s antisocial behavior and deviant associations, and in peer substance use each significantly predicted increased quantity of use.
Random Effects and R-Squared
The intercepts of both parts of the model were significantly associated (see Table 5), and both intercepts had significant residual variance and the quantity of marijuana used had significant residual variance. As fit statistics are not available for the model, we estimated the R-squared values for the outcomes and the two random intercepts (see Table 5). The binary use outcome had R-squared values that ranged from .77 at Grade 9 to .65 at Grade 12. The continuous (i.e., quantity of use) outcome had R-squared values that ranged from .68 at Grade 9 to .65 at Grade 11. The random intercept of Part 1 of the model had an R-square value of .68; whereas the random intercept of Part 2 of the model had an R-square value of .54. Although other model fit indices were not available for this analysis, the high R-squared values show the predictive power of the model.
Discussion
The present study partitioned two important aspects of marijuana use that typically have been confounded in studies of adolescence and examined prediction from change in the predictors across the high school years, as well a prediction from the same variables assessed at Grade 8 or Grade 9. Examining change scores as predictors is a very valuable approach when examining growth of a behavior, especially of behaviors showing such marked growth as marijuana use and quantity of use in the high school years. The approach yielded fine-grained results in predicting intercepts and growth in both the use and quantity of use of marijuana for a sample of boys who lived in higher delinquency neighborhoods as children. Although both general risk factors and marijuana-specific social influence from key developmental interactants (parents and peers) were associated with marijuana use and quantity of use across adolescence, there were substantial differences in the predictors for use versus nonuse and quantity of use. Suggesting that the conflation in most studies between the use versus nonuse of marijuana and the quantity of use of marijuana is problematic, and that variables of such a skewed nature as marijuana use would benefit from the use of a two-part semicontinuous model.
All of the Grade 8 and 9 predictors, except parental monitoring, were associated with either the initial intercept of use (i.e., probability of use versus nonuse) or with the intercept of quantity of use, and peer substance use predicted both. The fact that only peer substance use predicted the initial status of both parts of the model was unexpected but not without precedent. Given that 63% of seniors in the Monitoring the Future Study reported that they never smoked marijuana alone and 47% said they smoked with one or two other people most of the time or always (Bachman, Johnston, & O'Malley, 2011), it is less surprising that peer substance use was the most consistent predictor of both use and quantity of use. In addition, across the latent intercept, latent slope, and change score predictions to the outcomes, the predictors showed more significant associations with marijuana use (Part 1 of the model) then quantity of marijuana use (Part 2), suggesting that more research is needed regarding prediction of the quantity of marijuana use after controlling for use versus nonuse. Neither of the parent variables was found to be significantly associated with Part 2 of the model in any way, raising the likelihood of mediation effects with the potentially more proximal predictors.
Even with the outcome-specific risk factors in the model, the general developmental risk pathway factor of boys’ antisocial behavior and deviant associations was predictive of both marijuana use in the first year of high school and of increases in quantity of use over time (change scores). The strong (though not significant) standardized association for antisocial behavior and deviant peer association with the slopes of both parts of the model suggest an even stronger influence that would need to be replicated with a larger sample. Thus, such problem behaviors and associations are a key risk factor for marijuana use in high school. This is consistent with findings of previous studies (Flory, et al., 2004; Tarter, et al., 2008). Marijuana use is illegal in both adolescence and adulthood, compared with the use of alcohol and tobacco, which are only illegal in adolescence and therefore a somewhat milder form of delinquent behavior (i.e., a status offense) than is marijuana use. Thus, initiation of marijuana use and growth in use and quantity across the high school years are particularly strongly associated with these indicators of problem behavior and delinquency at adolescence.
Parental monitoring, which is arguably the strongest external protective factor overall against problem behavior in adolescence, was as expected protective against marijuana use, with increases in parental monitoring between time points predictive of lower probability of use. Parental monitoring, however, was only marginally protective of use at Grade 9 (the intercept). However, as the model was multivariate, the association between baseline parental monitoring and Grade 9 marijuana use may have been accounted for by parent and peer substance use. These findings add to the body of work indicating the importance of parental monitoring in protecting adolescents from engaging in problem and health-risking behaviors (Dekovi, 1999), including substance use (Windle & Wiesner, 2004). The role that parents may play in monitoring has been questioned, with some researchers arguing that this behavior is entirely driven by youth disclosure (Kerr & Stattin, 2003). However, the weight of evidence suggests that monitoring is an interactional process between the parent and youth that is built around a history of parental positive involvement in the youth’s life (Brody, 2003; Capaldi, 2003).
Prior evidence for the association of depressive symptoms with marijuana use at adolescence has been mixed. Whereas this predictor was not associated with use versus nonuse in the current study, it was associated with the intercept (positively) and slope (negatively) of quantity of marijuana use. Thus, evidence was consistent with the hypothesis that adolescents experiencing depressive symptoms may use marijuana to alleviate or self-medicate those symptoms, but that they do so less over the course of high school. This is particularly interesting in that the study only involved male youth, and depressive symptoms are generally considered to be more problematic for girls than for boys. Confidence in these findings is strengthened by the fact that antisocial behavior was included in the model, because depressive symptoms consistently show a low-to-moderate association with antisocial behavior (Capaldi, 1992), and antisocial behavior is associated with marijuana use.
Findings regarding the outcome-specific predictor of parent marijuana use were surprisingly weak – only being associated with use versus nonuse at Grade 9. The outcome-specific predictor of peer substance use, however, was very strongly predictive of both marijuana use and quantity of use. Overall, it appears, as suggested in prior studies, that in mid to late adolescence the influence of peers’ marijuana use is much stronger than is the influence of parental use (Flay et al., 1994). Furthermore, whereas peer marijuana use is likely to be growing substantially across the high school years, use by parents is likely to be flat or diminishing. Thus, it was not unexpected that parent use would show the hypothesized effects to the intercept of use but not to growth in quantity.
Given the strength of each of the predictors, the substantial difference between the associations for each model is confirmation of the importance of considering illegal substance use in adolescence in a two-part model, when possible. The study design was a significant advance over prior research in a number of respects – particularly with the examination of growth in both the probability of marijuana use and quantity of use over a critical developmental period, the high school years – along with the examination of a theoretically driven and comprehensive prediction model, including both general developmental risk pathway and substance specific predictors and effects.
The study has some limitations. First, the sample was predominantly White and included male adolescents only. The extent to which these findings would generalize to other ethnic groups and to girls requires testing. Second, reports of frequency of peer marijuana use were limited to reports by the adolescent. Third, predictors were not all assessed every year; therefore, some of the change-score predictors spanned more than 1 year. Fourth, fit statistics are not available for two-part growth models other then R-squared. Finally, the sample size was relatively modest for the models tested. Strengths of the study included the use of multimethod, multiagent measures and the repeated measurements of the dependent (and independent) variables across the period, both of which enhance reliability of these variables and the use of a two-part semicontinuous model to account for the heavily skewed nature of the data.
This study shows the important role that antisocial behavior and deviant associations play for male adolescents in initiation and growth in use of marijuana and the importance of parental monitoring in protecting youth from such growth in use. A major finding was the importance of peer substance use as a dynamic predictor of marijuana use across the high school years. The differences between predictors of initiation and quantity of marijuana use and the dynamic nature of the association of peer substance use, parental monitoring, and antisocial behavior and deviant peer association with marijuana use would have been missed in a simpler model, as they have been in the literature to date. Taken all together, these findings indicate the importance of addressing these risk factors for male youth from at-risk backgrounds in prevention programs (Brown, et al., 2005; Spaeth, Weichold, Silbereisen, & Wiesner, 2010).
Acknowledgments
The project described was supported by awards from National Institutes of Health, U.S. Public Health Service (NIH) to Dr. Capaldi: Award Number 1R01AA018669 (Understanding Alcohol Use over Time in Early Mid-Adulthood for At-Risk Men) from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA); R01 DA 015485 (Adjustment Problems and Substance Use in Three Generations) from the National Institute of Drug Abuse (NIDA); and HD 46364 (Risk for Dysfunctional Relationships in Young Adults) from the National Institute of Child Health and Development (NICHD). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH, NIDA, NIAAA, or NICHD. We thank Jane Wilson and the data collection staff for their commitment to high-quality data and Sally Schwader and David Kerr for editorial assistance.
Contributor Information
Isaac J. Washburn, Oregon Social Learning Center, 10 Shelton McMurphey Blvd, Eugene, OR 97401, isaacw@oslc.org, Phone 541-485-2711 FAX 541-485-7087
Deborah M. Capaldi, Oregon Social Learning Center, 10 Shelton McMurphey Blvd, Eugene, OR 97401, deborahc@oslc.org, Phone 541-485-2711 FAX 541-485-7087
References
- Achenbach TM. Manual for Teacher's Report Form and 1991 profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychology; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM, Edelbrock C. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist and Revised Child Behavior Profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Arseneault L, Cannon M, Witton J, Murray RM. Causal association between cannabis and psychosis: Examination of the evidence. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2004;184(2):110–117. doi: 10.1192/bjp.184.2.110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bachman JG, Johnston LD, O'Malley PM. Monitoring the future: Questionnaire responses from the nation's high school seniors, 2010. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Bahr S, Hoffmann J, Yang X. Parental and peer influences on the risk of adolescent drug use. The Journal of Primary Prevention. 2005;26(6):529–551. doi: 10.1007/s10935-005-0014-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bailey JA, Hill KG, Oesterle S, Hawkins JD. Linking substance use and problem behavior across three generations. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2006;34:263–282. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006-9033-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blozis SA, Feldman B, Conger RD. Adolescent alcohol use and adult alcohol disorders: A two-part random-effects model with diagnostic outcomes. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2007;88:S85–S96. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bossong MG, Niesink RJM. Adolescent brain maturation, the endogenous cannabinoid system and the neurobiology of cannabis-induced schizophrenia. Progress in Neurobiology. 2010;92:370–385. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2010.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brody GH. Parental monitoring: Action and reaction. In: Crouter AC, Booth A, editors. Children's influence on family dynamics: The neglected side of family relationships. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2003. pp. 163–169. [Google Scholar]
- Broman CL. The longitudinal impact of adolescent drug use on socioeconomic outcomes in young adulthood. Journal of Child and Adolescent Substance Abuse. 2009;18:131–143. [Google Scholar]
- Brook JS, Whiteman M, Finch SJ, Morojele NK, Cohen P. Individual latent growth curves in the development of marijuana use from childhood to young adulthood. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2000;23(5):451–464. doi: 10.1023/a:1005573031757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown EC, Catalano RF, Fleming CB, Haggerty KP, Abbott RD. Adolescent substance use outcomes in the Raising Healthy Children project: A two-part latent growth curve analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2005;73:699–710. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.4.699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capaldi DM. Co-occurrence of conduct problems and depressive symptoms in early adolescent boys: II. A 2-year follow-up at Grade 8. Development and Psychopathology. 1992;4:125–144. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499001959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capaldi DM. Parental monitoring: A person-environment interaction perspective on this key parenting skill. In: Crouter AC, Booth A, editors. Children's influence on family dynamics: The neglected side of family relationships. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2003. pp. 171–179. [Google Scholar]
- Capaldi DM, Chamberlain P, Fetrow RA, Wilson JE. Conducting ecologically valid prevention research: Recruiting and retaining a "whole village" in multimethod, multiagent studies. American Journal of Community Psychology. 1997;25:471–492. doi: 10.1023/a:1024607605690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capaldi DM, Crosby L, Stoolmiller M. Predicting the timing of first sexual intercourse for at-risk adolescent males. Child Development. 1996;67:344–359. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capaldi DM, Patterson GR. Psychometric properties of fourteen latent constructs from the Oregon Youth Study. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Capaldi DM, Shortt JW, Kim HK. A life span developmental systems perspective on aggression toward a partner. In: Pinsof W, Lebow J, editors. Family psychology: The art of the science. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press; 2005. pp. 141–167. [Google Scholar]
- Capaldi DM, Stoolmiller M, Kim HK, Yoerger K. Growth in alcohol use in at-risk adolescent boys: Two-part random effects prediction models. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2009;105:109–117. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.06.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Creemers HE, Dijkstra JK, Vollebergh WAM, Ormel J, Verhulst FC, Huizink AC. Predicting life-time and regular cannabis use during adolescence: The roles of temperament and peer substance use: The TRAILS Study. Addiction. 2010;105:699–708. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02819.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cronbach LS. Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika. 1951;16:297–334. [Google Scholar]
- D'agostino RB, Belanger A, D'Agostino RB., Jr A suggestion for using powerful and informative tests of normality. American Statistician. 1990;44:316–321. [Google Scholar]
- D'Amico EJ, McCarthy DM. Escalation and initiation of younger adolescents' substance use: The impact of perceived peer use. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2006;39:481–487. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2006.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Degenhardt L, Hall W, Lynskey M. Exploring the association between cannabis use and depression. Addiction. 2003;98:1493–1504. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2003.00437.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dekovi M. Risk and protective factors in the development of problem behavior during adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 1999;28:667–685. [Google Scholar]
- Dembo R, Wareham J, Greenbaum PE, Childs K, Schmeidler J. Marijuana use among juvenile arrestees: A two-part growth model analysis. Journal of Child and Adolescent Substance Abuse. 2009;18:19. [Google Scholar]
- Dishion TJ, Capaldi DM. Peer Involvement and Social Skills Questionaire. Eugene OR: Oregon Social Learning Center; 1985. Unpublished Instrument. [Google Scholar]
- Dishion TJ, Capaldi DM, Spracklen KM, Li F. Peer ecology of male adolescent drug use. Development and Psychopathology. 1995;7:803–824. [Google Scholar]
- Dishion TJ, Capaldi DM, Yoerger K. Middle childhood antecedents to progressions in male adolescent substance use: An ecological analysis of risk and protection. Journal of Adolescent Research. 1999;14(2):175–205. [Google Scholar]
- Dishion TJ, McMahon RJ. Parental monitoring and the prevention of child and adolescent problem behavior: A conceptual and empirical formulation. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 1998;1:61–75. doi: 10.1023/a:1021800432380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dishion TJ, Owen LD. A longitudinal analysis of friendships and substance use: Bidirectional influence from adolescence to adulthood. Developmental Psychology. 2002;38:480–491. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.38.4.480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dishion TJ, Patterson GR. The development and ecology of antisocial behavior in children and adolescents. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ, editors. Developmental psychopathology: Risk, disorder, and adaptation. Vol. 3. New York: Wiley; 2006. pp. 503–541. [Google Scholar]
- Duncan TE, Tildesley E, Duncan SC, Hops H. The consistency of family and peer influences on the development of substance use in adolescence. Addiction. 1995;90:1647–1660. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1995.901216477.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engels JM, Diehr P. Imputation of missing longitudinal data: a comparison of methods. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2003;56:968–976. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(03)00170-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flay BR, Hu FB, Siddiqui O, Day LE, Hedeker D, Petraitis J, Sussman S. Differential influence of parental smoking and friends' smoking on adolescent initiation and escalation and smoking. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1994;35:248–265. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleming CB, Mason WA, Mazza JJ, Abbott RD, Catalano RF. Latent growth modeling of the relationship between depressive symptoms and substance use during adolescence. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2008;22:186–197. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.22.2.186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flory K, Lynam D, Milich R, Leukefeld C, Clayton R. Early adolescent through young adult alcohol and marijuana use trajectories: Early predictors, young adult outcomes, and predictive utility. Development and Psychopathology. 2004;16:193–213. doi: 10.1017/s0954579404044475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnston LD, O'Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the future national results on adolescent drug use: Overview of key findings 2009. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse; 2010. (NIH Publication No. 10-7583 ed.) [Google Scholar]
- Kandel DB, Griesler PC, Lee G, Davies M, Schaffran C. Parental influences on adolescent marijuana use and the baby boom generation: Findings from the 1979 – 1996 National Household Surveys on Drug Abuse. Rockville, MD: Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, Office of Applied Studies; 2001. (DHHS Publication No. (SMA) 01-3531, Analytic Series: A-13) [Google Scholar]
- Kellam SG, Rebok GW, Mayer LS, Ialongo N, Kalodner CR. Depressive symptoms over first grade and their response to a developmental epidemiologically based preventive trial aimed at improving achievement. Developmental Psychopathology. 1994;6:463–481. [Google Scholar]
- Kerr M, Stattin H. Parenting of adolescents: Action or reaction? In: Crouter AC, Booth A, editors. Children's influence on family dynamics: The neglected side of family relationships. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2003. pp. 121–151. [Google Scholar]
- Khantzian EJ. The self-medication hypothesis of substance use disorders: A reconsideration and recent applications. Harvard Review of Psychiatry. 1997;4(5):231–244. doi: 10.3109/10673229709030550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiesner J, Poulin F, Dishion TJ. Adolescent substance use with friends: Moderating and mediating effects of parental monitoring and peer activity contexts. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 2010;56(4):529–556. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2010.0002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirisci L, Mezzich AC, Reynolds M, Tarter RE, Aytaclar S. Prospective study of the association between neurobehavior disinhibition and peer environment on illegal drug use in boys and girls. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 2009;35(3):145–150. doi: 10.1080/00952990902825405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lac A, Crano WD. Monitoring matters. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2009;4(6):578. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01166.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee C, Mun EY, White HR, Simon P. Substance use trajectories of black and white young men from adolescence to emerging adulthood: A two-part growth curve analysis. Journal of Ethnicity in Substance Abuse. 2010;9(4):301–319. doi: 10.1080/15332640.2010.522898. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee JO, Hill KG, Guttmannova K, Bailey JA, Hartigan LA, Hawkins JD, Catalano RF. The effects of general and alcohol-specific peer factors in adolescence on trajectories of alcohol abuse disorder symptoms from 21 to 33 years. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2011;121:213–219. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.08.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li C, Pentz MA, Chou CP. Parental substance use as a modifier of adolescent substance use risk. Addiction. 2002;97:1537–1550. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00238.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martins SS, Storr CL, Alexandre PK, Chilcoat HD. Adolescent ecstasy and other drug use in the National Survey of Parents and Youth: The role of sensation-seeking, parental monitoring and peer's drug use. Addictive Behaviors. 2008;33:919–933. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mauricio AM, Little M, Chassin L, Knight GP, Piquero AR, Losoya SH, Vargas-Chanes D. Juvenile offenders’ alcohol and marijuana trajectories: Risk and protective factor effects in the context of time in a supervised facility. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2009;38(3):440–453. doi: 10.1007/s10964-008-9324-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore THM, Zammit S, Lingford-Hughes A, Barnes TRE, Jones PB, Burke M, Lewis G. Cannabis use and risk of psychotic or affective mental health outcomes: A systematic review. The Lancet. 2007;370(9584):319–328. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)61162-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user's guide. 6th ed. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 1998–2010. [Google Scholar]
- Olsen MK, Schafer JL. A two-part random-effects model for semicontinuous longitudinal data. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 2001;96(454):730–745. [Google Scholar]
- Oregon Social Learning Center. Unpublished instruments. 1982–2012 Available from http://www.oslc.org/unpublished_oslc_instruments.pdf.
- Patterson GR, Reid JB, Dishion TJ. A social learning approach: Vol. 4. Antisocial boys. Eugene, OR: Castalia; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Podsakoff PM, MacKenzie SB, Lee JY, Podsakoff NP. Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology. 2003;88(5):879. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radloff L. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1(3):385–401. [Google Scholar]
- Reinherz HZ, Giaconia RM, Hauf AMC, Wasserman MS, Paradis AD. General and specific childhood risk factors for depression and drug disorders by early adulthood. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2000;39:223–231. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200002000-00023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snyder J. Reinforcement and coercion mechanisms in the development of antisocial behavior: Peer relationships. In: Reid JB, Patterson GR, Snyder J, editors. Antisocial behavior in children and adolescents: A developmental analysis and model for intervention. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2002. pp. 101–122. [Google Scholar]
- Spaeth M, Weichold K, Silbereisen RK, Wiesner M. Examining the differential effectiveness of a life skills program (IPSY) on alcohol use trajectories in early adolescence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2010;78:334. doi: 10.1037/a0019550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Rockville, MD: Office of Applied Studies; 2010. Results from the 2009 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: Volume I. Summary of national findings (NSDUH Series H-38A, HHS Publication No. SMA 10-4586 Findings) [Google Scholar]
- Tarter RE, Kirisci L, Ridenour T, Vanyukov M. Prediction of cannabis use disorder between childhood and young adulthood using the Child Behavior Checklist. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2008;30:272–278. [Google Scholar]
- Walker HM, McConnell S. Walker-McConnell scale of social competence and school adjustment. Austin, TX: PRO ED Incorporation; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Windle M, Wiesner M. Trajectories of marijuana use from adolescence to young adulthood: Predictors and outcomes. Development and Psychopathology. 2004;16:1007–1027. doi: 10.1017/s0954579404040118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization. Programme on substance abuse. Cannabis: A health perspective and research agenda. Geneva: Switzerland; 1997. [Google Scholar]